Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Dec 17.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatrics. 2008 Nov;122(5):961–970. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-3063

Cumulative Violence Exposure and Self-Rated Health: Longitudinal Study of Adolescents in the United States

Renée Boynton-Jarrett 1, Louise Ryan 2, Lisa Berkman 1, Rosalind Wright 1,3
PMCID: PMC8679309  NIHMSID: NIHMS541527  PMID: 18977974

Abstract

OBJECTIVE

The goal was to determine whether cumulative exposure to violence in childhood and adolescence contributes to disparities in self-rated health among a nationally representative sample of US adolescents.

METHODS

The National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1997 is an ongoing, 8-year (1997–2004), longitudinal, cohort study of youths who were 12 to 18 years of age at baseline (N = 8224). Generalized estimating equations were constructed to investigate the relationship between cumulative exposure to violence and risk for poor health.

RESULTS

At baseline, 75% of subjects reported excellent or very good health, 21.5% reported good health, and 4.5% reported fair or poor health. Cumulative violence exposures (witnessed gun violence, threat of violence, repeated bullying, perceived safety, and criminal victimization) were associated with a graded increase in risk for poor health and reduced the strength of the relationship between household income and poor health. In comparison with subjects with no violence exposure, risk for poor self-rated health was 4.6 times greater among subjects who reported ≥5 forms of cumulative exposure to violence, controlling for demographic features and household income. Trend analysis revealed that, for each additional violence exposure, the risk of poor health increased by 38%. Adjustment for alcohol use, drug use, smoking, depressive symptoms, and family and neighborhood environment reduced the strength of the relationships between household income and cumulative exposure to violence scores and poor self-rated health, which suggests partial mediation of the effects of socioeconomic status and cumulative exposure to violence by these factors.

CONCLUSIONS

In this nationally representative sample, social inequality in risk for poor self-rated health during the transition from adolescence to adulthood was partially attributable to disparities in cumulative exposure to violence. A strong graded association was noted between cumulative exposure to violence and poor self-rated health in adolescence and young adulthood.

Keywords: longitudinal study, socioeconomic status, self rated health, adolescents, exposure to violence

Introduction

Exposure to violence in childhood and adolescence constitutes a pervasive national public health problem,1,2 affecting an estimated 70% to 97% of US school-aged children and teens. Community violence disproportionately affects minority youths and youths living in urban environments.1 Youths living in poorer neighborhoods have increased exposure to violence in both their homes and their communities.314 Moreover, youths with lower socioeconomic status (SES) experience more-frequent social stressors.3,5,9,15 Disparate exposure to social adversities, such as violence, may contribute to social inequalities in health.

Research evidence has emerged that highlights the long-term effects of violence exposure in early life on morbidity and mortality rates in adulthood,16 as well as on physical and mental health and risk profiles.13,1619 Reducing adverse health outcomes related to violence exposure has been identified as one of the health promotion priorities for Healthy People 2010,20 but there is limited understanding of the mechanisms through which violence exposure leads to poor health outcomes. Life course epidemiology conceptualizes determinants of disease occurrence in terms of biological and social exposures experienced during different stages of life.17 Adverse childhood exposures during critical periods may have both latent effects21,22 and cumulative effects that negatively affect health outcomes and trajectory.23–,25

In this study, we evaluated the association between cumulative exposure to violence (CEV) and self-rated health (SRH) for adolescents transitioning into young adulthood. Our preliminary hypothesis was that, as individuals were exposed to increasing amounts of community violence, their SRH would decline. This study is unique in that we prospectively evaluated youth reports of SRH in a socioeconomically and racially/ethnically diverse national sample of youths and used a statistical method that accounts for correlations between repeated measures over time.

Methods

Survey Design

The National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1997 is an ongoing prospective cohort study initiated in 1997 to examine the transition from school to the workforce among US adolescents. A nationally representative sample of 8984 youths, 12 to 18 years of ages, was enrolled, after screening of 75 291 households in 147 nonoverlapping primary sampling units. A supplemental sample of black and Hispanic youths in that age range was also recruited. Computer-assisted personal interviews are conducted annually to collect information on labor market participation, training, income, family formation and background, behaviors, and health. Both youths and parents/guardians are surveyed. Retention rates between 1998 and 2004 were 93% to 86% and 94% to 88% for the cross-sectional and supplemental samples, respectively.26

Sample

We restricted our analyses to the first 8 waves of data (1997–2004) available at the time of analysis and youths who did not have a chronic condition at baseline (n = 570), because having a “physical, emotional, or mental condition that limited schoolwork” was highly correlated with SRH in our bivariate analyses (χ2 test, P < .001). We also excluded youths with incomplete baseline information on violence exposure, health behaviors, or demographic characteristics (n = 190). After exclusions, the final sample size was 8224 youths.

Measures

Definition of SRH

The outcome of interest, participants' SRH, was assessed annually by using a single question, that is, “How would you rate your health?,” on a Likert-type scale with 5 possible responses (excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor). SRH is commonly used to assess global health status and has been recommended by the World Health Organization and the European Union Commission for monitoring. Validation research has established SRH as a reliable valid predictor of health care utilization and physical health problems.27,28 SRH has several advantages, including the strength and consistency of its predictive power for morbidity and death and the potential to reflect human judgments, behaviors, and resources.28 It reliably predicts death independent of other medical, behavioral, and psychosocial risk factors among adults,28 and it has been associated with other health measures.29 In the Medical Expenditure Panel Survey, youths ≤17 years of age in fair or poor health had significantly higher average total expenditures ($2688 vs $805 per child) and were more likely to have office visits, inpatient hospitalizations, and prescription medications than were youths in good to excellent health.30 Responses were analyzed by using SRH as a dichotomous outcome. The variable was dichotomized into good (good, very good, or excellent) versus poor (fair or poor), comparable to previous studies.3134

Definition of Exposure to Violence

Exposure to violence was measured with 4 baseline questions (in 1997) and 3 follow-up questions, queried during the first survey after each participant's 18th birthday. These questions assessed 4 domains of violence exposure, that is, direct victimization, perceived safety, threat of violence, and witnessing violence. Perceived school safety was assessed with a single question in 1997, “Do you feel safe at school?,” rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale from strongly agree to strongly disagree. We dichotomized this variable as unsafe (disagree or strongly disagree) versus safe (agree or strongly agree). Threat of violence was assessed in 1997 as the number of times someone had “threatened to hurt” the respondent at school, on a scale of 0 to 50. Youths were asked about repeated bullying and witnessing gun violence before the age of 12 with 2 yes/no questions, as follows. (1) “Before you turned age 12, were you ever the victim of repeated bullying?” (2) “Before you turned age 12, did you ever see someone get shot or shot at with a gun?” Threat of violence, bullying, and witnessing gun violence were all dichotomized as ever/never variables. During follow-up interviews, participants were queried about gun violence and bullying at ages 12 to 18 with the following questions. (1) “Between the ages of 12 and 18, have you ever been shot at, or seen someone get shot or shot at, with a gun?” (2) “Between the ages of 12 and 18, were you ever the victim of repeated bullying?” In 2002, all youths were asked about criminal victimization between 1997 and 2002, as follows: “In the last 5 years, have you been the victim of a violent crime, for example, physical or sexual assault, robbery, or arson?”

The CEV score was created by summing the number of self-reported violence exposures, including school safety in 1997, threatened in 1997, witnessed shooting at <12 years of age, witnessed shooting at 12 to 18 years of age, repeated bullying at <12 years of age, repeated bullying at 12 to 18 years of age, and criminal victimization. Because of small sample sizes in the highest categories, we combined those with ≥5 exposures into a single group. We created the CEV score because we did not have access to a formal measure that assesses multiple sources of exposure to violence in this data set. There was a statistically significant correlation among all individual types of violence included in the CEV score (data not shown). For subanalyses, we also created separate CEV scores for violence exposures before 12 years of age and exposures between 12 and 18 years of age.

Demographic, Behavioral, Familial, and Neighborhood Characteristics

Potential confounding variables were identified on the basis of theoretical relevance, literature suggestions, or empirical evidence. Definitions of these variables are presented in Table 1. Gross household income was modeled as a categorical variable (less than $20 000, $20 000 to $50 000, or more than $50 000). Youths with incomplete or missing information were categorized as having missing data (~26% of the original sample; n = 2252). Youths with missing income data were more likely to be black (30% vs 24%) or Hispanic (26% vs 20%) and were less likely to smoke (37% vs 40%) than were those who reported income; however, they did not differ appreciably with respect to any other baseline characteristics. Parent SRH in 1997 was dichotomized as excellent, very good, or good versus fair or poor. Youths with missing values for parent SRH (n = 1022) accounted for 12% of the original sample. Parent SRH was positively correlated with youth SRH (r = 0.18; P = .001).

TABLE 1.

Demographic, Behavioral, Familial, and Neighborhood Characteristics

Variable Definition
Gross household income (1997)a Created by combining wages of respondent, wages of spouse/partner, child support, interest and dividends from stocks or mutual funds, rental income, retirement pension, alimony, Social Security income, public support, monetary gifts, and other income; based on parental report unless respondent was living independently; categorical variable (less than $20 000, $20 000 to $50 000, or more than $50 000).
Place of residencea Respondents’ zip codes were linked to Federal Information Processing standard geographic information codes for state and county (with Maptitude V4.7 software, Caliper Corporation, Newton, MA) in order to identify whether place of residence was urban or rural. Urbanized areas are based on the Census Bureau definition and include those with population >2,500. Urban residence was dichotomized and updated with each annual survey. Those whose zip code could not be precisely defined as urban or rural were listed as unknown.
Family structure Dichotomized as 2-parent household versus all other household types (including single parent).
Race/ethnicity Recoded as non-Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, Hispanic, Asian, or other (because of sample sizes, Asian and other combined as 1 category); categorical variable.
Parent SRH (1997) Dichotomized as excellent, very good, or good versus fair or poor.
Depressive symptoms (2000, 2002, and 2004) Participants reported frequency of depressive symptoms over past month, “How much of the time during the last month have you felt down in the dumps?”; dichotomized none vs some/most/all of the time.
Baseline depressive symptoms (1997) Subsample (n = 4988) of cohort was asked, “Are you unhappy, sad, or depressed?”; responses dichotomized as not true vs somewhat/often true.
BMI Self-reported weight and height at baseline rounded to the nearest centimeter and kilogram, respectively. BMI is calculated as weight (kg) divided by height squared (m2). A categorical variable was created for BMI-for-age percentile rankings: underweight <5th percentile, healthy weight 5th-85th percentile, overweight 85th-95th percentile, and obese >95th percentile, and missing as recommended by the Centers for Disease Control using age- and sex-standardized measures.
Smoking status Lifetime use (ever/never) in 1997 and updated annually; dichotomized
Alcohol use Lifetime use (ever/never) in 1997 and updated annually; dichotomized
Marijuana use Lifetime use (ever/never) in 1997 and updated annually; dichotomized
Neighborhood gangs All youths questioned about whether there were “gangs in their neighborhood”; yes/no response, updated annually; dichotomized
Homeless between 1997 and 2002 (2002) Participants asked whether they had been homeless or lived in homeless shelter for ≥2 nights in past 5 years; yes/no response; dichotomized
Household member incarcerated in 1997–2002 (2002) Subjects asked whether household member had been incarcerated in past 5 years; yes/no response; dichotomized
Family/Home Risk Index (1997)b Combines youth and parent interview questions and interviewer observations at baseline to provide overall assessment of youth's environment, ie, quality of physical home environment, neighborhood risk factors, availability of enriching educational activities in home and environment, religious behavior of family, parental involvement in schooling and family routines, television viewing, parental physical and mental health/disability, parental monitoring, and youth-parent relationship; higher scores indicate more-risky environment; dichotomized as >75th percentile versus all others.

Statistical Analyses

All statistical analyses were performed by using SAS (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). Participants with incomplete baseline information on violence exposure were excluded from the analysis. Those with missing information on violence exposure in the later survey years (1998–2004) were assumed not to have been exposed. Because inclusion of those with missing information who might have had exposure would result in misclassification and might bias results toward the null, we also performed analyses excluding those with missing information, to evaluate the potential impact, and we found no substantial difference in the results.

Univariate analyses were performed to evaluate the distributions of both SRH and CEV scores and all theoretically relevant independent variables, including measures of SES, sociodemographic characteristics, health behaviors, and individual violence exposures. Frequency distributions were performed to determine the correlation between dichotomized SRH in 1997 and potential categorical predictors (including, age, gender, parent SRH, and categorical household income). A covariate was included in the multivariate analyses if theoretical or empirical evidence supported its role as a risk factor for poor SRH, if it was a significant predictor of SRH in univariate regression models, or if including it in the full multivariate model led to a change in the odds ratio of ≥5%.35 Finally, we evaluated the correlations between different types of violence exposure by using χ2 tests.

Generalized estimating equation analyses, performed by using the SAS PROC GENMOD procedure, were used to estimate a logistic model for predictors of poor SRH while allowing for dependence between repeated measurements for the same subject.36,37 We used an exchangeable correlation structure and logistic link function. The estimated exchangeable correlation was fairly low, at 0.18. To prevent an imbalance attributable to missing data, we included a within-subject effect that specified the order of measurements for individuals. Analyses used all 8 waves of data to create a general linear model to predict poor SRH. The model estimates are population averaged.

Initially, we performed a series of multiple-predictor models estimating the impact of each early life adversity on SRH separately. Model 1 included the following variables: age, gender, race/ethnicity, household income, family structure, baseline BMI, and time-varying residence in an urban area. Model 2 included all variables in model 1 plus the CEV score. Model 3 included all variables in model 2 plus parent SRH, homelessness, incarcerated household member, depressive symptoms between 2000 and 2004, neighborhood gangs, family risk environment scale, and time-varying risky health behaviors (smoking, alcohol consumption, and marijuana use).

Mediation of the impact of household income on SRH by CEV was tested as outlined by Baron and Kenny,38 to ensure that the mediating variable was correlated with the outcome, the outcome was correlated with the antecedent, and the relationship between antecedent and outcome changed when the mediator was included. First, we evaluated the relationship between household income and CEV. Next, generalized estimating equation models with and without CEV assessed whether CEV mediated the relationship between SES and SRH. In addition, we tested whether there were significant interactions between CEV and time and between CEV and income.

In subanalyses, we evaluated the relative contributions of cumulative measures of violence in early life (<12 years of age) and adolescence (12–18 years of age) independently and within the same models. All analyses were repeated by excluding those with missing household income data, to assess differences between those with missing income data and the rest of the sample. Supplementary analyses were performed with the subset of the cohort screened for depressive symptoms at baseline in 1997 (N = 4988), to test whether baseline depressive symptoms confounded observed associations.

RESULTS

Study Group and Baseline Findings

The final sample used in analyses consisted of 8224 youths (49% female), with a mean age of 14 years (range: 12–18 years) in 1997 (Table 2). At baseline, the majority (75%) of the youths reported excellent or very good SRH, 21.5% reported good SRH, and ~ 4.5% reported fair (4.3%; n = 348) or poor (0.2%; n = 19) SRH. Whereas 14% of youths reported not feeling safe at school, 20% had been threatened, and 19% had been bullied repeatedly before age 12. The prevalence of youths reporting fair or poor SRH differed according to gender, race/ethnicity, household income, and parental health. Compared with those reporting good SRH, the prevalence of fair/poor SRH was higher among female (56%), low-income (32%), and black youths (39%).

TABLE 2.

Prevalence of Baseline Demographic Characteristics According to Baseline Fair/Poor SRH (N = 8224)

Characteristic Total Fair/Poor SRH
Gender, n (%)
    Male 4178 (51) 163 (44)
    Female 4046 (49) 210 (56)
Age, mean ± SD, y 14 ± 1.5
Race/ethnicity, n (%)
    Asian and other 75 (1) 1 (0.3)
    Hispanic 1781 (22) 89 (24)
    Non-Hispanic black 2139 (26) 146 (39)
    Non-Hispanic white 4229 (51) 137 (36.7)
Two-parent household, n (%)
    No 3008 (37) 189 (51)
    Yes 5216 (63) 184 (49)
SRH, n (%)
    Poor 19 (0.2) 19 (5)
    Fair 354 (4.3) 354 (95)
    Good 1781 (21.5)
    Very good 2803 (31)
    Excellent 3267 (44)
Household income, n (%)
    Low (less than $20 000) 1622 (20) 120 (32)
    Medium ($20 000 to $50 000) 2190 (26) 90 (24)
    High (more than $50 000) 2160 (26) 59 (16)
    Missing data 2252 (27) 104 (28)
Smoking ever, n (%)
    Yes 3162 (39) 188 (50)
    No 5062 (62) 185 (50)
Alcohol ever, n (%)
    Yes 3488 (42) 191 (51)
    No 4736 (58) 182 (49)
Marijuana ever, n (%)
    Yes 1602 (19) 114 (31)
    No 6622 (81) 259 (69)
Parent SRH, n (%)
    Poor/fair 1024 (13) 102 (27)
    Good 6179 (75) 218 (59)
    Missing data 1021 (12) 53 (14)
BMI, n (%)
    <5th percentile 185 (2) 12 (3)
    5th to 85th percentile 5642 (69) 187 (50)
    85th to 95th percentile 1151 (14) 62 (17)
    >95th percentile 863 (10) 83 (22)
    Missing data 383 (5) 29 (8)
Household member incarcerated, n (%)
    Yes 445 (5) 34 (9)
    No 7779 (95) 339 (91)
Homeless ever, n (%)
    Yes 96 (1) 14 (4)
    No 8128 (99) 359 (96)
Urban environment, n (%)
    Yes 6005 (73) 289 (77)
    No 2219 (27) 84 (23)
Neighborhood gangs, n (%)
    Yes 4852 (59) 251 (67)
    No 3372 (41) 122 (33)
Family/Home Risk Index, n (%)
    High risk 1025 (12) 96 (26)
    Low risk 7199 (88) 277 (74)

The prevalence of each type of violence exposure and the CEV scores according to SRH at baseline are presented in Table 3. Compared with youths who reported good SRH, those with fair/poor SRH had higher prevalence rates of not feeling safe at school (29% vs 13%), being threatened at school (31 vs 19%), being bullied repeatedly (31 vs 18%), and having witnessed shooting (23 vs 10%). Table 4 presents the distribution of SRH findings according to CEV scores. Of youths with CEV scores of ≥5, 18% reported fair/poor SRH, compared with only 3% of those with CEV scores of 0. Table 5 demonstrates the distribution of demographic characteristics and known risk factors for poor SRH according to CEV scores. Most variables varied significantly according to CEV scores.

TABLE 3.

Prevalence of Violence Exposures According to SRH at Baseline

Violence Exposure n (%)
Total (N = 8224) Good SRH (N = 7851) Poor SRH (N = 373)
Not safe at school 1146 (14) 1037 (13) 108 (29)a
Threatened at school 1636 (20) 1521 (19) 115 (31)a
Bullied repeatedly at <12 y 1554 (19) 1440 (18) 114 (31)a
Bullied at 12–18 y 784 (10) 729 (9) 55 (15)b
Witnessed shooting at <12 y 887 (11) 804 (10) 84 (23)a
Witnessed shooting at 12–18 y 966 (12) 890 (11) 76 (20)a
Victim of crime, attack, or assault 473 (6) 453 (6) 20 (6)
a

Χ2 test, P < .0001.

b

Χ2 test, P < .005.

TABLE 4.

CEV Scores and SRH at Baseline

CEV Score n (%)
Total (N = 8224) Good SRH (N = 7851) Fair/Poor SRH (N = 373)
0 4020 (49) 3917 (97) 103 (3)
1 2206 (27) 2092 (95) 114 (5)
2 1159 (14) 1091 (94) 67 (6)
3 542 (6.5) 492 (91) 50 (9)
4 221 (2.5) 196 (89) 25 (11)
5 77 (1) 63 (81) 14 (18)

CEV scores were created by summing the number of self-reported violence exposures, including school safety in 1997, threatened in 1997, witnessed shooting at <12 years, witnessed shooting at 12 to 18 years, repeated bullying at <12 years, repeated bullying at 12 to 18 years, and criminal victimization.

TABLE 5.

Prevalence of Violence Exposure Categories According to Demographic Characteristics (N = 8224)

Characteristic No. With Indicated Violence Categories (%)
Total 0 1 2 3 4 ≥5
Gendera
    Male 4178 (51) 1858 (44) 1163 (28) 659 (16) 320 (8) 128 (3) 50 (1)
    Female 4046 (49) 2162 (53.5) 1043 (26) 499 (12) 222 (5.5) 93 (2.3) 27 (0.7)
Age
    12 y 1080 (13) 547 (51) 262 (24) 174 (16) 63 (6) 24 (2) 10 (1)
    13 y 1585 (19.2) 783 (49) 434 (27) 208 (13) 111 (7) 36 (2) 13 (1)
    14 y 1733 (21) 846 (49) 474 (27) 255 (15) 96 (5.5) 46 (2.5) 16 (1)
    15 y 1714 (21) 823 (48) 476 (28) 223 (13) 121 (7) 54 (3) 17 (1)
    16 y 1552 (19) 736 (47) 423 (27) 215 (14) 117 (7.5) 46 (3) 15 (1)
    17 y 541 (6.6) 277 (51) 130 (24) 81 (15) 32 (6) 15 (3) 6 (1)
    18 y 19 (0.2) 8 (42) 7 (37) 2 (11) 2 (11) 0 0
Race/ethnicity
    Asian and other 75 (1) 37 (49) 17 (23) 8 (11) 7 (9) 3 (4) 3 (4)
    Hispanicb 1781 (22) 877 (49) 510 (29) 235 (13) 112 (6) 29 (2) 18 (1)
    Non-Hispanic blacka 2139 (26) 793 (37) 641 (30) 395 (18.5) 195 (9) 82 (4) 33 (1.5)
    Non-Hispanic white 4229 (51) 2313 (55) 1038 (25) 520 (12) 228 (5) 107 (2.5) 23 (0.5)
Two-parent householda
    No 3008 (37) 1203 (40) 859 (28.6) 513 (17) 269 (9) 123 (4) 41 (1.4)
    Yes 5216 (63) 2817 (54) 1347 (26) 645 (12) 273 (5.3) 98 (2) 36 (0.7)
SRHa
    Fair/poor 373 (4.5) 103 (28) 114 (30) 67 (18) 50 (13) 25 (7) 14 (4)
    Good/excellent 7851 (95.5) 3917 (50) 2092 (27) 1091 (14) 492 (6) 196 (2) 63 (1)
Household incomea
    Low 1622 (20) 634 (39) 472 (29) 279 (17) 147 (9) 60 (4) 30 (2)
    Medium 2190 (27) 1074 (49) 574 (26) 301 (14) 150 (7) 65 (3) 26 (1)
    High 2160 (26) 1220 (56) 532 (24) 263 (12) 96 (4.5) 40 (2) 9 (0.5)
    Missing data 2252 (27) 1092 (48) 628 (28) 315 (14) 149 (7) 56 (2.5) 12 (0.5)
Parent SRHa
    Poor/fair 1024 (12.5) 396 (39) 291 (28) 176 (17) 379 (6) 44 (4) 20 (2)
    Good 6179 (75) 3144 (51) 1609 (26) 841 (14) 97 (9.5) 152 (2.5) 54 (1)
    Missing data 1021 (12.5) 480 (47) 306 (30) 141 (14) 66 (6.5) 25 (2.5) 3 (0.3)
BMIa
    <5th percentile 185 (2) 90 (49) 44 (24) 28 (15) 17 (9) 4 (2) 2 (1)
    5th to 85th percentile 5642 (69) 2859 (51) 1486 (26) 770 (14) 339 (6) 143 (3) 45 (1)
    85th to 95th percentile 1151 (14) 516 (45) 333 (29) 170 (15) 85 (7) 37 (3) 10 (1)
    >95th percentile 863 (10) 363 (42) 244 (28) 144 (17) 72 (8) 23 (2) 17 (2)
    Missing data 383 (5) 192 (50) 99 (26) 46 (12) 29 (8) 14 (4) 3 (1)
Household member incarcerateda
    Yes 445 (5.4) 156 (35) 102 (23) 79 (18) 62 (14) 30 (7) 16 (4)
    No 7779 (94.6) 3864 (49.5) 2104 (27) 1079 (14) 480 (6) 191 (.5) 61 (1)
Homelessa
    Yes 96 (1.2) 15 (16) 17 (18) 23 (24) 25 (26) 6 (6) 10 (10)
    No 8128 (98.8) 4005 (49) 2189 (27) 1135 (14) 517 (6.3) 215 (2.7) 67 (1)
Family/Home Risk Indexa
    Low risk 7199 (87.5) 3679 (51) 1920 (26) 952 (13) 430 (6) 164 (2) 54 (1)
    High risk 1025 (12.5) 341 (33) 286 (28) 206 (20) 112 (11) 57 (6) 23 (2)
Smoking evera
    Yes 3162 (38) 1296 (41) 847 (27) 546 (17) 302 (10) 126 (4) 45 (1.4)
    No 5062 (62) 2724 (54) 1359 (27) 612 (12) 240 (5) 95 (2) 32 (0.6)
Alcohol evera
    Yes 3488 (42) 1509 (43) 912 (26) 583 (17) 307 (9) 140 (4) 37 (1)
    No 4736 (58) 2511 (53) 1294 (27) 575 (12) 235 (5) 81 (2) 40 (1)
Marijuana evera
    Yes 1602 (20) 563 (35) 440 (27) 288 (27) 188 (12) 92 (6) 31 (2)
    No 6622 (80) 3457 (52) 1766 (27) 870 (13) 354 (5) 129 (2) 46 (0.7)
Neighborhood gangsa
    Yes 4852 (59) 1943 (40) 1369 (28) 847 (17) 437 (9) 188 (4) 68 (1.4)
    No 3372 (41) 2077 (61.6) 837 (25) 311 (9) 105 (3.1) 33 (1) 9 (0.3)
Urban environmenta
    Yes 6005 (73) 2837 (47) 1644 (27) 892 (15) 409 (7) 166 (3) 57 (1)
    No 2219 (27) 1183 (53) 562 (25) 266 (12) 133 (6) 55 (2.5) 20 (1)
a

P < .0001.

b

P < .01.

Multivariate Analyses

To model the population-averaged effects of several social, demographic, and behavioral factors on SRH over time, we used the PROC GENMOD procedure. First, separate models were used to assess the relationships between each type of violence exposure, household adversity, and health risk behavior and SRH individually, controlling for baseline demographic features. When modeled individually, the odds of poor SRH increased by ~60% to 80% for each type of violence exposure (results not shown). Estimates of the odds ratio (OR) for each violence category were statistically significant (P< .01).

The impact of CEV was even more pronounced. We developed 3 predictive models, as shown in Table 6, controlling for potential confounders in the most parsimonious analysis. The analysis that included CEV scores demonstrated a graded association between the odds of poor SRH and CEV scores (model 2). Compared with youths with no CEV, youths with exposure to ≥5 types of violence had 4.63 times (95% confidence interval [CI]: 3.06–6.99 times) the odds of poor health. CEV was associated with a graded increase in risk for poor health and reduced the strength of the relationship between the lowest- and middle-income groups and poor health (ORs changed from 1.69 to 1.41 and from 1.43 to 1.28, respectively). Inclusion of potential mediators of the relationship between CEV and SRH in model 3 significantly reduced the impact of violence exposure on poor health by nearly twofold among those with ≥5 exposures. This suggests partial mediation of the impact of violence exposures on health through health risk behaviors, depression, and household dysfunction. In summary, adjustment for social stressors, CEV, and health behaviors did not fully account for the impact of household income on health. The deviance was also lower in model 3, in comparison with models 2 and 1 (18 212.7, 24 841.5, and 25 366.4, respectively), which suggests a better model fit with inclusion of CEV, health risk behaviors, family environment, and parental health. Finally, when the CEV score was modeled as a categorical variable, a strong dose-response relationship between CEV scores and poor SRH was observed (test for trend, P < .001). For each 1-unit increase in the CEV score, the risk for poor health increased by 38%.

The interaction between time and CEV suggested a diminishing impact of CEV on the odds for fair/poor SRH over time (OR: 0.98; 95% CI: 0.97–0.99). Next, we tested the interaction between continuous income and CEV in both the odds for fair/poor health and the risk for declining SRH by using the continuous scale. The 2 approaches gave qualitatively similar results, which suggests a protective effect of increasing income on the risk for fair/poor health among those exposed to CEV; however, the significance level was only marginal in the dichotomous model (OR: 0.99; 95% CI: 0.97–1.00).

Cumulative measures of violence in early life (<12 years of age) and adolescence (12–18 years of age) were evaluated in separate and the same generalized estimating equation models; graded relationships were found for both periods of exposure, but with a slightly greater propensity toward poor SRH with early-life CEV. Each 1-unit increase in early-life CEV and adolescent CEV was associated with 40% (P < .0001) and 30% (P < .0001) increases in odds for poor health, respectively (data not shown).

Additional Analyses

Although results are not presented in the text, all of the analyses described above were repeated by using SRH dichotomized as poor, fair, or good (n = 2154; 26%) versus very good or excellent (n = 6070; 74%). Similar graded relationships were found for CEV scores, household income, and risk for poor/fair/good SRH. In the fully adjusted model, trend analyses revealed that risk for poor/fair/good SRH increased 21% for each 1-unit increase in the CEV score.

Restriction of the analytic sample to youths who did not have missing data for household income yielded results analogous to those for the original sample (data available on request). Restriction of the sample to those with baseline data on depressive symptoms (n = 4988) revealed that, although depressive symptoms at baseline were a significant predictor of SRH in the fully adjusted model (OR: 1.29; 95% CI: 1.11–1.50), inclusion did not alter the estimates for income and only slightly attenuated the effects of CEV; moreover, depressive symptoms between 2000 and 2004 remained statistically significant (OR: 1.56; 95% CI: 1.39–1.76).

DISCUSSION

Our findings demonstrated a strong graded relationship between CEV scores and risk for poor SRH in a nationally representative sample of adolescents. In this study, witnessing gun violence, threat of violence, feeling unsafe, repeated bullying, and criminal victimization each independently and significantly increased risk for poor SRH. A dose-response relationship between CEV and poor SRH was established and is consistent with research that demonstrates the cumulative effect of multiple stressors on health risk.16,19,39

Previous research documented an association between child poverty and the incidence of violence exposure.8,10,40,41 In our study, witnessing gun violence and feeling unsafe significantly differed according to household income and partially mediated the relationship between household income and health. This confirms the findings of earlier prospective studies, wherein familial income has been consistently linked to childhood health conditions.42

Family social environment has also been linked to both SES and violence exposure. Impoverished children are at higher risk for neglect and abuse, exposure to familial violence, and inadequate nurturing.13 Elevated familial risk is associated with greater emotional reactivity, poorer coping responses, and low levels of parental support among youths.13 Our study demonstrated partial mediation of the impact of CEV on SRH by familial risk index, which may reflect an impaired ability to buffer stress in the presence of adversities. We also found that health risk behaviors and depressive symptoms partially mediated the relationship between CEV and SRH. These findings support previous research that established the enduring impact of early-life exposure to social stress and violence on health.1719 Being bullied43,44 and fear of crime have been linked to poor mental and physical health.45,46 Childhood adversities have been linked to smoking initiation and behavior,4749 as well as alcohol abuse, illegal substance use,5054 and depression.5560 Lo and Cheng found that depression mediated the relationship between physical abuse and alcohol and drug abuse.53 Nicotine and illicit substances may have beneficial psychoactive properties for mood regulation, affect, and coping.48,61 Chronic social stress has been linked to neuroendocrine system dysregulation18; moreover, primate62,63 and human64 studies have linked environmental risk to serotonergic dysfunction. Because smoking, alcohol use, and drug abuse are associated with enhanced serotonergic activity,6567 it is possible that health risk behaviors represent a form of self-medication in the wake of adverse exposures.13,55

School safety may indicate the role of age-specific aspects of the social environment that influence health. Vulnerability within the school environment may contribute directly to inequalities in health through injury, anxiety, and depression. Perceived safety may influence physical activity and indirectly affect health. School safety also may be a proxy for toxic environmental exposures, neighborhood deprivation, food insecurity, access to resources, and other factors that influence health.68,69 The “differential opportunity hypothesis” posits that access to illegitimate and legitimate opportunity structures depends on the social location.70,71 If school environments vary according to SES, then in relatively poorer schools with fewer resources, youths may resort to illegitimate means (such as gangs and drug trade) to obtain social status, which threatens safety, increases violence, and ultimately undermines health.

This study finds empirical evidence in support of the accumulation-of-risk model. This form of the life course model posits that insults, whether correlated or uncorrelated, have a cumulative impact on health outcomes and trajectory.17 Our findings reveal that violence exposure in childhood and adolescence has both strong independent associations and an additive impact on SRH over time. We also found a strong correlation between various forms of violence exposure over time. When testing the critical-period model by comparing exposures in early life (<12 years of age) and later life (12–18 years), we found that, although exposures during both periods had strong graded effects on risk for poor health, early life exposures seemed to have a slightly greater predictive value for poor health. Therefore, it is possible that exposures at critical periods may have differential effects on health risk, as well as cumulative effects over time.

This study has limitations. First, we cannot rule out the possibility of reverse causality, with poor health preceding CEV. Youths who have identifiable handicaps or poorer fitness because of an illness may be more likely to be the targets of violence in school. However, our analyses excluded children with chronic health conditions at baseline. Fear of violence may be associated with social isolation. Social networks were not assessed in this study; therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that the relationship between CEV and poor SRH is confounded by impaired social networks. Notably, previous research documented independent effects of repeated bullying and low levels of social support on the mental health of school-aged children.43 Second, we might have failed to capture residual confounding of CEV by SES. Third, to our knowledge, the measures of violence exposure used in this study are not validated.

Although SRH has been used extensively in the health inequality literature, its appropriateness as a metric for adolescent health and the dimensions of health it captures continue to be debated.27 SRH is a subjective measure, therefore individuals with objectively poor health may perceive and therefore rate their health differently. If individuals conceptualize and report health differently across social classes, then SRH may not estimate reliably the degree of inequality.72 Moreover, if health assessment varies between early and late adolescence or with growing awareness of social status, then the income gradient in SRH may be an artifact of this process. Future investigations should use more-objective measures of adolescent health (eg, medical visits, hospitalizations, or physiologic correlates of disease activity).

CONCLUSIONS

CEV had graded robust effects on SRH in this sample of adolescents. Income-related inequalities in health persisted as youths transitioned into adulthood, and they were partially accounted for by CEV, health risk behaviors, family environment, and parental health. These findings suggest that interventions aimed at promoting adolescent health and well-being cannot focus on behavior modification alone. Broader social policy changes aimed at reducing violence exposure in childhood and adolescence, including promoting safer schools and communities, may have greater effects on health trajectories during adolescence and young adulthood.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Dr Boynton-Jarrett was supported by National Institutes of Health grant F31 MH67538-01 and K12 HD043444. Dr Wright was supported by National Institutes of Health grants K08 HL04187 and R01 HL64108.

We thank the youth and parent participants in the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1997 cohort. We gratefully acknowledge Howard Bauchner, MD, who provided detailed commentary and review of several drafts of this manuscript.

ABBREVIATIONS

AAP

American Academy of Pediatrics

BMI

body mass index

CAPI

computer-assisted personal interviews

CI

confidence interval

ETV

exposure to violence

FPL

Federal Poverty Line

NLSY ’97

National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1997 cohort

SES

socioeconomic status

SRH

self rated health

REFERENCES

  • 1.Brener ND, Lowry R, Barrios L, Simon T, Eaton D. Violence-Related Behaviors among High School Students- -United States, 1991-2003. MMWR. 2004;53(29):651–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Buka SL, Stichick TL, Birdthistle I, Earls FJ. Youth exposure to violence: prevalence, risks, and consequences. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2001;71(3):298–310. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.71.3.298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Margolin G, Gordis EB. The effects of family and community violence on children. Annu Rev Psychol. 2000;51:445–79. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Nansel TR, Overpeck M, Pilla RS, Ruan WJ, Simons-Morton B, Scheidt P. Bullying behaviors among US youth: prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. JAMA. 2001;285(16):2094–100. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.16.2094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mijanovich T, Weitzman BC. Which “broken windows” matter? School, neighborhood, and family characteristics associated with youths' feelings of unsafety. J Urban Health. 2003;80(3):400–15. doi: 10.1093/jurban/jtg045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Attar B, Guerra N, Tolan P. Neighborhood disadvantage, stressful life events, and adjustment in urban elementary-school children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1994;23:391–400. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Duncan DF. Growing Up under the Gun: Children and Adolescents Coping with Violent Neighborhoods. Journal of Primary Prevention. 1996;16(4):343–356. doi: 10.1007/BF02411740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Fitzpatrick KM, Boldizar JP. The prevalence and consequences of exposure to violence among African-American youth. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1993;32(2):424–430. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199303000-00026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Garbarino J, Kostelny K. What do we need to know to understand children in war and community violence? In: Simon, editor. Apfel, Roberta J. Yale University Press; CT, US: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cooley-Quille MR, Turner SM, Beidel DC. Emotional impact of children's exposure to community violence: A preliminary study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1995;34(10):1362–1368. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199510000-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Horn JL, Trickett PK. In: Community violence and child development: A review of research. Trickett Penelope K., editor. American Psychological Association; Schellenbach:DC, US: 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Osofsky JD. Children in a violent society. The Guilford Press; New York, NY, US: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Martinez P, Richters JE. The NIMH Community Violence Project: II. Children's distress symptoms associated with violence exposure. Psychiatry: Interpersonal & Biological Processes. 1993 Feb;56(1) doi: 10.1080/00332747.1993.11024618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gabrino J, Kostelny K, Dubrow N. What children can tell us about living in danger. American Psychologist. 1991;46:376–383. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.46.4.376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Scheier LM, Botvin GJ, Miller NL. Life Events, Neighborhood Stress, Psychosocial Functioning, and Alcohol Use among Urban Minority Youth. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse. 1999 [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Aber JL. Nelson CA, editor. Poverty, violence, and child development: Untangling family and community level effects. Threats to optimal development: Integrating biological and social risk factors. 1994 [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Repetti R, Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Risky families: family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;128(2):330–366. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.McLoyd V. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. Am Psychol. 1998;53(2):185–204. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ben-Shlomo Y, Kuh D. A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;31:285–293. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.McEwen B, Seeman TE. Protective and damaging effects of mediators of stress: elaborating and testing the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896:30–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08103.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hertzman C. The biological embedding of early experience and its effects on health in adulthood. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896:85–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08107.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14(4):245–58. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(98)00017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Starfield B, Riley AW, Witt WP, Robertson J. Social class gradients in health during adolescence. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2002;56(5):354–342. doi: 10.1136/jech.56.5.354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Montgomery LE, Kiely JL, Pappas G. The effects of poverty, race, and family structure on US children's health: data from the NHIS, 1978 through 1980 and 1989 through 1991. American Journal of Public Health. 1996;86(10):1401–1405. doi: 10.2105/ajph.86.10.1401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Chen E, Matthews KA, Boyce WT. Socioeconomic differences in children's health: how and why do these relationships change with age? Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128(2):295–329. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Adler NE, Ostrove JM. Socioeconomic status and health: what we know and what we don't. Annals of the NY Academy of Sciences. 1999;896:3–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08101.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Marmot MG, Smith GD, Stansfeld S, et al. Health inequalities among British civil servants: the Whitehall II Study. Lancet. 1991;337:1387–1393. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)93068-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.West P. Health inequalities in the early years: is there equalisation in youth? Soc Sci Med. 1997;44(6):833–58. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00188-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Blane Social patterning of medical mortality in youth. Social Science and Medicine. 1994;39(3):361–366. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90132-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Macintyre S, West P. Lack of class variation in health in adolescence: an artefact of an occupational measure of social class? Soc Sci Med. 1991;32(4):395–402. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(91)90340-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Goodman E. The role of socioeconomic status gradients in explaining differences in US adolescents' health. Am J Public Health. 1999;89(10):1522–8. doi: 10.2105/ajph.89.10.1522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Glendinning A, Hendry L, Shucksmith J. Lifestyle, health and social class in adolescence. Soc Sci Med. 1995;41(2):235–48. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)00316-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lantz P LJ, House J, Lepkowski J, Mero R, Musick M, WIlliams D. Socioeconomic Disparities in health change in a longitudinal study of US adults: the role of health-risk behaviors. Social Science and Medicine. 2001;53:29–40. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00319-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Barker D. Fetal and infant origins of adult disease. British Medical Journal; London: 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Barker D. Mothers, babies and disease in later life. London: British Medical Journal. 1994 [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Power C HC. Social and biological pathways linking early life and adult disease. British Medical Bulletin. 1997;53(1):210–21. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a011601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Rutter M. Pathways from childhood to adult life. Journal of Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989;30:23–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00768.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kuh DLB-SY. A life course approach to adult disease. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1997 Sourcebook. Bureau of Labor Statistics; Washington, D.C.: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Wade TJ, Vingilis E. The development of self-rated health during adolescence: an exploration of inter- and intra-cohort effects. Can J Public Health. 1999;90(2):90–4. doi: 10.1007/BF03404108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1997;38:21–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kaplan GA, Camach T. Perceived health and mortality: a nine-year follow-up of the human population laboratory cohort. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1983;117(3):292–304. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a113541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Elixhauser A, Machlin SR, Zodet MW, Chevarley FM, Patel N, McCormick MC, Simpson L. Health care for children and youth in the United States: 2001 annual report on access, utilization, quality, and expenditures. Ambul Pediatr. 2002;6:419–437. doi: 10.1367/1539-4409(2002)002<0419:hcfcay>2.0.co;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.West P. Inequalities? Social class differentials in health in British youth. Soc Sci Med. 1988;27(4):291–6. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(88)90262-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Power C. Social and economic background and class inequalities in health among young adults. Soc Sci Med. 1991;32(4):411–7. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(91)90342-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kawachi I, Kennedy B, Glass R. Social Capital and Self-Rated Health: A Contextual Analysis. American Journal of Public Health. 1999;89:1187–1193. doi: 10.2105/ajph.89.8.1187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Greenland S. Modeling and variable selection in epidemiological analysis. AJPH. 1989;79:340–349. doi: 10.2105/ajph.79.3.340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Zerger SL, Liang KY. Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes. Biometrics. 1986;42:121–130. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Zerger SL, Liang KY, Albert PS. Models for longitudinal data: a generalized estimating equation approach. Biometrics. 1988;44:1049–1060. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Newacheck PW. Poverty and childhood chronic illness. Achieves of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 1994;148:1143–1149. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1994.02170110029005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Pamuk E, Makuc D, Heck K, Reuben C, Lochner K. Socioeconomic status and health chartbook: health, United States, 1998. National Center for Health Statistics; Hyattsville, MD: 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Glendinning A, Love JG, Hendry LB, Shucksmith J. Adolescence and health inequalities: extensions to Macintyre and West. Soc Sci Med. 1992;35(5):679–87. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(92)90006-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.West P, Macintyre S, Annandale E, Hunt K. Social class and health in youth: findings from the west of Scotland twenty-07 study. Soc Sci Med. 1990;30(6):665–73. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(88)90252-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Rahkonen O, Arber S, Lahelma E. Health inequalities in early adulthood: a comparison of young men and women in Britain and Finland. Soc Sci Med. 1995;41(2):163–71. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)00320-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Rigby K. Effects of peer victimization in schools and perceived social support on adolescent well-being. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2000;1:57–68. doi: 10.1006/jado.1999.0289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Forero R, McLellan L, Rissel C, Bauman A. Bullying behaviour and psychosocial health among school students in New South Wales, Australia: cross sectional survey. BMJ. 1999;319(7206):344–8. doi: 10.1136/bmj.319.7206.344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Chandola T. The fear of crime and area differences in health. Health Place. 2001;7(2):105–16. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8292(01)00002-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Green G, Gilbertson JM, Grimsley MF. Fear of crime and health in residential tower blocks. A case study in Liverpool, UK. Eur J Public Health. 2002;12(1):10–15. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/12.1.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Rosenthal BS. Exposure to Community Violence in Adolescence: Trauma Symptoms. Adolescence. 2000;138(35):271–284. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.O'Keefe M, Sela-Amit M. An Examination of the Effects of Race/Ethnicity and Social Class on Adolescents' Exposure to Violence. Journal of Social Service Research. 1997 [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Garmezy N. Children in poverty: resilience despite risk. Psychiatry. 1993;56:127–136. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1993.11024627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Gomez JE, Johnson BA, Selva M, Sallis JF. Violent crime and outdoor physical activity among inner-city youth. Prev Med. 2004;39(5):876–81. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Khoury-Kassabri M, Benbenishty R, Astor RA, Zeira A. The contributions of community, family, and school variables to student victimization. Am J Community Psychol. 2004;34(3-4):187–204. doi: 10.1007/s10464-004-7414-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Cloward R, Ohlin L. Delinquency and opportunity: A theory of delinquent gangs. Free Press; Glencoe, IL: 1960. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Cloward R. Illegitimate means, Anomie, and Deviant Behavior. American Sociological Review. 1960;24(2):164–176. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Goodman E, McEwen BS, Dolan LM, Schafer-Kalkhoff T, Adler NE. Social disadvantage and adolescent stress. J Adolesc Health. 2005;37(6):484–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.11.126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Power C, Matthews S, Manor O. Inequalities in self rated health in the 1958 birth cohort: lifetime social circumstances or social mobility? BMJ. 1996;313(7055):449–53. doi: 10.1136/bmj.313.7055.449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.The role of the pediatrician in youth violence prevention in clinical practice and at the community level. American Academy of Pediatrics Task Force on Violence. Pediatrics. 1999;103(1):173–81. doi: 10.1542/peds.103.1.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES