Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Dec 16.
Published in final edited form as: Int J Neurosci. 2014 Mar 24;124(12):912–918. doi: 10.3109/00207454.2014.895344

Physical activity and cognitive function in bariatric surgery candidates

Rachel Galioto 1, Wendy C King 2, Dale S Bond 3, Mary Beth Spitznagel 1, Gladys Strain 4, Michael Devlin 5, Ronald Cohen 6, Ross D Crosby 7,8, James E Mitchell 7,8, John Gunstad 1
PMCID: PMC8674741  NIHMSID: NIHMS1762716  PMID: 24547736

Abstract

Cognitive impairment is common in severe obesity. Lack of physical activity is a likely contributor to impairment in this population, as many obese persons are inactive and physical activity has been positively and independently associated with cognitive function in healthy and medically-ill samples. This study investigated whether physical activity, measured by self-report of aerobic physical activity in 85 bariatric surgery candidates, was associated with cognitive function. A subset of 31 participants also completed objective activity monitoring. Steps/d and high-cadence min/week, representative of ambulatory moderate to vigorous physical activity (MVPA), were calculated. Approximately one quarter of participants self-reported at least 30 min/d of aerobic MVPA, at least 5 d/week. Median steps/d was 7949 (IQR = 4572) and median MVPA min/week was 105 (IQR = 123). Cognitive deficits were found in 32% of participants (29% memory, 10% executive function, 13% language, 10% attention). Controlling for demographic and medical factors, self-reported aerobic physical activity was weakly correlated with lower attention (r = −0.21, p = 0.04) and executive function (r = −0.27, p < 0.01) and both self-reported aerobic physical activity and objectively-determined MVPA min/week were negatively correlated with memory (r = −0.20, p = 0.04; r = −0.46; p = 0.02, respectively). No other correlations between physical activity measures and cognitive function were significant. Contrary to expectations, greater levels of physical activity were not associated with better cognitive functioning. Such findings encourage future studies to clarify the association among cognitive function and physical activity in obese persons.

Keywords: exercise, obesity, attention, executive function, memory


Obesity is increasingly recognized as an independent risk factor for adverse neurocognitive outcomes. Elevated body mass index (BMI) is associated with increased risk for neurologic disorders such as stroke [1], Alzheimer’s disease, and vascular dementia [24]. Elevated BMI is also associated with structural and functional abnormalities on neuroimaging in the absence of these disorders, including decreased blood flow [5], reduced white matter integrity [6,7], and smaller grey matter and total brain volume relative to normal weight [8]. Consistent with these adverse brain changes, persons with elevated BMI often exhibit cognitive dysfunction on testing [914], with the most common deficits being the domains of memory and executive function [9,10,14]. Cognitive impairment is particularly prevalent in severely obese persons, with up to 25% of individuals demonstrating clinically meaningful levels of impairment on neuropsychological testing [10].

Past studies have shown multiple factors contribute to cognitive dysfunction in obese persons, including severity of obesity and comorbid conditions like hypertension and type 2 diabetes [15]. Though not previously examined, another likely contributor to cognitive impairment in adults with severe obesity is low levels of physical activity [1618]. Bariatric surgery patients are often sedentary [19] and numerous studies show regular physical activity reduces risk for dementia [2023] and cognitive decline [24]. Much of the research in this area has demonstrated an association between improved cardiovascular fitness and participation in moderate- to high-intensity exercise on structural and functional neurocognitive outcomes. For example, increased cardiorespiratory fitness is associated with reduced brain atrophy [25], preservation of gray and white matter [26,27], and greater hippocampal volumes [28]. Similarly, moderate- to high-intensity aerobic exercise is associated with increased gray and white matter volume [29] and functional connectivity in the prefrontal cortex [30]. Consistent with these findings, moderate-to high-intensity aerobic exercise has also been shown to improve cognitive functioning, including executive functioning, attention, visuospatial functioning, and processing speed, across patient and healthy samples [3033].

This preliminary study examined the contribution of physical activity to cognitive function in severely obese adults. Specifically, we examine whether physical activity is associated with memory, attention, executive function, and/or language among adults undergoing bariatric surgery. Based on previous findings, we hypothesized that higher levels of physical activity would be associated with better performance on tests of cognitive function.

Methods

All procedures were approved by the appropriate Institutional Review Boards and all participants provided written informed consent prior to study involvement.

Trial design and participants

The original sample included 122 adults who were recruited for an ancillary study focused on cognitive function from the Longitudinal Assessment of Bariatric Surgery-2 (LABS-2) parent project [34]. Of the original sample, 17 participants were excluded from analyses due to missing cognitive function data (n = 14) or self-reported physical activity data (n = 3). Inclusion criteria for the ancillary study included age between 20–70 years and being English-speaking. Exclusion criteria included a history of neurological disorder or injury (e.g. dementia, stroke, seizures), history of moderate to severe head injury (defined as > 10 min of loss of consciousness) [35], past or current history of alcohol or drug abuse (DSM-IV criteria) [36], history of a learning disorder or developmental disability (defined by DSM-IV criteria) [36], or impaired sensory function that precluded computerized testing. Some analyses were further limited to a subset of 31 of these individuals with valid objective physical activity data. All measurements were completed prior to bariatric surgery. Medical and demographic characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographic and medical characteristics of the study sample.

Full sample (n = 105) Subsample (n = 31)
Mean (SD) [range] or% Mean (SD) [range] or%
Age, years 43.3 (11.0) [20–66] 43.5 (11.1) [21–61]
BMI, kg/m2 46.8 (6.7) [35.7–75.0] 48.6 (8.7) [35.7–75.0]
Female 68.6% 67.7%
Diabetes 22.9% 32.3%
Hypertension 42.9% 51.6%
Asthma 15.2% 12.9%
RAPA 3.9 (1.4) [2–6] 4.2 (1.7) [2–6]

Note: RAPA – Rapid Assessment of Physical Activity

Measures

Cognitive function

The Integneuro cognitive test battery is a standardized and semi-automated computerized battery that estimates intellectual abilities and assesses performance in multiple cognitive domains. Tests are administered in a fixed order using pre-recorded instructions and a touch-screen computer. For each test, the task is introduced and the participant is provided with sample items to ensure comprehension. The battery has demonstrated good construct validity in comparison to standard neuropsychological measures and has been shown to have good test- retest reliability [37,38]. For this study, tests were categorized into four domains: Attention, Executive Function, Memory and Language. These tests included:

Attention

Digit span backward

Digit span assesses basic auditory attention. Participants are presented with a series of digits on the touch-screen, separated by a 1-s interval. Participants are then immediately asked to enter the digits on a numeric keypad on the touch-screen in the reverse order of presentation. The number of digits in each sequence is gradually increased from 3 to 9, with two sequences at each level. The score for this task is the total number of correct trials.

Span of visual memory

This task is similar to Spatial Span from the Wechsler Memory Scales [39]. Participants are asked to recreate highlighted forward and backward patterns on the screen. The score is the total number of correct trials.

Switching of attention – digits

This test is a computerized adaptation of the Trail Making Test A [40]. Participants are presented with a pattern of 25 numbers in circles and asked to touch them in ascending order. This test assesses attention and psychomotor speed. The score is time to completion.

Verbal interference – word

This test is similar to the Word trial on the Stroop Color Word Test [41]. Participants are presented with color words one at a time. Below each colored word is a response pad with the four possible words displayed in black and in a fixed format. During a 1-min period participants are required to identify the name of each word as quickly as possible after it has been presented on the screen, providing a measure of attention. The score is the number of words correctly identified.

Executive function

Switching of attention – digits and letters

This test is a computerized adaptation of the Trail Making Test B [42]. Participants are presented with an array of 13 numbers (1–13) and 12 letters (A–L). Participants are asked to touch numbers and letters alternately in ascending order. This test taps both attentional abilities and executive function. The score is time to completion.

Verbal interference – color

This test is similar to the Color Word trial of the Stroop Color Word Test [43]. Participants are presented with colored words one at a time. To measure executive function, the subject is required to name the color of the ink rather than the target word as quickly as possible during a 1-min period. The score is the number of words correctly identified.

Maze task

The maze task assesses executive function and is a computerized adaptation of the Austin Maze [44]. Participants are presented with an 8 × 8 matrix of circles and are asked to identify the hidden path through the grid. Unique auditory and visual cues are presented for correct and incorrect responses. When participants have completed the maze twice without error, or after 10 min has passed, the trial ends. The score is the total number of errors committed.

Memory

Verbal list – learning

This test has two components. Participants are read a list of 12 words a total of four times and asked to recall as many words as possible following each trial. Following presentation and recall of a distraction list, participants are asked to recall target words (i.e. words from the original list). After a 20-min delay, participants are again asked to recall target words. Finally, a recognition trial comprised of target words and foils is completed. The first score is the number of words recalled after a short delay. The second score is the number of words recalled after the 20-min delay. The third score is the number of words correctly identified as a target or foil word.

Language

Verbal fluency

This test has two components. For the letter fluency task, participants are asked to generate words beginning with a given letter of the alphabet (F, A and S), for 60 s. The test is done three times. The first score is the number of correct words generated across all three trials. For the animal fluency task, participants are asked to generate as many animal names as they can in 60 s. The second score is the total number of correct animal names.

Computing composite scores for cognitive domains

Raw scores for each measure were transformed into z-scores based on age, gender and estimated intelligence to promote generalizability and facilitate clinical interpretation. A composite score for each domain of cognitive function (memory, attention, executive function, and language) was created by averaging the z-scores from each of the specific tests within that domain.

Physical activity

Objective assessment of ambulatory physical activity

Detailed procedures for measuring physical activity for this study have been described elsewhere [16]. Participants were asked to wear the StepWatch 3 Activity Monitor (OrthoCare Innovations, Washington, DC) [45], a microprocessor-controlled biaxial activity monitor, fastened above the ankle, that combines acceleration, position and timing information to count steps per minute, for 7 d prior to surgery. Participants returned monitors via mail. Nonwear periods were identified by intervals of at least 120 min with no activity [46]. Daily wear time was calculated as 24 h minus the duration of nonwear periods. Days with less than 10 h of wear time were excluded. Step counts at the minute level were used to calculate mean daily steps and mean daily minutes of ambulatory moderate-vigorous activity (MVPA) (i.e. estimated from the number of minutes with 80 steps or more), as well as mean daily minutes of bout-related ambulatory MVPA (a bout was established when 8 min out of a 10 min window met the threshold of 80 steps or more. A bout ended with three consecutive minutes below this threshold) [47]. Daily MVPA values were standardized to 1-week periods by multiplying the mean daily values by 7.

Rapid assessment of physical activity (RAPA)

The RAPA is a nine-item self-report questionnaire designed to easily assess usual physical activity levels of adults 50 and older, which has been found to be comparable to other validated physical activity surveys among older adults [48]. It was selected for this study because the physical activity patterns of severely obese adults are similar to older adults [16]. For the current effort, we utilized the aerobic activity score, determined from responses to seven yes or no items, which are arranged in ascending order based on duration, frequency and level of usual aerobic activity (e.g. “I do 30 minutes or more a day of moderate physical activities, 5 or more days a week” is a score of 6). The highest item with an affirmative response equals the participant’s score (range 0 to 7). Higher scores reflect greater levels of physical activity. A score of 6 or above is considered to be active [48].

Other measures

Height and weight were measured by trained research staff using standardized protocols. BMI was calculated as height in meters divided by weight in kilograms squared. Age was determined from participant’s self-reported date of birth. Participants also self-reported sex and currently having or having a history of a diagnosis of asthma, diabetes or hypertension by a health care professional.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics were used to characterize the sample. Pearson and partial correlations, controlling for BMI, diabetes, hypertension and asthma status, were conducted to test associations among the indices of physical activity and the four cognitive function domain scores (attention, executive function, memory and language), which were already adjusted for age, gender and estimated intelligence. Statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05. The statistical software package used for all analyses was SPSS version 20.

Results

Low rates of physical activity in the sample

Among the subset of individuals with valid objective physical activity data (n = 31), median steps per day was 7949 steps/d (IQR = 4572). Using standard step per day indices [49], 16.1% of individuals were sedentary (less than 5000 steps/d), 29.0% were low active (5000 to 7499 steps/d), 32.3% were somewhat active (7500 to 9999 steps/d), 12.9% were active (10 000 to 12 499 steps/d) and 9.7% were highly active (12 500+ steps/d). Ambulatory MVPA for this sample ranged from 4 to 650 min/week (median = 105.0; IQR = 123.2). Bout-related MVPA ranged from 0 to 584 min/week (median = 0.0; IQR = 48). Only 41.9% of participants (n = 13) achieved any bout-related ambulatory MVPA; 35.5% (n = 11) achieved at least 30 min/week of bout-related MVPA, 19.2% (n = 6) achieved at least 60 min/week, and only 2 participants (6.4%) achieved at least 150 min/week.

Scores on the aerobic subscale of the RAPA ranged from 2 to 7, with the average being 3.9 (SD = 1.4. Based on this self-report measure, 21.9% of participants were considered active.

Cognitive impairment is prevalent among obese persons

Clinically meaningful levels of cognitive impairment (<1.5 SD below the mean) were found in all domains. In this sample, 32.3% (n = 10) exhibited deficits in at least one domain. Nine individuals (29.0%) demonstrated impairment in memory; composite scores ranged from −2.38 to 2.65 (median = −0.72, IQR = 2.70). Four individuals (12.9%) demonstrated impairment in language; composite scores ranged from −1.99 to 2.76 (median = 0.01, IQR = 1.25). Three individuals (9.7%) demonstrated impairment in executive function; composite scores ranged from −2.74 to 1.94 (median = −.03, IQR = 1.60). Three individuals (9.7%) demonstrated impairment in attention; composite scores ranged from −2.06 to 1.40 (median = 0.04, IQR = 1.46).

Physical activity is unrelated to cognitive function

Pearson and partial correlations between measures of physical activity and cognitive function, are presented in Table 2. After controlling for BMI, diabetes, hypertension and asthma status, there were weak negative correlations between RAPA scores and executive function (r = −0.27, p = 0.006), attention (r = −0.21, p = 0.04) and memory (r = −0.20, p = 0.04). Among the subsample, minutes of ambulatory MVPA per week was negatively associated with attention (r = −0.46, p = 0.02). No other significant correlations between physical activity and cognitive function emerged.

Table 2.

Unadjusted bivariate and partial correlations between physical activity and cognitive function.

Cognitive functiona
Attention Executive function Memory Language
Unadjusted Adjustedb Unadjusted Adjustedb Unadjusted Adjustedb Unadjusted Adjustedb
Self-reported physical activity (n = 105)
RAPA score −0.22* −0.21* −0.27** −0.27** −0.24* −.20* 0.11 −0.09
Objective physical activity (n = 31)
Steps/d −0.36* −0.34 −0.34 −0.28 −0.17 .07 −0.13 0.07
MVPA min/week −0.50** −0.46* −0.41* −0.41 −0.17 −.08 −0.16 0.04
Bout-related MVPA min/week −0.43* −0.36 −0.32 −0.27 −0.24 −.22 −0.20 < 0.01

Note: MVPA = moderate-vigorous intensity physical activity; RAPA = Rapid Assessment of Physical Activity;

a

Cognitive function measures adjusted for age, gender and estimated intelligence;

b

Correlation adjusted for body mass index, asthma, diabetes and hypertension;

*

p < 0.05,

**

p < 0.01.

Discussion

The current study examined the association between physical activity and cognitive function among severely obese individuals. Similar to previous work in this population, including the larger LABS-2 cohort from which participants were selected, [16, 50] physical activity levels were low, with nearly one-quarter (self-report) to less than one-tenth (objective monitoring) of bariatric surgery candidates meeting the national recommendation of at least 150 min MVPA/week [51]. The current findings are also consistent with previous research demonstrating that cognitive impairment is prevalent among obese individuals [9, 10, 14] with cognitive deficits exhibited in 22% of participants. However, contrary to expectations and findings in other samples, [20, 22, 52, 53] after adjusting for potential covariates, there was not a significant association between steps/day and bout-related min/week of MVPA and any of the cognitive function scores. More surprising, self-reported aerobic physical activity was weakly correlated with lower attention and executive function, and both self-reported aerobic physical activity and objectively-determined min/week of MVPA (not bout-related) were negatively correlated with memory performance. Such findings suggest that there is little association between physical activity and cognitive function among severely obese adults in an observational, naturalistic setting. Several aspects of these findings warrant brief discussion.

Though several possibilities exist, the most likely explanation for the current findings involves the low levels of physical activity observed in the sample. Previous research has suggested that cognitive function can be improved through physical activity that enhances cardiovascular fitness [54], and that intensity of physical activity (rather than duration) is most important for cognitive benefits [55, 56]. Similarly, much of the research supporting the association between activity and cognitive function examines the effects of moderate- to high-intensity aerobic training programs [29, 30]. However, low levels of bout-related MVPA (the type of physical activity to most likely improve levels of cardiovascular fitness [29, 30]) were observed in our sample (i.e. only 41.9% of participants achieved any bout-related ambulatory MVPA, only 2 (6.4%) of which achieved the suggested minimum of 150 min/week of bout-related MVPA) and it is possible that no (or very few) participants were above the threshold of physical activity duration or intensity needed to provide cognitive benefits.

