Skip to main content
NeuroImage : Clinical logoLink to NeuroImage : Clinical
. 2021 Dec 7;33:102906. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102906

Effective connectivity in subcortical visual structures in de novo Patients with Parkinson’s Disease

Emmanuelle Bellot a, Louise Kauffmann b, Véronique Coizet a, Sara Meoni a,b,c, Elena Moro a,b, Michel Dojat a,
PMCID: PMC8670854  PMID: 34891045

Graphical abstract

graphic file with name ga1.jpg

Abbreviations: BOLD, blood oxygen level-dependent; DCM, dynamic causal modeling; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus; PD, Parkinson’s disease; ROI, region of interest; SC, superior colliculus; V1, primary visual cortex

Keywords: Dynamic causal modeling, fMRI, Human vision, Neuroimaging, Superior colliculus

Highlights

  • First study addressing effective connectivity in the visual network of PD patients.

  • Superior colliculus (SC) has a causal role in luminance contrast processing.

  • Connectivity between SC, LGN and V1 is altered in PD patients.

  • Visual deficits seem due to a deficiency in connectivity from SC to LGN and V1.

  • L-Dopa treatment over a six-month period is insufficient to restore connectivity.

Abstract

Background

Parkinson’s disease (PD) manifests with the appearance of non-motor symptoms before motor symptoms onset. Among these, dysfunctioning visual structures have recently been reported to occur at early disease stages.

Objective

This study addresses effective connectivity in the visual network of PD patients.

Methods

Using functional MRI and dynamic causal modeling analysis, we evaluated the connectivity between the superior colliculus, the lateral geniculate nucleus and the primary visual area V1 in de novo untreated PD patients (n = 22). A subset of the PD patients (n = 8) was longitudinally assessed two times at two months and at six months after starting dopaminergic treatment. Results were compared to those of age-matched healthy controls (n = 22).

Results

Our results indicate that the superior colliculus drives cerebral activity for luminance contrast processing both in healthy controls and untreated PD patients. The same effective connectivity was observed with neuromodulatory differences in terms of neuronal dynamic interactions. Our main findings were that the modulation induced by luminance contrast changes of the superior colliculus connectivity (self-connectivity and connectivity to the lateral geniculate nucleus) was inhibited in PD patients (effect of contrast: p = 0.79 and p = 0.77 respectively). The introduction of dopaminergic medication in a subset (n = 8) of the PD patients failed to restore the effective connectivity modulation observed in the healthy controls.

Interpretation

The deficits in luminance contrast processing in PD was associated with a deficiency in connectivity adjustment from the superior colliculus to the lateral geniculate nucleus and to V1. No differences in cerebral blood flow were observed between controls and PD patients suggesting that the deficiency was at the neuronal level. Administration of a dopaminergic treatment over six months was not able to normalize the observed alterations in inter-regional coupling. These findings highlight the presence of early dysfunctions in primary visual areas, which might be used as early markers of the disease.

1. Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder affecting more than six million people worldwide (Collaborators, 2018). The onset of the pathological process and the so-called prodromal phase of PD is believed to start 10–20 years before the classical motor manifestations (tremor, rigidity and bradykinesia) (Mahlknecht et al., 2015). Several non-motor symptoms develop during this prolonged prodromal phase (Siderowf and Lang, 2012), especially sensory dysfunctions like hyposmia (Lerner and Bagic, 2008, Siderowf et al., 2012) and oculo-visual changes in pupil reactivity, color vision or visuo-motor adaptation (Armstrong, 2015). Several signs of a dysfunctioning visual pathway appear from early stages of PD (Weil et al., 2016) in the retina (Ahn et al., 2018) and progress along with the disease to higher visual brain areas, from V1 to the fusiform gyrus (Cardoso et al., 2010). PD patients show visuospatial and visuoperceptual deficits, which reflect structural changes in temporoparietal cortical regions (Pereira et al., 2009). Considering the systematic occurrence of neuron degeneration in subcortical structures before invasion of the cortex, alterations would be expected in brain regions involved in the first steps of visual processing such as the superior colliculus (SC) and the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN); such changes may constitute early biomarkers of the disease.

Neuroimaging is a powerful tool for improving our understanding of both regional-specific dysfunction (Grafton et al., 1994) and brain connectivity changes in PD (Rowe, 2010). Several studies have explored resting-state functional connectivity (i.e. the statistical dependency of interregional activity) (Hacker et al., 2012, Helmich et al., 2010, Skidmore et al., 2011, van Eimeren et al., 2009, Wu et al., 2009). Fewer studies have examined changes in effective connectivity, i.e. how an activity network involving different brain regions is affected by a pathology, focusing on the detection of changes in cortical motor network coupling (Buijink et al., 2015, Dirkx et al., 2016, Marreiros et al., 2013, Nettersheim et al., 2019, Palmer et al., 2009, Rowe et al., 2002, Rowe et al., 2010) and modulatory actions of deep brain stimulation (Kahan et al., 2012, Kahan et al., 2014). To our knowledge, no study has been published concerning the subcortical and cortical effective connectivity in the visual network and how it is affected by PD.

Using functional MRI (fMRI) in healthy subjects of different ages, we showed an increase in the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) responses along the visual pathway, within the SC, the LGN and V1, in response to increasing luminance contrast in the range of 1–9%, which changed with normal aging (see Fig. 1 for a schematic representation of the fMRI data) (Bellot et al., 2016). In a subsequent study performed in de novo PD patients, we found already present a functional deficit in both the SC and the LGN that was not compensated for by dopaminergic treatment at six months (see Supplementary Materiel, SM: Fig. S1) (Moro et al., 2020). The activation in our three regions of interest (ROIs) was correlated with luminance contrast modulation but did not inform us about the ROIs interactions. The main aim of the present study was to further investigate these findings by addressing brain connectivity, i.e., interactions between our visual ROIs, and exploring how visual information processing within this network might be already affected in de novo untreated PD patients. We hypothesized that the effective connectivity between the SC, the LGN and V1 would be altered in de novo PD patients compared to age-matched healthy controls and that the initiation of dopaminergic treatment over six months would not restore the inter-regional coupling. We analyzed effective connectivity using dynamic causal modeling (DCM), a probabilistic (Bayesian) modeling technique that estimates the coupling between selected brain regions based on fMRI data i.e., their BOLD responses to a specific stimulation, in our case visual stimuli. The present study is focused on the analysis of effective connectivity in PD patients and healthy controls based on the data published in (Bellot et al., 2016, Moro et al., 2020).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Illustration of the visual stimulus and the three subcortical and cortical regions of interest involved in the first image processing steps and studied with fMRI. Black arrows indicate the anatomical connections based on the literature. SC = superior colliculus, LGN = lateral geniculate nucleus, V1 = primary visual area.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Participants

Thirty healthy subjects (CT-30) and 22 de novo PD patients were recruited at the Movement Disorders Center of the Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Grenoble (Grenoble, France). Inclusion criteria were: recent diagnosis of PD (<1 year of motor symptoms’ onset) according to the Movement Disorder Society (MDS) clinical diagnostic criteria of Parkinson’s disease (Postuma et al., 2015), stage 1 or 2 of the Hoehn and Yahr scale (Hoehn and Yahr, 1967), with no psychiatric disorders, no antiparkinsonian treatment, absence of ophthalmological issues (cataract, retinal pathologies or glaucoma) and no contraindication to brain MRI (including relevant resting tremor). A subset of this control group, CT-22, was age-matched with the PD patients. Additionally, eight patients from the PD-dn group, who had started L-dopa or dopamine agonist treatment after the first brain MRI study, were further evaluated twice: at six weeks to two months (session S1, resp. PD-2), and again at six months (session S2, resp. PD-6) after anti-PD treatment onset. All the selected participants were enrolled in our two previous studies (Bellot et al., 2016, Moro et al., 2020). All participants provided written informed consent to the study. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (ID RCB-2012-A00310-43 and ID RCB-2014-A01835-42) and is registered at ClinicalTrial.gov (NCT02488395).

2.2. fMRI design and procedure

The techniques used for visual stimuli and MRI acquisition have been described in detail elsewhere (Bellot et al., 2016). Briefly, subjects were presented in each visual hemi-field with a series of achromatic checkerboards, viewed via a mirror fixed on the head coil and flashing at a frequency of 4 Hz on a grey background, with four levels of luminance contrast (1, 3, 5 and 9%) to avoid SC saturation (Schneider and Kastner, 2005). A block-design visual paradigm was used with each luminance contrast level presented in separate 12-s blocks. To ensure accurate activation localization in SC and limit partial volume effect, 32-channel SENSE coil imaging was performed at 3T, with a spatial resolution of 1.5 mm in each direction for EPI sequence and an acquisition volume centered on the SC structure. A high-resolution T1-weighted (MPRAGE) sequence and a T1-weighted FGATIR sequence were acquired to facilitate the manual delineation of SC and LGN (see details in SM). The cardiac signal was indirectly recorded at 100 Hz using finger photoplethysmography. Functional data analysis was performed using the single-participant general linear model (GLM) for block-designs with SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, U.K.) implemented in Matlab. The design matrix was constructed in order to remove possible motion components and cardio-respiratory effects, the main source of noise in the BOLD signal in the SC (see SM).

2.3. Statistical analysis

2.3.1. Dynamic causal modeling for BOLD responses

DCM (Friston et al., 2003, Penny et al., 2010) explores changes in the effective connectivity, i.e. how the influence exerted by one region over other regions can be affected by the experimental conditions. DCM allows inferences to be made on the neural mechanisms underlying the observations, in our study based on the BOLD time course in three ROIs (see SM for Time series extraction). Causal network models representing our hypotheses on these mechanisms were constructed and their parameters estimated (see SM about the specification of such models in DCM). Three sets of parameters were considered: the driving input parameter being visual stimulation in our models, the intrinsic connection parameters and the modulatory parameters that define how the effective connectivity may be modulated by, in our case, luminance contrast variations. In DCM, not all the intrinsic connections are necessarily affected by the modulation, leading to alternative plausible models that should be estimated.

The aim of this study was to assess whether differences observed in fMRI responses in PD patients compared to age-matched controls, could be explained by PD- and dopaminergic treatment- dependent alterations in the architecture of effective connectivity between the visual ROIs, and/or in the connectivity strength and its sensitivity to change by luminance contrast. To this end, the model best able to explain the data was first defined for each group (control, PD before and after dopamine treatment). Differences between groups in terms of the winning model was an indication of a change in connectivity architecture. The estimated parameters of each respective winning model were examined and compared across groups.

2.3.2. Model space

Six possible endogenous connections were considered between the three visual structures of interest, the LGN, SC and V1 (see Fig. 2, matrix A of the network). Neurophysiological studies in primates have shown bidirectional structural connections between the LGN and the V1 (Briggs and Usrey, 2011, Merigan and Maunsell, 1993, Sherman and Koch, 1986) as well as structural connections from the SC to the LGN (Benevento and Fallon, 1975, Harting et al., 1978, Munoz and Coe, 2011). Connections from the V1 to the SC have also been identified in macaque monkeys (Cerkevich et al., 2014, Künzle and Akert, 1977, Kuypers and Lawrence, 1967, Lock et al., 2003, Wurtz and Goldberg, 1972). Indeed, injections of anterograde tracers in non-human primates indicates that the SC receives input from most areas of visual cortex (Weller et al., 2002). However, to our knowledge, no direct inputs from the SC to the V1 or from the LGN to the SC have been reported so far. It should be noted that effective connectivity between two regions does not necessarily imply direct anatomical connections as the influence of one region over another can be mediated by other relay structures. For example, the SC and the V1 are connected via the pulvinar (Pennartz et al., 2019). The visual stimuli used in this study entered all models as a driving input to the two subcortical nodes, the LGN and the SC, both receiving direct visual inputs from the retina.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

DCM model space of the modulations of effective connectivity between our regions of interest. The intrinsic connectivity (Matrix A) was taken to be complete (bidirectional) between regions. Activations in response to the first luminance level (1% luminance contrast) were used as input in LGN and SC (matrix C) while activations to the other levels of luminance contrast (3, 5 and 9%) modulated effective connectivity (matrix B) depending on each model. Each model included unilateral modulation from one region to the two others (M1-M3). Additionally, there could be bilateral modulation from V1 to LGN or to SC (M4 and M5) or between LGN and SC (M6). Finally, all connections could be modulated (M7). Together, the different possibilities yielded seven models. In addition, the self-modulation in SC was included in each of the 7 models giving a total of 14 models. SC = superior colliculus, LGN = lateral geniculate nucleus, V1 = primary visual area. Solid arrows: intrinsic connections. Dashed arrows: connections modulated by luminance.

2.3.3. Connectivity modulation

A model space was designed by varying the subset of connections modulated by luminance contrast variations. This was based on our previous fMRI results (Bellot et al., 2016) showing that luminance contrast fluctuations modulated the activations in the visual ROIs (increased response with increasing contrast, see SM: Fig. S1). For the effective connectivity study, the lowest contrast condition (1%) was considered to constitute a baseline from which the response was further modulated by contrast increase. Based on this notion, the remaining contrast conditions (3%, 5% and 9%) were used as modulating inputs, to explore the connectivity modulation with increasing contrasts relative to the 1% contrast condition. These transient perturbations were implemented as bilinear terms assuming a bilinear evolution based on our previous results showing a linear relation between BOLD signal and luminance contrast in the three regions within the variation range used (Bellot et al., 2016). A series of seven models was considered to test the modulatory influence of luminance contrast on effective connectivity from LGN, V1 or SC, to the two other structures (V1 and SC, LGN and SC, or LGN and V1, cases 1 to 3 respectively), or from V1 + LGN, V1 + SC or LGN + SC to the SC, LGN or V1, respectively (cases 4 to 6 respectively), as well as all the possible modulations (case 7). Also considered were models with the same structure and modulations as models 1 to 7 but with an additional self-modulation of the SC, leading to a total of fourteen models to compare (see Fig. 2). These models (8 to 14) were used to account for the possibility of a dysfunction of the SC self-modulation in response to luminance contrast in PD. All 14 models were fed successively with the functional datasets from four groups: control subjects and untreated PD patients or treated PD patients with dopaminergic treatment (2- or 6-months uptake). Model selection and inference on parameters of the best model were made. For group-level inference on model structure, we then considered a Random-effects (RFX) BMS, taking into account pathophysiological variations across patients (Stephan et al., 2010) (see SM).

2.4. Data availability

MR data supporting the results of this study are available from the corresponding author, on a collaborative basis.

3. Results

3.1. Demography

Details of the clinical characteristics of PD patients are shown in Table 1. There were no significant differences between the CT-22 (9 women, age 55.5 y. ± 9.4) and PD-dn groups of patients (5 women, age 57.3 y. ± 10.5), in terms of age (two-sample t-test; p = 0.57) and gender (p = 0.2). The subset of the PD population (n = 8, 3 women, age 55,62 y. ± 7.3) with the follow-up at 2 and 6 months did not differ in terms of age (two sample t-test, p = 0.75) and gender (p = 0.53).

Table 1.

Clinical characteristics of PD patients. (this table is similar to Table 1 in (Moro et al., 2020)).

Patient Sex Age, yr H&Y Scale stage Year of Diagnosis Disease Onset (Body side) UPDRS Treatment at 2 and 6 mo
1 M 67 1 2016 Left 18 NA
2 F 45 1 2017 Left 11 L-dopa/benserazide
3 M 61 1 2017 Left 31 NA
4 M 59 1 2016 Left 11 Pramipexole
5 M 68 1 2016 Right 5 NA
6 M 76 1 2016 Left 17 NA
7 M 58 1 2015 Left 23 Rasagiline/pramipexole
8 M 59 1 2016 Right 15 Rasagiline/pramipexole
9 F 49 1 2014 Left 15 Rasagiline/L-dopa Benserazide
10 M 36 1 2016 Left 9 NA
11 M 47 2 2016 Right 11 NA
12 F 68 2 2014 Right 40 Rasagiline/L-dopa benserazide
13 M 72 2 2017 Left 23 NA
14 M 45 2 2017 Right 20 NA
15 M 68 2 2017 Right 25 NA
16 M 50 2 2015 Right 28 L-dopa/triehyphenidyl
17 M 52 2 2016 Left 21 NA
18 F 56 2 2016 Left 24 NA
19 M 67 2 2015 Right 14 NA
20 M 57 2 2014 Right 18 Rasagiline/ropinirole
21 M 44 2 2016 Right 27 NA
22 F 56 2 2016 Right 21 NA

F = female; M = male; H&Y: Hoehn and Yahr; NA = not applicable; PD = Parkinson’s disease; UPDRS = Unified PD Rating Scale.

3.2. Effective connectivity: Healthy controls and PD patients

3.2.1. The best effective connectivity model for controls

In the control group (CT-22), the exceedance probabilities (xp) for the 14 models are shown in Fig. 3A. As can be seen in this Figure, Model 10, which emphasizes the role of the SC, outperformed all competing models (xp = 0.93). It was characterized by self-modulation by luminance contrast in the SC and unilateral modulation from the SC to the LGN and the V1. The fitting quality of this model was indicated for one subject (see SM: Fig. S2). Its parameters are reported in SM: Table 1. Additional results on the effect of age on the intrinsic connectivity in healthy controls (CT-30) are provided in SM (see SM: Fig. S3 and Table 2).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Effective connectivity in control and patients. A. Exceedance probabilities of the models: from left to right: matched controls (CT-22); de novo PD patients (PD-dn); PD patients 2-months after treatment (PD-2); PD patients 6-months after treatment (PD-6). Model 10 (see Insert) was the best model with random effect Bayesian model selection (RFX) for CT-22, PD-dn and PD-2, whereas model 6 (see Insert) was the best model for PD-6. B. Intrinsic connections parameters of the selected model for PD patients vs. matched controls. Intrinsic connection and modulatory parameters are expressed in Hz. Same abbreviations as in Fig. 2. The vertical bars represent standard errors.

3.2.2. The best effective connectivity model for PD patients

As for the control group, the winning model for de novo (untreated) PD patients (PD-dn) was model 10 (see Fig. 3A, xp = 0.37). However, the exceedance probability was lower in these patients (xp = 0.37) compared to controls (xp ≥ 0.70). The significance of the different parameters of the model is reported in SM: Table 1.

3.2.3. Effective connectivity in PD

The finding that the best-fitting model was the same for the PD patient and control groups, suggests that PD does not impact the architecture of connectivity between the three ROIs. We therefore examined the connectivity parameters on this model for both groups. As for controls, all intrinsic connectivity parameters were positive, i.e. an increase in activity in one region resulted in an increase in each target region (see Fig. 3B). PD-dn patients’ intrinsic connectivity parameters did not significantly differ from controls, as confirmed by the 2x6 ANOVA with Group (Controls CT-22, age-matched with PD patients- vs. PD-dn patients) as the between-subjects factor and Connection as within-subjects factor; this revealed a significant effect of Connection (F(5, 210) = 5.82, p < 10-4, ηp2 = 0.23) but no significant effect of Group (p = 0.72) or interaction between Group and Connection (p = 0.89).

3.2.4. Effect of luminance contrast modulation on the effective connectivity

PD was revealed as a factor that significantly affected the SC self-modulation (F(1,42) = 14.03, p < 10-3, ηp2 = 0.40) and the modulation of the SC to LGN connection (F(1,42) = 8.46, p < 10-2, ηp2 = 0.29) but not the modulation of the SC to V1 connection (p = 0.15). Moreover, luminance contrast was another factor significantly affecting these three intrinsic connections (SC ↔ SC self-modulation: F(3,126) = 8.08, p < 10-4, ηp2 = 0.28; SC → LGN: F(3,126) = 3.78, p < 10-2, ηp2 = 0.15; SC → V1: F(3,126) = 6.07, p < 10-3, ηp2 = 0.22). Interaction between Group and Contrast factors was only significant for the self-modulation of the SC (F(3,126) = 6.28, p < 10-3, ηp2 = 0.23; SC → LGN: p = 0.14; SC → V1: p = 0.11) (2x4 ANOVA with Group, i.e. Controls vs. PD-dn patients, as between-subjects factor and Contrast as within-subjects factor, performed for each connection). To further investigate the effect of the modulation of the connections in PD, an ANOVA with Contrast as repeated-measure was performed for each connection. These analyses revealed a much-reduced weak effect of the luminance contrast modulation on the SC connections in PD-dn patients compared to controls (see Fig. 4). Indeed, the pathology (PD) appeared to have hampered the self-modulation of SC (effect of contrast: p = 0.79) and the modulation of the SC to LGN connection (effect of contrast: p = 0.77). However, the luminance contrast significantly modulated the SC to V1 connection (F(3,63) = 3.66, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.29) in PD-dn patients (see Fig. 4).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Intrinsic connection modulation with luminance contrast. Each diagram corresponds to connections present in model 10 and model 6 and shows the corresponding modulation of the connectivity strength (expressed in Hz) by luminance contrast variations for Controls (CT-22), PD patients without treatment (PD-dn) and with 2 (PD-2) or 6 (PD-6) months of dopaminergic treatment. Dashed line and stars: CT-22. Bold line: PD patients; Circle: PD-dn, Square: PD-2, Triangle: PD-6. Intrinsic connection and modulatory parameters are expressed in Hz. Same abbreviations as in Fig. 2. The vertical bars represent standard errors.

3.3. Effective connectivity in PD: Medication effects

A subset of the PD participants (n = 8) was assessed longitudinally after two (PD-2) and six (PD-6) months of dopaminergic treatment.

3.3.1. The best effective connectivity model

For PD-2, the same model (Model 10) was selected (xp = 0.22) (see Fig. 3A). However, a different model, Model 6, was selected for PD-6 (xp = 0.23) (see Fig. 3A right). The SC plays a less central role in model 6 compared to in model 10. This suggests a reduced response of SC following six months of treatment. On the other hand, LGN is more implicated in model 6 with additional connections to the V1 and to the SC. The significance of the different parameters of these models (6 and 10) is reported in SM: Table 1.

3.3.2. Effect of the dopaminergic treatment on the effective connectivity

At two and six months of treatment, intrinsic connectivity parameters from models 6 and 10 were positive (see Fig. 3B). To test the effect of dopaminergic treatment on these parameters, a 3x6 ANOVA with Group (PD-dn vs. PD-2 vs. PD-6) as between-subjects factor and Connection as within-subjects factor was performed. Connection (F(5,175) = 6.56, p = 10-4, ηp2 = 0.25) was revealed as a significant factor but not Group (i.e. treatment) (p = 0.43), and no interaction between Group and Connection (p = 0.75) was observed (see Fig. 3B).

3.3.3. Effects of the dopaminergic treatment on the modulation of the effective connectivity by luminance contrast

At two months of treatment, only the modulation by luminance contrast of the connection SC ↔ SC was significantly affected by the medication (F(1,28) = 6.87, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.31; SC → LGN: p = 0.48; SC → V1: p = 0.46). Contrast had no significant effect on the three intrinsic connections (SC ↔ SC: p = 0.69; SC → LGN: p = 0.43; SC → V1: p = 0.07) and no interaction was found between Group and Contrast (SC ↔ SC: p = 0.59; SC → LGN: p = 0.59; SC → V1: p = 0.12) (2x4 ANOVA with Group, i.e. PD-dn vs. PD-2, as between-subjects factor and Contrast as within-subject factor, performed for each connection) (see Fig. 4).

After six months of treatment, as characterized by the selected model 6, four connections seemed modulated by luminance contrast, i.e. SC → LGN, SC → V1, LGN → SC and LGN → V1. A 2x4 ANOVA was performed for each of the two common connections of models 6 and 10 (SC → LGN and SC → V1) with Group (PD-dn vs. PD-6) as between-subjects factor and Contrast as within-subjects factor. Neither Group (SC → LGN: p = 0.91; SC → V1: p = 0.24) nor Contrast (SC → LGN: p = 0.98; SC → V1: p = 0.32) as factors showed any significant effect on these connections. An interaction was found between Group and Contrast for connection SC → V1 (F(3,84) = 2.87, p = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.17; SC → LGN: p = 0.80), showing a significant difference between the two groups at 9% contrast (p = 0.01) (see Fig. 4).

4. Discussion

This is the first study addressing the effective connectivity in the visual network of de novo PD patients. By testing competing models of connectivity, we have demonstrated that the SC drives cerebral activity in the visual network for luminance contrast processing, both in healthy controls and PD patients off medication. Our results reveal that the corresponding deficit in contrast processing reported early in PD patients seems related to a deficiency in connectivity adjustment from the SC to both the LGN and the primary visual area V1. Dopaminergic treatment over six months is not sufficient to normalize this abnormal connectivity. The DCM model space we considered is based on our previous works demonstrating: 1) the three regions of interest SC, LGN and V1 exhibited a BOLD response modulated by luminance contrast in the healthy population; (Bellot et al., 2016) and 2) the functioning of SC was altered in de novo PD patients (Moro et al., 2020). This latter study motivated our introducing models of connectivity that included the self-modulation of the SC.

Pivotal role of the SC for luminance contrast processing. For healthy subjects, the DCM analysis supports a model with a high excellence probability (xp = 0.93) where luminance contrast modulated the SC self-connectivity, as well as effective connectivity from the SC to both the V1 and the LGN. For these subjects, an increase in luminance contrast resulted in an increase of the SC self-connectivity for all age ranges (except the elderly) and an increase of the SC to LGN and V1 couplings (see SM: Fig. S4C and SM: Table 2). Our data also indicate that connectivity strength within and from the SC decreases with normal aging (see SM: Fig. S4B and SM: Table 2). This decrease may explain the age-related luminance contrast sensitivity loss observed in these regions with BOLD measurements and also based on behavioral responses (Bellot et al., 2016). Due to the small size of our regions of interest, and in order to maximize the signal-to-noise ratio, we pooled the right and left hemisphere data for each ROI for each subject. Indeed, for the SC that plays a central role in our study, visual information from the right to the left colliculus can be transferred via the inter-collicular commissure or other inter-hemispheric tracts (Georgy et al., 2016).

Effect of PD on effective connectivity. We addressed the nonmotor impact of PD and studied the effective connectivity of the visual system as measured by DCM, based on fMRI measurements. In fMRI, the measured BOLD signal reflects changes in hemodynamic parameters induced by neural activation and is considered as an index of cerebral activity. However, aging, pathological states or drugs may influence hemodynamics and alter the neurovascular coupling. We have previously shown that observed BOLD signal variations both in healthy subjects (Bellot et al., 2016) and in PD patients (Moro et al., 2020) were due to neural activity changes and not a consequence of perfusion alterations. We measured CBF using the ASL technique to verify that the basal brain perfusion level was similar between the two groups in each region of interest and under anti-PD treatment. Thus, BOLD changes reflected neural activity changes and were not a consequence of a neurovascular coupling deficiency.

The present study has investigated the alterations in visual processing due to PD at the first stages, both at the cortical level in the V1 and at the subcortical level in the LGN and the SC. Metabolic differences can be present in PD patients compared to controls that remain unobserved by the MRI techniques we used. The observed BOLD signal modulation due to luminance changes and correlated to visual perception was similar in PD patients and healthy controls in V1 suggesting corresponding neural activity changes at the cortical level. Then, it is unlikely that the observed differences in the two other structures SC and LGN were not due to changes in brain activity but to metabolic alterations only present at this subcortical level.

The SC is a complex sensory-motor brainstem structure that acts as a sentinel in detecting sudden environmental changes and responding to these changes. It integrates multimodal sensory information from visual, auditory, and tactile sources; generates outputs for gaze, head, and arm movement; and sends priority signals to the substantia nigra pars compacta and the intralaminar nucleus of the thalamus. Abnormal responses of SC to visual stimuli have been demonstrated in a PD rat model (Rolland et al., 2013) and more recently in humans (Moro et al., 2020). In a post-mortem study (Erskine et al., 2017), neurodegenerative changes in the SC tissue were found in Lewy bodies dementia that may contribute to visual attention deficits and visual hallucinations; manifestations which are also present in PD patients (Diederich et al., 2009, Frei and Truong, 2017).

Our DCM analysis revealed that the best model explaining the data in terms of intrinsic and modulated connectivity was the same for healthy controls and de novo untreated PD patients (see Fig. 3A). In concordance with previous studies, this model emphasized the pivotal role of the SC for luminance contrast processing. However, we found that the modulation of the SC connectivity (self-connectivity and connectivity to the LGN) by luminance contrast was inhibited in these PD patients (see SM: Table 1). This suggests that while PD patients have the same effective connectivity as healthy controls, the neuromodulation is different in terms of neuronal dynamic interactions. The SC-V1 connection modulation by luminance contrast was preserved at this early stage of PD, thus explaining why de novo PD patients did not report about visual deficits.

The best-fitting models for PD patients and for healthy controls were identical but the exceedance probability was rather low for the former (xp = 0.37) compared to the latter (xp = 0.93). A methodological alternative would have been to consider Bayesian Model Averaging. In this method, the parameters of all plausible models are averaged while taking into account the respective weight for each model (Stephan et al., 2010). This method also allows to compare the differences between models. Although it could have been useful to compare differences between models 6 and 10 after 6-months treatment of PD patients, this alternative would also have rendered the inter-group comparison (i.e. healthy controls vs PD patients), which was the main focus of the study, more difficult to interpret. We thus chose to identify the model which best fitted the observed data in healthy controls and revealed the effect of the pathology (and that of the drug, see below) on the same “best” model. In accordance with that previously observed (Moro et al., 2020), the decrease in xp for the best-fitting PD model reflects that luminance processing becomes dysfunctional in PD patients likely due to a dysfunctioning SC, the central structure in this model. The decrease in xp could also be due to the greater inter-subject variability in the BOLD signal for the PD group compared to the control group (Moro et al., 2020) and to possible metabolic alterations that affect PD patients (Sonninen et al., 2020).

Effect of PD treatment. For brain regions involved in controlling movement, some studies (Nettersheim et al., 2019, Palmer et al., 2009) showed that dopaminergic medication normalized to some extent effective connectivity in PD patients. Similarly, a relative normalization of visuo-parietal connectivity in PD patients with dopaminergic medication during cued and uncued writing was found (Nackaerts et al., 2018). The results from our present study show that dopaminergic treatment, whether taken during 2 or 6 months, did not permit the recovery from abnormal SC connectivity to other brain regions. Findings from our analyses do however suggest that after six months of anti-PD treatment, the LGN plays a more important role (see model 6, Fig. 3A) in the investigated network, despite no evidence of recovery of the effective connectivity modulation by luminance contrast (see Fig. 4). This is consistent with the reduced response in the SC conjointly with an increased response of the LGN to luminance contrast after six month of treatment as observed in (Moro et al., 2020).

It has been suggested (Rolland et al., 2013) that the alteration of the SC response may occur as a compensatory mechanism in response to inhibition exerted by the loss of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra reticulata. Therefore, dopaminergic treatment may not be adapted to rapidly (after 6 months administration) reverse this process (Palmer et al., 2009). Additionally, brain regions are part of complex inter-network interactions (Furl, 2015) suggesting that the anti-PD treatments may have, at this stage of the pathology, no effect on the functional responses of networks interacting and influencing the visual regions studied.

Our study has several limitations. Although the reasonable power for DCM analysis was reached (Goulden et al., 2010), the present study remains limited by the small size of the subject samples. Indeed, the number of patients on which we could assess the effect of medication on inter-regional coupling was only half that of the initial patient sample size. Our results should thus be considered as exploratory and require replication on a larger population. Furthermore, we did not consider a hemispheric difference in effective connectivity that could be induced by the lateralization of the disease. The BOLD signal used in the present study provides indirect neuronal information based on which connectivity models could be defined at a systems level using DCM but which hampers the interpretation of effective connectivity in terms of inhibition or excitation at the neuronal level. The poor temporal resolution of the BOLD response did not allow the assessment of temporal dynamics of the neuronal model. Based on the literature and our previous results both in humans and PD animal models, we consider the colliculus as a key sensory structure impacted at early stages of PD. Recently, we demonstrated alterations in the function of the superior SC, involved in visual information processing, at early stages of PD. The main goal of the present study was to investigate the role of SC in regional coupling both in normal and pathological conditions. To this end, we introduced models with or without SC self-modulation. Introduction of more models, for instance with self-modulation on the other ROIs, could be explored in future studies, although the robustness of the results obtained would diminish with increasing number of tested connections. Finally, considering the decreasing robustness of DCM analyses with increasing number of ROIs, we restricted our hypothesis testing to a small number of ROI, namely SC, LGN and V1, which we considered the most informative. It should be noted that the missing regions do not affect the estimates in the ROIs because influences from outside the restricted network are modelled as endogenous fluctuations (Daunizeau et al., 2011, Stephan et al., 2010). Nevertheless, we left several other activated regions unexplored. For example, neuroanatomical studies suggest that the SC forms a subcortical pathway that bypasses the striate cortex V1 and projects to the amygdala via the pulvinar. While the role of Pulvinar remains elusive (Fiebelkorn and Kastner, 2019), it is clearly a region of interest, at least in its inferior and ventrolateral parts, when studying vision in PD. The recent availability of digital atlases of the human thalamic nuclei (for instance (Najdenovska et al., 2018) would facilitate its automatic delineation and then its functional exploration. However, the functional heterogeneity of the pulvinar (Guedy and Vuilleumier, 2020) and its proximity to the choroidal artery could hamper the detection on the Bold modulation induced by luminance contrast variations, at least at 3 T. Further research concerning the impact of the SC alterations on connectivity with these structures may help finely tune our understanding of the effects of the disease.

In summary, these findings provide useful insights into brain alterations occurring at early stages of PD and in particular highlight the presence of early dysfunctions in primary visual areas, which might be used as early markers of the disease.

Clearly, the measurement of such dysfunctions using an fMRI paradigm, as proposed in the present study, does not fit with a routine clinical evaluation. We currently explore the use of a specific intra-saccadic motion paradigm where the magnocellular pathway (including SC and LGN) is mainly involved (Nicolas et al., JOV 2021). The rationale is that the motion detection performance measured in the psycho-physical experiment using such a paradigm, would be poorer in PD patients compared to age-matched healthy controls due to the functional deficiency of the SC and the LGN.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Emmanuelle Bellot: Investigation, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. Louise Kauffmann: Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. Véronique Coizet: Conceptualization, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. Sara Meoni: Resources, Writing – review & editing. Elena Moro: Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. Michel Dojat: Conceptualization, Methodology, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to the patients and to the healthy control group of individuals who took part in this study.

Funding sources

Emmanuelle Bellot is the recipient of a grant from the France Parkinson Foundation (France) and from the Université Grenoble Alpes. This work was partly supported by a grant from ‘La Fondation de l’Avenir’ (France). The Grenoble MRI facility IRMaGe is partly funded by the French program ‘Investissement d’Avenir’ run by the Agence Nationale pour la Recherche (ANR-11-INBS-0006).

Footnotes

Appendix A

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102906.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

The following are the Supplementary data to this article:

Supplementary data 1
mmc1.docx (2.1MB, docx)

References

  1. Ahn J., Lee J.-Y., Kim T.W., Yoon E.J., Oh S., Kim Y.K., Kim J.-M., Woo S.J., Kim K.W., Jeon B. Retinal thinning associates with nigral dopaminergic loss in de novo Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2018;91(11):e1003–e1012. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000006157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Armstrong R.A. Oculo-Visual Dysfunction in Parkinson's Disease. J. Parkinsons Dis. 2015;5(4):715–726. doi: 10.3233/JPD-150686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bellot E., Coizet V., Warnking J., Knoblauch K., Moro E., Dojat M. Effects of aging on low luminance contrast processing in humans. Neuroimage. 2016;139:415–426. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Benevento L.A., Fallon J.H. The ascending projections of the superior colliculus in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) J. Comp. Neurol. 1975;160(3):339–361. doi: 10.1002/cne.901600306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Briggs F., Usrey W.M. Corticogeniculate feedback and visual processing in the primate. J. Physiol. 2011;589(1):33–40. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.193599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Buijink A.W.G., van der Stouwe A.M.M., Broersma M., Sharifi S., Groot P.F.C., Speelman J.D., Maurits N.M., van Rootselaar A.-F. Motor network disruption in essential tremor: a functional and effective connectivity study. Brain. 2015;138(10):2934–2947. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cardoso E.F., Fregni F., Maia F.M., Melo L.M., Sato J.R., Cruz A.C., Bianchi E.T., Fernandes D.B., Monteiro M.L.R., Barbosa E.R., Amaro E. Abnormal visual activation in Parkinson's disease patients. Mov. Disord. 2010;25(11):1590–1596. doi: 10.1002/mds.23101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cerkevich C.M., Lyon D.C., Balaram P., Kaas J.H. Distribution of cortical neurons projecting to the superior colliculus in macaque monkeys. Eye Brain. 2014;2014:121–137. doi: 10.2147/EB.S53613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Collaborators G.P.s.D. Global, regional, and national burden of Parkinson's disease, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17:939–953. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30295-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Daunizeau J., David O., Stephan K.E. Dynamic causal modelling: a critical review of the biophysical and statistical foundations. Neuroimage. 2011;58(2):312–322. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Diederich Nico J., Fénelon Gilles, Stebbins Glenn, Goetz Christopher G. Hallucinations in Parkinson disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2009;5(6):331–342. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2009.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dirkx Michiel F., den Ouden Hanneke, Aarts Esther, Timmer Monique, Bloem Bastiaan R., Toni Ivan, Helmich Rick C. The Cerebral Network of Parkinson's Tremor: An Effective Connectivity fMRI Study. J. Neurosci. 2016;36(19):5362–5372. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3634-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Erskine D., Thomas A.J., Taylor J.P., Savage M.A., Attems J., McKeith I.G., Morris C.M., Khundakar A.A. Neuronal loss and alpha-synuclein pathology in the superior colliculus and its relationship to visual hallucinations in Dementia with Lewy Bodies. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2017;25:595–604. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2017.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fiebelkorn Ian C., Kastner Sabine. The Puzzling Pulvinar. Neuron. 2019;101(2):201–203. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.12.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Frei Karen, Truong Daniel D. Hallucinations and the spectrum of psychosis in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 2017;374:56–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2017.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Friston K.J., Harrison L., Penny W. Dynamic causal modelling. Neuroimage. 2003;19(4):1273–1302. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00202-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Furl N. Structural and effective connectivity reveals potential network-based influences on category-sensitive visual areas. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015;9:253. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Georgy Loraine, Celeghin Alessia, Marzi Carlo A., Tamietto Marco, Ptito Alain. The superior colliculus is sensitive to gestalt-like stimulus configuration in hemispherectomy patients. Cortex. 2016;81:151–161. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Goulden Nia, McKie Shane, Suckling John, Williams Stephen Ross, Anderson Ian Muir, Deakin John Francis William, Elliott Rebecca. A comparison of permutation and parametric testing for between group effective connectivity differences using DCM. Neuroimage. 2010;50(2):509–515. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Grafton Scot. T., Woods Roger P., Tyszka Mike. Functional imaging of procedural motor learning: Relating cerebral blood flow with individual subject performance. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1994;1(3):221–234. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460010307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Guedy C., Vuilleumier P. Functional connectivity fingerprints of the human pulvinar: Decoding its role in cognition. Neuroimage. 2020;221:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hacker, C.D., Perlmutter, J.S., Criswell, S.R., Ances, B.M., Snyder, A.Z., 2012. Resting state functional connectivity of the striatum in Parkinson's disease. Brain 135, 3699–3711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  23. Harting John K., Casagrande V.A., Weber Joseph T. The projection of the primate superior colliculus upon the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus: autoradiographic demonstration of interlaminar distribution of tectogeniculate axons. Brain Res. 1978;150(3):593–599. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90822-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Helmich, R.C., Derikx, L.C., Bakker, M., Scheeringa, R., Bloem, B.R., Toni, I., 2010. Spatial remapping of cortico-striatal connectivity in Parkinson's disease. Cereb Cortex 20, 1175-1186. [DOI] [PubMed]
  25. Hoehn, M.M., Yahr, M.D., 1967. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology 17, 427–442. [DOI] [PubMed]
  26. Kahan Josh, Mancini Laura, Urner Maren, Friston Karl, Hariz Marwan, Holl Etienne, White Mark, Ruge Diane, Jahanshahi Marjan, Boertien Tessel, Yousry Tarek, Thornton John S., Limousin Patricia, Zrinzo Ludvic, Foltynie Tom, Baron Jean-Claude. Therapeutic subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation reverses cortico-thalamic coupling during voluntary movements in Parkinson's disease. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(12):e50270. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.005027010.1371/journal.pone.0050270.g00110.1371/journal.pone.0050270.g00210.1371/journal.pone.0050270.g00310.1371/journal.pone.0050270.g00410.1371/journal.pone.0050270.t00110.1371/journal.pone.0050270.t002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kahan, J., Urner, M., Moran, R., Flandin, G., Marreiros, A., Mancini, L., White, M., Thornton, J., Yousry, T., Zrinzo, L., Hariz, M., Limousin, P., Friston, K., Foltynie, T., 2014. Resting state functional MRI in Parkinson's disease: the impact of deep brain stimulation on 'effective' connectivity. Brain 137, 1130–1144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  28. Künzle H., Akert K. Efferent connections of cortical, area 8 (frontal eye field) in Macaca fascicularis. A reinvestigation using the autoradiographic technique. J. Comp. Neurol. 1977;173(1):147–163. doi: 10.1002/cne.901730108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kuypers Henricus G.J.M., Lawrence Donald G. Cortical projections to the red nucleus and the brain stem in the Rhesus monkey. Brain Res. 1967;4(2-3):151–188. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(67)90004-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lerner Alicja, Bagic Anto. Olfactory pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson disease revisited. Mov. Disord. 2008;23(8):1076–1084. doi: 10.1002/mds.22066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lock T.M., Baizer J.S., Bender D.B. Distribution of corticotectal cells in macaque. Exp. Brain Res. 2003;151(4):455–470. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1500-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Mahlknecht Philipp, Seppi Klaus, Poewe Werner. The Concept of Prodromal Parkinson's Disease. J. Parkinsons Dis. 2015;5(4):681–697. doi: 10.3233/JPD-150685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Marreiros André C., Cagnan Hayriye, Moran Rosalyn J., Friston Karl J., Brown Peter. Basal ganglia-cortical interactions in Parkinsonian patients. Neuroimage. 2013;66:301–310. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Merigan W.H., Maunsell J.H. How parallel are the primate visual pathway ? Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1993;16:369–402. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Moro Elena, Bellot Emmanuelle, Meoni Sara, Pelissier Pierre, Hera Ruxandra, Dojat Michel, Coizet Veronique. Visual dysfunction of the superior colliculus in de novo Parkinsonian patients. Ann. Neurol. 2020;87(4):533–546. doi: 10.1002/ana.25696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Munoz D.P., Coe B.C. Saccade, search and orient–the neural control of saccadic eye movements. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011;33:1945–1947. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07739.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Nackaerts Evelien, Michely Jochen, Heremans Elke, Swinnen Stephan, Smits-Engelsman Bouwien, Vandenberghe Wim, Grefkes Christian, Nieuwboer Alice. Being on Target: Visual Information during Writing Affects Effective Connectivity in Parkinson's Disease. Neuroscience. 2018;371:484–494. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Najdenovska Elena, Alemán-Gómez Yasser, Battistella Giovanni, Descoteaux Maxime, Hagmann Patric, Jacquemont Sebastien, Maeder Philippe, Thiran Jean-Philippe, Fornari Eleonora, Bach Cuadra Meritxell. In-vivo probabilistic atlas of human thalamic nuclei based on diffusion- weighted magnetic resonance imaging. Sci. Data. 2018;5(1) doi: 10.1038/sdata.2018.270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Nettersheim Felix Sebastian, Loehrer Philipp Alexander, Weber Immo, Jung Fabienne, Dembek Till Anselm, Pelzer Esther Annegret, Dafsari Haidar Salimi, Huber Carlo Andreas, Tittgemeyer Marc, Timmermann Lars. Dopamine substitution alters effective connectivity of cortical prefrontal, premotor, and motor regions during complex bimanual finger movements in Parkinson's disease. Neuroimage. 2019;190:118–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Nicolas G., Castet E., Rabier A., Kristensen E., Dojat M., Guerin-Dugue A. Neural Correlates of Intra-saccadic Motion Perception. J. Vision. 2021;21:1–24. doi: 10.1167/jov.21.11.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Palmer S.J., Eigenraam L., Hoque T., McCaig R.G., Troiano A., McKeown M.J. Levodopa-sensitive, dynamic changes in effective connectivity during simultaneous movements in Parkinson's disease. Neuroscience. 2009;158(2):693–704. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.06.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Pennartz Cyriel M.A., Dora Shirin, Muckli Lars, Lorteije Jeannette A.M. Towards a unified view on pathways and functions of neural recurrent processing. Trends Neurosci. 2019;42(9):589–603. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2019.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Penny Will D., Stephan Klaas E., Daunizeau Jean, Rosa Maria J., Friston Karl J., Schofield Thomas M., Leff Alex P., Kording Konrad P. Comparing families of dynamic causal models. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2010;6(3):e1000709. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pereira Joana B., Junqué Carme, Martí María-Jośe, Ramirez-Ruiz Blanca, Bargalló Nuria, Tolosa Eduardo. Neuroanatomical substrate of visuospatial and visuoperceptual impairment in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 2009;24(8):1193–1199. doi: 10.1002/mds.22560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Postuma Ronald B., Berg Daniela, Stern Matthew, Poewe Werner, Olanow C. Warren, Oertel Wolfgang, Obeso José, Marek Kenneth, Litvan Irene, Lang Anthony E., Halliday Glenda, Goetz Christopher G., Gasser Thomas, Dubois Bruno, Chan Piu, Bloem Bastiaan R., Adler Charles H., Deuschl Günther. MDS clinical diagnostic criteria for Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 2015;30(12):1591–1601. doi: 10.1002/mds.26424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rolland M., Carcenac C., Overton P.G., Savasta M., Coizet V. Enhanced visual responses in the superior colliculus and subthalamic nucleus in an animal model of Parkinson's disease. Neuroscience. 2013;252:277–288. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.07.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rowe, J., Stephan, K.E., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R., Lees, A., Passingham, R., 2002. Attention to action in Parkinson's disease: impaired effective connectivity among frontal cortical regions. Brain 125, 276-289. [DOI] [PubMed]
  48. Rowe J.B. Connectivity analysis is essential to understand neurological disorders. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2010;4 doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rowe J.B., Hughes L.E., Barker R.A., Owen A.M. Dynamic causal modelling of effective connectivity from fMRI: are results reproducible and sensitive to Parkinson's disease and its treatment? Neuroimage. 2010;52(3):1015–1026. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Schneider Keith A., Kastner Sabine. Visual responses of the human superior colliculus: a high-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;94(4):2491–2503. doi: 10.1152/jn.00288.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sherman S.M., Koch C. The control of retinogeniculate transmission in the mammalian lateral geniculate nucleus. Exp. Brain Res. 1986;63:1–20. doi: 10.1007/BF00235642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Siderowf A., Jennings D., Eberly S., Oakes D., Hawkins K.A., Ascherio A., Stern M.B., Marek K., Investigators P. Impaired olfaction and other prodromal features in the Parkinson At-Risk Syndrome Study. Mov. Disord. 2012;27:406–412. doi: 10.1002/mds.24892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Siderowf A., Lang A.E. Premotor Parkinson's disease: concepts and definitions. Mov. Disord. 2012;27:608–616. doi: 10.1002/mds.24954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Skidmore F., Korenkevych D., Liu Y., He G., Bullmore E., Pardalos P.M. Connectivity brain networks based on wavelet correlation analysis in Parkinson fMRI data. Neurosci. Lett. 2011;499:47–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.05.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sonninen T.M., Hamalainen R.H., Koskuvi M., Oksanen M., Shakirzyanova A., Wojciechowski S., Puttonen K., Naumenko N., Goldsteins G., Laham-Karam N., Lehtonen M., Tavi P., Koistinaho J., Lehtonen S. Metabolic alterations in Parkinson's disease astrocytes. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:14474. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-71329-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Stephan K.E., Penny W.D., Moran R.J., den Ouden H.E., Daunizeau J., Friston K.J. Ten simple rules for dynamic causal modeling. Neuroimage. 2010;49:3099–3109. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. van Eimeren T., Monchi O., Ballanger B., Strafella A.P. Dysfunction of the default mode network in Parkinson disease: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch. Neurol. 2009;66:877–883. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Weil R.S., Schrag A.E., Warren J.D., Crutch S.J., Lees A.J., Morris H.R. Visual dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. Brain. 2016;139:2827–2843. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Weller R.E., Steele G.E., Kaas J.H. Pulvinar and other subcortical connections of dorsolateral visual cortex in monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 2002;450:215–240. doi: 10.1002/cne.10298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wu T., Wang L., Chen Y., Zhao C., Li K., Chan P. Changes of functional connectivity of the motor network in the resting state in Parkinson's disease. Neurosci. Lett. 2009;460:6–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.05.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wurtz R.H., Goldberg M.E. The role of the superior colliculus in visually-evoked eye movements. Bibl. Ophthalmol. 1972;82:149–158. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary data 1
mmc1.docx (2.1MB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

MR data supporting the results of this study are available from the corresponding author, on a collaborative basis.


Articles from NeuroImage : Clinical are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES