SUMMARY
Neural circuits often exhibit sequences of activity, but the contribution of local networks to their generation remains unclear. In the zebra finch, song-related premotor sequences within HVC may result from some combination of local connectivity and long-range thalamic inputs from nucleus uvaeformis (Uva). Because lesions to either structure abolish song, we examine ‘sleep replay’ using high-density recording methods to reconstruct precise song-related events. Replay activity persists after the upstream nucleus interfacialis of the nidopallium is lesioned and slows when HVC is cooled, demonstrating that HVC provides temporal structure for these events. To further gauge the importance of intra-HVC connectivity for shaping network dynamics, we lesion Uva during sleep and find that residual replay sequences can span syllable boundaries, supporting a model in which HVC can propagate sequences throughout the duration of the song. Our results highlight the power of studying offline activity for studying behaviorally relevant circuit organization.
Keywords: birdsong, sequences, network, vocal production, zebra finch, motor control, circuit models
Graphical Abstract
eTOC:
Elmaleh et al. use high density recordings to measure the activity of the song control pathway in the zebra finch. They precisely decode snippets of song-related activity during sleep and leverage these ‘offline’ dynamics to test three models of network connectivity responsible for sequence generation.
INTRODUCTION
Neuronal sequences have been proposed to perform important functions in many brain regions, such as navigation (Skaggs et al., 1996), time generation (MacDonald et al., 2011; Pastalkova et al., 2008), decision making (Harvey et al., 2012; Schmitt et al., 2017), and skilled movement (Peters et al., 2014). The zebra finch has emerged as an excellent model system for studying the mechanisms underlying neural sequences that drive a well-defined complex behavior (Hahnloser et al., 2002). Through months of intensive practice (Tchernichovski et al., 2001), male zebra finches learn to produce a highly stereotyped courtship song, consisting of a repeated series of 2–7 distinct vocal elements called syllables. During singing, a sparse sequence of activity is generated within the forebrain nucleus HVC (proper name) in which each premotor neuron produces a single high-frequency burst of spikes (duration: ~10ms) (Hahnloser et al., 2002; Kozhevnikov and Fee, 2007); as a population, these neurons form a temporally precise (jitter with respect to song: < 1 ms) neural sequence tiling the length of the song (Egger et al., 2020; Lynch et al., 2016; Picardo et al., 2016) (Figure 1A). HVC activity is then sent to the robust nucleus of the arcopallium (RA) and ultimately to motor neurons and song-production musculature (Wild, 1993) (Figure 1B).
Previous electrophysiological (Kosche et al., 2015; Mooney and Prather, 2005) and ultrastructural (Kornfeld et al., 2017) work has demonstrated that premotor HVC neurons form synapses with each other, and these local axon collaterals have been proposed to act as a means of propagating activity by sequentially activating premotor neurons during song (Egger et al., 2020; Fee et al., 2004; Long et al., 2010). In addition, local circuit GABAergic interneurons (Scotto-Lomassese et al., 2007; Wild et al., 2005) provide inhibition that may also contribute to premotor patterning (Gibb et al., 2009; Kosche et al., 2015; Markowitz et al., 2015).
Along with this intrinsic circuitry, HVC receives input from a multi-region feedback loop dedicated to song production. Feedback signals must pass through the vocal thalamic nucleus uvaeformis (Uva) that projects both directly to HVC and indirectly via a cortical sensorimotor region known as the nucleus interfacialis of the nidopallium (NIf) (Figure 1B) (Coleman and Mooney, 2004; Hahnloser and Fee, 2007; Hahnloser et al., 2008; Reinke and Wild, 1998; Striedter and Vu, 1998). Together, NIf and Uva provide the two main sources of external input to HVC. While NIf has been demonstrated to be important for song learning in juvenile birds (Mackevicius et al., 2020; Roberts et al., 2012; Zhao et al., 2019), it does not play a critical role in adult song production; complete lesions of NIf only temporarily perturb singing (Cardin et al., 2005; Otchy et al., 2015). In contrast, lesions to either HVC or Uva can permanently abolish song production (Coleman and Vu, 2005; Danish et al., 2017; Nottebohm et al., 1976; Williams and Vicario, 1993). A clear understanding of how local connectivity and external inputs (e.g., from Uva) work together to enable sequence generation and propagation in HVC remains elusive (Andalman et al., 2011; Galvis et al., 2017; Hamaguchi et al., 2016; Long et al., 2010). A similar barrier also exists in many other systems, where elimination of either cortical or thalamic activity disrupts both behavior and circuit dynamics in the intact region (Bolkan et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2017; Sauerbrei et al., 2020; Schmitt et al., 2017).
There are three existing classes of models for how local circuitry and long-range inputs contribute to the premotor sequence observed during song, with the key difference being the timescale represented within the HVC microcircuit. In the distributed model (Figure 1C), sequence progression is critically reliant upon moment-to-moment feedback through Uva (Hamaguchi et al., 2016). Here, local HVC circuitry is not sufficient to advance the sequence; thus, the relevant timescale produced within HVC is limited to the duration of a premotor burst (~ 10 ms). The remaining models rely on intrinsic HVC connections to propagate sequential activity (Figures 1D and 1E). In the subnetwork model (Figure 1D), HVC is composed of multiple discrete subcircuits that encode individual song elements (~ 100–200 ms) (Glaze and Troyer, 2006; Long and Fee, 2008; Wang et al., 2008). In this view, HVC lacks the local connectivity to link the subnetworks, a role performed by extrinsic influences, likely from the feedback loop (Andalman et al., 2011). A subnetwork organization based on syllables is consistent with previous behavioral perturbations that abolish singing at syllable boundaries rather than at arbitrary moments during the song (Cynx, 1990; Galvis et al., 2017). In contrast, the HVC microcircuit may have sufficient information in its intrinsic connections to form a single continuous network sequence (~ 1 s) representing a concatenation of each song element within the finch’s repertoire (continuous model, Figure 1E).
To address these models, we first performed high-density recordings within the zebra finch song control pathway during song performance. We found that Uva lesions not only abolished singing (Coleman and Vu, 2005; Danish et al., 2017; Williams and Vicario, 1993) but also eliminated all song-related activity, preventing our ability to study HVC circuit structure. We therefore turned our attention to previously observed ‘replay’ events occurring during sleep (Dave and Margoliash, 2000; Hahnloser et al., 2006; Long et al., 2010) as a means of testing models of HVC circuit organization outside of the context of singing. We decoded the activity of neural ensembles within the song control pathway, revealing precise song-related replay fragments. This replay persisted in the absence of NIf input and was dilated in time when we applied a cooling probe to HVC, highlighting the importance of HVC for providing temporal structure for these events. We then lesioned Uva during sleep and found that song-related sequences within HVC remained despite catastrophic changes to singing on the following day. These results support the notion that the HVC microcircuit has sufficient connectivity to sustain sequences in the absence of thalamic feedback, highlighting the value of offline activity for determining the functional structure of neural circuits.
RESULTS
Thalamic lesions eliminate behaviorally relevant network activity
Existing models of HVC circuit organization (Figure 1C-E) differ regarding the role of the polysynaptic feedback loop in sequence production. For instance, thalamic inputs from Uva driven through the feedback loop may be necessary to link individual burst events in HVC (e.g., Figure 1C) or isolated syllable-length sequences (e.g., Figure 1D). We evaluated these possibilities by lesioning Uva and examining the effect of this manipulation on vocal behavior and neural activity in the song system (Schmidt et al., 2004) using high-density silicon probes (Egger et al., 2020). Rather than record sparse premotor activity in HVC, we placed our electrodes into the more densely firing downstream structure RA (Hahnloser et al., 2002; Kozhevnikov and Fee, 2007; Leonardo and Fee, 2005; Yu and Margoliash, 1996) (Figure 1B and 2A-C). Because RA song-related bursts are largely driven by HVC (Figure 2A) (Fee et al., 2004), we used RA activity as an efficient means of monitoring the HVC premotor network.
We found that Uva lesions had a profound impact on both behavior and motor-related activity (Figures 2D-M). In agreement with previous studies (Coleman and Vu, 2005; Danish et al., 2017; Williams and Vicario, 1993), birds were unable to produce normal song following Uva ablation (Figure 2G), though introductory notes – distinct vocalizations which precede song – were considerably less affected (Figures 2F-I). Importantly, song-related bursting events (e.g., Figure 2F) were eliminated following Uva lesions (e.g., Figure 2G, 2J and 2K, proportion of burst spikes: 0.86 ± 0.15 control; 0.02 ± 0.04 lesion). Instead, RA neurons exhibited regular firing patterns typically found outside the context of vocalization (Figure 2F) (Leonardo and Fee, 2005; Yu and Margoliash, 1996), which are likely the result of intrinsic cellular properties (Mooney, 1992). Unlike burst events, the tonic firing patterns lacked trial-to-trial regularity across vocalizations (control: r = 0.66 ± 0.20; Uva lesion: r = 0.07 ± 0.09) (Figures 2H, 2I, 2L and 2M). These data indicate that, in addition to strongly perturbing singing behavior, lesions to the motor thalamic nucleus Uva result in a complete disappearance of patterned premotor activity during vocal production, rendering us unable to test the previously introduced models regarding the functional organization of HVC (Figure 1C-E).
Robust song-related replay occurs during sleep
To assess the contribution of local circuit connectivity to the propagation of neural sequences, we needed an assay in which the structure of behavior-related network activity could be examined following thalamic ablation. The lack of such dynamics during vocalization prompted us to consider sleep activity, which has been shown to display episodes of burst firing in single neurons resembling patterns observed during singing in the zebra finch (Dave and Margoliash, 2000; Hahnloser et al., 2002; Long et al., 2010). Although singing and sleep are vastly different states whose dynamics are strongly influenced by changes in neuromodulatory tone (Cardin and Schmidt, 2004; Schmidt and Konishi, 1998), the stability of singing (Deregnaucourt et al., 2005; Lombardino and Nottebohm, 2000) and related neural activity (Katlowitz et al., 2018; Rauske et al., 2010) from one day to the next in the adult zebra finch provides the foundation to leverage sleep as a means of examining network organization within the song control pathway.
We used our multielectrode probe to measure activity during singing (Figures 3A and 3B) and sleep (Figures 3C and 3D) in both HVC and RA. Indeed, both regions exhibited population activity during sleep highly resembling that observed during song, supporting the notion that song-related replay is a network-wide event (Fee et al., 2004; Hahnloser and Fee, 2007). We found that RA recordings provided a more precise means of estimating the position of sleep replay events with respect to song (3.2 ± 1.1 ms) than those in HVC (62.5 ± 52.8 ms) (see Methods; Figure 3E) despite having recorded a similar number of neurons in each structure (RA: 52.1 ± 14.8 cells/bird, 8 birds; HVC projection neurons: 49.6 ± 17.6 cells/bird, 5 birds). Distinguishing between our models (Figures 1C-E) requires a highly precise measurement of replayed song activity in the temporal domain (~ 10 ms, or roughly the duration of an HVC burst). We therefore used RA recordings to decode activity in the zebra finch motor pathway during sleep.
We directly compared the activity of RA neurons in 4 birds during sleep and singing by recording for 15.3 ± 2.2 hours, including a continuous dark period (9.7 ± 0.9 hours) when birds spent the majority of time sleeping, and RA exhibited typical ‘sleep bursts’ (Dave and Margoliash, 2000; Hahnloser et al., 2006). We first tested whether individual neurons could be recorded continuously during this period using our high-density recording approach. In addition to our spike sorting protocol (see Methods), the unique electrophysiological properties of RA neurons provided further evidence for recording stability. Because each RA neuron exhibits its own invariant pattern of bursting during vocalization (or temporal ‘barcode’) (e.g., Figure S1) (Leonardo and Fee, 2005; Yu and Margoliash, 1996), we could directly compare song-related activity before and after the sleep session (Rauske et al., 2010). In all cases, we found the activity of individual RA neurons was preserved (Figure S1), confirming that neurons could be stably recorded throughout the duration of our experiment.
To precisely decode the song content of network activity during sleep, we used a template-matching strategy in which candidate ‘replay’ sleep epochs (Figure 3D, arrows) – defined by the presence of high-frequency bursts (see Methods) – were discretized into 6 ms bins and compared to population activity recorded during song (Figure S2). Often, sequential bins recorded during sleep closely resembled sequential bins of the song template, allowing us to detect and analyze population-wide sleep replay events (Figures 3F, 3G and S2, see Methods). Population activity during replay events was highly correlated with that observed during song (median: 0.74, interquartile range: 0.65 – 0.83), approaching the degree of regularity observed across different song renditions (median: 0.82, interquartile range: 0.74 – 0.90) and considerably greater than expected by chance (median: 0.00, interquartile range: −0.11 – 0.12) (n = 7 birds) (Figure S2K). A direct comparison of template correlation values reveals that sleep events have 89.9 ± 2.8% of the precision inherent in song events (Figure S2L). Single replay events occurred with a frequency of 11.3 ± 7.9 events/minute (Figure 3H). These events rarely spanned the entire length of the song (range: 0.51 – 0.95 sec) and instead formed shorter sequence fragments (interquartile range: 56 – 140 ms) (Figure 3I) (n = 4 birds).
We observed that the network progressed forward through the song sequence during sleep at nearly the same speed as it does during song itself (replay event slope: 0.96 ± 0.08, n = 26,215 events from 4 birds) (Figure 3J), which is in contrast to the temporally compressed (slope: > 1) and reversed (slope: < 0) replay events observed in the rodent hippocampus (Diba and Buzsaki, 2007; Lee and Wilson, 2002; Wilson and McNaughton, 1994) and consistent with the lack of temporal distortion of replay observed during REM sleep (Louie and Wilson, 2001). Unlike what occurs during song production where sequence start times are limited to syllable boundaries, we found that sleep replay events are initiated throughout the entirety of the song (Figure S3) (88.5 ± 7.7% of analyzed time bins, n = 4 birds). Taken together, these observations demonstrate that replay events in RA during sleep accurately reflect fragments of song-related neural activity, suggesting the circuit architecture responsible for controlling song patterning is engaged during this offline state.
HVC establishes temporal structure for sleep replay
Our study aims to use sleep activity to examine the contribution of local HVC circuit organization to sequence generation (Figure 1). To accomplish this goal, we must ensure that sleep replay in RA is driven by activity within HVC, rather than by dynamics originating within an upstream structure. Previous work used pharmacological inactivation and electrophysiological recordings to conclude that the upstream cortical nucleus NIf may be initiating sleep sequences within HVC (Hahnloser and Fee, 2007). To directly test the role of NIf in sleep replay, we performed excitotoxic lesions within that structure in 2 birds and confirmed the completeness of our manipulation with subsequent histological analysis (Figure S4A). Although the NIf lesions resulted in transient behavioral abnormalities (Figure S4B) (Cardin et al., 2005; Otchy et al., 2015), we found that robust replay (Figure S4C-G) remained, demonstrating that these dynamics do not require upstream inputs from NIf.
In addition to NIf, several other structures, such as the medial magnocellular nucleus of the anterior neostriatum (Foster et al., 1997; Nottebohm et al., 1976) and the nucleus avalanche (Akutagawa and Konishi, 2010) directly project to HVC and may shape sleep-related activity. To determine the collective contribution of all upstream nuclei to HVC sleep sequences, we measured the impact of mild HVC cooling on replay activity. Previous work has demonstrated that cooling axonal inputs conveying important sequential timing information in a downstream nucleus did not alter the speed of the resultant behavioral sequence (Long and Fee, 2008). Therefore, if a region upstream of HVC were acting as the pattern generator during sleep, we would not expect stretching of sleep-related dynamics upon simply cooling the axon targets within HVC. Alternatively, if the time scale of sleep replay is shaped by local HVC activity, then cooling that structure should measurably slow down replay events (Banerjee et al., 2021).
We distinguish between these possibilities by placing a Peltier device capable of producing graded temperature changes in HVC (ΔT range: 2.8 to 8.7∆C) while measuring sleep activity in RA (n = 5 birds) (Figure 4). The slope of detected replay events during the cooled periods decreased systematically as a function of HVC temperature (Figures 4A-C) with an average decrease in replay speed of 2.6 ± 1.8 %/°C (Figure 4D), a value consistent with the impact of both induced (Hamaguchi et al., 2016; Long and Fee, 2008) and naturally occurring (Aronov and Fee, 2012) temperature changes on singing behavior. We conclude that HVC dynamics set the tempo for RA replay events, further reinforcing the strategy of testing models of HVC network structure by measuring sleep replay in RA.
Sleep replay events do not require thalamic input
To directly determine the contribution of intrinsic HVC connectivity to sequence propagation, we compared the activity of 134 RA neurons (n = 3 birds) recorded before and after performing overnight electrolytic Uva lesions using a chronically implanted bipolar electrode (Figures 5A, 5B, S5 and S6). If inputs from Uva specifically, or feedback from the song production loop generally, are central to the moment-to-moment burst generation and sequence propagation in the song system (Fig 1C, distributed model), we would expect a significant decrease in sleep bursting and replay events after lesioning, as observed during singing (Figure 2). However, we found that the percentage of spikes in high-frequency bursts did not decrease postlesion (Figures 5A, S5 and S6) (prelesion: 7.5 ± 6.5%, postlesion: 9.7 ± 7.1%; Wilcoxon signed-rank test). To test whether replay was preserved following the Uva lesion, we returned to our template matching strategy (Figures 3 and S2) and measured the incidence of replay events. We found that the frequency (prelesion: 4.1 ± 3.2 events/min, postlesion: 5.4 ± 3.2 events/min; Wilcoxon signed-rank test) and duration (prelesion: 56.3 ± 20.1 ms, postlesion: 62.1 ± 34.0 ms; Wilcoxon rank-sum test) of detected events did not decrease following the lesion (Figures 5B, S5 and S6).
We next examined the fine structure of replay events in the absence of thalamic feedback to HVC by aligning the spiking activity of individual neurons during song and sleep (Figures 5C-F, see Methods). The temporal precision of postlesion sleep activity during replay, defined by similarity to song aligned activity, was consistent with prelesion levels (prelesion: 0.85 ± 0.09, postlesion: 0.82 ± 0.11) (Figure 5G), even though the same neurons no longer exhibited precisely timed bursts during vocalization (Figures 2 and 5H, prelesion: r = 0.66 ± 0.20, postlesion: r = 0.12 ± 0.19). Therefore, despite depriving the network of its thalamic feedback, HVC retained its ability to propagate sequences of song-related activity during sleep, indicating HVC does not require moment-to-moment thalamic input for sequence propagation as described in the distributed model (Figure 1C).
Replay events continue to cross syllable boundaries following thalamic lesion
The retention of patterned bursting activity in the absence of Uva input confirms a role for local HVC circuitry in sequence propagation, which is consistent with the remaining two proposed models. In the subnetwork model (Figure 1D), each vocal element (e.g., syllable) is represented by a distinct HVC circuit which is activated and linked through thalamic feedback (Andalman et al., 2011). This model predicts that following the Uva lesion, replay events should be unable to cross syllable boundaries (Figure 6A). On the other hand, the continuous model, in which the full sequence is reflected in the organization of the HVC circuit, predicts that replay events should not be constrained by the time scales of distinct behavioral units (Figure 6B).
To test these models, we delineated syllable onsets within the corresponding moments of the song template, accounting for the delay between neural activity and vocal onset (Fee et al., 2004) (see Methods). In the absence of thalamic input from Uva, individual replay events continued to cross these boundaries, as in the prelesion period (Figures 6C-D). We estimated the expected boundary crossings according to the subnetwork model by shuffling the syllable order of song templates, which accounts for those crossings that would be expected by chance if independent subnetworks were responsible for each syllable (prelesion: 0.3 ± 0.5%, postlesion: 0.6 ± 0.5%) (Figures 6E and 6F). Instead, we found that replay events crossed syllable boundaries with a frequency much greater than the shuffled estimates (prelesion: 27.1 ± 9.6%, postlesion: 31.1 ± 17.8%) (Figures 6E and 6F). The proportion of crossings for each syllable boundary in the prelesion and postlesion data sets did not differ (Wilcoxon rank-sum test: p = 0.70). The measured slope of the resultant linear regression (0.72, 95% CI [0.43 1.01]) was inconsistent with the subnetwork model (Figure 6G, predicted slope: 0). Instead, our findings support a synaptic architecture in which the HVC microcircuit can produce a single continuous sequence (Figure 6G, predicted slope: 1) representing all song elements (Figure 1E).
DISCUSSION
In this study, we examined the circuit organization of a behaviorally relevant premotor structure underlying song production in the adult zebra finch. To determine the contributions of local and long-range connections to observed dynamics, we lesioned a thalamic node in a feedback pathway critical for song production. In this ‘open loop’ configuration, both behavior and the associated neural activity were severely compromised. We therefore turned to network reactivation during sleep and observed that local HVC circuit connectivity contains sufficient information to propagate throughout the entirety of the song sequence. Our approach could be instructive for other systems in which the reactivation of behaviorally relevant neural ensembles has been observed during offline states, such as sleep (Gardner et al., 2019; Lee and Wilson, 2002; Trettel et al., 2019; Wilson and McNaughton, 1994) or quiet wakefulness (Diba and Buzsaki, 2007; Foster and Wilson, 2006). Using this strategy, offline circuit examination may become a broadly applicable approach for understanding complex network function, especially in cases where the specific contributions of interdependent structures remain ambiguous (Bolkan et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2017; Sauerbrei et al., 2020; Schmitt et al., 2017).
Our results highlight the importance of local HVC circuit connectivity for sequence generation. Previous work examining the fine structure of HVC network activity suggested a ‘polychronous’ architecture (Egger et al., 2020) in which sequentially active HVC premotor neurons are linked through local axon collaterals (Kornfeld et al., 2017), likely with an additional contribution from HVC inhibitory interneurons (Gibb et al., 2009; Kosche et al., 2015; Markowitz et al., 2015; Wild et al., 2005). We advance this model by indicating that premotor synaptic architecture is capable of continuously representing the entire syllable repertoire of the finch (Figures 1E and 6B). We find that the sequence sustaining circuitry within HVC is not subdivided into dedicated networks related to individual vocal elements, despite the fact that song syllables are functionally (Andalman et al., 2011; Cynx, 1990; Glaze and Troyer, 2006) and developmentally chunked (Lipkind et al., 2017; Okubo et al., 2015). Therefore, in the zebra finch, identified behavioral components (Wiltschko et al., 2015) are not overtly reflected within the functional organization of this critical premotor circuit.
Circuitry within HVC appears to be differentially engaged across singing and sleep. Activity occurring during sleep can be initiated at nearly every time point along the sequence (Figure S3), forming fragmented events. Our observation that sleep replay persists in the absence of NIf raises important questions regarding the factors that initiate these sleep sequences, which may be impacted by sleep-related changes in neuromodulatory tone (Cardin and Schmidt, 2004). In contrast, during singing, the sequence appears to be ballistically engaged at the level of syllables (Cynx, 1990), initiated at a small number of points corresponding to the onsets of vocal elements (Glaze and Troyer, 2006). Although, the mechanisms underlying this process remain unknown, we propose that Uva may be central to this function; the expression of individual behaviors (i.e., syllables) may arise through precisely timed Uva activity (i.e., at syllable onsets) (Danish et al., 2017) and/or through a dedicated thalamocortical pathway onto a subset of HVC premotor neurons capable of initiating the sequences for each vocal element (Andalman et al., 2011; Schmidt, 2003).
The necessity for thalamic input at the level of the syllable may reflect specific requirements for expert song performance. For example, studies using lesions (Williams et al., 1992), cooling (Long and Fee, 2008), and stimulation (Wang et al., 2008) have indicated that neural activity across hemispheres is required for proper song production. In the zebra finch, however, most song control nuclei on the left and right hemispheres (including HVC, RA, NIf, and Uva) do not directly communicate with each other (Schmidt et al., 2004). We therefore propose that bilaterally coordinated brainstem inputs working through Uva can align the activity of otherwise independent song production systems, thereby ensuring that both hemispheres engage the same part of the sequence (Schmidt, 2003). The requirement of hemispheric coordination during vocal performance could account for the catastrophic impact of Uva lesions on song and the modest (or nonexistent) effects of the same manipulation on sleep when the hemispheres need not be synchronized (Young et al., 2017).
Our results make specific predictions concerning the organization of motor networks in other systems. Although the syntax of the zebra finch is comparatively simple (i.e., invariant syllable order across renditions), we propose a similar network organization in other songbird species that exhibit more variable vocal behavior (Cohen et al., 2020; Fujimoto et al., 2011), with an upstream region playing an important role in determining the order of syllables (Hosino and Okanoya, 2000). While the underlying substrate may be more complex than the continuous representation we observe in the zebra finch, the initiation of discrete local motor programs could be more universal. Future work can determine whether such thalamic engagement of mammalian cortical regions can dynamically coordinate neural assemblies subserving multiple tasks (Dacre et al., 2021; Harvey et al., 2012; Schmitt et al., 2017).
METHODS
RESOURCE AVAILABILITY
Lead contact
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Michael Long (mlong@med.nyu.edu).
Materials availability
This study did not generate new unique reagents.
Data and Code availability
All code necessary to run template matching as described here can be found at <https://github.com/LongLabGit/TemplateMatching.git> (DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.5500262). The data sets generated during this study and any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper are available upon request from the Lead Contact.
EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS
We used adult (> 90 days posthatch) male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) that were obtained from an outside breeder and maintained in a temperature- and humidity-controlled environment with a 12/12 hr light/dark schedule. All animal maintenance and experimental procedures were performed according to the guidelines established by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the New York University Langone Medical Center.
METHOD DETAILS
Surgical procedures
All surgical procedures were performed under isoflurane anesthesia (1%–3% in oxygen) following established guidelines. Details pertaining to the use of high-density silicon probes for chronic recordings have been described elsewhere (Egger et al., 2020). Briefly, for all chronic recordings, silicon probes were implanted directly into the target area using the bifurcation of the sagittal sinus as the stereotaxic origin. For recordings in the robust nucleus of the archipallium (RA) the craniotomies were 2.35 mm lateral and 0.1 mm posterior; for recordings in HVC, the craniotomies were 2.25 mm lateral and 0.25 mm anterior. The ground wire was inserted between the skull and the dura above the cerebellum contralateral to the probe implantation. Silicon elastomer (Kwik-Cast, WPI) was applied to the craniotomy once the target region was successfully identified on all channels (depth: ~2.5–3 mm for RA and 0.5–0.8 mm for HVC) using the Intan Recording system (RHD USB interphase evaluation board or RHD Recording Controller).
For both acute and chronic lesions to Uvaeformis (Uva), coordinates varied from 1.5–1.6 mm lateral, 3.0–3.6 mm anterior and 5.1–5.4 mm ventral. An ipsilateral craniotomy above HVC centered at 2.3 mm lateral and 0.25 mm anterior of the superior sagittal sinus were made for the injection of dextran Alexa Fluor 488 to retrogradely label projections from Uva to HVC allowing for post hoc assessment of lesion success. HVC and Uva craniotomies were done at different head angles: beak bar positioned 45 degrees down from horizontal and 5 degrees up from horizontal for HVC and Uva respectively. Acute lesions were made using custom made bipolar stainless steel electrodes lowered into the Uva craniotomy 24–48 h prior to probe implantation. Both craniotomies were covered with Kwik-Cast (WPI), and the behavioral effects of the lesion were monitored until probe implantation.
For chronic Uva lesions, a platinum-iridium or tungsten search electrode (0.5 MOhm, MicroProbes) was used under isoflurane anesthesia to precisely locate Uva through antidromic activation of HVC-projecting Uva neurons. Next, a custom-built bipolar electrode, composed of a platinum-iridium electrode (0.5 MOhm, MicroProbes) and an insulated stainless steel wire (0.002”, California Fine Wire) was lowered into Uva and secured with dental acrylic. The uninsulated tip of the stainless steel wire and the tip of the platinum-iridium electrode were ~ 400 μm apart. Birds recovered from implantation surgery and were monitored to ensure no disruptions to song production following bipolar electrode implantation. Lesions were performed in the middle of the dark period following silicon probe implantation. For both acute and chronic lesion approaches the implantation of silicon probes was carried out as described above, and electrolytic lesions were achieved with 30 second biphasic pulses (acute: 50 μA, 6 total pulses; chronic: 50–500 μA, 6–10 total pulses).
NIf lesions were performed using the excitotoxin N-methyl-DL-aspartic acid (NMA, 4%), as described previously (Otchy et al., 2015). The extent and exact position of NIf was mapped using a 0.5 MΩ tungsten electrode (MicroProbes, WE30030.5B3) to measure antidromic responses evoked by a stainless steel bipolar electrode in HVC that was constructed from 50 µm Teflon coated stainless steel wire (California Fine Wire). NMA injections (12 sites, 30 nL each. 200 μm spacing) were then made along the anterior-posterior axis of NIf. Prior to application of NMA, dextran was injected into HVC (see Uva lesion protocol) to retrogradely label HVC-projecting NIf and Uva neurons. Following surgery, all vocalizations were recorded until song recovered to prelesion performance (Cardin et al., 2005; Otchy et al., 2015), at which time high-density silicon probes were implanted into RA.
Extracellular Recordings
Chronic recordings were performed using integrated 128-channel high-density silicon probes (Diagnostic Biochips, 128–5). Prior to surgery, integrated probes were mounted to a microdrive (NeuroNexus, d-XL) and a stainless steel ground wire (0.001” AM systems) was soldered to the reference of the headstage, which was held in place by a custom-made 3D printed enclosure (Formlabs). Within the hour following surgery, we began continuously recording neural activity from RA (Intan Recording Controller) and all vocalizations using an omnidirectional microphone (Audio Technica) and an analog preamplifier (Presonus). Free movement was enabled by an electrically assisted commutator (Doric Lenses). Female zebra finches were placed in an adjacent cage to elicit song. In a subset of experiments, electrolytic lesions were performed following 3–6 hours of sleep.
Focal Cooling
Cooling experiments required a preparatory surgery, during which a small stainless steel headplate was secured to the anterior-most position on the skull. A small craniotomy and durotomy were made for access to RA 2.3 mm lateral and 1.8 mm posterior to the superior sagittal sinus, and a large craniotomy 2 mm medial-lateral x 1.3 mm anterior-posterior centered on HVC was made to accommodate the cooling device as described previously (Long and Fee, 2008).
For the cooling experiments, birds were head-fixed and held in a foam restraint prior to recording. At the beginning of the recording session, a Peltier device (Custom Thermoelectric) was lowered onto the surface of the brain within the HVC craniotomy. This device was wired to a power source (Kepko) that allowed for careful control of applied current. A thin copper pouch whose temperature was maintained through flowing water was used as a heat sink (Long and Fee, 2008). Next, 128-channel acute high-density silicon probes (S. Masmanidis, UCLA; 128-K) (Yang et al., 2020) were lowered into the RA craniotomy. The temperature was varied in a block fashion (~1 hour per condition) with a return to baseline temperature between each cooling session. Following recording, a thermocouple probe (Omega) was inserted into HVC (depth: 0.5 mm) to measure the temperature changes resulting from each current level administered during the experiment.
Histology
After recording, birds were perfused with 4% PFA and probes were recovered. Brains were fixed overnight and sliced at 50–100 μm using a vibrating microtome (VT1200S, Leica). Slices were mounted (Vectashield) and imaged using a Zeiss Confocal microscope (LSM800).
DATA ANALYSIS
Recording neural activity during song
Custom MATLAB software was used to analyze song recordings and to align single unit data to behavior. To characterize song elements, the audio signal was bandpass filtered (low freq. = 500 Hz, high freq. = 8 kHz) and down sampled to 300 Hz to extract the amplitude envelope. This signal was then divided into three second segments; those segments with amplitude above of the noise floor for greater than 30 samples were assessed for vocal content. The start and stop times of all male vocalizations in these segments as well as the identity of the vocalization (i.e., syllable A, introductory note, call) were manually annotated.
For electrophysiological recordings, automated spike detection and sorting was carried out using Kilosort software (Pachitariu et al., 2016) and manual post-processing was performed using Phy (Rossant et al., 2016) as described previously (Egger et al., 2020). To confirm that we were holding the same neurons throughout our recording, in addition to amplitude and waveform stability (Phy), we compared singing activity in a subset of birds where song was recorded before (day 1) and after (day 2) sleep. For each bird, we calculated the Pearson correlation between the average song-related instantaneous firing rate on day 1 and day 2 for each neuron after aligning all song-related activity to the beginning and end of the song motif.
Both singing and sleep replay are associated with high frequency burst firing in the song production pathway. For each neuron, we calculated the percent of spikes within all song-related vocalizations belonging to a burst (instantaneous firing rate: > 100 Hz) within the window of vocal production after accounting for a 15 ms motor delay (Fee et al., 2004). Vocalizations defined as ‘song-related’ included both introductory notes and either song syllables (intact: control and chronic prelesion) or song attempts (postlesion: acute lesion and chronic postlesion) and excluded calls. To measure spiking precision during vocalizations, all spikes occurring within introductory notes were aligned to the warped start and stop times of each vocalization. The instantaneous firing rates for each aligned trial were smoothed with a 10 ms boxcar kernel and compared within condition (intact or postlesion) using the Pearson correlation. This firing rate threshold and smoothing procedure was used for all instantaneous firing rate measurements throughout the study.
To calculate temporal uncertainty, we compared song-related RA data collected as part of this study with a previously acquired HVC data set (Egger et al., 2020). The population activity during song was temporally subsampled every 2 ms and the probability of a given unique active neural ensemble was used to calculate entropy (I) (Leonardo and Fee, 2005); uncertainty was then defined as song length divided by 2I for each recording.
Template matching
A single template was chosen from the burst activity of the population of RA neurons during the performance of one song rendition and subdivided into bins (bin width: 6 ms, step size: 2 ms). The data were then further binarized; if a neuron was bursting within that bin, it was assigned a value of 1; no bursting was assigned a 0. Following the creation of the template, activity throughout the recording period was similarly binarized, segmented into bins, and assigned to the template bin it most resembles using Pearson correlation.
Sleep replay events were defined as fragments of sleep activity resembling significant portions of the song template using the following guidelines during the lights-off portion of the recording. Individual replay events were detected by traversing the two-dimensional space (template bin vs. recording time); a given bin assignment was joined with its neighbor if the absolute value of the distance between them was less than 4 ms, thus allowing for the detection of both forward and reverse replay events. Adjacent detected events were joined if the distance between them was less than 10 ms.
We calculated the slope of each event using a best fit linear approximation of the bin assignments within the detected event. Initial manual annotation of robust replay events suggested that most slope values were near 1. We therefore used outlier slope values (less than 0 and greater than 2) as a means of identifying spurious events. Using this metric, we found that replay events of sufficient length (i.e., 40 ms) very rarely exhibited an outlier slope value (Figure S2G), and we therefore established 40 ms as a minimum requirement for sleep replay events upon which we performed further analysis. We estimated the spectrogram of sleep replay events by assigning the portion of the song spectrogram corresponding to the template bin with the highest correlation value. For the cooling experiments, where no song template was available, we created a template from a period of robust bursting during a baseline temperature recording block, and slopes for the events detected during the cooled portions were normalized to the median slope of the detected baseline events.
To compare the similarity between song activity and sleep replay activity, we looked at the template matching results for detected sleep replay events as well as the non-template song repeats (i.e., the neural activity occurring during song renditions not used for the template). We compared all correlation scores assigned during non-template song repeats with those assigned during sleep events. We produced a shuffled distribution of correlation scores by comparing the neural activity during non-template song repeats and detected replay events to random portions of the song (rather than the winner-take-all approach used for event detection). We also performed a direct comparison in which we measured the correlation scores across sleep and song for each bin of our template, using the mean values for each bird to calculate a ratio (i.e., rsleep/rsong).
Sleep activity and replay measurements
Population bursting during sleep was measured as the percent of all spikes occurring within a burst (instantaneous firing rate: > 100Hz; bin width: 1 minute). To compare spiking precision within detected replay events pre- and postlesion, template matching was performed on neural data collected during awake singing trials. All spiking activity for a given neuron was aligned to all the detected events during song, prelesion replay, and postlesion replay. Average instantaneous firing rates for pre- and postlesion replay activity were compared to that of singing activity (Pearson correlation). To quantify potential differences in replay events before and after the lesion, we narrowed our analysis to the hour preceding the lesion (prelesion) and the second hour following the lesion (postlesion).
Syllable boundary crossings
All detected sleep replay events were assessed for boundary crossing by comparing the event’s template location with the corresponding position in song. A syllable was defined as the onset of the vocalization to the onset of the following syllable vocalization or the end of that vocalization for the last syllable in the song. Replay event starting times were normalized to their location within the starting syllable and stop times were adjusted to the position in the ending syllable. We also quantify the proportion of reconstructed replay events that cross syllable boundaries (e.g., sleep events initiated in syllable A that cross over into syllable B). To create replay events predicted by the subnetwork model, we shuffled the template such that the corresponding neural activity for each syllable remained intact, but that previously adjacent syllables were no longer continuous. For each bird, we generated either ten such templates meeting this criterion or all shuffled templates when fewer than ten were possible. Using the shuffled template bin assignments, events were detected and syllable boundary crossings were measured as above.
QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
All statistical details of experiments can be found in figure legends and the Results section, including the statistical tests used, exact value of n and what n represents (e.g., number of animals, number of cells, etc.). Values are reported as mean ± SD unless mentioned otherwise, and values are plotted as mean with error bars representing SD unless mentioned otherwise. Significance was defined at a level of 0.05. Normal distribution of data was not assumed. No data were excluded from analysis. Statistical calculations were performed using MATLAB R2017a.
Supplementary Material
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Antibodies | ||||
Bacterial and virus strains | ||||
Biological samples | ||||
Chemicals, peptides, and recombinant proteins | ||||
N-methyl-DL-aspartic acid (NMA, 4%) | AA Blocks | 17833–53-3 | ||
Critical commercial assays | ||||
Deposited data | ||||
Experimental models: Cell lines | ||||
Experimental models: Organisms/strains | ||||
Zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) | Magnolia Bird Farm, Anaheim, CA | N/A | ||
Oligonucleotides | ||||
Recombinant DNA | ||||
Software and algorithms | ||||
MATLAB | MathWorks | https://www.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html | ||
KiloSort spike sorting software | Pachitariu et al., 2016 | https://github.com/cortex-lab/KiloSort | ||
Sleep replay decoder (Template-matching tool) | This paper |
https://github.com/LongLabGit/TemplateMatching.git
DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.5500262 |
||
Other | ||||
High-density silicon probe (with integrated headstage) | Diagnostic Biochips | 128–5 integrated | ||
Chronic microdrive | Neuronexus | dDrive-xL | ||
Assisted Fiber-optic & Electric Rotary Joint | Doric Lenses | AHRJ-OE_FC_AD_12_HARW | ||
Omnetics cable adaptor | Doric Lenses | ADAPTER_HO12 | ||
Acquisition board | Intan Technologies | RHD Recording Controller (512 channels) | ||
Stimulus generator | A-M Systems | Model 2100 | ||
Omnidirectional microphone | Audio-Technica | AT803 | ||
Audio amplifier | Presonus | Studio Channel |
Highlights:
Thalamic lesions eliminate singing behavior and underlying network dynamics
High-density recordings during song enable precise decoding of sleep replay
Temporal dynamics during sleep are controlled by premotor nucleus HVC
Structure of sleep replay events highlights role of local HVC connectivity
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research was supported by NIH F31 NS116933–01 (M.E.), NIH R01 NS075044 (M.A.L.), and Simons Global Brain (M.A.L.). We thank Arkarup Banerjee, György Buzsáki, Dezhe Jin, Katherine Nagel, and members of the Long laboratory for comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. Ralph Peterson helped with data collection, and Abby Paulson provided technical assistance. We acknowledge helpful conversations with Robert Egger, Liam Paninski, Josh Glaser, and Erdem Varol.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Declaration of interests: The authors declare no competing interests.
REFERENCES
- Akutagawa E, and Konishi M (2010). New brain pathways found in the vocal control system of a songbird. J Comp Neurol 518, 3086–3100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andalman AS, Foerster JN, and Fee MS (2011). Control of vocal and respiratory patterns in birdsong: dissection of forebrain and brainstem mechanisms using temperature. PLoS One 6, e25461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aronov D, and Fee MS (2012). Natural changes in brain temperature underlie variations in song tempo during a mating behavior. PLoS One 7, e47856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banerjee A, Egger R, and Long MA (2021). Using focal cooling to link neural dynamics and behavior. Neuron [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolkan SS, Stujenske JM, Parnaudeau S, Spellman TJ, Rauffenbart C, Abbas AI, Harris AZ, Gordon JA, and Kellendonk C (2017). Thalamic projections sustain prefrontal activity during working memory maintenance. Nat Neurosci 20, 987–996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, Raksin JN, and Schmidt MF (2005). Sensorimotor nucleus NIf is necessary for auditory processing but not vocal motor output in the avian song system. J Neurophysiol 93, 2157–2166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, and Schmidt MF (2004). Noradrenergic inputs mediate state dependence of auditory responses in the avian song system. J Neurosci 24, 7745–7753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen Y, Shen J, Semu D, Leman DP, Liberti WA 3rd, Perkins LN, Liberti DC, Kotton DN, and Gardner TJ (2020). Hidden neural states underlie canary song syntax. Nature 582, 539–544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman MJ, and Mooney R (2004). Synaptic transformations underlying highly selective auditory representations of learned birdsong. J Neurosci 24, 7251–7265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman MJ, and Vu ET (2005). Recovery of impaired songs following unilateral but not bilateral lesions of nucleus uvaeformis of adult zebra finches. J Neurobiol 63, 70–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cynx J (1990). Experimental determination of a unit of song production in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). J Comp Psychol 104, 3–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacre J, Colligan M, Clarke T, Ammer JJ, Schiemann J, Chamosa-Pino V, Claudi F, Harston JA, Eleftheriou C, Pakan JMP, et al. (2021). A cerebellar-thalamocortical pathway drives behavioral context-dependent movement initiation. Neuron 109, 2326–2338 e2328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danish HH, Aronov D, and Fee MS (2017). Rhythmic syllable-related activity in a songbird motor thalamic nucleus necessary for learned vocalizations. PLoS One 12, e0169568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dave AS, and Margoliash D (2000). Song replay during sleep and computational rules for sensorimotor vocal learning. Science 290, 812–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deregnaucourt S, Mitra PP, Feher O, Pytte C, and Tchernichovski O (2005). How sleep affects the developmental learning of bird song. Nature 433, 710–716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diba K, and Buzsaki G (2007). Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripples. Nat Neurosci 10, 1241–1242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egger R, Tupikov Y, Elmaleh M, Katlowitz KA, Benezra SE, Picardo MA, Moll F, Kornfeld J, Jin DZ, and Long MA (2020). Local Axonal Conduction Shapes the Spatiotemporal Properties of Neural Sequences. Cell 183, 537–548 e512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fee MS, Kozhevnikov AA, and Hahnloser RH (2004). Neural mechanisms of vocal sequence generation in the songbird. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1016, 153–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster DJ, and Wilson MA (2006). Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature 440, 680–683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster EF, Mehta RP, and Bottjer SW (1997). Axonal connections of the medial magnocellular nucleus of the anterior neostriatum in zebra finches. J Comp Neurol 382, 364–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujimoto H, Hasegawa T, and Watanabe D (2011). Neural coding of syntactic structure in learned vocalizations in the songbird. J Neurosci 31, 10023–10033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galvis D, Wu W, Hyson RL, Johnson F, and Bertram R (2017). A distributed neural network model for the distinct roles of medial and lateral HVC in zebra finch song production. J Neurophysiol 118, 677–692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner RJ, Lu L, Wernle T, Moser MB, and Moser EI (2019). Correlation structure of grid cells is preserved during sleep. Nat Neurosci 22, 598–608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibb L, Gentner TQ, and Abarbanel HD (2009). Inhibition and recurrent excitation in a computational model of sparse bursting in song nucleus HVC. J Neurophysiol 102, 1748–1762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glaze CM, and Troyer TW (2006). Temporal structure in zebra finch song: implications for motor coding. J Neurosci 26, 991–1005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo ZV, Inagaki HK, Daie K, Druckmann S, Gerfen CR, and Svoboda K (2017). Maintenance of persistent activity in a frontal thalamocortical loop. Nature 545, 181–186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, and Fee MS (2007). Sleep-related spike bursts in HVC are driven by the nucleus interface of the nidopallium. J Neurophysiol 97, 423–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, Kozhevnikov AA, and Fee MS (2002). An ultra-sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird. Nature 419, 65–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, Kozhevnikov AA, and Fee MS (2006). Sleep-related neural activity in a premotor and a basal-ganglia pathway of the songbird. J Neurophysiol 96, 794–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, Wang CZ, Nager A, and Naie K (2008). Spikes and bursts in two types of thalamic projection neurons differentially shape sleep patterns and auditory responses in a songbird. J Neurosci 28, 5040–5052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamaguchi K, Tanaka M, and Mooney R (2016). A Distributed Recurrent Network Contributes to Temporally Precise Vocalizations. Neuron 91, 680–693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey CD, Coen P, and Tank DW (2012). Choice-specific sequences in parietal cortex during a virtual-navigation decision task. Nature 484, 62–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hosino T, and Okanoya K (2000). Lesion of a higher-order song nucleus disrupts phrase level complexity in Bengalese finches. Neuroreport 11, 2091–2095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katlowitz KA, Picardo MA, and Long MA (2018). Stable Sequential Activity Underlying the Maintenance of a Precisely Executed Skilled Behavior. Neuron 98, 1133–1140 e1133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kornfeld J, Benezra SE, Narayanan RT, Svara F, Egger R, Oberlaender M, Denk W, and Long MA (2017). EM connectomics reveals axonal target variation in a sequence-generating network. Elife 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosche G, Vallentin D, and Long MA (2015). Interplay of inhibition and excitation shapes a premotor neural sequence. J Neurosci 35, 1217–1227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kozhevnikov AA, and Fee MS (2007). Singing-related activity of identified HVC neurons in the zebra finch. J Neurophysiol 97, 4271–4283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee AK, and Wilson MA (2002). Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron 36, 1183–1194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonardo A, and Fee MS (2005). Ensemble coding of vocal control in birdsong. J Neurosci 25, 652–661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipkind D, Zai AT, Hanuschkin A, Marcus GF, Tchernichovski O, and Hahnloser RHR (2017). Songbirds work around computational complexity by learning song vocabulary independently of sequence. Nat Commun 8, 1247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lombardino AJ, and Nottebohm F (2000). Age at deafening affects the stability of learned song in adult male zebra finches. J Neurosci 20, 5054–5064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long MA, and Fee MS (2008). Using temperature to analyse temporal dynamics in the songbird motor pathway. Nature 456, 189–194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long MA, Jin DZ, and Fee MS (2010). Support for a synaptic chain model of neuronal sequence generation. Nature 468, 394–399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Louie K, and Wilson MA (2001). Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron 29, 145–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch GF, Okubo TS, Hanuschkin A, Hahnloser RH, and Fee MS (2016). Rhythmic Continuous-Time Coding in the Songbird Analog of Vocal Motor Cortex. Neuron 90, 877–892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacDonald CJ, Lepage KQ, Eden UT, and Eichenbaum H (2011). Hippocampal “time cells” bridge the gap in memory for discontiguous events. Neuron 71, 737–749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackevicius EL, Happ MTL, and Fee MS (2020). An avian cortical circuit for chunking tutor song syllables into simple vocal-motor units. Nat Commun 11, 5029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markowitz JE, Liberti WA 3rd, Guitchounts G, Velho T, Lois C, and Gardner TJ (2015). Mesoscopic patterns of neural activity support songbird cortical sequences. PLoS Biol 13, e1002158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mooney R (1992). Synaptic basis for developmental plasticity in a birdsong nucleus. J Neurosci 12, 2464–2477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mooney R, and Prather JF (2005). The HVC microcircuit: the synaptic basis for interactions between song motor and vocal plasticity pathways. J Neurosci 25, 1952–1964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nottebohm F, Stokes TM, and Leonard CM (1976). Central control of song in the canary, Serinus canarius. J Comp Neurol 165, 457–486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okubo TS, Mackevicius EL, Payne HL, Lynch GF, and Fee MS (2015). Growth and splitting of neural sequences in songbird vocal development. Nature 528, 352–357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Otchy TM, Wolff SB, Rhee JY, Pehlevan C, Kawai R, Kempf A, Gobes SM, and Olveczky BP (2015). Acute off-target effects of neural circuit manipulations. Nature 528, 358–363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pachitariu M, Steinmetz N, Kadir S, Carandini M, and Kenneth D, H. (2016). Kilosort: realtime spike-sorting for extracellular electrophysiology with hundreds of channels. bioRxiv, 061481. [Google Scholar]
- Pastalkova E, Itskov V, Amarasingham A, and Buzsaki G (2008). Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus. Science 321, 1322–1327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters AJ, Chen SX, and Komiyama T (2014). Emergence of reproducible spatiotemporal activity during motor learning. Nature 510, 263–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picardo MA, Merel J, Katlowitz KA, Vallentin D, Okobi DE, Benezra SE, Clary RC, Pnevmatikakis EA, Paninski L, and Long MA (2016). Population-Level Representation of a Temporal Sequence Underlying Song Production in the Zebra Finch. Neuron 90, 866–876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauske PL, Chi Z, Dave AS, and Margoliash D (2010). Neuronal stability and drift across periods of sleep: premotor activity patterns in a vocal control nucleus of adult zebra finches. J Neurosci 30, 2783–2794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinke H, and Wild JM (1998). Identification and connections of inspiratory premotor neurons in songbirds and budgerigar. J Comp Neurol 391, 147–163. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts TF, Gobes SM, Murugan M, Olveczky BP, and Mooney R (2012). Motor circuits are required to encode a sensory model for imitative learning. Nat Neurosci 15, 1454–1459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossant C, Kadir SN, Goodman DFM, Schulman J, Hunter MLD, Saleem AB, Grosmark A, Belluscio M, Denfield GH, Ecker AS, et al. (2016). Spike sorting for large, dense electrode arrays. Nat Neurosci 19, 634–641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sauerbrei BA, Guo JZ, Cohen JD, Mischiati M, Guo W, Kabra M, Verma N, Mensh B, Branson K, and Hantman AW (2020). Cortical pattern generation during dexterous movement is input-driven. Nature 577, 386–391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt MF (2003). Pattern of interhemispheric synchronization in HVc during singing correlates with key transitions in the song pattern. J Neurophysiol 90, 3931–3949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt MF, Ashmore RC, and Vu ET (2004). Bilateral control and interhemispheric coordination in the avian song motor system. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1016, 171–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt MF, and Konishi M (1998). Gating of auditory responses in the vocal control system of awake songbirds. Nat Neurosci 1, 513–518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitt LI, Wimmer RD, Nakajima M, Happ M, Mofakham S, and Halassa MM (2017). Thalamic amplification of cortical connectivity sustains attentional control. Nature 545, 219–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scotto-Lomassese S, Rochefort C, Nshdejan A, and Scharff C (2007). HVC interneurons are not renewed in adult male zebra finches. Eur J Neurosci 25, 1663–1668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Wilson MA, and Barnes CA (1996). Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences. Hippocampus 6, 149–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Striedter GF, and Vu ET (1998). Bilateral feedback projections to the forebrain in the premotor network for singing in zebra finches. J Neurobiol 34, 27–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchernichovski O, Mitra PP, Lints T, and Nottebohm F (2001). Dynamics of the vocal imitation process: how a zebra finch learns its song. Science 291, 2564–2569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trettel SG, Trimper JB, Hwaun E, Fiete IR, and Colgin LL (2019). Grid cell co-activity patterns during sleep reflect spatial overlap of grid fields during active behaviors. Nat Neurosci 22, 609–617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang CZ, Herbst JA, Keller GB, and Hahnloser RH (2008). Rapid interhemispheric switching during vocal production in a songbird. PLoS Biol 6, e250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wild JM (1993). Descending projections of the songbird nucleus robustus archistriatalis. J Comp Neurol 338, 225–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wild JM, Williams MN, Howie GJ, and Mooney R (2005). Calcium-binding proteins define interneurons in HVC of the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). J Comp Neurol 483, 76–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams H, Crane LA, Hale TK, Esposito MA, and Nottebohm F (1992). Right-side dominance for song control in the zebra finch. J Neurobiol 23, 1006–1020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams H, and Vicario DS (1993). Temporal patterning of song production: participation of nucleus uvaeformis of the thalamus. J Neurobiol 24, 903–912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson MA, and McNaughton BL (1994). Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science 265, 676–679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiltschko AB, Johnson MJ, Iurilli G, Peterson RE, Katon JM, Pashkovski SL, Abraira VE, Adams RP, and Datta SR (2015). Mapping Sub-Second Structure in Mouse Behavior. Neuron 88, 1121–1135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang L, Lee K, Villagracia J, and Masmanidis SC (2020). Open source silicon microprobes for high throughput neural recording. J Neural Eng 17, 016036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young BK, Mindlin GB, Arneodo E, and Goller F (2017). Adult zebra finches rehearse highly variable song patterns during sleep. PeerJ 5, e4052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu AC, and Margoliash D (1996). Temporal hierarchical control of singing in birds. Science 273, 1871–1875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao W, Garcia-Oscos F, Dinh D, and Roberts TF (2019). Inception of memories that guide vocal learning in the songbird. Science 366, 83–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All code necessary to run template matching as described here can be found at <https://github.com/LongLabGit/TemplateMatching.git> (DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.5500262). The data sets generated during this study and any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper are available upon request from the Lead Contact.