Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Emerg Adulthood. 2021 Mar 12;9(5):531–540. doi: 10.1177/2167696820986742

Changes in college student alcohol use during the COVID-19 pandemic: Are perceived drinking norms still relevant?

Scott Graupensperger 1,*, Anna E Jaffe 2, Charles N B Fleming 1, Jason R Kilmer 1, Christine M Lee 1, Mary E Larimer 1
PMCID: PMC8664006  NIHMSID: NIHMS1724499  PMID: 34900403

Abstract

With widespread concern for increased alcohol use during the COVID-19 pandemic, there is a pressing need to examine changes in young adults’ alcohol use and to identify antecedents of increased use. We tested the hypothesis that self-reported changes in alcohol use during the pandemic (frequency, quantity, heavy episodic drinking) would relate to perceptions of peers’ changes in alcohol use. In April of 2020, 507 college students self-reported changes in their alcohol use and perceived changes in use for typical students at their university (i.e., norms). Most students in our sample reported decreased alcohol use and perceived decreases in peers’ alcohol use. Perceptions of peers’ changes in alcohol use behavior strongly related to changes in students’ own alcohol use. Findings provide strong support for norms-based strategies that can correct normative misperceptions by highlighting the fact that most college students are not in fact engaging in heavier alcohol use during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Keywords: COVID-19, Social Norms, Peer Influence, Personalized Normative Feedback, Heavy Episodic Drinking


The novel Coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic has had a tremendous impact on society. Within the United States, college students have faced notable lifestyle changes as a result of campus closures. While such physical distancing and isolation measures are critical to combating the spread of the virus, there is an urgent need to examine and understand the effects that these distressing times have had on health behaviors (Holmes et al., 2020). Notably, there is concern among alcohol researchers for increased alcohol use during the COVID-19 pandemic (Clay & Parker, 2020), and evidence that college students may be drinking more than prior to the pandemic (Charles et al., 2020; Lechner et al., 2020). Emerging evidence suggests, however, that adolescents and young adults may not be increasing their overall alcohol use, instead drinking more frequently but in lower quantities per occasion (Dumas et al., 2020; Graupensperger, Fleming, et al., 2020). The current study addresses the call to action for research examining pandemic-specific changes in alcohol use among college students.

Prior to restrictions related to COVID-19, alcohol use among college students was known to be prevalent and problematic, leading to many adverse health consequences (Hingson et al., 2017). It is nevertheless unclear how college student alcohol use has changed during this unique time. As campuses closed, many students may be living with their parents, which is associated with less alcohol use relative to living on or near college campuses (Patrick et al., 2020; Patrick & Terry-McElrath, 2017). Indeed, students who had easy access to alcohol and engaged in more social alcohol use prior to the pandemic, such as those in Greek fraternities and sororities (Scott-Sheldon et al., 2008), may drink less during the pandemic. Although physical distancing measures have likely led to fewer social gatherings involving alcohol use (e.g., house parties), students may have more free time while taking courses online and some may engage in more alcohol use to combat boredom and loneliness. Given that college students are a high-risk group for alcohol misuse, it is critical to examine the extent that alcohol use patterns have changed during the COVID-19 pandemic, and to identify potential risk and protective factors. Such information is needed to inform alcohol use interventions that are tailored to the unique circumstances surrounding the COVID-19 pandemic. Indeed, college students are in a critical stage for intervention as alcohol-related lifestyle habits are largely formed during early adulthood (Arria et al., 2016), meaning that increased alcohol use during the pandemic could have long-term negative effects.

The news media has highlighted recent upticks in alcohol sales and use during the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., Bote, 2020; Bremner, 2020; Lofton, 2020; Polkavic, 2020), which may send a message to college students that alcohol use among peers and the general public has increased during this time. Media messaging can have a salient influence on young adults’ substance use behaviors via social learning processes as reporting population-level increases in alcohol use portrays this behavior as normative (Jackson et al., 2018). The focus theory of normative conduct (Cialdini et al., 1990) highlights descriptive norms – perceptions of how others behave in a given situation – as a powerful influence on individuals’ behaviors. Young adults are particularly susceptible to peer influences and, as such, perceived peer drinking norms play a central role in shaping college students’ decisions to engage in alcohol use (Burnett et al., 2011; Teunissen et al., 2012). Perceived norms pertaining to alcohol use are indeed considered to be among the strongest influences on students’ drinking (Neighbors et al., 2007; Perkins, 2002); the belief that others engage in more alcohol use than oneself, corresponds with heavier drinking patterns (Lewis et al., 2015; Litt et al., 2015).

Social norms theory (Berkowitz, 2004) acknowledges that perceptions of others’ behaviors are often inaccurate, which is particularly problematic given that college students tend to overestimate their peers’ alcohol use (Cox et al., 2019; Perkins et al., 2015). Accordingly, norms-based interventions such as personalized normative feedback (PNF) have been designed to correct over-inflated normative perceptions, which can subsequently reduce students’ alcohol use (Dotson et al., 2015; Neighbors et al., 2018; Patrick et al., 2014). Largescale norm-correcting campaigns have also been shown to reduce college students’ alcohol use, but only among those who believed that their peers had decreased alcohol use following the campaign (Mattern & Neighbors, 2003). This finding indicates that changes in alcohol use behaviors are closely related to perceived changes among peers. Researchers have also found that perceived drinking norms may have within-person associations with young adults own alcohol use, such that young adults report relatively greater alcohol use at timepoints in which they perceived peers were drinking more than usual or were more approving of alcohol use than usual (Dumas et al., 2019; Graupensperger, Jaffe, et al., 2020).

There is strong theoretical support and empirical evidence of positive associations between perceived drinking norms and alcohol use, but it is unclear how event-specific changes in descriptive norms may relate to changes in college students’ alcohol use behaviors. Given that peer drinking norms may be a key antecedent for college students’ drinking during COVID-19, there is a clear rationale for examining potential influences of perceived norms regarding changes in peers’ alcohol use during the COVID-19 pandemic. Establishing a link between perceived changes in drinking norms and changes in individuals’ alcohol use specifically during this time of physical distancing is a logical next step toward developing COVID-19-specific harm-reduction interventions.

Current Study

The current study entailed a cross-sectional design in which data were collected during the first month of mitigation strategies for the COVID-19 pandemic. We examined college students’ reports of how their alcohol use, as well as the alcohol use of typical students at their university (i.e., perceived norms), have changed in terms of frequency, quantity, and heavy episodic drinking behaviors (i.e., 4+ drinks per occasion for women / 5+ drinks per occasion for men). The first aim was to descriptively examine the extent to which college students reported increasing/decreasing alcohol use, relative to before the pandemic. Although we anticipated that students, on average, may increase alcohol use during this time (Charles et al., 2020; Lechner et al., 2020), we also expected heterogeneity in these self-reported changes in students’ drinking patterns. Related to this first aim, we explored whether changes in alcohol use behaviors differed for students who reported living with their parents during the pandemic, and whether changes differed by Greek status (i.e., those in fraternity/sorority groups). The second aim of this study was to descriptively examine perceptions of peers’ drinking changes during the COVID-19 pandemic. Due in part to media reports of increased alcohol use, we anticipated that students would hold over-inflated perceptions regarding increases in peers’ alcohol use. Finally, the primary aim of this research was to examine the extent to which changes in students’ own alcohol use behaviors were associated with perceived changes in peer drinking norms. We hypothesized that perceptions of peers’ changes in alcohol use would be positively associated with changes in participants’ own alcohol use, even after controlling for relevant covariates regarding demographics (age, birth sex, living with parents) and typical alcohol use. Findings from this research will inform the timely development of a PNF intervention that is specific to normative influences on young adults’ alcohol use during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Method

Procedure and participants

In April of 2020, just over a month after local campus closures and shelter-in-place orders, we sent e-mail invitations to 1,603 randomly selected first, second, and third-year undergraduates from a large public university on the west coast to participate in a web-based survey. This survey served as a screener for enrollment into an ongoing alcohol use intervention study and was adapted to include items specific to alcohol use behaviors during COVID-19. Participation in this survey was incentivized by a $10 gift card and complete responses were received from 507 students (31.63% response rate; 65.48% women; Mage = 18.90 ± 0.93). The sample comprised 35.31% first-year students, 41.42% second-year students, and 22.88% third-year students. Most of the sample reported living at home with their parents during the COVID-19 pandemic (63.71%), which is much higher than reported in previous studies from this specific university (i.e., 10.95% and 13.90% reported living at home with parents in Lee et al., 2020 and Larimer et al., 2009, respectively). We can thus infer that most of the students who reported living with parents during the pandemic had moved back in from their usual college living arrangement (e.g., dorms). Others in the sample reported living in campus residence halls (9.07%), in fraternity/sorority housing (6.11%), in off-campus housing (19.53%), and other (1.58%). Approximately a quarter of the sample (23.67%) reported being affiliated with a Greek organization, which is slightly oversampled in relation to the university website that estimates approximately 15% of undergraduates are in a Greek organization. Pertaining to race, 46.35% of the sample identified as White/Caucasian, 37.48% identified as Asian/Asian-American, 8.48% identified as mixed-race, 2.37% identified as African American, and 5.32% identified as another race. Moreover, 9.47% identified as ethnically Hispanic. All study procedures received approval from the authors’ Institutional Review Board.

Measures

Alongside standard demographic items, six items were created to examine students’ self-reported changes in alcohol use and perceptions of peers’ changes in alcohol use. Three items asked participants to report changes in their alcohol use: Compared to before COVID-19, the: (1) Frequency of my drinking (how often I drink alcohol) has been… (2) Amount I drink on each drinking occasion has been… (3) Number of occasions on which I have had 4+(women)/5+(men) drinks has been… Response options were 1 (a lot less), 2 (less), 3 (the same), 4 (more) and 5 (a lot more). Using the same response options, three similar items assessed participants’ perceptions of how peers’ alcohol use has changed during the pandemic: Compared to before COVID-19, the: (1) Frequency of drinking for the typical [University Name] student (how often they drink alcohol) has been… (2) Amount the typical [University Name] student drinks on each drinking occasion has been… (3) Number of occasions on which the typical [University Name] student has had 4+(women)/5+(men) drinks has been… We also assessed the typical number of drinks that participants consumed in a given week during the last 30 days (during-COVID) using the Daily Drinking Questionnaire (DDQ) (Collins et al., 1985). This instrument asks participants to report the number of drinks they typically consumed on each day of a typical week in the past 30 days, and values are summed to create an index of typical weekly alcohol consumption.

Analysis

As a first step, response frequencies were examined for each item pertaining to changes in drinking and perceived changes in peers’ drinking. Bivariate correlations were estimated among study variables. Spearman’s rank-order correlations were computed for associations between the change in alcohol use and perceived change in drinking norms variables, whereas Kendall’s rank correlation was computed to estimated correlations between ranked change variables and other study variables. Next, separate multiple regression models were estimated predicting the three measures of change in respondents’ drinking. These models were specified as multivariable ordinal logistic regression models (sometimes referred to as cumulative probability models) to match the ordinal response options, and the proportional odds assumption was tested for each independent variable. Because this assumption is often violated in practice, researchers can instead use generalized ordinal logistic regression (i.e., partial proportional odds models) that can be used to restrict or control the distance between consecutive thresholds between ordinal response options (Williams, 2016).

The independent variable of interest was participants’ perceptions of peers’ changes in the drinking behavior being predicted in the respective model. For example, the model predicting participants’ changes in heavy episodic drinking occasions featured perceptions of peers’ changes in heavy episodic drinking occasions as the independent variable. Age, birth sex, whether participants were living at home with their parents, Greek status, and typical number of weekly drinks were entered as covariates in all models. Students who reported not living with their parents during the pandemic were collapsed to enable contrasting living with parents vs. not living with parents, as there were not enough students reporting other living arrangements to make specific comparisons (e.g., living on-campus).

Results

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations are presented in Table 1. Student reports of typical number of drinks per week during the COVID-19 pandemic were generally lower than anticipated. Only 10.38% of men reported consuming 14 drinks or more per week, and only 16.72% of women reported consuming 7 drinks or more per week (i.e., 14 drinks per week for men and 7 drinks per week for women are the recommended upper limits for low-risk alcohol use consumption suggested by the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). Moreover, students living at home during the pandemic reported significantly lower weekly alcohol use (Mdrinks = 2.45) relative to those living in fraternity/sorority housing (Mdrinks = 12.70, p < .001), but not significantly lower than those in on-campus housing (Mdrinks = 2.73, p = .99) or in off-campus housing (Mdrinks = 3.34, p = .61).

Table 1.

Bivariate correlations and descriptive statistics for variables (N = 507).

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Change in Alcohol Use Frequency
2. Change in Number of Drinks per Occasion .79**
3. Change in Heavy Episodic Drinking Occasions .70** .80**
4. Norms for Peers’ Change in Alcohol Use Frequency .47 ** .38** .36**
5. Norms for Peers’ Change in Number of Drinks per Occasion .39** .42 ** .36** .82**
6. Norms for Peers’ Change in Heavy Episodic Drinking Occasions .34** .34** .36 ** .81** .83**
7. Typical Number of Drinks in a Week (DDQ) −.06 −.11** −.16** −.13** −.11* −.10*
8. Age .06 .03 −.01 .06 .05 .04 .08
9. Birth Sex (M = 0; F = 1) −.01 −.07 −.03 −.06 −.04 −.09* −.08 −.02
10. Living with Parents (No = 0; Yes = 1) −.09* −.06 −.04 −.11* −.06 −.10* −.18** −.25** −.01
11. Greek Affiliation (No = 0; Yes = 1) −.24** −.24** −.20** −.20** −.20** −.15** .28** .11* .07 −.05
Mean 2.27 2.17 2.02 2.44 2.42 2.25 3.20 18.90 - -
SD 1.10 0.97 1.01 1.24 1.13 1.15 5.43 0.93 - -
Range 1 – 5 1 – 5 1 – 5 1 – 5 1 – 5 1 – 5 0 – 34 18 – 23 - -
Frequencies of Response Options (%)
 1. A lot less 183
(36.09)
176
(34.78)
232
(45.85)
136
(26.82)
124
(24.51)
160
(31.62)
 2. Less 69
(13.61)
91
(17.98)
47
(9.29)
176
(34.71)
173
(34.19)
171
(33.79)
 3. The same 202
(39.84)
218
(43.08)
214
(42.29)
52
(10.26)
97
(19.17)
74
(14.62)
 4. More 43
(8.48)
18
(3.56)
12
(2.47)
120
(23.67)
99
(19.57)
88
(17.39)
 5. A lot more 10
(1.97)
3
(0.59)
1
(0.20)
23
(4.54)
13
(2.57)
13
(2.57)

Note: Bivariate associations among change in alcohol use and perceived change in drinking norms variables were calculated using Spearman’s rank-order correlations, and associations between ranked change variables and other study variables were calculated using Kendall’s rank correlation. Bolded cells shaded grey show correlations between individual change and norms for peers’ change in corresponding alcohol use behaviors.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

Mean values for participants’ reported changes in alcohol use and perceptions of peers’ changes were all below the scale midpoint, indicating that most participants in this sample reported drinking less alcohol during the COVID-19 pandemic and reported perceptions that peers were also engaging in less alcohol use, on average. There was nevertheless variability in these perceptions: some participants reported increased alcohol use and many perceived peers to have increased alcohol use (a visual display of the frequencies is shown in Figure 1). The frequencies listed in Table 1 also suggest that participants may hold over-inflated misperceptions pertaining to whether peers who have increased alcohol use during the pandemic, with 19.96% to 28.21% perceiving an increase in use, compared to only 2.67% to 10.45% reporting increases in personal alcohol use (depending on the specific outcome).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Histograms displaying the frequencies of responses to changes in alcohol use and perceived changes in descriptive norms. HED = Heavy episodic drinking. 1 = A lot less, 2 = Less, 3 = The same amount, 4 = More, 5 = A lot more.

For each discrete form of alcohol use behavior, there were moderate positive correlations between self-reported changes and perceived changes among peers. Significant negative correlations indicated that college students living at home with their parents during the pandemic were more likely to have decreased their alcohol use frequency, and more likely to have perceived decreases in peers’ drinking frequency and heavy episodic drinking. Moreover, students living at home with parents reported less weekly alcohol use (i.e., typical number of drinks per week) during the pandemic. Greek-affiliated students were more likely to have decreased their alcohol use across all three indicators and were more likely to have perceived decreases in peers’ alcohol use, but still reported more weekly drinks during the pandemic, relative to non-Greek-affiliated students.

Pertaining to the multivariable ordinal regression models, a preliminary check of the proportional odds assumption revealed that the weekly drinking variable (DDQ) violated this assumption in all three models, likely given the large variability in participants’ weekly alcohol consumption (SD = 5.43). As such, we employed a generalized ordinal regression approach and specified an ‘equidistant’ threshold, which restricts the distance between consecutive response options to be equally spaced (Christensen, 2018).1 The resulting models met the assumption of proportional odds for all covariates, indicating that the coefficients for each predictor value are consistent, or have parallel slopes, across each level of the outcome variable.

The results from the generalized ordinal logistic regression models are presented in Table 2. Changes in alcohol use behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic were not associated with age, sex, living with parents, or number of drinks students consumed in a typical week during the pandemic. Strong negative associations indicated that Greek-affiliated students reported decreasing all three indices of alcohol use to a greater extent than those who did not report a Greek affiliation. Perceived changes in peer drinking norms were positively associated with participants’ own changes in alcohol use behaviors. This effect held across changes in (a) frequency of alcohol use (β = .72), (b) quantity of alcohol use per occasion (β = .69), and (c) the number of heavy episodic drinking occasions (β = .62). The adjusted odds ratios of these effects were around 2 (i.e., 2.06, 1.99, and 1.86), indicating that a one-unit increase in category of perceived change in peers’ alcohol use (e.g., ‘a lot more’ vs. ‘more’) doubled or nearly doubled the odds of being in a higher category of self-reported change in alcohol use.

Table 2.

Multivariable generalized ordinal logistic regression models estimating associations between changes in alcohol use behaviors and perceived changes in peers’ alcohol use behaviors (N = 507).

Change in Alcohol Use
Frequency
Change in Amount of Alcohol Use
per Occasion
Change in Heavy Episodic
Drinking Occasions
Independent Variables β SE OR 95% CI β SE OR 95% CI β SE OR 95% CI



  Age .03 .09 1.03 (0.86, 1.23) .03 .09 1.03 (0.86, 1.24) −.01 .09 0.99 (0.82, 1.19)
  Birth Sex (M = 0, F = 1) .05 .13 1.05 (0.82, 1.36) −.05 .13 0.95 (0.73, 1.22) .11 .13 1.12 (0.85, 1.44)
  Living with Parents (No = 0, Yes = 1) −.23 .18 0.79 (0.56, 1.13) −.21 .18 0.81 (0.57, 1.16) −.06 .18 0.95 (0.66, 1.36)
  Greek Affiliation (No = 0; Yes = 1) −.91*** .23 0.40 (0.26, 0.62) −.86*** .22 0.42 (0.27, 0.65) −.82*** .23 0.44 (0.28, 0.69)
  Typical Number of Weekly Drinks (DDQ) .02 .02 1.02 (0.98, 1.05) .01 .02 1.01 (0.97, 1.04) .01 .02 1.01 (0.98, 1.05)
  Perceived Norm for Peers’ Change in Drinking Behavior .72*** .08 2.06 (1.78, 2.40) .69*** .08 1.99 (1.70, 2.34) .62*** .08 1.86 (1.59, 2.17)
Threshold Coefficients (specified to be ‘equidistan’)
  First Threshold 1.02 1.75 0.82 1.78 0.62 1.84
 Spacing of Consecutive Thresholds 1.54 0.08 1.75 0.09 1.43 0.08
Nagelkerke Pseudo-R2 .29 .24 .17

Note:

The perceived norm variable in each model refers to perceptions of peers’ changes for the behavior that is being predicted. β = Logistic regression coefficient. SE = Standard error. OR = Proportional odds ratio – for every one-unit increase in the independent variable, the odds of increasing one’s alcohol use behavior is multiplied by the OR value, holding constant all other variables.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < 0.001.

Discussion

Despite being a high-risk population for alcohol use, descriptive findings from this study suggest that college students, on average, reported decreased alcohol use during the initial weeks of the COVID-19 pandemic. In fact, many students reported drinking ‘a lot less’ during this time. There was nevertheless heterogeneity indicating that some students are indeed engaging in more alcohol use, relative to before the COVID-19 pandemic. Furthermore, decreased use on average may not mean safe or non-problematic levels of alcohol use for everyone, as 10.38% of men and 16.72% of women reported drinking at levels above NIAAA recommendations. Students generally perceived decreases in peers’ alcohol use, but many also indicted that they thought peers have increased their alcohol use during the pandemic, which represent a potentially at-risk subgroup of college students.

In support of the primary study hypothesis, perceptions of peers’ changes in alcohol use behavior were strongly associated with self-reported changes in students’ own alcohol use and this association held across several indices: alcohol use frequency, quantity of alcohol use during a drinking occasion, and heavy episodic drinking. This robust positive association between college students’ changes in alcohol use behaviors and perceptions of changes in peer drinking norms also held after controlling for demographic covariates and typical alcohol use. While this shows that those who perceived peers as having increased alcohol use may be at risk for increasing their own alcohol use, it also follows that those who believe their peers have decreased their alcohol use may be more likely to have decreased their own use. These findings build on prior literature detailing strong associations between behaviors and perceived norms (Neighbors et al., 2007; Perkins, 2002), and suggest that these associations may persist even in the context of physical distancing, when most students are no longer located on or near campus. Given evidence that drinking norms for proximal referents (e.g., close friends) may be a stronger predictor of students’ alcohol use than distal referents (e.g., typical university students), an important future direction is to examine students’ perceptions of how their close friends’ alcohol use may have changed during the pandemic (Neighbors et al., 2008). These proximal norms may be particularly salient while students are engaging in physical distancing as they are likely only keeping in touch with closer friends and having less exposure to other students on campus.

Alongside the pressing need to examine health-risk behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic (Holmes et al., 2020), the current study makes novel theoretical and practical contributions to our understanding of normative influences and how they can be employed within harm-reduction strategies. Findings revealed that although most students perceived a decrease in peers’ use, a number of students (20-28%) thought that peers had increased their alcohol use during the pandemic, whereas self-report data showed that in fact very few students (3-10%) reported having increased their alcohol use. Considering that young adults may hold overinflated normative perceptions pertaining to the extent that peers have increased alcohol use, norms-based interventions must be rapidly adapted to address high-risk drinking during the COVID-19 pandemic. Because there was a subset of college students who reported increased alcohol use and held normative perceptions that peers have increased their alcohol use, PNF strategies that correct normative misperceptions may be particularly valuable for this at-risk subset of students who hold misperceptions that peers have increased their alcohol use.

Whereas we have strong theoretical and empirical support that perceived norms predict one’s own behavior at a static-level (Cialdini et al., 1990; Neighbors et al., 2007; Perkins, 2002), these novel findings extend this association to behavioral changes around a specific event. Alongside perceptions of what others typically do, people may be particularly motivated to change their behavior if they believe that others are changing their behavior as well (Sparkman & Walton, 2019). Although additional translational studies are needed, there may be value in tailoring norms-based approaches to include normative information regarding peers’ reductions in alcohol use, which aligns with findings that norm-correcting campaigns may only work for those who believe others are reducing their alcohol use as well (Mattern & Neighbors, 2003). Additional research is therefore needed to test whether change-specific norms are comparable to, or even more influential than static norms.

Finally, it is important to consider the unique impact of campus closures on students’ alcohol use behaviors. Many college students are not able to legally purchase alcohol and, as such, students may have had greater access to alcohol prior to COVID-19 when living on or near campus (e.g., house parties). This may explain why Greek-affiliated students, who tend to drink more than non-Greek-affiliated students, reported greater decreases in alcohol use. That is, because Greek-affiliation is strongly associated with heavy alcohol use, Greek-affiliated students in our sample had more room to decrease. Interestingly, however, the Greek-affiliated students still reported engaging in more weekly alcohol use relative to their non-Greek peers.

We also included an indicator of students’ current living situation to examine the potential effects of living at home with parents. Bivariate associations indeed revealed that those who lived with their parents during the pandemic engaged in lower levels of alcohol use, but the regression models indicated that there were no statistically significant effects of living situation on changes in students’ alcohol use behaviors after accounting for change-related norms, typical drinking, age, and birth sex. However, it is important to note that we only asked about current living situation, whereas specifically asking if students had moved back in with parents may have enabled deeper exploration of how living with parents relates to student alcohol use during the COVID-19 pandemic. An additional next step would be to examine parental permissiveness as this factor could play an important role in terms of accessibility to alcohol while living at home.

Limitations

Despite the timeliness of this research, several limitations should be considered. Although there was a strong theoretical rationale for examining norms as a predictor of behavior, our cross-sectional design precludes making causal inferences or assessing the direction of these effects. It is indeed possible that students who have increased their drinking during the pandemic assume that their peers must also be engaging in increased use, for example. Also related to the cross-sectional design, we had no access to students’ level of alcohol use prior to the pandemic, which would have allowed us to confirm self-reported changes in alcohol use. Changes in drinking during the pandemic is an emerging area of study, and although the current study focused on normative influences as one of the most robust predictors of college students’ alcohol use (Neighbors et al., 2007), additional antecedents should be studied in future research. For example, some students may be facing more extreme distress during this time, which could potentially moderate associations between perceived changes in norms and changes in alcohol use behavior. We also note that the response rate for the current study (31.63%) was relatively low, likely due to random invitation via e-mail and modest incentive ($10 gift card). Also, circumstances regarding the COVID-19 pandemic may have contributed to the low response rate (e.g., students receiving many e-mails from university administration). Non-response bias may be a concern for all COVID-19 research studies. Although participation in college student surveys has declined over recent decades, low response rates do not necessarily bias the results of survey-based studies (Fosnacht et al., 2017). Because the data were collected from a single U.S. university, additional studies are needed to assess the extent to which these findings generalize. A final consideration is that these data were collected roughly one month following campus closures and shelter-in-place orders, which may provide only an initial glimpse at how the early stages of the pandemic impacted students’ alcohol use and other health behaviors. As the pandemic is ongoing, continuing to monitor changes in students’ health behaviors is crucial.

Conclusion

Despite concerns of increases in alcohol use, the average college student in the current study self-reported decreases in alcohol use behavior since the COVID-19 pandemic began. Building on prior theoretical and empirical research demonstrating normative perceptions as a predictor of behavior, normative perceptions of changes in drinking for a typical college student were associated with personal changes in drinking. These findings highlight the importance of normative perceptions for drinking behavior in college students, even while students are physically distanced from campus and peers. Although norms and personal drinking were strongly associated on average, a sizable number of students may have overestimated their peers’ drinking during the pandemic, indicating a need for norm-correcting interventions (e.g., PNF) to further reduce alcohol-related harm. Such strategies may be especially important for those students who may have increased their own alcohol use in response to normative influences (i.e., thinking that the typical university student has increased drinking).

Certainly, when campuses return to “normal” with fully occupied residence halls, fraternities, sororities, and off-campus houses, it will be equally important to assess behavior related to alcohol use and for campuses to have a prevention plan in place. For students who were drinking heavily prior to the pandemic and experienced a reduction in tolerance to alcohol, resuming drinking the way they did before schools closed could pose increased risk for harmful outcomes. For students who historically report alcohol use to manage social anxiety or social awkwardness, the reality of having had limited face-to-face interaction with peers for months could lead to excessive use upon a return to social gatherings. Moreover, those who experienced increased distress during the pandemic (and who reported increased substance use in response to unwanted affect), providing access to support services upon a return to campus will be essential. Additional research to understand college student alcohol use patterns and predictors during the COVID-19 pandemic is nevertheless critical to further reduce health-risk behavior during these challenging times, as will be researching possible changes once the pandemic is behind us.

Acknowledgements:

This research was funded by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (R01AA012547, R37AA012547, and T32AA007455). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

Footnotes

1

Models estimated using ordinal logistic regression were nearly identical to those using generalized ordinal logistic regression, but the latter approach is warranted given minor violation of the proportional odds assumption.

References

  1. Arria AM, Caldeira KM, Allen HK, Vincent KB, Bugbee BA, & O’Grady KE (2016). Drinking like an adult? Trajectories of alcohol use patterns before and after college graduation. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 40, 583–590. 10.1111/acer.12973 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Berkowitz AD (2004). The social norms approach: Theory, research and annotated bibliography. Higher Education Center for Alcohol and Other Drug Abuse and Violence Prevention. US Department of Education. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bote J (2020). Americans are drinking more during the COVID-19 pandemic. But how much alcohol is too much? USA Today, https://chicago.suntimes.com/coronavirus/2020/8/14. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bremner J (2020). U.S. alcohol sales increase 55 percent in one week amid coronavirus pandemic. Newsweek. [Google Scholar]
  5. Burnett S, Sebastian C, Cohen Kadosh K, & Blakemore SJ (2011). The social brain in adolescence: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1654–1664. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Charles N, Strong SJ, Burns LC, Bullerjahn MR, & Serafine KM (2020). Increased mood disorder symptoms, perceived stress, and alcohol use among college students during the COVID-19 pandemic. PsyArXiv, Pre-Print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Christensen RHB (2018). Cumulative Link Models for Ordinal Regression with the R Package ordinal. Journal of Statistical Software, https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/ordinal/vi. [Google Scholar]
  8. Cialdini RB, Reno RR, & Kallgren CA (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1015–1026. 10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Clay JM, & Parker MO (2020). Alcohol use and misuse during the COVID-19 pandemic: A potential public health crisis? The Lancet Public Health, 2667(20), 30088. 10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30088-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Collins RL, Parks GA, & Marlatt GA (1985). Social determinants of alcohol consumption: The effects of social interaction and model status on the self-administration of alcohol. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 189–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cox MJ, DiBello AM, Meisel MK, Ott MQ, Kenney SR, Clark MA, & Barnett NP (2019). Do misperceptions of peer drinking influence personal drinking behavior? Results from a complete social network of first-year college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, Online First, 10.1037/adb0000455 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dotson KB, Dunn ME, & Bowers CA (2015). Stand-alone personalized normative feedback for college student drinkers: A meta-analytic review, 2004 to 2014. PLoS ONE, 10, 1–17. 10.1371/journal.pone.0139518 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dumas TM, Davis JP, & Neighbors C (2019). How much does your peer group really drink? Examining the relative impact of overestimation, actual group drinking and perceived campus norms on university students’ heavy alcohol use. Addictive Behaviors, 40, 409–414. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.11.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dumas TM, Ph D, Ellis W, Ph D, Litt DM, & Ph D (2020). What does adolescent substance use look Llike during the COVID-19 pandemic? Examining changes in frequency, social contexts, and pandemic-related predictors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 67, 354–361. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.06.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fosnacht K, Howe E, & Peck LK (2017). How important are high response rates for college surveys? The Review of Higher Education, 40, 245–265. [Google Scholar]
  16. Graupensperger S, Fleming CB, Jaffe AE, Rhew IC, Patrick ME, & Lee CM (2020). Changes in young adults’ alcohol and marijuana use, norms, and motives from before to during the COVID-19 pandemic. Manuscript Submitted for Publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Graupensperger S, Jaffe AE, Hultgren BA, Rhew IC, Lee CM, & Larimer ME (2020). The dynamic nature of injunctive norms and time-varying associations with college student drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, Advance Ahead of Print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hingson R, Zha W, & Smyth DP (2017). Magnitude and trends in heavy episodic drinking, alcohol-impaired driving, and alcohol-related mortality and overdose hospitalizations among emerging adults of college ages 18–24 in the United States. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 78, 540–548. 10.15288/jsad.2017.78.540 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Holmes EA, O’Connor RC, Perry VH, Tracey I, Wessely S, Arseneault L, Ballard C, Christensen H, Cohen Silver R, Everall I, Ford T, John A, Kabir T, King K, Madan I, Michie S, Przybylski AK, Shafran R, Sweeney A, … Bullmore E (2020). Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: A call for action for mental health science. The Lancet Psychiatry, 0366, 1–14. 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30168-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jackson KM, Janssen T, & Gabrielli J (2018). Media/marketing influences on adolescent and young adult substance abuse. Current Addiction Reports, 5, 146–157. 10.1007/s40429-018-0199-6.Media/Marketing [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Larimer ME, Kaysen DL, Lee CM, Kilmer JR, Lewis MA, Dillworth T, Montoya HD, & Neighbors C (2009). Evaluating level of specificity of normative referents in relation to personal drinking behavior. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. Supplement, 16, 115–121. 10.15288/jsads.2009.s16.115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lechner WV, Laurene KR, Patel S, Anderson M, Grega C, & Kenne DR (2020). Changes in alcohol use as a function of psychological distress and social support following COVID-19 related university closings. Addictive Behaviors, 110, 106527. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106527 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lee CM, Kilmer JR, Neighbors C, Cadigan JM, Fairlie AM, Patrick ME, Logan DE, Walter T, White HR, & Lee CM (2020). A Marijuana consequences checklist for young adults with implications for brief motivational intervention research. Prevention Science, Advance Ahead of Print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lewis MA, Litt DM, & Neighbors C (2015). The chicken or the egg: Examining temporal precedence among attitudes, injunctive norms, and college student drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 76, 594–601. 10.15288/jsad.2015.76.594 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Litt DM, Lewis MA, Rhew IC, Hodge KA, & Kaysen DL (2015). Reciprocal relationships over time between descriptive norms and alcohol use in young adult sexual minority women. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29, 885–893. 10.1037/adb0000122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lofton J (2020). How much more is the U.S. drinking during coronavirus spread? A lot. Michigan Live, https://www.mlive.com/coronavirus/2020/04/how-much. [Google Scholar]
  27. Mattern JL, & Neighbors C (2003). Social norms campaigns: Examining the relationship between changes in perceived norms and changes in drinking levels. 489–493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, (n.d.). NIAAA Newsletter, https://www.rethinkingdrinking.niaaa.nih.gov/How-m. Retrieved May 29, 2020, from https://www.rethinkingdrinking.niaaa.nih.gov/How-much-is-too-much/Is-your-drinking-pattern-risky/Drinking-Levels.aspx
  29. Neighbors C, DiBello AM, Young CM, Steers M-LN, Rinker DV, Rodriguez LM, Blanton H, & Lewis MA (2018). Personalized normative feedback for heavy drinking: An application of deviance regulation theory. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2018.11.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Neighbors C, Lee CM, Lewis MA, Fossos N, & Larimer ME (2007). Are social norms the best predictor of outcomes among heavy-drinking college students? Journal of Studies on Alcohol & Drugs, 68, 556–565. 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.05.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Neighbors C, O’Connor RM, Lewis MA, Chawla N, Lee CM, & Fossos N (2008). The relative impact of injunctive norms on college student drinking: The role of reference group. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 22, 576–581. 10.1037/a0013043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Patrick ME, Lee CM, & Neighbors C (2014). Web-based intervention to change perceived norms of college student alcohol use and sexual behavior on Spring Break. Addictive Behaviors, 39(3), 600–606. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.11.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Patrick ME, & Terry-McElrath YM (2017). High-intensity drinking by underage young adults in the United States. Addiction, 112, 82–93. 10.1111/add.13556 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Patrick ME, Terry-McElrath YM, Evans-Polce RJ, & Schulenberg JE (2020). Negative alcohol-related consequences experienced by young adults in the past 12 months: Differences by college attendance, living situation, binge drinking, and sex. Addictive Behaviors, 105, 106320. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Perkins HW (2002). Social norms and the prevention of alcohol misuse in collegiate contexts. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Supplement, 14, 164–172. 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Perkins HW, Haines MP, & Rice R (2015). Misperceiving the college drinking norm and related problems: A nationwide study of exposure to prevention information, perceived norms and student alcohol misuse. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66, 470–478. 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.470 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Polkavic G (2020). Pandemic drives alcohol sales — and raises concerns about substance abuse. USC News, https://news.usc.edu/168549/covid-19-alcohol-sales-abuse-stress-relapse-usc-experts/ [Google Scholar]
  38. Scott-Sheldon LAJ, Carey KB, & Carey MP (2008). Health behaviors and college students: Does Greek affiliation matter? Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 31, 61–70. 10.1038/jid.2014.371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Sparkman G, & Walton GM (2019). Witnessing change: Dynamic norms help resolve diverse barriers to personal change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 82, 238–252. 10.1016/j.jesp.2019.01.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Teunissen HA, Spijkerman R, Prinstein MJ, Cohen GL, Engels RCME, & Scholte RHJ (2012). Adolescents’ conformity to their peers’ pro-alcohol and anti-alcohol norms: The power of popularity. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 36, 1257–1267. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01728.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Williams R (2016). Understanding and interpreting generalized ordered logit models. The Journal of Mathematical Sociology, 40, 7–20. 10.1080/0022250X.2015.1112384 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES