Abstract
The dopamine receptor 4 (DRD4) in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) acts to modulate behaviours including cognitive control and motivation, and has been implicated in behavioral inhibition and responsivity to food cues. Adolescence is a sensitive period for the development of habitual eating behaviors and obesity risk, with potential mediation by development of the PFC. We previously found that genetic variations influencing DRD4 function or expression were associated with measures of laboratory and real-world eating behavior in girls and boys. Here we investigated brain responses to high energy–density (ED) and low-ED food cues using an fMRI task conducted in the satiated state. We used the gene-based association method PrediXcan to estimate tissue-specific DRD4 gene expression in prefrontal brain areas from individual genotypes. Among girls, those with lower vs. higher predicted prefrontal DRD4 expression showed lesser activation to high-ED and low-ED vs. non-food cues in a distributed network of regions implicated in attention and sensorimotor processing including middle frontal gyrus, and lesser activation to low-ED vs non-food cues in key regions implicated in valuation including orbitofrontal cortex and ventromedial PFC. In contrast, males with lower vs. higher predicted prefrontal DRD4 expression showed minimal differences in food cue response, namely relatively greater activation to high-ED and low-ED vs. non-food cues in the inferior parietal lobule. Our data suggest sex-specific effects of prefrontal DRD4 on brain food responsiveness in adolescence, with modulation of distributed regions relevant to cognitive control and motivation observable in female adolescents.
Subject terms: Cognitive neuroscience, Feeding behaviour, Genetics of the nervous system, Motivation, Neural circuits, Reward
Introduction
Adolescence is a key period for the development of eating behaviors that confer obesity risk1,2. Early in life, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) begins to form critical direct and indirect connections to distributed cortical and sub-cortical brain regions3,4. This process continues apace throughout adolescence, with the PFC among the last brain regions to reach maturity5–7. Through its interactions with other brain regions, the PFC regulates multiple cognitive functions with potential relevance for eating behavior and thereby body weight, including behavioral inhibition and flexibility, and motivation and reward processing8. Consistent with this, studies have shown that obesity is associated with increased impulsive decision-making and attention bias in response to food cues, as well as with altered patterns of activation across the brain in response to food cues presented within fMRI tasks9.
Cortical neuronal maturation and the balance between neural excitation and inhibition during adolescence is hypothesized to be dopamine dependent10. The dopamine receptor 4 gene (DRD4) functionally produces inhibitory effects, and is expressed in brain regions implicated in planning, executive function and reward, including frontal cortex11. Human studies have found that diminished dopamine inhibitory feedback in DRD4-7R carriers is linked to weaker physiological dopamine signaling compared to non-carriers12–15. Other studies have shown associations of genetic variants associated with impoverished dopamine function (TaqI A1 (vs. A2)), with higher food reinforcement and energy intake in adults16, greater emotional eating and snack food reinforcement in adult females with obesity17, and lower satiety and fullness before a meal in children with obesity18.
Our previous work has demonstrated that genetic variation on DRD4 is associated with body weight and eating behavior in children, moderating the influence of the quality of the environment19–26. For example, the presence of hypofunctional variant DRD4-7R allele in combination with lower maternal sensitivity during the early postnatal period was associated with higher body weight in early childhood23. Extending this work, we have used a novel genomic approach that imputes the gene expression of DRD4 in PFC using individual level genomic information27 to find that genetically regulated expression of the DRD4 gene in PFC moderated effects of socioeconomic background on emotional eating in 48-month old children, and desire to drink in 60-month old children22. A similar differential susceptibility effect was found in the current adolescent cohort, such that lower predicted PFC DRD4 expression was associated with greater food intake in the satiated state, in the context of lower socioeconomic status20. This approach represents a significant addition to existing work by examining specific effects of genetically-driven mechanisms on the biological substrate of interest, in this case brain circuits including and interacting with prefrontal cortex.
The above evidence suggests a role of differential DRD4 expression in PFC in influencing obesity risk by affecting the response of individuals to the environment. Studies in females using fMRI have demonstrated that weaker responses in the brain reward circuitry to imagined and actual intake of palatable foods are more strongly associated with greater future increases in body mass in individuals carrying low functioning variants of dopamine receptor genes, such as the DRD2 TaqIA A1 allele and the DRD47R allele28,29. However, the specific effects of genetically regulated prefrontal expression of the DRD4 gene on neural responses to food cues have not yet been examined. We therefore investigated whether predicted PFC DRD4 expression would be associated with altered brain food cue responsiveness in adolescents. Given known sex differences in brain maturation during adolescence30 as well as in eating behavior20, obesity23, and neural responses to food cues31,32, likely driven by social environment as well as biological factors25, we examined relationships in females and males separately. Since obesity is associated with eating in the presence of satiety33, and with reduced neural satiety responses32, we examined effects of DRD4 PFC expression on neural food cue responses in the satiated/fed, rather than the fasted, state.
Methods
Participants
Adolescents and their mothers were recruited for a study investigating the neurobehavioral basis of obesity and familial obesity risk. Details of this study have been published elsewhere20,34 and are also described here. In summary, recruitment was via flyers posted at the Johns Hopkins Hospital in Baltimore, MD and online advertisements. Adolescent-mother dyads were required to speak English fluently. For adolescents, exclusion criteria included: being outside our target age range of 14–18 years old, current diagnosis of a significant health problem (e.g. eating disorder, learning disability), use of medication affecting appetite and body weight (e.g. stimulants, antidepressants), participation in a structured weight loss program, medical contraindications to MRI (e.g. metal implants), and food allergies. For mothers, exclusion criteria included: current pregnancy, and excessive smoking, recreational drug use or alcohol intake.
Potential participants completed a telephone screening. Eligible dyads were then invited for a total of three visits: an initial consultation, a scan visit in a fasted condition and a scan visit in a fed condition (condition of interest). During the initial consultation, informed consent was obtained from the 98 adolescent participants. Adolescents 18 years or over completed informed consent for themselves. For adolescents under 18 years, we obtained informed consent from parents, and adolescents themselves provided assent. Fifteen completed neither test day and were excluded from further analysis. Of the remaining 83 participants, we excluded adolescent-mother pairs whose saliva samples were not collected and had incomplete scan data, resulting in a final analytic sample of 73 adolescent-mother pairs who completed the initial consultation and at least one of the test days (no data were imputed).
This study was approved by the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine Institutional Review Board and all the procedures followed guidelines and regulation in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Initial consultation
Anthropometric measures
Body weight and fat percentage were assessed at the initial consultation using a SC-331S Total Body Composition Analyzer (TANITA Corp., Tokyo) to measure body weight and estimates fat percentage via Bio-Impedance Analysis. Height was assessed using a wall-mounted stadiometer after removal of shoes. BMI values (kg/m2) were calculated, and BMI z scores and percentiles derived for adolescents based on Center for Disease Control (CDC) growth charts from 200035. Adolescents under the 85th percentile were classified as normal-weight, adolescents between the 85th and 95th percentiles as overweight, and adolescents at the 95th percentile or above as obese. During the initial consultation, saliva samples were collected from all adolescents using the Oragene OG500 (DNAGenotek, Ottawa, Canada) saliva collection kits. Adolescents completed the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS)36. Mothers completed a demographic questionnaire reporting their own and their child’s race/ethnicity.
Scan visits
Overview
For the fed scan visit, participants were instructed to arrive fasted (for at least 4 h). Adolescent participants underwent an MRI scan that included a food cue reactivity (FCR) task. Prior to the scan, participants were given a fixed liquid meal preload of 16 fl oz (480 kcal) of the high-protein drink BOOST (480 kcal, 30 g protein, 12 g fat, 36 g sugar per 16 fl oz), which was consumed 38 ± 11 min before onset of the Food Cue Reactivity task.
Food cue reactivity (FCR) task
Participants completed an FCR task during the MRI scan. This task was programmed in E-Prime (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, Pennsylvania). The paradigm comprised an instruction screen that explained the task to the participants and 2 runs with 45 trials each. Each run included 15 high energy density (ED) food trials, 15 low-ED food trials, and 15 non-food trials, appearing in a pseudorandomized order with no more than 2 stimuli of each category in a row. Each trial lasted 4 s (including stimulus + stimulus rating period) and was followed by a fixation period (central crosshair for 1 s). For each trial, a food or non-food picture was presented in the middle of the screen with a wanting rating question and response options at the bottom of the screen. For food pictures the question read: “Do you want to eat this right now?” and for non-food stimuli it read: “Do you want to use this right now?” with response options as follows: Not at all, Not really, A little bit, Very much. Participants responded using a button box with two fingers (index and middle finger) on each hand. FCR task wanting ratings for each stimulus were coded from 1 to 4. High-ED food, low-ED food and non-food pictures were colored photos matched on size, shape, contrast, and resolution. High-ED food pictures included items such as a donut, chocolate chip cookies, a cinnamon bun, a slice of pizza, and tater tots (10 sweet and 5 savory items per run). Low-ED food photos included items such as a green apple, mushrooms, cooked peas and carrots, and strawberries. Non-food photos included stationery items and other objects such as post-it notes, a highlighter, masking tape, and gold screws. Verbal ratings for 1) hunger, 2) fullness, 3) desire to eat, 4) thirst, 5) stress, and 6) boredom preceded and followed the MRI scan. These ratings were reported on an analog scale ranging from 0–100, where “0” represented “Not at All” and “100” represented “Extremely”.
fMRI acquisition
Images were acquired using a 3.0 Tesla scanner (Phillips HealthCare, Best, the Netherlands) with a multi-element 32 channel receiver head coil. Anatomical images were acquired using a T1-weighted 3D magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence (TR/TE = 8.0/3.70 ms; flip angle = 8°; 1 × 1x1 mm3 resolution; acquisition matrix, 212 × 172; 1-mm thick slices; field of view (FOV), 212 × 172x150; 150 slices). BOLD-weighted functional images were acquired using single-shot SENSE-EPI (TR/TE = 2500/30 ms; flip angle = 70°; 3 × 3x3 mm3 resolution; acquisition matrix, 84 × 81; 3-mm thick slices; FOV, 256 × 256x141 mm; acquisition of 47 contiguous slices). Slices were acquired in ascending order.
Genetically predicted prefrontal DRD4 gene expression
DNA was extracted from saliva samples collected at the initial consultation. Expression of DRD4 in prefrontal brain regions was computed using PrediXcan, a prediction method that estimates tissue-specific gene expression based on individual-level genotype data27. Genotyping was conducted using the genome-wide Illumina Infinium Multi-Ethnic Global Array (MEGA), with clusters for the SNPs defined using GenomeStudio version 2011.1 and GenTrain 1.0. Quality control on the genotyping calls has been previously described37. SNPs were verified for a genotyping rate ≥ 95% and no deviation from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (P < 0.001), and minor allele frequency ≥ 0.05, using PLINK38,39. After quality control procedures and imputation, which included ascertainment of participant sex, 1,767,525 SNPs were available for use in PrediXcan. To describe the population stratification, we performed principal component analysis using SMARTPCA40 on this pruned dataset of genotyped SNPs (with r2 < 0.20, sliding window of 50 and an increment of 5 SNPs).
PrediXcan uses a machine learning approach to generate algorithms that estimate the genetically determined component of gene expression in specific brain regions at the individual level from the subject's genotype. The PrediXcan algorithm was built using a reference dataset from deceased human brain donors, comprising data from the GTEx project (version 7)41, GEUVADIS42 and DGN43, containing both genotype and gene expression levels. The PrediXcan method was executed according to methods available in27, using GTEX version 7 frontal cortex eQTL model41.
Socioeconomic status (SES) composite score
To assess socioeconomic environment, we used a combination of variables collected as part of the larger study. Detailed description of the composite score calculation is described elsewhere20. Briefly, mothers completed a demographic questionnaire including questions on annual household income, maternal education, food insecurity, perceived resource availability, and receipt of public assistance. To ensure that these five variables reflected the same underlying theoretical structure, and to derive a composite score reflecting multiple dimensions of socioeconomic environment, we conducted a principal component analysis (PCA) with Promax rotation. Each of the variables was standardized and weighted by its factor loading to create a composite score in which a higher score indicates higher socioeconomic status.
Statistical analysis
Behavioral data analysis
Analysis of behavioral data was conducted using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, version 25.0). Adolescents were divided into two groups representing higher vs. lower predicted prefrontal DRD4 gene expression levels (mean split). Wanting ratings for high-ED foods were averaged to create a single value for each participant. This calculation was repeated for low-ED foods and non-foods. Two-way ANOVAs (sex x DRD4 group) were used to compare between-group differences in continuous variables (age, BMI, BMI z-score, BMI percentile, body fat percent, socioeconomic environment score, FCR task wanting ratings, and internal state verbal ratings). Results were corrected for multiple comparisons with Bonferroni adjustment. Chi-square tests were used to assess between-group differences in categorical variables (pubertal status, race). Results were considered significant at p < 0.05 after Bonferroni adjustment.
fMRI data analysis
Analysis of fMRI data was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM version 12; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK) in the MATLAB R2017a programming environment. The Artifact Detection Tools (ART) toolbox for MATLAB (Gabrieli Laboratory, MIT, Cambridge, MA) was used to detect global mean and motion outliers in the functional data. A participant’s functional data were excluded if > 20% of all the volumes were tagged as high motion (motion > 3 mm in any direction). Subsequently, for each participant’s run, functional images were slice-time corrected using the median slice as the reference slice, realigned to the mean of the images after the initial realignment, normalized to the MNI-EPI template, and smoothed with a 6 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. For first-level statistical analysis, we constructed mass-univariate general linear regression models for each participant. The regressors included the task conditions (“high-ED”, “low-ED”, “non-food”, “fixation”) as events of interest and the realignment motion parameters as covariates. Task-related regressors were convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) and a high-pass filter of 128 s was applied. The following contrasts of interest were calculated at the single subject level: 1) high-ED vs. non-food; 2) low-ED vs. non-food. For group-level statistical analysis, we assessed the random effects of task-related activity between groups (high DRD4 vs. low DRD4 groups) using Bayesian posterior inference44 applied to the contrast images generated from the first-level analysis, including two principal components reflecting population stratification (PC1, PC2) as covariates in the model. The Bayesian approach is optimal for exploratory whole brain analyses since it infers the posterior probability of detecting the observed group effects given the observed activation map without making strong assumptions about effect size44, and therefore does not require adjustment for multiple comparisons and the associated risk of over-correction45,46. Per47, clusters were reported if they had an effect size Cohen’s d > 0.2, a Bayes factor logBF > 3.0, and a cluster extent threshold k > 10.
Results
Baseline characteristics
Participant demographics, anthropometrics and means for behavioral variables for our analytic sample are reported in Table 1. Demographic characteristics (child age, child sex, child race, child BMI z-score, and household income) were similar between the final analytic (n = 73) sample and the entire consented (n = 98) sample. However, maternal education was higher in the analytic sample (64.4% graduated from college) compared with the entire sample (55.7% graduated from college) (p = 0.003). Age, BMI z-score, and SES composite score did not differ significantly by sex or DRD4 group, and no sex by DRD4 group interactions were present. Mother-reported race for adolescents did not differ by sex or DRD4 group. As expected, females had significantly higher body fat percent compared to males (F(1,69) = 25.07, p < 0.001) across both DRD4 groups, and pubertal stage was more advanced in females, with females predominantly at the post pubertal stage, and males predominantly in mid and late pubertal stages (p < 0.001), with no evidence for differences by DRD4 group.
Table 1.
Descriptive characteristics.
Low DRD4 group (n = 40) | High DRD4 group (n = 33) | Whole sample (n = 73) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD) or N (%) | |||||
Female (n = 22) | Male (n = 18) | Female (n = 17) | Male (n = 16) | ||
Age | 16.2 (1.4) | 16.0 (1.2) | 16.6 (1.0) | 16.0 (1.3) | 16.2 (1.2) |
Pubertal stagea | |||||
Early pubertal stage | 0 (0) | 1 (5.9) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 1 (1.5) |
Mid pubertal stage | 0 (0) | 2 (11.8) | 0 (0) | 4 (33.3) | 6 (9.0) |
Late pubertal stage | 2 (9.5) | 14 (82.4) | 0 (0) | 7 (58.3) | 23 (34.3) |
Post pubertal stage | 19 (90.5) | 0 (0) | 17 (100.0) | 1 (8.3) | 37 (55.2) |
Race/Ethnicity | |||||
White | 8 (36.4) | 11 (61.1) | 11 (64.7) | 12 (75.0) | 42 (57.5) |
Black or African American | 11 (50.0) | 6 (33.3) | 2 (11.8) | 4 (25.0) | 23 (31.5) |
Asian | 2 (9.1) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 2 (2.7) |
More than one race | 1 (4.5) | 1 (5.6) | 3 (17.6) | 0 (0) | 5 (6.8) |
Other/unknown | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 1 (5.9) | 0 (0) | 1 (1.4) |
BMI | 25.4 (7.4) | 24.0 (5.8) | 23.3 (4.7) | 23.8 (6.0) | 24.2 (6.1) |
BMI z-score | 0.76 (1.04) | 0.58 (1.27) | 0.40 (1.12) | 0.49 (1.38) | 0.57 (1.18) |
Body Fat % | 29.8 (10.2) | 18.3 (10.4) | 27.4 (8.5) | 16.2 (8.8) | 23.4 (11.1) |
SES composite score | − 0.322 (1.147) | 0.139 (0.797) | 0.328 (0.869) | 0.067 (1.060) | 0.028 (0.999) |
Food cue reactivity task wanting ratings | |||||
High-ED foods | 2.51 (0.65) | 2.61 (0.52) | 2.01 (0.64) | 2.75 (0.65) | 2.47 (0.66) |
Low-ED foods | 2.57 (0.42) | 2.47 (0.54) | 2.27 (0.52) | 2.47 (0.53) | 2.45 (0.50) |
Non-foods | 1.61 (0.48) | 1.57 (0.57) | 1.71 (0.45) | 1.64 (0.55) | 1.63 (0.50) |
Internal state verbal ratingsb | |||||
Hunger | 34.4 (24.4) | 48.9 (31.9) | 24.4 (27.2) | 44.5 (33.3)c | 37.9 (29.9) |
Fullness | 66.5 (26.2) | 52.5 (28.3) | 65.4 (30.3) | 48.1 (32.1)c | 58.8 (29.5) |
Desire to eat | 37.0 (29.5) | 45.0 (32.0) | 30.3 (29.5) | 41.6 (35.1) | 38.3 (31.2) |
Thirst | 35.9 (27.8) | 42.8 (28.9) | 26.5 (25.8) | 31.9 (35.2) | 34.5 (29.4) |
Stress | 21.6 (32.4) | 19.3 (22.8) | 29.4 (29.9) | 25.0 (34.0) | 23.6 (29.7) |
Boredom | 54.1 (29.0) | 44.2 (31.9) | 26.9 (18.9) | 34.7 (31.2) | 41.1 (29.6) |
an = 21f/17 m for low group; 17f/12 m for high group; 67 for whole sample.
bInternal State Verbal Ratings collected before the FCR task.
cMales reported significantly higher ratings for hunger (p = 0.009) and significantly lower ratings for fullness (p = 0.029) relative to females before the FCR task, independent of DRD4 group.
Internal state ratings
Males reported significantly higher ratings for hunger (F(1,67) = 7.13, p = 0.009) and significantly lower ratings for fullness (F(1,67) = 4.98, p = 0.029) relative to females prior to the FCR task. No sex differences were found for ratings of desire to eat, thirst, stress and boredom.
Adolescents in the low DRD4 group reported significantly higher ratings for boredom (F(1,67) = 7.64, p = 0.007) relative to the adolescents in the high DRD4 group. No other DRD4 group differences were identified. No sex by DRD4 group interactions were identified for any of the internal state verbal ratings assessed prior to the FCR task.
FCR task wanting ratings
Males reported significantly higher wanting ratings for the high-ED food cues (F(1,67) = 8.55, p = 0.005) relative to females. Wanting ratings for the high-ED food, low-ED food and non-food cues did not differ significantly by DRD4 group. There was a significant sex by DRD4 group interaction for the high-ED food cue wanting ratings, such that males in the high DRD4 group reported significantly higher ratings relative to females in the high DRD4 group, while males and females in the low DRD4 group had similar high-ED food cue wanting ratings (F(1,67) = 4.06, p = 0.048). FCR wanting ratings are depicted in Fig. 1.
Figure 1.
Food cue reactivity task wanting ratings (Mean ± SEM) in adolescents by predicted prefrontal DRD4 expression level (high vs. low) and sex (female vs. male).
Imaging results
In response to high-ED food compared with non-food cues, females in the low DRD4 group showed lower BOLD response in the cuneus, middle occipital gyrus, superior occipital gyrus, precuneus, inferior parietal lobule, superior temporal gyrus, postcentral gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum. For the same contrast, males in the low DRD4 group showed greater BOLD response in the inferior parietal lobule relative to males in the high DRD4 group. For the contrast of low-ED food vs. non-food cues, females in the low DRD4 group showed lower BOLD response in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), precentral gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, midcingulate cortex, and superior parietal lobule relative to females in the high DRD4 group. For the same contrast, males in the low DRD4 group showed greater BOLD response in the inferior parietal lobule relative to males in the high DRD4 group. Results can be found in Table 2 and Figs. 2a,b.
Table 2.
Areas showing differential BOLD Response to High-ED Food vs. Non-Food and Low-ED Food vs. Non-Food Cues in FCR task between groups with low vs. high predicted prefrontal DRD4 expression.
Contrast | Cluster size k | Peak Log odds (logBF) | Xa | Y | Z |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Low > High DRD4 prefrontal expression | |||||
FEMALE | |||||
High-ED food > non-food cue | |||||
n.s | |||||
High-ED food < non-food cue | |||||
L Middle Occipital Gyrus | 76 | 4.51 | − 16 | − 90 | − 4 |
R Cuneus | 39 | 4.95 | 20 | − 66 | 30 |
R Cerebellum VI | 37 | 4.35 | 38 | − 50 | − 32 |
L Middle Frontal Gyrus | 28 | 4.01 | − 30 | 12 | 42 |
L Inferior Parietal Lobule | 28 | 4.08 | − 34 | − 58 | 40 |
R Postcentral Gyrus | 28 | 4.47 | 44 | − 22 | 34 |
L Superior Temporal Gyrus | 25 | 3.82 | − 60 | − 20 | 2 |
R Medial Prefrontal Cortex | 15 | 3.93 | 12 | 46 | 6 |
R Precuneus | 14 | 3.32 | 14 | − 48 | 60 |
R Hippocampus | 13 | 3.90 | 32 | − 16 | − 20 |
R Middle Frontal Gyrus | 11 | 3.39 | 38 | 6 | 44 |
L Superior Occipital Gyrus | 11 | 4.03 | − 16 | − 86 | 24 |
Low-ED food > non-food cue | |||||
n.s | |||||
Low-ED Food < Non-Food cue | |||||
L Precentral Gyrus | 153 | 5.80 | − 36 | − 12 | 38 |
R Precentral Gyrus | 91 | 5.13 | 36 | − 8 | 46 |
L Middle Frontal Gyrus | 56 | 4.57 | − 26 | 14 | 42 |
R Middle Frontal Gyrus | 23 | 3.46 | 36 | 8 | 46 |
R Midcingulate Cortex | 19 | 4.82 | 6 | 12 | 38 |
R Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex | 19 | 5.31 | 14 | 50 | − 4 |
L Orbitofrontal Cortex | 19 | 3.74 | − 18 | 34 | − 16 |
R Superior Parietal Lobule | 17 | 3.60 | 16 | − 48 | 54 |
MALE | |||||
High-ED food > non-food cue | |||||
R Inferior Parietal Lobule | 18 | 4.32 | 34 | − 52 | 52 |
High-ED food < non-food cue | |||||
n.s | |||||
Low-ED food > non-food cue | |||||
R Inferior Parietal Lobule | 37 | 4.06 | 30 | − 40 | 52 |
Low-ED food < non-food cue | |||||
n.s |
Results adjusted for Population Stratification 1, Population Stratification 2 effect size = 0.2, logBF > 3, k > = 10.
aCoordinates in MNI space.
Figure 2.
(a) Areas showing differential BOLD Response to High-ED Food vs. Non-Food and Low-ED Food vs. Non-Food Cues between Groups with Low vs. High Predicted Prefrontal DRD4 Expression among Adolescent Females. Figure depicts all clusters with effect size Cohen’s d > 0.2 and Bayes factor logBF > 3.0. Clusters surviving cluster extent threshold k > 10 are labelled. IPL inferior parietal lobule, Mid Occ middle occipital gyrus, Cer cerebellum, MFG middle frontal gyrus, STG superior temporal gyrus, mPFC medial prefrontal cortex, Hipp hippocampus, vmPFC ventromedial prefrontal cortex, OFC orbitofrontal cortex, MCC midcingulate cortex. (b) Areas showing differential BOLD Response to High-ED Food vs. Non-Food and Low-ED Food vs. Non-Food Cues between Groups with Low vs. High Predicted Prefrontal DRD4 Expression among Adolescent Males. Figure depicts all clusters with effect size Cohen’s d > 0.2 and Bayes factor logBF > 3.0. Clusters surviving cluster extent threshold k > 10 are labelled. IPL inferior parietal lobule.
Discussion
Our results demonstrate that differential genetically-predicted DRD4 expression in the PFC is associated with differential patterns of brain activation in response to food cues of varying energy density (ED), when presented in the fed state. These differences appear to be sex dependent, such that adolescent girls with low predicted expression of DRD4 in PFC showed a pronounced pattern of lower activation to high-ED food cues across multiple brain regions implicated in diverse higher-order and lower-order cognitive processes including attention, memory, visual processing, taste processing and motor processing. Differences also vary somewhat by stimulus, such that adolescent girls with low predicted expression of DRD4 in the PFC showed lower activation of orbitofrontal cortex and ventromedial PFC, regions critical for valuation of stimuli, in response to low-ED cues. No sex by DRD4 interactions were apparent for ratings of internal state, or stimuli-related wanting during the FCR task. However, males reported more hunger and less fullness pre-scan compared to females, and adolescents with low DRD4 reported higher pre-scan boredom. Taken together, our results provide initial support for a model in which genetically-determined dopamine function in prefrontal cortex, and biological and social components associated with sex, act together to influence neural processing of food cues in the satiated state among adolescent girls and boys.
Our findings are broadly consistent with previous mechanistic and clinical studies on function of the prefrontal cortex and its modulation by dopamine through development. Specifically, the prefrontal cortex is known to modulate behaviors relevant to eating behavior and obesity via anatomical and functional connections with other cortical and sub-cortical brain areas8,9. Further, during adolescence the process of cortical neuronal maturation and synaptic neurotransmission has been found to be dopamine dependent10,48,49. Altered expression of dopamine receptor genes could therefore be associated with dysregulated activity in cortical ensembles, affecting the capacity for cortical inhibition and thereby influencing the functioning of neural circuits underlying food preference and selection related behaviour50–52. The differentiation of our neuroimaging association results by sex is also broadly consistent with a substantial literature demonstrating sex differences in the rate of structural and functional maturation of prefrontal cortex during adolescence30 as well as in neural and behavioral responses to food cues31,32.
Against this general background, certain features of our results merit further discussion. First, we found much more evidence for differentiation in neural activation patterns among girls, as opposed to boys, with relatively low predicted DRD4 expression. Specifically, girls with low predicted expression of DRD4 in the PFC showed a pronounced pattern of reduced activation to high-ED food cues across a widespread appetitive circuit of brain regions implicated in diverse processes including attention, memory, visual processing, taste processing and motor processing. This suggests that girls with relatively reduced dopamine receptor 4 (DRD4) expression in the PFC may exhibit a relative blunting, or suppression, of appetitive processing in the brain in the context of a task requiring evaluation of subjective appetite in response to high-calorie stimuli. In contrast, girls with relatively high DRD4 expression may be most likely to mount a complex response to a highly palatable food stimulus in the environment, engaging multiple regions associated with stimulus salience, motivation, and attentional control. The emergence of these differential patterns of activation among girls, but not boys, is consistent with greater likelihood of a complex appetitive response among girls. The behavioral consequences of differentiation according to prefrontal DRD4 expression cannot be inferred within this cross-sectional study. However it was notable that mean wanting ratings for high-ED foods for girls with low expression (2.5) were more similar to the higher ratings among boys with either low (2.7) or high (2.8) expression, than for girls with high expression (2.0). Larger longitudinal studies may be able to determine whether low PFC DRD4 expression in girls could in some individuals or settings act to increase obesity risk by decreasing complex processing of high-ED foods, or, alternatively, to increase obesity resilience by decreasing the salience of high-ED foods during evaluation of appetite.
Also striking was the differentiation we observed between patterns of activation in response to high-ED and low-ED foods compared to non-food in the case of girls. For girls, group differences in activation were largely distinct for each stimulus category, with the exception of the middle frontal gyrus, which showed relatively reduced activation bilaterally in response to both high-ED and low-ED foods in comparison with non-foods. Since the middle frontal gyrus plays a role in reorienting attention from externally-driven to endogenous attentional control53, diminished activation here may reflect a relatively reduced inclination to attend to internal satiety signals when evaluating the desire to eat both higher and lower energy foods. However, the diminished response to high-ED foods across a much broader array of regions implicated in motivation and attention suggests that evaluating appetite for high-energy foods engages a much more complex neural response than the same process for low-ED foods, among girls with lower DRD4 expression in PFC. In contrast, boys with lower predicted DRD4 expression showed relatively increased activation of right inferior parietal lobe for both high-ED and low-ED foods compared with non-foods. This result was in contrast to results in females, who showed relatively decreased activation of left inferior parietal lobule in response to high-ED foods vs. non-foods together with decreased activation across a large number of attentional processing regions. Given the IPL’s role in maintaining attentional control on current task goals while simultaneously preparing a response to salient stimuli in the environment54, this phenomenon suggests that decreased PFC dopamine function may act to increase simple attention toward foods compared with non-foods in boys, and to decrease more complex attentional processing specifically toward high-ED foods in girls.
Also of note was that adolescent girls with lower DRD4 expression showed lower activation of vmPFC and OFC in response to low-ED compared with non-foods. Both vmPFC and OFC have been implicated in goal directed behaviours especially in determining the value of a goal such as food reward during the process of decision making55,56. Interestingly, neuronal activity in the vmPFC during evaluation of value has been found to be irrelevant to the degree of self-control executed by participants57. Studies of neural responses to food cues have also shown activation of the OFC among healthy-weight adults, consistent with involvement in neural representation of value58–61. The DRD4 group difference we saw here may therefore reflect a relatively decreased valuation of healthy low-ED foods among girls with lower dopamine function in PFC.
While our imaging analyses revealed diverging effects of DRD4 expression in females and males, our behavioral analyses showed main effects of sex on appetite ratings, and of DRD4 on ratings of emotion, namely boredom. Regardless of predicted PFC DRD4 group, adolescent males reported greater pre-scan hunger, lower pre-scan fullness, and higher wanting ratings for the high-ED food cues, compared with adolescent girls. This is consistent with other evidence for enhanced food motivation in men compared with women throughout the lifespan, and especially during puberty and adolescence when sex differences in energy demands become pronounced62,63. In our data it therefore appears that the effect of sex generally trumps that of DRD4 expression to influence subjective appetite. Since our goal for the current work was to explore effects of DRD4 expression within each sex rather than to test main effects of sex, and DRD4 status did not significantly influence appetite reports in either sex, we here present our results unadjusted for internal state ratings. It was notable, however, that adolescent girls with low predicted DRD4 showed wanting ratings for high-ED foods (and to some degree wanting ratings for low-ED foods and pre-scan hunger and desire to eat) that were more similar to those of adolescent boys than to their peers with high predicted DRD4, suggesting that behavioral indices of appetite in this group may also show some evidence for heightening. Since the current investigation was a secondary analysis, our cell sizes for these comparisons were small and no a priori power calculation was conducted to generate the target sample size. It is therefore possible that a larger study would be more sensitive to such effects. Future research could also consider repeating analyses with control for baseline appetite to distinguish whether observed results are substantially attributable to effects of predicted DRD4 on global appetite, or to effects of predicted DRD4 on neural food cue responses independent of general subjective appetite. Potential influence of PFC DRD4 expression on emotion-driven eating should also be investigated further. Our fMRI task paradigm for this study was not designed to explicitly examine the effect of emotion on neural food cue response. However, consistent with the role of dopamine in motivation, we found that adolescents with low predicted PFC DRD4 expression expressed higher levels of boredom before beginning the task, and boredom has been shown in some individuals to trigger consumption of highly palatable snack foods64.
Our results suggest that prefrontal DRD4 expression and sex may influence neural and behavioral food cue responses, and that prefrontal DRD4 expression may show unique impacts on these outcomes among adolescent girls. One potential explanation for the divergence of our results by sex may be sex differences in relative stages of brain development rather than enduring sex differences persisting through development65. For example, brain circuits subserving executive function, including the PFC, are relatively late-maturing in comparison with other brain regions, and mature more rapidly in females66. Variability in the functioning of brain regions engaged during decision making may therefore be more pronounced in girls, both mediated by dopamine function, and allowing differences driven by dopamine function to emerge. It would therefore be of interest to test whether these sex differences are also apparent in adulthood, when both sexes have reached the same level of maturation. A related potential explanation is hormonal. Hormonal transition periods such as adolescence are marked by structural and functional maturation of cortical networks and increased vulnerability to behavioral disorders involving dopamine function8, and girls in our sample were more advanced in pubertal stage. Further, dopamine function is influenced by sex and previous evidence shows a differential role of sex hormones such as insulin, leptin, progesterone and estrogen on dopamine-regulated behaviours including motivation, reward and emotion processing63,67,68. Estrogen levels have been proposed to modulate brain regions subserving emotional regulation69,70. Emotional eating behavior may also be regulated by estrogen through estrogen receptor-alpha (ERalpha)71. Other mechanisms by which sex hormones could be regulating emotional eating behaviours include gastric mechanoreceptors, glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1), gustatory sensations and orosensensory hedonics72. Estrogen has been proposed to facilitate dopaminergic neurotransmission in cortical and sub-cortical brain regions73,74. Estrogen may also facilitate glutamatergic and suppress GABAergic inhibitory transmission through its action at GABAA receptors75. A PET study conducted during the postpartum period (a period with increased hormonal and emotional dysregulation) found reduced levels of estrogen to be associated with increased levels of an enzyme involved in metabolising dopamine in the female brain76. Future studies examining the impacts of puberty are warranted to further investigate hormonal effects on cortical dopaminergic transmission and its effect on food cue response. Given evidence that neural food cue responses vary with menstrual cycle in adult women77–79, studies comparing young girls in specific phases of the menstrual cycle may also help to clarify the current findings. It should also be noted that the sex differences by DRD4 expression that we observed here could in part be attributable to social as well as hormonal forces such as increased perceived pressure for girls to display prosocial behaviors80, or to restrict dietary intake and attain a low body weight69,72. Such environmental factors could result in a more complex, conflicting cognitive processing of food cues in girls which is selectively minimized among girls who show a blunted response to environmental stimuli due to decreased genetically-driven DRD4 function in PFC.
The present results should be considered in the context of an emerging literature demonstrating that genetically-influenced dopamine function can modulate the effects of sex, and environmental factors, on variables associated with obesity risk25,81,82. For example, a sex by DRD2 rs6722 genotype interaction was previously observed such that females carrying the TT allele performed significantly better on an attention task than males carrying the CC allele82, while another study found that the VNTR DRD4-7R-hypofunctional allele was more strongly associated with high caloric food intake in young girls than boys83. Despite evidence in the same cohort that DRD4 expression interacted with SES and to influence food intake20, exploratory analyses did not support interactive effects of SES and DRD4 group on neural responses to food cues. However, as previously discussed, the sex-specific effects we observed are consistent with environmental, as well as biological, correlates of sex modulating effects of DRD4 expression, supporting the overall model that DRD4 expression modulates environmental effects on obesity risk.
Limitations of our study include the sample size. For example, studies with considerably larger sample sizes would be required to investigate potential three-way interactions between DRD4 expression, sex, and socioeconomic variation. Also, our comparisons by sex and DRD4 group were not able to show that sex and predicted DRD4 expression interacted to influence food cue wanting ratings, or BMI z score. This suggests that the brain activation effects we observed may be more proximally and therefore strongly related to the effects of DRD4 expression than downstream behavioral or anthropometric outcomes. Since perceptual uncertainty influences decision making and is dependent on dopamine84, and reactivity to cues is predicted by the value of the reward and its availability in the given environment85, neural responses within our food cue task, which centered on unavailable rewards, were likely most optimal for capturing effects of DRD4 expression. Another limitation was that we did not investigate potential social and environmental variables that could explain the sex differences we observed. Nevertheless, taken in the context of complementary research, our results support sex-specific effects of prefrontal DRD4 on brain food responsiveness in adolescence, with modulation of appetitive circuit responses to food cues most apparent in female adolescents.
Acknowledgements
This study was funded by R00DK088360 (PI: SC). Further support for SC and AP from R01DK113286 and UG3OD023313. Additional funding for LD from NIH (5U01HD061973-03), FQRSC Team(2015-SE-179342), CIHR-India Team, FRN(110726) and SSHRC Operating(410-2010-2258).
Author contributions
A.K.P., T.J. and A.P. wrote the first draft of the paper, and all authors contributed to subsequent drafts. A.P., L.C., A.K.P. and P.P.S. were responsible for statistical analyses, and R.L. and A.P. led the genotyping. L.D., A.K.P., P.P.S. and S.C. conceived the study. S.C. designed the larger study and obtained funding. All authors reviewed and approved the final submitted manuscript.
Data availability
The datasets used for the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors jointly supervised this work: Laurette Dube and Susan Carnell.
References
- 1.Libbey HP, Story MT, Neumark-Sztainer DR, Boutelle KN. Teasing, disordered eating behaviors, and psychological morbidities among overweight adolescents. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2008;16(Suppl 2):S24–S29. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Boutelle KN, Hannan P, Fulkerson JA, Crow SJ, Stice E. Obesity as a prospective predictor of depression in adolescent females. Health Psychol. 2010;29(3):293–298. doi: 10.1037/a0018645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Thierry AM, Gioanni Y, Dégénétais E, Glowinski J. Hippocampo-prefrontal cortex pathway: Anatomical and electrophysiological characteristics. Hippocampus. 2000;10(4):411–419. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4<411::AID-HIPO7>3.0.CO;2-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Brockmann MD, Pöschel B, Cichon N, Hanganu-Opatz IL. Coupled oscillations mediate directed interactions between prefrontal cortex and hippocampus of the neonatal rat. Neuron. 2011;71(2):332–347. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2004;101(21):8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6(3):309–315. doi: 10.1038/nn1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Petanjek Z, Judaš M, Šimic G, Rasin MR, Uylings HBM, Rakic P, et al. Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108(32):13281–13286. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105108108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Larsen B, Luna B. Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018;94:179–195. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Michaud A, Vainik U, Garcia-Garcia I, Dagher A. Overlapping neural endophenotypes in addiction and obesity. Front. Endocrinol. (Lausanne). 2017;8:127. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2017.00127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Tseng KY, O’Donnell P. D2 dopamine receptors recruit a GABA component for their attenuation of excitatory synaptic transmission in the adult rat prefrontal cortex. Synapse. 2007;61(10):843–850. doi: 10.1002/syn.20432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Oak JN, Oldenhof J, Van Tol HH. The dopamine D(4) receptor: One decade of research. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2000;405(1–3):303–327. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(00)00562-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Asghari V, Sanyal S, Buchwaldt S, Paterson A, Jovanovic V, Van Tol HH. Modulation of intracellular cyclic AMP levels by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants. J. Neurochem. 1995;65(3):1157–1165. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1995.65031157.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Van Tol HH, Wu CM, Guan HC, Ohara K, Bunzow JR, Civelli O, et al. Multiple dopamine D4 receptor variants in the human population. Nature. 1992;358(6382):149–152. doi: 10.1038/358149a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.D’Souza UM, Russ C, Tahir E, Mill J, McGuffin P, Asherson PJ, et al. Functional effects of a tandem duplication polymorphism in the 5’flanking region of the DRD4 gene. Biol. Psychiatry. 2004;56(9):691–697. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Nikolova YS, Ferrell RE, Manuck SB, Hariri AR. Multilocus genetic profile for dopamine signaling predicts ventral striatum reactivity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(9):1940–1947. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Epstein LH, Temple JL, Neaderhiser BJ, Salis RJ, Erbe RW, Leddy JJ. Food reinforcement, the dopamine D2 receptor genotype, and energy intake in obese and nonobese humans. Behav. Neurosci. 2007;121(5):877–886. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.5.877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Obregón AM, Oyarce K, García-Robles MA, Valladares M, Pettinelli P, Goldfield GS. Association of the dopamine D2 receptor rs1800497 polymorphism with food addiction, food reinforcement, and eating behavior in Chilean adults. Eat Weight Disord. 2021 doi: 10.1007/s40519-021-01136-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Obregón AM, Valladares Vega MA, Goldfield G, Llewellyn C. Genetic variation of the dopamine D2 receptor gene: Association with the reinforcing value of food and eating in the absence of hunger in Chilean children. Nutr. Hosp. 2020;34(3):524–533. doi: 10.20960/nh.02897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Paquet C, Portella AK, Moore S, Ma Y, Dagher A, Meaney MJ, et al. Dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates real-world behavioural response to the food retail environment in children. BMC Public Health. 2021;21(1):145. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-10160-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Portella AK, Papantoni A, Paquet C, Moore S, Rosch KS, Mostofsky S, et al. Predicted DRD4 prefrontal gene expression moderates snack intake and stress perception in response to the environment in adolescents. PLoS ONE. 2020;15(6):e0234601. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0234601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Mandelbaum J, Moore S, Silveira PP, Meaney MJ, Levitan RD, Dubé L. Does social capital moderate the association between children’s emotional overeating and parental stress? A cross-sectional study of the stress-buffering hypothesis in a sample of mother-child dyads. Soc. Sci. Med. 2020;257:112082. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.12.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Barth B, Bizarro L, Miguel PM, Dubé L, Levitan R, O’Donnell K, et al. Genetically predicted gene expression of prefrontal DRD4 gene and the differential susceptibility to childhood emotional eating in response to positive environment. Appetite. 2020;148:104594. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Levitan RD, Jansen P, Wendland B, Tiemeier H, Jaddoe VW, Silveira PP, et al. A DRD4 gene by maternal sensitivity interaction predicts risk for overweight or obesity in two independent cohorts of preschool children. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry. 2017;58(2):180–188. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nagy E, Moore S, Gruber R, Paquet C, Arora N, Dubé L. Parental social capital and children’s sleep disturbances. Sleep Health. 2016;2(4):330–334. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2016.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Silveira PP, Gaudreau H, Atkinson L, Fleming AS, Sokolowski MB, Steiner M, et al. Genetic differential susceptibility to socioeconomic status and childhood obesogenic behavior: Why targeted prevention may be the best societal investment. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(4):359–364. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.4253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Silveira PP, Meaney MJ, Dubé L. DRD4, income, and children’s food choices: Plasticity allele or different opportunities? Reply. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(8):810–811. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gamazon ER, Wheeler HE, Shah KP, Mozaffari SV, Aquino-Michaels K, Carroll RJ, et al. A gene-based association method for mapping traits using reference transcriptome data. Nat. Genet. 2015;47(9):1091–1098. doi: 10.1038/ng.3367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Stice E, Yokum S, Bohon C, Marti N, Smolen A. Reward circuitry responsivity to food predicts future increases in body mass: Moderating effects of DRD2 and DRD4. Neuroimage. 2010;50(4):1618–1625. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Stice E, Spoor S, Bohon C, Small DM. Relation between obesity and blunted striatal response to food is moderated by TaqIA A1 allele. Science. 2008;322(5900):449–452. doi: 10.1126/science.1161550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Koolschijn PCMP, Crone EA. Sex differences and structural brain maturation from childhood to early adulthood. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013;5:106–118. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wang G-J, Volkow ND, Telang F, Jayne M, Ma Y, Pradhan K, et al. Evidence of gender differences in the ability to inhibit brain activation elicited by food stimulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106(4):1249–1254. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0807423106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gobbi S, Weber SC, Graf G, Hinz D, Asarian L, Geary N, et al. Reduced neural satiety responses in women affected by obesity. Neuroscience. 2020;447:94–112. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.07.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lansigan RK, Emond JA, Gilbert-Diamond D. Understanding eating in the absence of hunger among young children: A systematic review of existing studies. Appetite. 2015;85:36–47. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Smith KR, Papantoni A, Chen L, Benson L, Carnell S. Familial risk and sex influence ad libitum meal intake and neural food cue responses in adolescence. Obesity. 2020;28:98–98. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, Grummer-Strawn LM, Flegal KM, Mei Z, et al. 2000 CDC Growth Charts for the United States: Methods and development. Vital Health Stat. 2002;11(246):1–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Carskadon MA, Acebo C. A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development. J. Adolesc. Health. 1993;14(3):190–195. doi: 10.1016/1054-139x(93)90004-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Qiu A, Tuan TA, Ong ML, Li Y, Chen H, Rifkin-Graboi A, et al. COMT haplotypes modulate associations of antenatal maternal anxiety and neonatal cortical morphology. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2015;172(2):163–172. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14030313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Qiu A, Shen M, Buss C, Chong Y-S, Kwek K, Saw S-M, et al. Effects of antenatal maternal depressive symptoms and socio-economic status on neonatal brain development are modulated by genetic risk. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27(5):3080–3092. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MAR, Bender D, et al. PLINK: A tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2007;81(3):559–575. doi: 10.1086/519795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Patterson N, Price AL, Reich D. Population structure and eigenanalysis. PLoS Genet. 2006;2(12):e190. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0020190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.GTEx Consortium The genotype-tissue expression (GTEx) project. Nat Genet. 2013;45(6):580–585. doi: 10.1038/ng.2653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lappalainen T, Sammeth M, Friedländer MR, ’t Hoen PAC, Monlong J, Rivas MA, et al. Transcriptome and genome sequencing uncovers functional variation in humans. Nature. 2013;501(7468):506–511. doi: 10.1038/nature12531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Battle A, Mostafavi S, Zhu X, Potash JB, Weissman MM, McCormick C, et al. Characterizing the genetic basis of transcriptome diversity through RNA-sequencing of 922 individuals. Genome Res. 2014;24(1):14–24. doi: 10.1101/gr.155192.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Neumann J, Lohmann G. Bayesian second-level analysis of functional magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage. 2003;20(2):1346–1355. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00443-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Friston KJ, Glaser DE, Henson RNA, Kiebel S, Phillips C, Ashburner J. Classical and Bayesian inference in neuroimaging: Applications. Neuroimage. 2002;16(2):484–512. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gelman A, Hill J, Yajima M. Why we (usually) don’t have to worry about multiple comparisons. J. Res. Educ. Eff. 2012;5(2):189–211. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Han H, Park J. Using SPM 12’s second-level Bayesian inference procedure for fMRI analysis: Practical guidelines for end users. Front. Neuroinform. 2018;12:1. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2018.00001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Grobin AC, Deutch AY. Dopaminergic regulation of extracellular gamma-aminobutyric acid levels in the prefrontal cortex of the rat. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1998;285(1):350–357. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Andersson RH, Johnston A, Herman PA, Winzer-Serhan UH, Karavanova I, Vullhorst D, et al. Neuregulin and dopamine modulation of hippocampal gamma oscillations is dependent on dopamine D4 receptors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109(32):13118–13123. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201011109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ting AK, Chen Y, Wen L, Yin D-M, Shen C, Tao Y, et al. Neuregulin 1 promotes excitatory synapse development and function in GABAergic interneurons. J. Neurosci. 2011;31(1):15–25. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2538-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Jaaro-Peled H, Hayashi-Takagi A, Seshadri S, Kamiya A, Brandon NJ, Sawa A. Neurodevelopmental mechanisms of schizophrenia: Understanding disturbed postnatal brain maturation through neuregulin-1-ErbB4 and DISC1. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32(9):485–495. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bhardwaj SK, Baharnoori M, Sharif-Askari B, Kamath A, Williams S, Srivastava LK. Behavioral characterization of dysbindin-1 deficient sandy mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2009;197(2):435–441. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Japee S, Holiday K, Satyshur MD, Mukai I, Ungerleider LG. A role of right middle frontal gyrus in reorienting of attention: A case study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2015;9:23. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Singh-Curry V, Husain M. The functional role of the inferior parietal lobe in the dorsal and ventral stream dichotomy. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(6):1434–1448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kable JW, Glimcher PW. The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10(12):1625–1633. doi: 10.1038/nn2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Plassmann H, O’Doherty J, Rangel A. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. J. Neurosci. 2007;27(37):9984–9988. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2131-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science. 2009;324(5927):646–648. doi: 10.1126/science.1168450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.van der Laan LN, de Ridder DTD, Viergever MA, Smeets PAM. The first taste is always with the eyes: A meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. Neuroimage. 2011;55(1):296–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Siep N, Roefs A, Roebroeck A, Havermans R, Bonte ML, Jansen A. Hunger is the best spice: An fMRI study of the effects of attention, hunger and calorie content on food reward processing in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 2009;198(1):149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Pohl TM, Tempelmann C, Noesselt T. How task demands shape brain responses to visual food cues. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38(6):2897–2912. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Luo S, Alves J, Hardy K, Wang X, Monterosso J, Xiang AH, et al. Neural processing of food cues in pre-pubertal children. Pediatr. Obes. 2019;14(2):e12435. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Nu CT, MacLeod P, Barthelemy J. Effects of age and gender on adolescents’ food habits and preferences. Food Qual Prefer. 1996;7(3–4):251–262. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Woods SC, Gotoh K, Clegg DJ. Gender differences in the control of energy homeostasis. Exp. Biol. Med. 2003;228(10):1175–1180. doi: 10.1177/153537020322801012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Moynihan AB, van Tilburg WAP, Igou ER, Wisman A, Donnelly AE, Mulcaire JB. Eaten up by boredom: Consuming food to escape awareness of the bored self. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:369. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Koolschijn PC, Peper JS, Crone EA. The influence of sex steroids on structural brain maturation in adolescence. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(1):e83929. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. Sex differences in the adolescent brain. Brain Cogn. 2010;72(1):46–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Becker JB. Gender differences in dopaminergic function in striatum and nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1999;64(4):803–812. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00168-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Brinton RD, Thompson RF, Foy MR, Baudry M, Wang J, Finch CE, et al. Progesterone receptors: Form and function in brain. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2008;29(2):313–339. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Del Parigi A, Chen K, Gautier J-F, Salbe AD, Pratley RE, Ravussin E, et al. Sex differences in the human brain’s response to hunger and satiation. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2002;75(6):1017–1022. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/75.6.1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Dreher J-C, Schmidt PJ, Kohn P, Furman D, Rubinow D, Berman KF. Menstrual cycle phase modulates reward-related neural function in women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104(7):2465–2470. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605569104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Foryst-Ludwig A, Kintscher U. Metabolic impact of estrogen signalling through ERalpha and ERbeta. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 2010;122(1–3):74–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2010.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Asarian L, Geary N. Sex differences in the physiology of eating. Am. J. Physiol. 2013;305(11):R1215–R1267. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00446.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Uban KA, Rummel J, Floresco SB, Galea LAM. Estradiol modulates effort-based decision making in female rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(2):390–401. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Peper JS, van den Heuvel MP, Mandl RCW, Hulshoff Pol HE, van Honk J. Sex steroids and connectivity in the human brain: A review of neuroimaging studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36(8):1101–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Smith SS, Woolley CS. Cellular and molecular effects of steroid hormones on CNS excitability. Cleve Clin J Med. 2004;71(Suppl 2):S4–10. doi: 10.3949/ccjm.71.suppl_2.s4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Sacher J, Wilson AA, Houle S, Rusjan P, Hassan S, Bloomfield PM, et al. Elevated brain monoamine oxidase A binding in the early postpartum period. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(5):468–474. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Frank TC, Kim GL, Krzemien A, Van Vugt DA. Effect of menstrual cycle phase on corticolimbic brain activation by visual food cues. Brain Res. 2010;1363:81–92. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Arnoni-Bauer Y, Bick A, Raz N, Imbar T, Amos S, Agmon O, Marko L, Levin N, Weiss R. Is it me or my hormones? Neuroendocrine activation profiles to visual food stimuli across the menstrual cycle. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2017;102(9):3406–3414. doi: 10.1210/jc.2016-3921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Alonso-Alonso M, Ziemke F, Magkos F, Barrios FA, Brinkoetter M, Boyd I, Rifkin-Graboi A, Yannakoulia M, Rojas R, Pascual-Leone A, Mantzoros CS. Brain responses to food images during the early and late follicular phase of the menstrual cycle in healthy young women: Relation to fasting and feeding. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2011;94(2):377–384. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.010736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Kitayama S, King A, Yoon C, Tompson S, Huff S, Liberzon I. The dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) moderates cultural difference in independent versus interdependent social orientation. Psychol. Sci. 2014;25(6):1169–1177. doi: 10.1177/0956797614528338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Levitan RD, Kaplan AS, Davis C, Lam RW, Kennedy JL. A season-of-birth/DRD4 interaction predicts maximal body mass index in women with bulimia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(8):1729–1733. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Gurvich C, Rossell SL. Dopamine and cognitive control: Sex-by-genotype interactions influence the capacity to switch attention. Behav. Brain Res. 2015;281:96–101. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.11.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Silveira PP, Portella AK, Kennedy JL, Gaudreau H, Davis C, Steiner M, et al. Association between the seven-repeat allele of the dopamine-4 receptor gene (DRD4) and spontaneous food intake in pre-school children. Appetite. 2014;73:15–22. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275(5306):1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Nomoto K, Schultz W, Watanabe T, Sakagami M. Temporally extended dopamine responses to perceptually demanding reward-predictive stimuli. J. Neurosci. 2010;30(32):10692–10702. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4828-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets used for the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.