Because this was an exploratory study with a small sample size we did not adjust for multiple comparisons, but this did make the study more susceptible to type I errors. Given this fact, coupled with the very low levels of MVPA exhibited by the current sample, it appears likely that the observed associations between some measures of physical activity and cognitive function, which were weak to low, are due to statistical artifact instead [57] of a possible adverse effect of physical activity in this population. Finally, given the known adverse effects of elevated BMI on cognitive function, it is possible that, in this sample, the adverse effects of severe obesity out-weigh the potential benefits of low levels of physical activity on cognitive function.

The mechanisms by which higher levels of physical activity may lead to improved cognitive function among obese are not entirely understood and are likely to be multifactorial. One likely possibility is that increased levels of physical activity may lead to improvement or resolution of obesity-related medical conditions that are known to impair cognition such as type 2 diabetes mellitus [58], heart disease [59] and sleep apnea [60]. Additionally, greater physical activity and cardiovascular fitness are related to reduced systemic inflammation and circulating biomarkers (e.g. brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) [61], leptin [62], ghrelin [63]) which are implicated in obesity and related to cognitive dysfunction. Finally, increased physical activity levels are likely related to improved cognitive function through their association with improved structural and functional brain outcomes [2530]. Future studies are needed to clarify the possibility that physical activity can attenuate the adverse neurocognitive effects of obesity.

Several aspects of this preliminary, naturalistic study may limit generalizability. A primary limitation involves the small sample size and use of cross-sectional data. Past studies of physical activity and cognitive function often follow individuals for periods of several weeks to months [52, 64] and it is possible that activity levels over an extended period of time are better predictors of neurocognitive outcomes than briefer periods of monitoring. Thus, another possible explanation for the limited findings involves the short duration of objective physical activity assessment (i.e. 3–7 d), which may not be fully representative of typical physical activity among these individuals. To address this limitation, a self-report measure of usual aerobic physical activity was also evaluated. However, given the bias of subjective physical activity assessment [65], it is not clear whether it better represented actual usual physical activity behavior compared to the objective monitor. Furthermore, despite having an adequate range of values, it appears likely that many individuals were not physically active enough to improve cognitive function. In effect, it is likely that a threshold exists for the benefits of physical activity on cognitive function. Randomized clinical trials of physical activity interventions which promote a high level of MVPA would provide clearer insight into the cognitive benefits of physical activity in this population. Larger, prospective studies may also clarify the association between physical activity and cognition, especially in light of findings that suggest both physical activity and cognition improve following bariatric surgery [66,67].

Conclusions

The current study supports previous research demonstrating low levels of physical activity and high rates of cognitive impairment among severely obese individuals. Although higher physical activity levels were not associated with better cognition in this study, future research should examine this relationship using randomized clinical trials designed to evaluate the effect of MVPA on change in cognitive function over time. Such findings would directly determine the contribution of improved physical activity and cardiovascular fitness to cognitive impairment in obese persons.

Footnotes

Declaration of Interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of this paper.

References

  • 1.Hu G, Tuomilehto J, Silventoinen K, et al. Body mass index, waist circumference, and waist-hip ratio on the risk of total and type-specific stroke. Arch Int Med 2007;167:1420–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gustafson D, Rothenberg E, Blennow K, et al. I An 18-year follow-up of overweight and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Int Med 2003;163:1524–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kivipelto M, Ngandu T, Fratiglioni L, et al. Obesity and vascular risk factors at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 2005;62:1556–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Whitmer R, Gunderson E, Quesenberry CP Jr, et al. Body mass index in midlife and risk of Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia. Curr Alzheimer Res 2007;4:103–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Willeumier K, Taylor D, Amen D. Elevated BMI is associated with decreased blood flow in the prefrontal cortex using SPECT imaging in healthy adults. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2011;19:1095–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Stanek K, Grieve S, Brickman A, et al. Obesity is associated with reduced white matter integrity in otherwise healthy adults. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2011;19:500–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Mueller K, Anwander A, Möller HE, et al. Sex-dependent influences of obesity on cerebral white matter investigated by diffusion-tensor imaging. PLoS One 2011;6:e18544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gunstad J, Paul R, Cohen R, et al. Relationship between body mass index and brain volume in healthy adults. Int J Neurosci 2008;118:1582–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gunstad J, Paul R, Cohen R, et al. Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Compr Psychiatry 2007;48:57–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gunstad J, Lhotsky A, Wendell C, et al. Longitudinal examination of obesity and cognitive function: results from the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Neuroepidemiology 2007;34:222–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gunstad J, Paul R, Cohen R, et al. Obesity is associated with memory deficits in young and middle-aged adults. Eat Weight Disord-St 2006;11:e15–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lokken K, Boeka A, Yellumahanthi K, et al. Cognitive performance of morbidly obese patients seeking bariatric surgery. Am Surg 2010;76:55–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Boeka A, Lokken K. Neuropsychological performance of a clinical sample of extremely obese individuals. Arch Clin Neuropsych 2008;23:467–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Waldstein SR, Katzel LL. Interactive relations of central versus total obesity and blood pressure to cognitive function. Int J Obes 2006;30:201–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Debette S, Seshadri S, Beiser A, et al. Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurology 2011;77:461–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.King WC, Belle SH, Eid GM, et al. Physical activity levels of patients undergoing bariatric surgery in the Longitudinal Assessment of Bariatric Surgery (LABS) study. Surg Obes Relat Dis 2008;4:721–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Jakicic JM, Gregg E, Knowler W, et al. Activity patterns of obese adults with type 2 diabetes in the look AHEAD study. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2010;42:1995–2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bond DS, Unick JL, Jakicic JM, et al. Objective assessment of time spent being sedentary in bariatric surgery candidates. Obes Surg 2011;21:811–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bond DS, Thomas JG, Unick JL, et al. Self-reported and objectively measured sedentary behavior in bariatric surgery candidates. Surg Obes Relat Dis 2013;9:123–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Abbott RD, White LR, Ross W, et al. Walking and dementia in physically capable men. JAMA 2004;292:1447–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen JD, et al. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older. Ann Intern Med 2006;144:73–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Laurin D, Verreault R, Lindsay J, et al. Physical activity and risk of cognition and dementia in elderly persons. Arch Neurol 2001;58:498–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kramer AF, Hahn S, Cohen NJ, et al. Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature 1999;400:418–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gow AJ, Mortensen EL, Avlund K. Activity participation and cognitive aging from age 50 to 80 in the glostrup 1914 cohort. J Am Geriatr Sociol 2012;60:1831–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci 2003;14:125–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rovio S, Spulber G, Nieminen LJ, et al. The effect of midlife physical activity on structural brain changes in the elderly. Neurobiol Aging 2010;31:1927–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Marks BL, Madden DJ, Bucur B, et al. Role of aerobic fitness and aging on cerebral white matter integrity. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007;1097:171–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Voss MW, et al. Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus 2009;19:1030–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Colcombe SJ, Erickson KL, Scalf PE, et al. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;61:1166–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Voss MW, Heo S, Prakash RS, et al. The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Hum Brain Mapp 2013;34:2972–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Dustman RE, Ruhling RO, Russell EM, et al. Aerobic exercise training and improved neuropsychological function of older individuals. Neurobiol Aging 1984;5:35–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Blumenthal JA, Emery CF, Madden DJ, et al. Long-term effects of exercise on psychological functioning in older men and women. J Gerontol 1991;46:352–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Albinet C, Boucard G, Bouquet C, Audiffren M. Increased heart rate variability and executive performance after aerobic training in the elderly. Eur J Appl Physiol 2010;109:617–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Belle SH, Berk PD, Courcoulas AP, et al. Safety and efficacy of bariatric surgery: longitudinal assessment of bariatric surgery. Surg Obes Relat Dis 2007;3:116–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Alexander M Mild traumatic brain injury: pathophysiology, natural history, and clinical management. Neurology 1985;45:1253–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.American Psychiatric Association – APA. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Paul R, Lawrence J, Williams L, et al. Validity of IntegNeuroTM: a new automated, computerized and standardized battery of neurocognitive tests. J Integr Neurosci 2005;115:1549–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Williams LM, Simms E, Clark CR, et al. The test-retest reliability of a standardized neurocognitive and neurophysiological test battery: “neuromarker.” Int J Neurosci 2005;115:1605–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wechsler D Wechsler memory scale – three. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Reitan R Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Motor Skills 1958;8:271–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Golden C Stroop color and word task: a manual for clinical and experimental uses. Chicago, IL: Stoeling; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Walsh K Understanding brain damage – a primer of neuropsychological evaluation. Melbourne: Churchill Livingstone; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Gillberg IC, Billstedt E, Wentz E, et al. Attention, executive functions, and mentalizing in anorexia nervosa eighteen years after onset of eating disorder. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2010;32:358–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Peñas-Lledó E, Loeb K, Martin L, Fan J. Anterior cingulate activity in bulimia nervosa: a fMRI case study. Eat Weight Disord 2007;12:78–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Boone DA, Coleman KL. Use of a step activity monitor in determining outcomes. J Prosthet Orthot 2006;18:86–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.King WC, Li J, Leishear K, et al. Determining activity monitor wear timeL an influential decision rule. J Phys Act Health 2011;8:566–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.King WC, Engel SG, Elder KA, et al. Walking capacity of bariatric surgery candidates Surg Obes Relat Dis 2012;8:48–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Topolski TD, LoGerfo J, Patrick DL, et al. The Rapid Assessment of Physical Activity (RAPA) among older adults. Prev Chronic Dis 2006;3:A118. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Tudor-Locke C, Bassett DR Jr. How many steps/day are enough? Preliminary pedometer indices for public health. Sports Med 2004;34:1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Bond DS, Jakicic JM, Unick JL, et al. Pre- to postoperative physical activity changes in bariatric surgery patients: self-report vs. objective measures. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2010;18: 2395–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Centers for Disease Control, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Physical activity guidelines for Americans. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion: 2008, 6–17- = 2010; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Anderson-Hanley C, Arciero PJ, Westen SC, et al. Neuropsychological benefits of stationary bike exercise and a cybercycle exergame for older adults with diabetes: an exploratory analysis. J Diabetes Sci Technol 2012;6:849–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.van Gelder BM, Tijhuis MA, Kalmijn S, et al. Physical activity in relation to cognitive decline in elderly men: the FINE Study. Neurology 2004;63:2316–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Angevaren M, Aufdemkampe G, Verhaar HJJ, et al. Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Sys Rev 2008;(2):CD005381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Angevaren M, Vanhees L, Wendel-Vos W, et al. Intensity, but not duration, of physical activities is related to cognitive function. Eur J Cardiovasc Prev Rehabil 2007;14:825–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Brown BM, Peiffer JJ, Sohrabi HR, et al. Intense physical activity is associated with cognitive performance in the elderly. Transl Psychiatry 2012:e191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Wiseman S The effect of restriction of range upon correlation coefficients. Brit J Educ Psychol 1967;37:248–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Reiner M, Niermann C, Jekauc D, Woll A. Long-term health benefits of physical activity – a systematic review of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health 2013;13:813. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Conti AA, Macchi C. Protective effects of regular physical activity on human vascular system. Clin Ter 2013;164:293–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Quan SF, O’Connor GT, Quan JS, et al. Association of physical activity with sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep Breath 2007;11:149–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Jung SH, Kim J, Davis JM, et al. Association among basal serum BDNF, cardiorespiratory fitness and cardiovascular disease risk factors in untrained healthy Korean men. Eur J Appl Physiol 2011;111:303–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Reseland JE, Anderssen SA, Solvoll K, et al. Effects of long term changes in diet and exercise on plasma leptin concentrations. Am J Clin Nutr 2001;73:240–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Spitznagel MB, Benitez A, Updegraff J, et al. Serum ghrelin is inversely associated with cognitive function in a sample of non-demented elderly. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2010;64: 608–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hansen AL, Johnsen BH, Sollers JJ 3rd, et al. Heart rate variability and its relation to prefrontal cognitive function: the effects of training and detraining. Eur J Appl Physiol 2004;93:263–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Westerterp KR. Assessment of physical activity: a critical appraisal. Eur J Appl Physiol 2009;105:823–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.King WC, Hsu JY, Belle SH, et al. Pre- to postoperative changes in physical activity: report from the longitudinal assessment of bariatric surgery-2 (LABS-2). Surg Obes Relat Dis 2012;8:522–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Gunstad J, Strain G, Devlin MJ, et al. Improved memory function 12 weeks after bariatric surgery. Surg Obes Relat Dis 2011;7:465–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES