Abstract
Objective
Symptoms of problematic Internet use (PIU) may be increased in people who display an excessive involvement in Massively Multiplayer Online Role-Playing Games (MMORPGs). Notably, impulsivity and alexithymia have not been investigated together in the literature addressing the predictors of PIU among gamers, despite evidence that these personality traits may play a pivotal role in the development of problematic gaming patterns. The purpose of this research was to explore the relationship between facets of alexithymia and impulsivity and PIU scores among MMORPG players.
Method
In the current study, 364 World of Warcraft (WoW) players (272 males, 74.7%) aged 18 to 48 years old provided socio-demographic information and completed questionnaires on PIU, time spent online playing WoW, alexithymia, and impulsivity.
Results
PIU scores were negatively associated with age and positively associated with alexithymia scores, impulsivity scores, and time spent online playing WoW. A linear regression analysis showed that PIU scores were predicted by time spent online playing WoW, the alexithymic features concerning difficulties identifying and describing feelings, and attentional impulsivity.
Conclusions
Our findings suggest that MMORPG players with difficulties concerning affect awareness and a tendency to be distracted by novel stimuli may spend an excessive amount of time playing video games and may also show prominent symptoms of PIU.
Keywords: problematic gaming, problematic Internet use, impulsivity, alexithymia, World of Warcraft, gaming disorder, Internet gaming disorder
1. Introduction
Massively Multiplayer Online Role-Playing Games (MMORPGs) are computer games that are played on the Internet and that allow many gamers to play simultaneously in a virtual world. Players from all over the world can explore, communicate, cooperate and fight to achieve in-game goals, to develop the plot of the game and to improve the characteristics of their virtual representation (i.e., the avatar). In an MMORPG, the world continues to exist when the user is not logged in, and both events and interactions continue to occur when the user is not connected.
One of the most played MMORPGs is World of Warcraft (WoW). WoW is an MMORPG set in the fantasy world of Azeroth, which offers a wide selection of contents and quests. The quest is a task assigned to a player character, which generates a reward when completed, such as experience points (XP), objects and gold (gold is the in-game currency of WoW).
Players begin their adventure in WoW, like in any other MMORPG, by creating a character. This character is largely customizable in terms of appearance and roles played in game dynamics. There are two in-game factions that the player can choose: Horde and Alliance. Also, WoW includes twelve playable classes (e.g. Death Knight, Monks, Druids) and thirteen races (e.g. Dwarfs, Gnomes, Humans). WoW entails different game modes, such as Player versus Environment (PvE) and Player versus Player (PvP). In addition, its universe is constantly expanding, and new contents and in-game activities are frequently provided to the player. These attractive characteristics of WoW might explain its huge success among its players.
However, research on WoW and other MMORPGs suggests that there is a dark side of the coin: in fact, it is reported that a minority of MMORPG players develop symptoms of problematic gaming (Hahn et al., 2014; Leménager et al., 2014; Stavropoulos, Kuss, Griffiths, Wilson, & Motti-Stefanidi, 2017). Psychological features associated with problematic use of MMORPGs, and especially WoW, have been extensively examined in research (Billieux et al., 2013, 2015; Deleuze et al., 2019; Fuster, Chamarro, Carbonell, & Vallerand, 2014; Kuss, Louws, & Wiers, 2012; Ryan, Rigby, & Przybylski, 2006; Yee, 2006). Research shows that motivational factors such as a desire for competition, advancement and escapism may lead the player to spend excessive time in the virtual world and may foster an excessive and problematic use of the game (Billieux et al., 2011, 2013, 2015; Dauriat et al., 2011; Hellström, Nilsson, Leppert, & Åslund, 2012; Király et al., 2015; Kirby, Jones, & Copello, 2014; Schimmenti, Infanti, Badoud, Laloyaux, & Billieux, 2017a; Yee, 2006).
It is noteworthy that people who spend many hours playing MMORPG do not necessarily display a problematic use associated with negative outcomes. In fact, it has been observed that a great amount of time spent by individuals in virtual worlds can represent the result of a healthy passion, rather than a problematic behavior (Billieux et al., 2013; Burnay, Billieux, Blairy, & Larøi, 2015; Charlton & Danforth, 2007); nonetheless, it has also been observed that an excessive time spent in virtual worlds, combined with other symptoms related to excessive and dysregulated game use, may generate problematic gaming patterns. Notably, the conflicting interpretations of the psychological and behavioral correlates of an excessive involvement in online games fostered an intense scientific debate, especially concerning the adequacy of classifying problematic gaming patterns as a specific psychiatric disorder. The American Psychiatric Association (APA, 2013) proposed the diagnosis of Internet gaming disorder (IGD) as a condition for further study, in order to identify a putative form of behavioral addiction characterized by loss of control and a persistent and recurrent use of Internet games that lead to significant impairments in psychological and psychosocial functioning. More recently, WHO experts recommended to include Gaming Disorder (GD) in the section of disorders due to addictive behaviors in the ICD-11 (World Health Organization [WHO], 2018). WHO experts described GD as a disorder characterized by symptoms such as impairment control over gaming, a priority given to gaming compared to other interests and daily activities, and continuation or escalation of gaming despite its negative consequences. Just to provide an example of the intense debate concerning the diagnostic classification of problematic gaming, Dullur and Starcevic (2018) recently highlighted that there is no clear consensus to establish that a problematic use of online games can be classified as a mental disorder. King and colleagues (2018) replied that there are sufficient reasons, especially from a public health perspective, to include the diagnosis of gaming disorder in the diagnostic manuals. Then, Schimmenti and Starcevic (2019) identified some prominent logical fallacies (that is, logical errors in the scientific discourse) that have been used by King and colleagues to promote the inclusion of IGD and GD among mental disorders.
From a psychological perspective, Kardefelt-Winther (2014a, 2014b) has proposed to consider some Internet activities, such an excessive use of online games, as a compensatory strategy to satisfy needs and to cope with psychosocial problems. In this context, problematic Internet use (PIU) has been conceptualized as a coping strategy to escape from real life problems and difficulties, through a temporary retreat in Internet activities (Deleuze et al., 2019; Di Blasi et al., 2019; Kardefelt-Winther, 2014c; Maroney, Williams, Thomas, Skues, & Moulding, 2018; Reinecke, 2009; Schimmenti, & Caretti 2010, 2017; Schimmenti et al., 2019a; Snodgrass et al., 2018).
PIU can be defined as an excessive, dysregulated, and potentially addictive use of Internet services and applications (Schimmenti & Caretti, 2017). However, following a compensatory approach, a great amount of time spent in video games can be conceived either as a problematic or non-problematic activity, depending on the psychological factors that lead an individual to “in the virtual worlds.
Research has shown that males, especially adolescents and young adults, are more likely to develop PIU (Anderson, Steen, & Stavropoulos, 2018), including excessive use of online games (Borgonovi, 2016; Durkee et al., 2012; Ha & Hwang, 2014; Laconi, Pires, & Chabrol, 2017; Li, Yu, Zhang, & Jin, 2015). Men tend to spend more time than women on video games (Elliott, Ream, McGinsky, & Dunlap, 2012; Laconi et al., 2017; Puerta-Cortés, Panova, Carbonell, & Chamarro, 2017; Rehbein, Staudt, Hanslmaier, & Kliem, 2016), even though recent data suggest that the use of video games among women is increasing (Entertainment Software Association, 2018). Studies suggest that men prefer MMORPG and sports games, while women prefer simulation games and casual games (Laconi et al., 2017; Puerta-Cortés et al., 2017; Rehbein et al., 2016).
Importantly, research has already highlighted the role of personality features in the development of problematic gaming patterns, beyond the effects of gender preferences and time spent online (Gervasi et al., 2017a). Among the personality factors that may affect the development of problematic gaming, alexithymia and impulsivity may represent two relevant constructs that deserve further investigation in their relationship with excessive gaming patterns and PIU.
The term alexithymia (a composed Greek term which means “no word for emotions”) describes a personality trait characterized by difficulty identifying and describing one’s own feelings, together with a limited capacity to fantasize and to use one’s emotions to guide actions (Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994a). Impulsivity is a personality trait that includes a difficulty focusing on tasks and a tendency to act without self-control, on the spur of the moment (Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995).
Alexithymia and impulsivity are closely linked with difficulties in emotion regulation. Emotion regulation is the psychological process that allows the individuals to recognize, differentiate, and modulate their emotions in response to internal, environmental, and relational stimuli (David, 2016; Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist, & Target, 2002; Gross & Thompson, 2007). The emotional difficulties related to alexithymic traits and impulsivity have been theoretically conceptualized as specific components of an emotion dysregulation process, with research findings supporting this conceptualization (Di Blasi et al., 2019; D’Agostino, Covanti, Monti, & Starcevic, 2017; Schimmenti, Santoro, La Marca, Costanzo, & Gervasi, 2019b).
Literature findings show that emotion dysregulation is a potential risk factor for PIU (Estévez Gutiérrez, Herrero Fernández, Sarabia Gonzalvo, & Jàuregui Bilbao, 2014; Estévez, Jáuregui, Sánchez-Marcos, Lopez-González, & Griffiths, 2017; Hollett & Harris, 2019). In particular, alexithymia and impulsivity are positively associated with PIU (Casale, Caplan, & Fioravanti, 2016; Dalbudak et al., 2013; Hormes, Kearns, & Timko, 2014; Kandri, Bonotis, Floros, & Zafiropoulou, 2014; Meerkerk, van den Eijnden, Franken, & Garretsen, 2010; Mottram & Fleming, 2009; Schimmenti et al., 2017b; Scimeca et al., 2014; Yates, Gregor, & Haviland, 2012; Yildiz, 2017), and especially with problematic gaming (Bonnaire & Baptista, 2018; Gentile et al., 2011; Gervasi et al., 2017a). For example, Bonnaire and Baptista (2018) found that people who displayed problematic MMORPG use were more alexithymic than non-problematic players. Also, research has shown that regular gamers display higher levels of alexithymia and are less emotionally reactive than irregular gamers (Gaetan, Bréjard, & Bonnet, 2016). Accordingly, it has been proposed that video games can be used to aid emotion regulation in people who display alexithymic traits and emotion dysregulation (Di Blasi et al., 2019; Gaetan et al., 2016; Hemenover & Bowman 2018; Hussain & Griffiths, 2009; Villani et al., 2018).
Furthermore, a longitudinal study conducted on a large sample of adolescents identified impulsivity as a risk factor for the development of problematic use of videogames (Gentile et al., 2011). A recent systematic review on the relationship between problematic gaming patterns and personality traits (Gervasi et al., 2017a) also showed that impulsivity is moderately to strongly associated with problematic gaming. However, it was also suggested that features such as impulsivity, sensation seeking, diminished inhibitory control and poor self-control might qualify both problematic and non-problematic gamers (Billieux et al., 2011, 2015; Billieux & Van der Linden, 2012; Choi et al., 2014; Collins, Freeman, & Chamarro-Premuzic, 2012; Metcalf & Pammer, 2014; Zhou, Zhu, Li, & Wang, 2014), and some studies suggested that MMORPG players may display different impulsivity profiles with respect to other videogame players (Collins et al., 2012, Puerta-Cortés et al., 2017), such as players of action games (Metcalf & Pammer, 2014).
In sum, research suggests that alexithymic features and impulsivity may contribute to the development of PIU, including problematic gaming, but it has to be understood which domains of alexithymia and impulsivity are associated with problematic gaming in MMORPG players. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to explore if specific features of alexithymia and impulsivity predicted PIU in a sample of MMORPG players, controlling for gender, age, household arrangement, and time spent in WoW.
1. Method
2.1. Participants
The study sample included 364 WoW players (272 males, 74.7%) aged 18 to 48 years old (M = 25.39; SD = 7.03). All participants were recruited via an online online survey. An advertisement was shared on different platforms, such as official WoW forums, general video game forums, and Facebook groups and pages dedicated to World of Warcraft. Before taking the survey, participants were informed about the nature of the study. The anonymity of the data was guaranteed: no information that could reveal the identity of the players, such as real names or Internet protocol address, was collected.
2.2. Measures
Internet Addiction Test (IAT; Young, 1998)
The IAT is a self-report measure that assesses the presence and the extent of PIU. It includes items addressing the incapability to control Internet use, the degree of preoccupation with the Internet, the extent of hiding or lying about online use, and its continued use despite negative effects. The IAT comprises 20 items rated on a five-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). IAT scores can range from 20 to 100, with higher scores indicating higher levels of PIU. The IAT includes questions such as “How often do you block out disturbing thoughts about your life with soothing thoughts of the Internet?” The IAT has demonstrated adequate validity and reliability in Italy (Ferraro, Caci, D’Amico, & Di Blasi, 2007) and worldwide (Ha & Hwang, 2006; Widyanto & McMurran, 2004), and it is one of the most widely used scales in research on PIU. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the IAT in the current study was .89.
Twenty-Items Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; Bagby et al., 1994a; Bagby, Taylor, & Parker, 1994b)
The TAS-20 is a self-report measure used to assess alexithymia. It consists of 20 items that address three factors of alexithymia: difficulty identifying feelings (DIF, 7 items), difficulty describing feelings (DDF, 5 items) and externally oriented thinking (EOT, 8 items). Participants indicate how much they agree with each statement on a five-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree). The TAS-20 total score can range from 20 to 100, with higher scores indicating higher levels of alexithymic traits. The TAS-20 includes statements such as: “I have feelings that I can’t quite identify” (related to the DIF factor); “It is difficult for me to find the right words for my feelings” (related to the DDF factor); and “I prefer talking to people about their daily activities rather than their feelings” (related to the EOT factor). The TAS-20 has demonstrated good psychometric properties in many studies (Parker, Keefer, Taylor, & Bagby, 2008; Taylor & Bagby, 2013), including Italy (Bressi et al., 1996). In the current study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was .83 for TAS-20 total score (alpha value of factor scores: .84 for DIF, .79 for DDF, and .63 for EOT).
The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11; Patton et al., 1995)
The BIS-11 is a widely used self-report measure for the assessment of impulsivity. It comprises 30 items presented as first-person statement rated on a 4-point scale (Rarely/Never, Occasionally, Often, Almost Always/Always). In addition to a total score ranged from 30 to 120, the BIS-11 provides three subscale scores addressing different domains of impulsivity: attentional impulsiveness (8 items; e.g., “I don’t ‘pay attention”), motor impulsiveness (11 items; e.g., “I act on impulse”), and non-planning impulsiveness (11 items; e.g., “I get easily bored when solving through problems”). Higher scores indicate higher levels of impulsivity. The psychometric properties of the BIS-11 have been tested with positive results in many countries, including Italy (Fossati, Di Ceglie, Acquarini, & Barratt, 2001). In the current study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was .83 for BIS-11 total score, .60 for attentional impulsiveness, .69 for motor impulsiveness and .70 for non-planning impulsiveness.
Socio-demographic data and average numbers of hours spent each day playing WoW were collected by means of an ad hoc-questionnaire.
2.3. Statistical analyses
Descriptive statistics were computed for all study variables. Group differences were examined through t-test. Pearson’s r correlation coefficients were calculated to examine the associations between time spent in WoW, IAT scores, TAS-20 scores, and BIS-11 scores. A multiple linear regression analysis was conducted to analyze the effects of gender, age, household arrangement, time spent online playing WoW, alexithymia factors (DIF, DDF and EOT) and impulsivity domains (attentional impulsiveness, motor impulsiveness and non-planning impulsiveness) on IAT scores.
3. Results
Descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1, for the full sample and differentiated by gender. Most of the participants (92%) lived with other people in the household, such as family members and colleagues, whereas the others lived alone. Male players were younger than females, and they reported higher scores on TAS-20 and its DDF and EOT factors.
Table 1.
Full sample | Males | Females | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(N= 364) | (n= 272) | (n= 92) | |||||||
M | (SD) | Range | M | (SD) | M | (SD) | t( 362) | p | |
Age | 25.39 | (7.03) | 18-48 | 24.52 | (6.45) | 27.97 | (8.03) | 4.15 | <.01 |
Hours spent online playing World of Warcraft | 3.22 | (1.43) | .5-5 | 3.18 | (1.45) | 3.34 | (1.37) | -.97 | .33 |
Internet Addiction Test | 52.03 | (14.18) | 23-100 | 52.32 | (13.85) | 51.20 | (15.17) | .66 | .51 |
TAS-20 (total score) | 44.54 | (12.71) | 21-83 | 45.56 | (12.17) | 41.53 | (13.84) | 2.65 | .01 |
Difficulty Identifying Feelings | 13.82 | (6.30) | 7-34 | 13.68 | (6.20) | 14.24 | (6.59) | -.74 | .46 |
Difficulty Describing Feelings | 12.74 | (5.22) | 5-25 | 13.15 | (5.11) | 11.53 | (5.38) | 2.59 | .01 |
Externally Oriented Thinking | 17.98 | (5.24) | 8-36 | 18.73 | (5.36) | 15.76 | (4.15) | 4.84 | <.01 |
BIS-11 (total score) | 61.26 | (11.18) | 36-105 | 61.54 | (11.48) | 60.40 | (10.25) | .85 | .40 |
Attentional Impulsiveness | 16.12 | (3.64) | 10-30 | 16.27 | (3.66) | 15.66 | (3.57) | 1.39 | .17 |
Motor Impulsiveness | 20.80 | (4.58) | 12-42 | 20.72 | (4.69) | 21.03 | (4.23) | -.57 | .57 |
Non-planning Impulsiveness | 24.34 | (5.30) | 12-41 | 24.56 | (5.36) | 23.71 | (5.11) | 1.33 | .19 |
Note. TAS-20: Twenty-Items Toronto Alexithymia Scale; BIS-11: The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11.
Subsequently, the correlations between the investigated variables were examined (see Table 2). A pattern of significant and positive associations emerged among time spent online playing WoW and scores on IAT, TAS-20, and BIS-11. All these variables were significantly and negatively associated with age.
Table 2.
2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Age | .12* | .22** | .31** | -.24** | .29** | .19** | .14** | .24** | .04 | .17** |
2. Time spent online playing World of Warcraft | - | .38** | .20** | .16** | .09 | .21** | .25** | .13* | .25** | .22** |
3. Internet Addiction Test | - | .51** | .51** | .40** | .24** | .47** | .48** | .40** | .31** | |
4. TAS-20 (total score) | - | .82** | .82** | .63** | .52** | .52** | .38** | .42** | ||
5. Difficulty Identifying Feelings | - | .58** | .21** | .48** | .50** | .40** | .32** | |||
6. Difficulty Describing Feelings | - | .29** | .30** | .38** | .18** | .23** | ||||
7. Externally Oriented Thinking | - | .40** | .29** | .26** | .42** | |||||
8. BIS- 11 total (score) | - | .78** | .84** | .84** | ||||||
9. Attentional Impulsiveness | - | .57** | .47** | |||||||
10. Motor Impulsiveness | - | .52** | ||||||||
11. Non-planning Impulsiveness | - |
Note. TAS-20: Twenty-Items Toronto Alexithymia Scale; BIS-11: The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11; *p < .05, **p < .01
A multiple linear regression analysis was performed to examine the role of gender, age, household arrangement, time spent online playing WoW, TAS-20 factors and BIS-11 factors in predicting IAT scores in the sample. The time spent online playing WoW, the TAS-20 factors concerning difficulty identifying feelings and difficulty describing feelings, and the BIS-11 domain of attentional impulsiveness were significant and positive predictors of IAT scores in the sample (see Table 3) . The regression model was significant (F(10,353) = 26.33; p < .001) and it explained 43% of variance in IAT scores. An additional exploration of the interactions between alexithymia factor scores and impulsivity factor scores displayed no significant interaction effects, indicating that difficulty identifying feelings, difficulty describing feelings, and attentional impulsiveness independently predicted PIU in our sample of WoW players.
Table 3.
B | SE | Partial r | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | -.60 | 1.41 | -.02 | -.43 | .67 |
Age | -.10 | .09 | -.06 | -1.03 | .30 |
Living with family or peers | -1.55 | 2.17 | -.04 | -.72 | .48 |
Hours spent online playing World of Warcraft | 2.71 | .43 | .32 | 6.38 | <.01 |
Difficulty Identifying Feelings | .57 | .13 | .24 | 4.57 | <.01 |
Difficulty Describing Feelings | .31 | .14 | .12 | 2.17 | .03 |
Externally Oriented Thinking | .00 | .13 | .00 | -.01 | .99 |
Attentional Impulsiveness | .86 | .22 | .21 | 3.94 | <.01 |
Motor Impulsiveness | .29 | .18 | .09 | 1.62 | .11 |
Non-planning Impulsiveness | -.04 | .14 | -.02 | -.31 | .76 |
Model: F= 26.33<. 001R2= .43 |
4. Discussion
The present study examined the role of alexithymia and impulsivity facets in PIU scores reported by players of WoW. Three out of four participants in our study were males. This is in line with research showing that men prefer MMORPGs, sport games, and competitive online videogames (Elliott et al., 2012; Laconi et al., 2017; Puerta-Cortés et al., 2017; Rehbein et al., 2016), whereas women tend to play casual games for entertainment (Ko, Yen, Chen, Chen, & Yen, 2005) and they prefer videogames involving simulation, strategy games and mini-games (Laconi et al., 2017; Puerta-Cortés et al., 2017; Rehbein et al., 2016). Our results also showed that males were younger than females in our sample. According to Yee (2006), male players may have earlier access to MMORPGs than females, and females are frequently introduced to MMORPGs in later time by friends or partners.
Many participants in our study reported IAT scores indicating the presence of difficulties controlling their Internet use and an excessive involvement with Internet activities (Young, 1998). This finding supports the view that some WoW players may display PIU because of their excessive involvement with the game and their consequent problematic gaming patterns. In contrast with the majority of studies (e.g., Dong et al., 2018; Durkee et al., 2012; Greenberg, Lewis, & Dodd, 1999; Kormas, Critselis, Janikian, Kafetzis, & Tsitsika, 2011; Tsitsika, Critselis, Janikian, Kormas, & Kafetzis, 2011), men and women in our sample did not differ in their PIU scores, nor they displayed significant differences in the time spent online playing WoW. However, in line with other studies, in our sample younger age was associated with higher levels of PIU (Gervasi et al., 2017b; Schimmenti, Passanisi, Gervasi, Manzella, & Fama, 2014; Schimmenti et al., 2019a; Scimeca et al., 2014) and increased time spent in WoW (Englund, Egeland, Oliva, & Collins, 2008). Moreover, younger participants showed increased levels of alexithymic features and impulsivity traits. Probably, emotion regulation abilities were not entirely developed in some of the youngest people in our sample, and this might have led some of them to use WoW to increase their positive emotions or to facilitate affect modulation and regulation (Di Blasi et al., 2019). This would be in line with theory (Schimmenti & Caretti, 2010, 2017) suggesting that vulnerable people may use the Internet as a “psychic retreat” to protect themselves from distressing experiences.
Finally, a linear regression analysis showed that time spent online playing WoW, difficulty identifying feelings, difficulty describing feelings, and attentional impulsivity predicted the scores on PIU in our sample. This suggests that video games may represent a facilitating environment to cope with negative feelings for some individuals, and that gaming might also help some players to reduce their difficulties identifying, describing and processing their feelings (Gaetan et al., 2016). In fact, research suggests that playing online can be used to access emotions and to reduce alexithymic dynamics (Di Blasi et al., 2019; Gaetan et al., 2016; Hemenover & Bowman, 2018; Villani et al., 2018). However, playing MMORPGs may also trigger an excessive emotional involvement and a need to excessively and continuously play the game in some vulnerable people (Gentile et al., 2011), thus increasing the risk of problematic and dysregulated gaming patterns. In fact, immediate gratification due to in-game reinforcements and an increasing level of challenges can lead some vulnerable and emotionally dysregulated MMORPG players to a compulsive and impulsive use of the game (Choi & Kim, 2004; Hull, Williams, & Griffiths, 2013; Wan & Chiou, 2006). In this context, the tendency to seek novel and attractive stimuli may foster the absorption in the video game and, consequently, its excessive use, starting a maladaptive pathway that may result in the development of PIU.
In sum, these considerations are in line with research suggesting that PIU is linked to an interference with processing emotional stimuli (Schimmenti, Starcevic, Gervasi, Deleuze, & Billieux, 2018) and support the view that video games may represent a particularly attractive environment for some vulnerable individuals. In MMORPGs, these individuals may find a way to cope with emotion difficulties and, at the same time, to fulfill their needs for stimulation and novelty (Deleuze et al., 2019; Demetrovics et al., 2011). Especially, people who have difficulties identifying and communicating their feelings may be prone to use video games as a way to escape from negative feelings and boredom (Billieux & Van der Linden, 2012; Billieux et al., 2015; Di Blasi et al., 2019; Hussain & Griffiths, 2009; Kardefelt-Winther 2014a, 2014c; Kuo, Lutz, & Hiler, 2016; Maroney et al., 2018). Therefore, a potential interpretation of our findings is that the tendency to be distracted by different and novel stimuli and the continuous search for stimulation are linked, in a vicious circle, to increased difficulties identifying and describing feelings. This, in turn, may foster an excessive use of the video game, and may ultimately generate PIU.
Some limitations of the current study must be reported. First, we used the IAT to examine PIU, but the questions included in this measure did not allow us to disentangle the effects of problematic gaming patterns from other features of an excessive and dysregulated involvement with the Internet, thus we relied on participants’ self-reported time playing WoW to speculate on the role of excessive gaming in PIU. Future research might use specific measures of IGD or GD symptoms together with measures of PIU. Second, our study relied on self-reported measures, which can result in bias effects. Third, the self-selected nature of the sample limited the possibility to generalize our findings to other video game players; additionally, our results refer only to WoW players. Fourth, direct causation cannot be inferred due to the cross-sectional nature of the study. Finally, our sample was recruited in the community, so our results cannot be immediately extended to individuals who seek treatment for problematic gaming.
5. Conclusions
Its limitations notwithstanding, our study can shed new lights on how time spent online, alexithymic features and impulsivity traits can contribute to the development of PIU in WoW players. Our findings suggest that difficulties in understanding and communicating feelings, together with the tendency to be distracted by novel and attractive stimuli, can contribute to the development of problematic gaming patterns. Moreover, our study suggests that investigating the domains of emotion dysregulation in people who display an excessive use of video games can be useful to identify their specific psychological vulnerabilities.
All these considerations are in line with a theoretical framework that conceptualizes problematic gaming as a compensatory strategy to deal with psychological and psychosocial problems (Di Blasi et al., 2019; Kardefelt-Winther, 2014b; Schimmenti & Caretti, 2010; Snodgrass et al., 2018). On the clinical level, some implications can be derived when such framework is applied to our findings. These clinical implications particularly concern the opportunity to treat the reduced affect awareness and the tendency to become easily distracted by external stimuli of some video game players. In fact, for some people who display an excessive use of video games associated with alexithymic features and impulsivity, treatment interventions should aim to promote emotion understanding and affect regulation (Schimmenti & Caretti, 2010). In fact these interventions might serve to help this type of dysregulated gamers to balance their interest about in-game attractive stimuli with an interest about those mental states and psychological motives that brought them to excessively use the game.
References
- American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson, E. L., Steen, E., & Stavropoulos, V. (2017). Internet use and Problematic Internet Use: A systematic review of longitudinal research trends in adolescence and emergent adulthood. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 22(4), 430-454. [Google Scholar]
- Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994a). The Twenty-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 23-32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., & Parker, J. D. A. (1994b). The Twenty-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-II. Convergent discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 33-40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billieux, J., Chanal, J., Khazaal, Y., Rochat, L., Gay, P., Zullino, D., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Psychological predictors of problematic involvement in massively multiplayer online role-playing games: Illustration in a sample of male cybercafe players. Psychopathology, 44(3), 165-171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billieux, J., Thorens, G., Khazaal, Y., Zullino, D., Achab, S., & Van der Linden, M. (2015). Problematic involvement in online games: A cluster analytic approach. Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 242–250. [Google Scholar]
- Billieux, J., & Van der Linden, M. (2012). Problematic use of the Internet and self-regulation: A review of the initial studies. The Open Addiction Journal, 5(1), 24-29. [Google Scholar]
- Billieux, J., Van der Linden, M., Achab, S., Khazaal, Y., Paraskevopoulos, L., Zullino, D., & Thorens, G. (2013). Why do you play World of Warcraft? An in-depth exploration of self-reported motivations to play online and in-game behaviours in the virtual world of Azeroth. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(1), 103-109. [Google Scholar]
- Bonnaire, C., & Baptista, D. (2018). Internet gaming disorder in male and female young adults: The role of alexithymia, depression, anxiety and gaming type. Psychiatry Research, 272, 521–530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borgonovi, F. (2016). Video gaming and gender differences in digital and printed reading performance among 15-year-olds students in 26 countries. Journal of adolescence, 48, 45–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bressi, C., Taylor, G., Parker, J., Bressi, S., Brambilla, V., Aguglia, E., … Invernizzi, G. (1996). Cross validation of the factor structure of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: An Italian multicenter study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41(6), 551-559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burnay, J., Billieux, J., Blairy, S., & Larøi, F. (2015). Which psychological factors influence Internet addiction? Evidence through an integrative model. Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 28–34. [Google Scholar]
- Casale, S., Caplan, S. E., & Fioravanti, G. (2016). Positive metacognitions about Internet use: The mediating role in the relationship between emotional dysregulation and problematic use. Addictive Behaviors, 59, 84–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charlton, J. P., & Danforth, I. D. (2007). Distinguishing addiction and high engagement in the context of online game playing. Computers in Human Behavior, 23(3), 1531-1548. [Google Scholar]
- Choi, D., & Kim, J. (2004). Why people continue to play online games: in search of critical design factors to increase customer loyalty to online contents. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 7(1), 11-24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi, S. W., Kim, H., Kim, G. Y., Jeon, Y., Park, S., Lee, J. Y., … Kim, D. J. (2014). Similarities and differences among Internet gaming disorder, gambling disorder and alcohol use disorder: A focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3(4), 246-253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins, E., Freeman, J., & Chamarro-Premuzic, T. (2012). Personality traits associated with problematic and non-problematic massively multiplayer online role playing game use. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(2), 133-138. [Google Scholar]
- D’Agostino, A., Covanti, S., Monti, M. R., & Starcevic, V. (2017). Reconsidering emotion dysregulation. Psychiatric Quarterly, 88(4), 807-825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalbudak, E., Evren, C., Topcu, M., Aldemir, S., Coskun, K. S., Bozkurt, M., … Canbal, M. (2013). Relationship of Internet addiction with impulsivity and severity of psychopathology among Turkish university students. Psychiatry Research, 210(3), 1086-1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dauriat, F. Z., Zermatten, A., Billieux, J., Thorens, G., Bondolfi, G., Zullino, D., & Khazaal, Y. (2011). Motivations to play specifically predict excessive involvement in massively multiplayer online role-playing games: Evidence from an online survey. European Addiction Research, 17(4), 185-189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- David, S. (2016). Emotional agility: Get unstuck, embrace change, and thrive in work and life. London, UK: Penguin. [Google Scholar]
- Deleuze, J., Maurage, P., Schimmenti, A., Nuyens, F., Melzer, A., & Billieux, J. (2019). Escaping reality through videogames is linked to an implicit preference for virtual over real-life stimuli. Journal of Affective Disorders, 245, 1024–1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demetrovics, Z., Urbán, R., Nagygyörgy, K., Farkas, J., Zilahy, D., Mervó, B., … Harmath, E. (2011). Why do you play? The development of the motives for online gaming questionnaire (MOGQ). Behavior research methods, 43(3), 814-825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Blasi, M., Giardina, A., Giordano, C., Coco, G. L., Tosto, C., Billieux, J., & Schimmenti, A. (2019). Problematic video game use as an emotional coping strategy: Evidence from a sample of MMORPG gamers. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 8(1), 25-34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dong, G., Zheng, H., Liu, X., Wang, Y., Du, X., & Potenza, M. N. (2018) Gender-related differences in cue-elicited cravings in Internet gaming disorder: the effects of deprivation. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(4), 953–964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dullur, P., & Starcevic, V. (2018). Internet gaming disorder does not qualify as a mental disorder. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 52(2), 110-111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durkee, T., Kaess, M., Carli, V., Parzer, P., Wasserman, C., Floderus B, … Wasserman, D. (2012). Prevalence of pathological Internet use among adolescents in Europe: Demographic and social factors. Addiction, 107(12), 2210-2222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott, L., Ream, G., McGinsky, E., & Dunlap, E. (2012). The contribution of game genre and other use patterns to problem video game play among adult video gamers. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 10(6), 948-969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Englund, M. M., Egeland, B., Oliva, E. M., & Collins, W. A. (2008). Childhood and adolescent predictors of heavy drinking and alcohol use disorders in early adulthood: A longitudinal developmental analysis. Addiction, 103(1), 23-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Entertainment Software Association (2018). Essential facts about the computer and video game industry. Retrieved May 16, 2019, from URL: http://www.theesa.com/wpcontent/uploads/2018/05/EF2018_FINAL.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Estévez Gutiérrez, A., Herrero Fernández, D., Sarabia Gonzalvo, I., & Jàuregui Bilbao P., (2014). Mediating role of emotional regulation between impulsive behavior in gambling, Internet and videogame abuse, and dysfunctional symptomatology in young adults and adolescents. Adicciones, 26(4), 282-290. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Estévez, A., Jáuregui, P., Sánchez-Marcos, I., Lopez-González, H., & Griffiths, M .D. (2017). Attachment and emotion regulation in substance addictions and behavioral addictions. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 534–544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferraro, G., Caci, B., D’Amico, A., & Di Blasi, M. (2007). Internet addiction disorder: An Italian study. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 10(2), 170-175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. New York, NY: Other Press. [Google Scholar]
- Fossati, A., Di Ceglie, A., Acquarini, E., & Barratt, E. S. (2001). Psychometric properties of an Italian version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) in nonclinical subjects. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57(6), 815-828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster, H., Chamarro, A., Carbonell, X., & Vallerand, R. J. (2014). Relationship between passion and motivation for gaming in players of massively multiplayer online role-playing games. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17(5), 292-297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaetan, S., Bréjard, V., & Bonnet, A. (2016). Video games in adolescence and emotional functioning: Emotion regulation, emotion intensity, emotion expression, and alexithymia. Computers in Human Behavior, 61, 344–349. [Google Scholar]
- Gentile, D. A., Choo, H., Liau, A., Sim, T., Li, D., Fung, D., & Khoo, A. (2011). Pathological video game use among youths: A two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 127(2), e319-e329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gervasi, A. M., La Marca, L., Lombardo, E., Mannino, G., Iacolino, C., & Schimmenti, A. (2017b). Maladaptive personality traits and Internet addiction symptoms among young adults: A study based on the alternative DSM-5 model for personality disorders. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 14(1), 20-28. [Google Scholar]
- Gervasi, A. M., La Marca, L., Costanzo, A., Pace, U., Guglielmucci, F., & Schimmenti, A. (2017a). Personality and internet gaming disorder: a systematic review of recent literature. Current Addiction Reports, 4(3), 293-307. [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg, J. L., Lewis, S. E., & Dodd, D. K. (1999). Overlapping addictions and self-esteem among college man and women. Addictive Behaviors, 24(4), 565-571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion Regulation: Conceptual Foundations. In Gross J. J. (Ed.) Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3-24). New York, NY: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Guglielmucci, F., Saroldi, M., Zullo, G., Munno, D., & Granieri, A. (2017). Personality profiles and problematic Internet use in a sample of italian adolescents. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 14(1), 94-103. [Google Scholar]
- Ha, Y. M., & Hwang, W. J. (2014). Gender differences in Internet addiction associated with psychological health indicators among adolescents using a national web-based survey. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 12(5), 660–669. [Google Scholar]
- Ha, J. H., Yoo, H. J., Cho, I. H., Chin, B., Shin, D., & Kim, J. H. (2006). Psychiatric comorbidity assessed in Korean children and adolescents who screen positive for Internet addiction. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67(10), 821-826. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahn, T., Notebaert, K. H., Dresler, T., Kowarsch, L., Reif, A., & Fallgatter, A. J. (2014). Linking online gaming and addictive behavior: Converging evidence for a general reward deficiency in frequent online gamers. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hellström, C., Nilsson, K. W., Leppert, J., & Åslund, C. (2012). Influences of motives to play and time spent gaming on the negative consequences of adolescent online computer gaming. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(4), 1379-1387. [Google Scholar]
- Hemenover, S. H., & Bowman, N. D. (2018). Video games, emotion, and emotion regulation: Expanding the scope. Annals of the International Communication Association, 42(2), 125–143. [Google Scholar]
- Hollett, K. B., & Harris, N. (2019). Dimensions of emotion dysregulationassociatedwithproblemvideogaming. Addiction Research & Theory. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2019.1579801. [DOI]
- Hormes, J. M., Kearns, B., & Timko, C. A. (2014). Craving Facebook? Behavioral addiction to online social networking and its association with emotion regulation deficits. Addiction, 109(12), 2079-2088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hull, D. C., Williams, G. A., & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Video game characteristics, happiness and flow as predictors of addiction among video game players: A pilot study. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2(3), 145-152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hussain, Z., & Griffiths, M. D. (2009). The attitudes, feelings, and experiences of online gamers: A qualitative analysis. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12(6), 747-753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandri, T. A., Bonotis, K. S., Floros, G. D., & Zafiropoulou, M. M. (2014). Alexithymia components in excessive Internet users: A multi-factorial analysis. Psychiatry Research, 220(1-2), 348-355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014a). A conceptual and methodological critique of Internet addiction research: Towards a model of compensatory Internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 351–354. [Google Scholar]
- Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014b). Problematizing excessive online gaming and its psychological predictors. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 118–122. [Google Scholar]
- Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014c). The moderating role of psychosocial well-being on the relationship between escapism and excessive online gaming. Computers in Human Behavior, 38, 68–74. [Google Scholar]
- King, D. L., Delfabbro, P. H., Potenza, M. N., Demetrovics, Z., Billieux, J., & Brand, M. (2018). Internet gaming disorder should qualify as a mental disorder. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 52(7), 615-617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Király, O., Urbán, R., Griffiths, M. D., Ágoston, C., Nagygyörgy, K., Kökönyei, G., & Demetrovics, Z. (2015). The mediating effect of gaming motivation between psychiatric symptoms and problematic online gaming: An online survey. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 17(4), e88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirby, A., Jones, C., & Copello, A. (2014). The impact of massively multiplayer online role playing games (MMORPGs) on psychological wellbeing and the role of play motivations and problematic use. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 12(1), 36-51. [Google Scholar]
- Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Chen, C. C., Chen, S. H., & Yen, C. F. (2005). Gender differences and related factors affecting online gaming addiction among Taiwanese adolescents. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 193(4), 273-277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kormas, G., Critselis, E., Janikian, M., Kafetzis, D., & Tsitsika, A. (2011). Risk factors and psychosocial characteristics of potential problematic and problematic Internet use among adolescents: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health, 11, 595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuo, A., Lutz, R. J., & Hiler, J. L. (2016). Brave new World of Warcraft: A conceptual framework for active escapism. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 33(7), 498-506. [Google Scholar]
- Kuss, D. J., Louws, J., & Wiers, R. W. (2012). Online gaming addiction? Motives predict addictive play behavior in massively multiplayer online role-playing games. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 15(9), 480-485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laconi, S., Pires, S., & Chabrol, H. (2017). Internet gaming disorder, motives, game genres and psychopathology. Computers in Human Behavior, 75, 652–659. [Google Scholar]
- Leménager, T., Dieter, J., Hill, H., Koopmann, A., Reinhard, I., Sell, M., … Mann, K. (2014). Neurobiological correlates of physical self-concept and self-identification with avatars in addicted players of massively multiplayer online role-playing games (MMORPGs). Addictive Behaviors, 39(12), 1789-1797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, L., Yu, Q., Zhang, L., & Jin, S. (2015). The gender difference on Internet addictive among adolescent: The mediation effect of the differentiation of social and psychological situation in school. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23(6), 1044–1048. [Google Scholar]
- Maroney, N., Williams, B. J., Thomas, A., Skues, J., & Moulding, R. (2018). A stress-coping model of problem online video game use. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. doi: 10.1007/s11469-018-9887-7 [DOI]
- Meerkerk, G. J., van den Eijnden, R. J., Franken, I. H. A., & Garretsen, H. F. L. (2010). Is compulsive internet use related to sensitivity to reward and punishment, and impulsivity?. Computers in Human Behavior, 26(4), 729-735. [Google Scholar]
- Metcalf, O., & Pammer, K. (2014). Impulsivity and related neuropsychological features in regular and addictive first person shooter gaming. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 17(3), 147-152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mottram, A. J., & Fleming, M. J. (2009). Extraversion, impulsivity, and online group membership as predictors of problematic Internet use. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12(3), 319-321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parker, J. D., Keefer, K. V., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2008). Latent structure of the alexithymia construct: A taxometric investigation. Psychological Assessment, 20(4), 385-396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(6), 768-774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puerta-Cortés, D. X., Panova, T., Carbonell, X., & Chamarro, A. (2017). How passion and impulsivity influence a player’s choice of videogame, intensity of playing and time spent playing. Computers in Human Behavior, 66, 122–128. [Google Scholar]
- Rehbein, F., Staudt, A., Hanslmaier, M., & Kliem, S. (2016). Video game playing in the general adult population of Germany: Can higher gaming time of males be explained by gender specific genre preferences? Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 729–735. [Google Scholar]
- Reinecke, L. (2009). Games and recovery: The use of video and computer games to recuperate from stress and strain. Journal of Media Psychology, 21(3), 126–142. [Google Scholar]
- Ryan, R. M., Rigby, C. S., & Przybylski, A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: A self-determination theory approach. Motivation and Emotion, 30(4), 344-360. [Google Scholar]
- Schimmenti, A., & Caretti, V. (2010). Psychic retreats or psychic pits? Unbearable states of mind and technological addiction. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 27(2), 115-132. [Google Scholar]
- Schimmenti, A., & Caretti, V. (2017). Video-terminal dissociative trance: Toward a psychodynamic understanding of problematic Internet use. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 14(1), 64-72. [Google Scholar]
- Schimmenti, A., Infanti, A., Badoud, D., Laloyaux, J., & Billieux, J. (2017a). Schizotypal personality traits and problematic use of massively-multiplayer online role-playing games (MMORPGs). Computers in Human Behavior, 74, 286–293. [Google Scholar]
- Schimmenti, A., Musetti, A., Costanzo, A., Terrone, G., Maganuco, N. R., Rinella, C. A., & Gervasi, A. M. (2019a). The unfabulous four: Maladaptive personality functioning, insecure attachment, dissociative experiences, and problematic Internet use among young adults. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. doi: 10.1007/s11469-019-00079-0. [DOI]
- Schimmenti, A., Passanisi, A., Caretti, V., La Marca, L., Granieri, A., Iacolino, C., … Billieux, J. (2017b). Traumatic experiences, alexithymia, and Internet addiction symptoms among late adolescents: A moderated mediation analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 314–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schimmenti, A., Passanisi, A., Gervasi, A. M., Manzella, S., & Fama, F. I. (2014). Insecure attachment attitudes in the onset of problematic Internet use among late adolescents. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 45(5), 588-595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schimmenti, A., Santoro, G., La Marca, L., Costanzo, A., & Gervasi, A. M. (2019b). Emotion dysregulation: A correlation network analysis. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. Advance online publication.
- Schimmenti, A., & Starcevic, V. (2019). Logical fallacies in justifying problematic gaming as a mental disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. doi: 10.1177/0004867418821431 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Schimmenti, A., Starcevic, V., Gervasi, A., Deleuze, J., & Billieux, J. (2018). Interference with processing negative stimuli in problematic Internet users: Preliminary evidence from an emotional Stroop task. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 7(7), 177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scimeca, G., Bruno, A., Cava, L., Pandolfo, G., Muscatello, M. R. A., & Zoccali, R. (2014). The relationship between alexithymia, anxiety, depression, and Internet addiction severity in a sample of Italian high school students. The Scientific World Journal, Art. ID 504376, 1-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snodgrass, J. G., Bagwell, A., Patry, J. M., Dengah II, H. F., Smarr-Foster, C., Van Oostenburg, M., & Lacy, M. G. (2018). The partial truths of compensatory and poor-get-poorer Internet use theories: More highly involved videogame players experience greater psychosocial benefits. Computers in Human Behavior, 78, 10–25. [Google Scholar]
- Stavropoulos, V., Kuss, D. J., Griffiths, M. D., Wilson, P., & Motti-Stefanidi, F. (2017). MMORPG gaming and hostility predict Internet addiction symptoms in adolescents: An empirical multilevel longitudinal study. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 294–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M. (2013). Psychoanalysis and empirical research: The example of alexithymia. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 61(1), 99-133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsitsika, A., Critselis, E., Janikian, M., Kormas, G., & Kafetzis, D. A. (2011). Association between Internet gambling and problematic Internet use among adolescents. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27(3), 389-400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villani, D., Carissoli, C., Triberti, S., Marchetti, A., Gilli, G., & Riva, G. (2018). Videogames for emotion regulation: A systematic review. Games for Health Journal, 7(2), 85-99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wan, C. S., & Chiou, W. B. (2006). Why are adolescents addicted to online gaming? An interview study in Taiwan. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9(6), 762-766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Widyanto, L., & McMurran, M. (2004). The psychometric properties of Internet Addiction Test. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 7(4), 443-450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization (2018). ICD-11 beta draft—mortality and morbidity statistics. https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/enAccessed 12 February 2019.
- Yates, T. M., Gregor, M. A., & Haviland, M. G. (2012). Child maltreatment, alexithymia, and problematic Internet use in young adulthood. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 15(4), 219-225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yee, N. (2006). Motivations for play in online games. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9(6), 772-775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yildiz, M. A. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies as predictors of Internet addiction and smartphone addiction in adolescents. Journal of Educational Sciences & Psychology, 7(1), 66-78. [Google Scholar]
- Young, K. S. (1998). Caught in the net. New York: Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, Z., Zhu, H., Li, C., & Wang, J. (2014). Internet addictive individuals share impulsivity and executive dysfunction with alcohol-dependent patients. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]