Abstract
Low positive affect has been identified as an antecedent of binge-eating episodes among individuals with binge-eating disorder (BED), yet positive affect has received far less attention in eating disorders research than its counterpart, negative affect. In this article, we argue that the low levels of positive affect which occur with anhedonia (i.e., loss of interest or pleasure in activities) may contribute to the onset and maintenance of BED. We introduce a theoretical model in which anhedonia increases risk for BED through its inter-relationships with dysregulated eating and weight gain, and we describe potential direct (e.g., reward-related processes) as well as indirect (e.g., influences on depressive symptoms and physical activity) pathways by which anhedonia may lead to adverse eating- and weight-related outcomes. We also propose a momentary maintenance model in which low positive affect and positive affect dysregulation occurring with anhedonia maintain binge eating directly and indirectly through maladaptive health behaviors, such as decreased physical activity, less healthy eating, and fewer social interactions, which in turn maintain anhedonia. We draw upon outside literatures to present evidence that aligns with the proposed risk and maintenance models and conclude by outlining avenues for future research—including methodological/measurement, theoretical, and clinical research directions.
Keywords: binge-eating disorder, anhedonia, positive affect, theoretical models, maintenance, risk
Anhedonia, defined as a loss of interest or pleasure in activities, involves low levels of positive affect (PA; De Fruyt et al., 2020). While PA has begun to receive some recognition in the eating disorders (EDs) literature (e.g., Coniglio et al., 2019), the role of low PA has historically been overlooked in ED research in lieu of predominant focus on negative affect (Selby et al., 2019). Although little research has explicitly examined the role of anhedonia in binge-eating disorder (BED), some work has investigated the more general and dimensional role of PA. Consistent with affect regulation models of eating pathology, evidence indicates that low PA is an antecedent of binge-eating episodes among individuals with BED (Schaefer et al., 2020). Baseline PA dynamics have also been found to predict BED treatment outcomes, including changes in binge-eating frequency and weight (Mason, Smith, Anderson, et al., 2020; Mason, Smith, Williams-Kerver, et al., 2020). In this article, we propose that anhedonia and PA dysregulation may contribute to risk and maintenance of BED and outline critical methodological/measurement, theoretical, and treatment considerations.
Anhedonia as a Risk Factor for BED
We propose that inter-relationships between anhedonia, dysregulated eating, and weight gain may contribute to the development of BED and outline potential mechanisms. Growing evidence suggests that anhedonia predicts eating- and weight-related outcomes. In adolescents and adults, anhedonia predicted greater weight gain over naturalistic follow-ups (Cho et al., 2018; Ibrahim et al., 2016). Particularly, adults with both high depressive symptoms and high anhedonia were most likely to gain weight (Ibrahim et al., 2016). In a study of adults with overweight/obesity participating in a weight loss intervention, those with anhedonia reported greater binge eating at baseline and follow-up and exhibited poorer weight outcomes (Keränen et al., 2010). Relatedly, social anhedonia (i.e., disinterest in and lack of reward from social interaction) has been demonstrated among individuals with EDs (Barkus & Badcock, 2019). For example, among those with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa, social anhedonia is negatively correlated with hedonic responses to sucrose, though in these populations, evidence suggests fear of weight gain may drive the association more so than decreased ability to derive pleasure from sweet foods (Eiber et al., 2002). To date, there remains limited longitudinal research in this area, and no research exists on the role of social anhedonia in BED and obesity.
Both direct and indirect mechanisms may predispose individuals with anhedonia to develop binge eating and gain excess weight. Anhedonia is thought to arise from reductions in dopamine transmission and dysfunction within the reward system, particularly regarding anticipation of reward (wanting), consumption of reward (liking), and learning of reward (Der-Avakian & Markou, 2012; Rømer Thomsen et al., 2015). Outside of EDs and obesity, neuroscience research indicates anhedonia is characterized by reduced reward anticipation and willingness to exert effort to get a reward, decreased liking of rewards, and disruptions in reinforcement learning–i.e., the ability to modify behaviors as a function of reward (Craske et al., 2016; De Fruyt et al., 2020).
Accordingly, studies among individuals with depression have shown that anhedonia is associated with reduced activation in regions associated with value-based decision-making during reward anticipation (Stoy et al., 2011; Ubl et al., 2015), hypoactivity in ventral striatum in the context of positive stimuli (Epstein et al., 2006; Keedwell et al., 2005; Pizzagalli et al., 2009; Wacker et al., 2009), and reduced ventral striatal responding during Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning tasks (Gradin et al., 2011; Kumar et al., 2008). Given this evidence for difficulty with reward learning in anhedonia, one possibility is that individuals with anhedonia have an increased drive to obtain comfort and pleasure from primary reinforcers such as food (versus secondary, or learned, reinforcers) in order to compensate for these deficits and improve affective states, which in turn may increase risk of BED symptoms and weight gain (Giel et al., 2017; Kessler et al., 2016; Reiter et al., 2017).
In addition to potential direct effects of reward dysfunction on appetite regulation, anhedonia may indirectly influence binge eating and obesity development by exacerbating other relevant etiological factors. Research suggests anhedonia, in comparison to other depressive symptoms, is a marker of greater symptom severity and poorer prognosis among individuals with depression (Gabbay et al., 2015; McMakin et al., 2012); as such, anhedonia may compound negative affectivity and depressive symptoms that are known to predict binge-eating pathology and obesity (Luppino et al., 2010; Puccio et al., 2018). Anhedonia also may contribute to lower physical activity, which is central for energy balance, appetite regulation, and neurocognitive functioning (Beaulieu et al., 2018; Cox et al., 2016). Taken together, current evidence suggests anhedonia represents risk for negative eating- and weight-related outcomes, which may be due to altered reward-related processes and secondary effects of anhedonia on other proximal risk factors for binge eating and obesity.
Anhedonia as a Maintenance Factor for BED
Figure 1 depicts a momentary maintenance model of anhedonia and binge eating in BED. This model includes three components: (1) the link between anhedonia and momentary PA; (2) momentary process of low PA, behaviors, and binge eating; and (3) maintenance of binge eating by dysregulated PA. These components are described below.
Figure 1.
Momentary maintenance process model of anhedonia and binge eating in binge-eating disorder
Anhedonia and momentary PA.
Anhedonia has demonstrated associations with lower levels of PA and less intense pleasure experiences in ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies of young adults with versus without anhedonia and adults with major depressive disorder and anhedonia versus controls (Heininga et al., 2017; 2019). Contrary to beliefs that PA reactivity may be blunted in anhedonia (De Fruyt et al., 2020), young adults with anhedonia versus without anhedonia exhibited greater PA variability with no signs of blunted PA reactivity in one EMA study (Heininga et al., 2017), though another EMA study found no differences in PA dynamics between individuals with major depressive disorder and anhedonia compared to controls (Heininga et al., 2019). Moreover, when exploring high-arousal (e.g., energetic) and low-arousal PA (e.g., calm), young adults with anhedonia versus without anhedonia demonstrated greater reactivity to pleasurable experiences in high-arousal PA (Heininga et al., 2017). Taken together, evidence suggests that anhedonia is associated with dysregulation of PA in daily life.
PA, behaviors, and binge eating.
The broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 2001) suggests that subjective experiences of PA expand one’s attentional scope and promote engagement in behaviors that further engender PA. Consistent with this theory, evidence from general samples show bi-directional associations between momentary PA and adaptive behaviors, such that higher PA predicts healthy eating (e.g., increased fruit/vegetable intake), physical activity, and social interactions, and these behaviors predict higher subsequent PA (Conner et al., 2015; Liao et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2019; Mason et al., 2019). Conversely, individuals with BED typically engage in less healthy eating patterns, consume fewer healthy foods, engage in less physical activity, and have more interpersonal difficulties (e.g., Brugner et al., 2019; Engel et al., 2009; Hrabosky et al., 2007; Masheb et al., 2011), which may serve to maintain lower overall and within-day PA. Binge eating may be used as a way to momentarily increase PA in individuals with BED. Consistently, in a recent EMA study of adults with BED, trajectories of PA surrounding binge eating showed that, on average, PA decreased prior to binge eating and stabilized following binge eating, providing evidence for a PA-enhancing effect of binge eating in BED (Schaefer et al., 2020).
PA dysregulation and binge-eating maintenance.
In addition to low PA being associated with binge eating, data suggests elevated levels of PA compared to one’s average level interact with other state or trait risk factors to predict binge eating (Smith et al., 2019; 2020). That is, individuals with BED may engage in binge eating when PA is lower than their average level as well as when PA is higher than their average level. Therefore, rather than engaging in healthy behaviors to maintain PA, at moments when individuals with BED experience higher-than-usual PA, they may be more likely to engage in binge eating as a way to maintain PA, given that they may not experience PA from many other activities due to underlying anhedonia. Binge eating for PA maintenance may be learned through repeated instances of binge eating increasing low levels of momentary PA. Consistently, a laboratory study found preliminary support for this explanation, such that women with BED with greater baseline expectations that food is rewarding consumed more calories after a PA induction (Dingemans et al., 2009).
Future Directions for Research
Methodological/measurement directions.
Testing and extending the proposed models represent vital steps toward increasing understanding of the role of anhedonia in BED. Much research in the ED field focuses on composite measures of depressive symptoms, but it is key to specifically include measures that assess anhedonia and PA dynamics (including intensity, instability, differentiation, and inertia; Trull et al., 2015). Also, future work should utilize intensive longitudinal designs (e.g., EMA) to examine maintenance mechanisms of binge eating implicated in proposed models of BED. EMA studies may offer critical insights regarding momentary processes, including altered reward learning and maladaptive patterns of positive reinforcement in the maintenance of binge eating.
It is critical for EMA analyses to disaggregate PA into between- and within-subjects components (Curran & Bauer, 2011) to study associations between low and high PA compared to one’s average. Given assertions in our model that both high and low PA may predict binge eating, non-linear models will be important to evaluate. In addition, studies should incorporate sensors and measures to objectively capture PA-promoting behaviors (e.g., accelerometers, 24-hour dietary recalls, electronically activated recorders). Further, longitudinal multi-wave EMA designs will enable researchers to examine changes in anhedonia, behaviors, and reward-related processes, and the extent to which these mechanisms represent BED prevention and treatment targets.
Consistent with calls for elucidation of risk factors across various levels of analysis, future research should also evaluate biological factors associated with anhedonia and altered reward processes that may contribute to the development and maintenance of binge eating. In particular, identifying neurobiological mechanisms may provide insight regarding promising treatment targets for individuals with BED. For instance, findings from mood disorders field suggest that anhedonia is associated with altered dopaminergic function that may influence reward learning and related processes (Cooper et al., 2018). Recent developments in neurobiological, pharmacological, and computational approaches provide the opportunity to examine dopaminergic deficits that might contribute to the neurobiological factors underlying anhedonia-related reward deficits in BED (Cooper et al., 2018). Given evidence outside of BED samples suggesting that behavioral and neural responses may differ when processing primary (e.g., food) vs. secondary (e.g., money) rewards (e.g., Amlung et al., 2016; Herpertz et al., 2015; Valentine & O’Doherty, 2009), elucidating the extent to which anhedonia in BED is specific to aberrant processing of primary versus secondary rewards will be another important future direction. Identifying neurobiological factors that contribute to elevated anhedonia and maintain altered reward processes offers the potential to identify candidate biological factors that could be targeted to bolster treatment outcomes for BED.
Theoretical directions.
Overall, theoretical models focused on BED are lacking, and theoretical models in the ED field do not explicitly include anhedonia and PA (Pennesi & Wade, 2016). This leaves an urgent need to incorporate recent research on anhedonia and potential deficits in reward processes within risk and maintenance models of BED. Research is needed to empirically identify the causal pathways by which anhedonia may increase risk for BED. While reward system dysfunction (both reward surfeit and deficit models) has been proposed to account for overconsumption and excess weight, neither the surfeit or deficit model has demonstrated unequivocal support across the literature. Future research should investigate the extent to which binge eating may initially result from heightened reward responsiveness to food (potentially due to anhedonia-related reward dysfunction) but over time, lead to down-regulation of dopamine receptors within reward regions and compound risk of future binge eating and weight gain (Stice et al., 2010). Further, in addition to elucidating the role of PA and anhedonia, future theoretical and empirical research should consider interactional associations between negative affect and PA.
With respect to potential indirect mechanisms, future research should investigate the roles exacerbation of depressive symptoms and decreased physical activity may play. In addition, research on social anhedonia should examine whether having a detached or uninterested interpersonal style may increase interpersonal problems and thereby act as an additional indirect pathway, as interpersonal problems are considered a central component within the interpersonal model of binge eating (Wilfley et al., 2002). In addition to refining our proposed risk model of anhedonia and BED, future research is needed to examine possible moderators of BED risk among those with anhedonia. Further, we call for more research on maintenance models of BED, which are lacking. Specifically, research is needed to test each of the processes we outline in our momentary maintenance model of binge eating in BED. Finally, while the models in the current paper focus on how anhedonia promotes and maintains BED, anhedonia may occur across EDs (Tchanturia et al., 2012) and thus future research should elucidate how these models may apply to other EDs. Also, future research should examine bi-directionality of associations; that is, how BED symptoms promote and maintain anhedonia, as noted in Figure 1.
Treatment directions.
The proposed risk and maintenance models may also provide a guiding framework for treatment planning and intervention development for BED. For instance, if findings support the role of anhedonia as a risk and/or prognostic factor, clinicians should assess for elevated anhedonia prior to treatment. In the case that future research supports the proposed mechanistic maintenance model, this would inform treatment development efforts. For example, efforts to adapt PA-focused interventions (e.g., PA treatment for depression and anxiety; Craske et al., 2019) may prove particularly beneficial for individuals who demonstrate deficits in reward anticipation, consumption, and/or learning processes posited to contribute to BED. Finally, identifying neurobiological processes underlying altered reward processes may also inform the development of brain-based interventions (e.g., non-invasive neuromodulation, pharmacological interventions).
Conclusions
Initial evidence suggests that anhedonia contributes to risk and maintenance of BED. However, systematic research is needed to better understand the role of anhedonia-related reward processes in BED. To address gaps, we have proposed the first risk and maintenance models of BED, providing foundation for future research examining anhedonia-related models of BED.
Acknowledgments
This work was funded partially by the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases (Award Number K01DK124435) and the National Institute of Mental Health (Award Numbers K23MH123910 and T32MH082761).
Footnotes
The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
Data Availability Statement: Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analyzed in this study.
References
- Amlung M, Petker T, Jackson J, Balodis I, & MacKillop J. (2016). Steep discounting of delayed monetary and food rewards in obesity: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46, 2423–2434. 10.1017/S0033291716000866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkus E, & Badcock JC (2019). A transdiagnostic perspective on social anhedonia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 216. 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beaulieu K, Hopkins M, Blundell J, & Finlayson G. (2018). Homeostatic and non-homeostatic appetite control along the spectrum of physical activity levels: An updated perspective. Physiology & Behavior, 192, 23–29. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.12.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brugnera A, Carlucci S, Compare A, & Tasca GA (2019). Persistence of friendly and submissive interpersonal styles among those with binge-eating disorder: Comparisons with matched controls and outcomes after group therapy. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 26, 603–615. 10.1002/cpp.2385 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho J, Goldenson NI, Pester MS, Khoddam R, Bello MS, Dunton GF, … & Leventhal AM (2018). Longitudinal associations between anhedonia and body mass index trajectory groups among adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 63, 81–87. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.12.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coniglio KA, Christensen KA, Haynos AF, Rienecke RD, & Selby EA (2019). The posited effect of positive affect in anorexia nervosa: Advocating for a forgotten piece of a puzzling disease. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 52, 971–976. 10.1002/eat.23147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conner TS, Brookie KL, Richardson AC, & Polak MA (2015). On carrots and curiosity: Eating fruit and vegetables is associated with greater flourishing in daily life. British Journal of Health Psychology, 20, 413–427. 10.1111/bjhp.12113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper JA, Arulpragasam AR, & Treadway MT (2018). Anhedonia in depression: biological mechanisms and computational models. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 22, 128–135. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craske MG, Meuret AE, Ritz T, Treanor M, & Dour HJ (2016). Treatment for anhedonia: a neuroscience driven approach. Depression and Anxiety, 33, 927–938. 10.1002/da.22490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craske MG, Meuret AE, Ritz T, Treanor M, Dour H, & Rosenfield D. (2019). Positive affect treatment for depression and anxiety: A randomized clinical trial for a core feature of anhedonia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 87, 457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox EP, O’Dwyer N, Cook R, Vetter M, Cheng HL, Rooney K, & O’Connor H. (2016). Relationship between physical activity and cognitive function in apparently healthy young to middle-aged adults: a systematic review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 19, 616–628. 10.1016/j.jsams.2015.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran PJ, & Bauer DJ (2011). The disaggregation of within-person and between-person effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 583–619. 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100356 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Fruyt J, Sabbe B, & Demyttenaere K. (2020). Anhedonia in depressive disorder: A narrative review. Psychopathology, 1–8. 10.1159/000508773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Der-Avakian A, & Markou A. (2012). The neurobiology of anhedonia and other reward-related deficits. Trends in Neurosciences, 35, 68–77. 10.1016/j.tins.2011.11.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dingemans AE, Martijn C, van Furth EF, & Jansen AT (2009). Expectations, mood, and eating behavior in binge eating disorder. Beware of the bright side. Appetite, 53, 166–173. 10.1016/j.appet.2009.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eiber R, Berlin I, de Brettes B, Foulon C, & Guelfi JD (2002). Hedonic response to sucrose solutions and the fear of weight gain in patients with eating disorders. Psychiatry Research, 113, 173–180. 10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00232-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel SG, Kahler KA, Lystad CM, Crosby RD, Simonich HK, Wonderlich SA, … & Mitchell JE (2009). Eating behavior in obese BED, obese non-BED, and non-obese control participants: A naturalistic study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 897–900. 10.1016/j.brat.2009.06.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein J, Pan H, Kocsis JH, Yang Y, Butler T, Chusid J, … & Silbersweig DA (2006). Lack of ventral striatal response to positive stimuli in depressed versus normal subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1784–1790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabbay V, Johnson AR, Alonso CM, Evans LK, Babb JS, & Klein RG (2015). Anhedonia, but not irritability, is associated with illness severity outcomes in adolescent major depression. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 25(3), 194–200. 10.1089/cap.2014.0105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giel KE, Teufel M, Junne F, Zipfel S, & Schag K. (2017). Food-related impulsivity in obesity and binge eating disorder—a systematic update of the evidence. Nutrients, 9, 1170. 10.3390/nu9111170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gradin VB, Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Stickle C, Milders M, … & Steele JD (2011). Expected value and prediction error abnormalities in depression and schizophrenia. Brain, 134, 1751–1764. 10.1093/brain/awr059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison A, Mountford VA, & Tchanturia K. (2014). Social anhedonia and work and social functioning in the acute and recovered phases of eating disorders. Psychiatry Research, 218, 187–194. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heininga VE, Dejonckheere E, Houben M, Obbels J, Sienaert P, Leroy B, … & Kuppens P. (2019). The dynamical signature of anhedonia in major depressive disorder: positive emotion dynamics, reactivity, and recovery. BMC Psychiatry, 19, 59. 10.1186/s12888-018-1983-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heininga VE, Van Roekel E, Ahles JJ, Oldehinkel AJ, & Mezulis AH (2017). Positive affective functioning in anhedonic individuals’ daily life: Anything but flat and blunted. Journal of Affective Disorders, 218, 437–445. 10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heller AS, Johnstone T, Shackman AJ, Light SN, Peterson MJ, Kolden GG, … & Davidson RJ (2009). Reduced capacity to sustain positive emotion in major depression reflects diminished maintenance of fronto-striatal brain activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 22445–22450. 10.1073/pnas.0910651106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hrabosky JI, White MA, Masheb RM, & Grilo CM (2007). Physical activity and its correlates in treatment-seeking obese patients with binge eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 40, 72–76. 10.1002/eat.20323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ibrahim M, Thearle MS, Krakoff J, & Gluck ME (2016). Perceived stress and anhedonia predict short-and long-term weight change, respectively, in healthy adults. Eating Behaviors, 21, 214–219. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.03.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keedwell PA, Andrew C, Williams SC, Brammer MJ, & Phillips ML (2005). The neural correlates of anhedonia in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 843–853. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keränen AM, Rasinaho E, Hakko H, Savolainen M, & Lindeman S. (2010). Eating behavior in obese and overweight persons with and without anhedonia. Appetite, 55(3), 726–729. 10.1016/j.appet.2010.08.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RM, Hutson PH, Herman BK, & Potenza MN (2016). The neurobiological basis of binge-eating disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 63, 223–238. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.01.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Milders M, Reid I, & Steele JD (2008). Abnormal temporal difference reward-learning signals in major depression. Brain, 131, 2084–2093. 10.1093/brain/awn136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liao Y, Shonkoff ET, & Dunton GF (2015). The acute relationships between affect, physical feeling states, and physical activity in daily life: a review of current evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1975. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01975 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu H, Xie QW, & Lou VW (2019). Everyday social interactions and intra-individual variability in affect: A systematic review and meta-analysis of ecological momentary assessment studies. Motivation and Emotion, 43, 339–353. 10.1007/s11031-018-9735-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Luppino FS, de Wit LM, Bouvy PF, Stijnen T, Cuijpers P, Penninx BW, & Zitman FG (2010). Overweight, obesity, and depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 220–229. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masheb RM, Grilo CM, & Rolls BJ (2011). A randomized controlled trial for obesity and binge eating disorder: Low-energy-density dietary counseling and cognitive-behavioral therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 821–829. 10.1016/j.brat.2011.09.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason TB, Smith KE, Anderson L, Schaefer L. Crosby RD, Engel SG, … & Wonderlich SA (in press). Affective response to binge eating as a predictor of binge eating disorder treatment outcome. Clinical Psychological Science. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason TB, Smith KE, Williams-Kerver G, Crosby RD, Engel SG, Crow SJ, Wonderlich SA, & Peterson CB (2020). Descriptives and predictors of weight change over the course of psychological treatments for binge eating disorder. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McMakin DL, Olino TM, Porta G, Dietz LJ, Emslie G, Clarke G, … & Shamseddeen W. (2012). Anhedonia predicts poorer recovery among youth with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment–resistant depression. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 404–411. 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.01.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzagalli DA, Holmes AJ, Dillon DG, Goetz EL, Birk JL, Bogdan R, … & Fava M. (2009). Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 702–710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puccio F, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz M, Ong D, & Krug I. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis on the longitudinal relationship between eating pathology and depression. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 49, 439–454. 10.1002/eat.22506 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiter AM, Heinze HJ, Schlagenhauf F, & Deserno L. (2017). Impaired flexible reward-based decision-making in binge eating disorder: evidence from computational modeling and functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42, 628–637. 10.1038/npp.2016.95 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rømer Thomsen K, Whybrow PC, & Kringelbach ML (2015). Reconceptualizing anhedonia: novel perspectives on balancing the pleasure networks in the human brain. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 49. 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer LM, Smith KE, Anderson LM, Cao L, Crosby RD, Engel SG, Crow SJ, Peterson CB, & Wonderlich SA (2020). The role of affect in the maintenance of binge-eating disorder: Evidence from an ecological momentary assessment study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129, 387–396. 10.1037/abn0000517 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Selby EA, Panza E, & Plasencia M. (2019). Positive emotion dysregulation in eating disorders and obesity. In Gruber J. (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Positive Emotion and Psychopathology (pp. 424–443). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Simon JJ, Skunde M, Wu M, Schnell K, Herpertz SC, Bendszus M, … & Friederich HC (2015). Neural dissociation of food-and money-related reward processing using an abstract incentive delay task. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 1113–1120. 10.1093/scan/nsu162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith KE, Mason TB, Anderson LM, Schaefer LM, Crosby RD, Engel SG, … & Peterson CB (2020). Naturalistically assessed associations between physical activity, affective functioning, and binge eating among adults with binge-eating disorder. Eating Disorders, 1–14. 10.1080/10640266.2020.1746121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith KE, Mason TB, Crosby RD, Engel SG, & Wonderlich SA (2019). A multimodal, naturalistic investigation of relationships between behavioral impulsivity, affect, and binge eating. Appetite, 136, 50–57. 10.1016/j.appet.2019.01.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith KE, Mason TB, Juarascio A, Weinbach N, Dvorak R, Crosby RD, & Wonderlich SA (2020). The momentary interplay of affect, attention bias, and expectancies as predictors of binge eating in the natural environment. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 53, 586–594. 10.1002/eat.23235 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith KE, Mason TB, Crosby RD, Engel SG, Crow SJ, Wonderlich SA, & Peterson CB (2018). State and trait positive and negative affectivity in relation to restraint intention and binge eating among adults with obesity. Appetite, 120, 327–334. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.09.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stice E, Yokum S, Bohon C, Marti N, & Smolen A. (2010). Reward circuitry responsivity to food predicts future increases in body mass: moderating effects of DRD2 and DRD4. Neuroimage, 50, 1618–1625. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoy M, Schlagenhauf F, Sterzer P, Bermpohl F, Hägele C, Suchotzki K, … & Adli M. (2012). Hyporeactivity of ventral striatum towards incentive stimuli in unmedicated depressed patients normalizes after treatment with escitalopram. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 26, 677–688. 10.1177/0269881111416686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchanturia K, Davies H, Harrison A, Fox JR, Treasure J, & Schmidt U. (2012). Altered social hedonic processing in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45, 962–969. 10.1002/eat.22032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trull TJ, Lane SP, Koval P, & Ebner-Priemer UW (2015). Affective dynamics in psychopathology. Emotion Review, 7, 355–361. 10.1177/1754073915590617 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ubl B, Kuehner C, Kirsch P, Ruttorf M, Diener C, & Flor H. (2015). Altered neural reward and loss processing and prediction error signalling in depression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 1102–1112. 10.1093/scan/nsu158 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valentin VV, & O’Doherty JP (2009). Overlapping prediction errors in dorsal striatum during instrumental learning with juice and money reward in the human brain. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 3384–3391. 10.1152/jn.91195.2008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wacker J, Dillon DG, & Pizzagalli DA (2009). The role of the nucleus accumbens and rostral anterior cingulate cortex in anhedonia: integration of resting EEG, fMRI, and volumetric techniques. Neuroimage, 46, 327–337. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilfley DE, Welch RR, Stein RI, Spurrell EB, Cohen LR, Saelens BE, … & Matt GE (2002). A randomized comparison of group cognitive-behavioral therapy and group interpersonal psychotherapy for the treatment of overweight individuals with binge-eating disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 713–721. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.8.713 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]