Abstract
Innate behavioral biases such as human handedness are a ubiquitous form of inter-individual variation that are not strictly hardwired into the genome and are influenced by diverse internal and external cues. Yet, genetic and environmental factors modulating behavioral variation remain poorly understood, especially in vertebrates. To identify genetic and environmental factors that influence behavioral variation, we take advantage of larval zebrafish light-search behavior. During light-search, individuals preferentially turn in leftward or rightward loops, in which directional bias is sustained and non-heritable. Our previous work has shown that bias is maintained by a habenula-rostral PT circuit and genes associated with Notch signaling. Here we use a medium-throughput recording strategy and unbiased analysis to show that significant individual to individual variation exists in wildtype larval zebrafish turning preference. We classify stable left, right, and unbiased turning types, with most individuals exhibiting a directional preference. We show unbiased behavior is not due to a loss of photo-responsiveness but reduced persistence in same-direction turning. Raising larvae at elevated temperature selectively reduces the leftward turning type and impacts rostral PT neurons, specifically. Exposure to conspecifics, variable salinity, environmental enrichment, and physical disturbance does not significantly impact inter-individual turning bias. Pharmacological manipulation of Notch signaling disrupts habenula development and turn bias individuality in a dose dependent manner, establishing a direct role of Notch signaling. Last, a mutant allele of a known Notch pathway affecter gene, gsx2, disrupts turn bias individuality, implicating that brain regions independent of the previously established habenula-rostral PT likely contribute to inter-individual variation. These results establish that larval zebrafish is a powerful vertebrate model for inter-individual variation with established neural targets showing sensitivity to specific environmental and gene signaling disruptions. Our results provide new insight into how variation is generated in the vertebrate nervous system.
Keywords: zebrafish, inter-individual variation, individuality, environment, Notch, Gsx, modulation, thermoregulation
Introduction
Inter-individual variation, or individuality, is a hallmark of nearly all animal species and contributes to the population’s fitness and ability to adapt when confronted with environmental change (Dingemanse et al., 2004; Klein et al., 2017; Horváth et al., 2020). One form of inter-individual variation is sensory and motor biases. Handedness in humans is a familiar example, expressed as a sustained preference for left- or right-hand use, which the expression of a specific hand preference is only partially explained by genetics, suggesting complex interactions contribute to imposing handed phenotypes (Cuellar-Partida et al., 2020). Indeed, significant complexity underlies human handedness. The preferred hand usage is context-dependent, largely independent of other behavioral biases, and shows variable consistency – observed as consistent or inconsistent hand dominance in a task dependent manner (Watson and Kimura, 1989; Souman et al., 2009; Chu et al., 2012). Handed biases are also a conserved form of individual behavioral variation with species as diverse as hagfish (Miyashita and Palmer, 2014), Drosophila (Kain et al., 2012; Buchanan et al., 2015), chicken (Rogers, 1982; Casey and Karpinski, 1999), and various vertebrate paw/foot biases (Bulman-Fleming et al., 1997; Brown and Magat, 2011; Giljov et al., 2013; Schiffner and Srinivasan, 2013; Manns et al., 2021) showing sustained individual motor preferences. Despite the prevalence of handed behaviors, mechanisms instructing these behaviors and the variation observed across individuals are still poorly understood.
Research to date shows that binary handed-like behavioral variation is observed in isogenic Drosophila (Kain et al., 2012; Buchanan et al., 2015; Linneweber et al., 2020) and clonal fish (Izvekov et al., 2012; Bierbach et al., 2017). Even more complex behavioral modalities in isogenic mouse strains (Freund et al., 2013, 2015; Hager et al., 2014), Caenorhabditis elegans (Stern et al., 2017), Drosophila (Linneweber et al., 2020), and clonal crayfish (Vogt et al., 2008) species display stable individual phenotypes with significant inter-individual variation at the population level, suggesting external events contribute to behavioral diversity across individuals. Even in humans, external or stochastic factors are likely important as discordant handedness is frequently observed in monozygotic twins (Jäncke and Steinmetz, 1995). These examples suggest that environmental, chemical, or physical events during development, even at early developmental stages, could produce inter-individual differences. In Drosophila, the availability of numerous isogenic strains and the ability to assay large numbers of individuals have been instrumental in elucidating key components generating inter-individual variation (Buchanan et al., 2015). When navigating in their environment, Drosophila display a turn bias, where individuals preferentially use same-direction turns, and the magnitude of this bias is modulated by genetic background, activity in the central complex, and exposure to environmental enrichment as well as social experiences (Ayroles et al., 2015; Buchanan et al., 2015; Akhund-Zade et al., 2019; Versace et al., 2020). These findings demonstrate that functional variation in the invertebrate nervous system is maintained by specific neural substrates and further modified by gene and environment interaction. In murine models, exploratory behavior is a thoroughly investigated example of inter-individual variation, where phenotype variation is enhanced by environmental enrichment and correlated changes in hippocampal neurogenesis (Freund et al., 2013; Körholz et al., 2018; Zocher et al., 2020). Despite this well-studied mammalian model and other known handed behaviors that suggest changes in neuron number or activity patterns may regulate inter-individual variation, the mechanisms instructing inter-individual differences remain poorly understood. Therefore, two prevailing questions are what neural substrates generate biases and what mechanisms instruct specific bias types, i.e., left versus right-handed or consistent versus inconsistent handedness.
Zebrafish have emerged as a powerful model for elucidating mechanisms that instruct visceral and neural differences between individuals (Gamse et al., 2003, 2005; Dreosti et al., 2014). Moreover, similar to other teleost species, zebrafish have a visual bias, preferentially using the left eye to assess novelty (Bisazza et al., 1997; De Santi et al., 2001; Sovrano, 2004; Andrew et al., 2009). However, this behavioral bias is primarily fixed in the population and offers little insight into inter-individual variation. Larval zebrafish also perform a light-search behavior that is onset by the loss of visual navigating cues, which drives a period of stereotypic leftward or rightward circling (Horstick et al., 2017), consistent with search patterns observed in other species (Bell et al., 1985; Hills et al., 2004, 2013; Gray et al., 2005). An individual’s leftward or rightward circling direction is persistent over at least multiple days, observed at equal proportions in the population, and is not heritable (Horstick et al., 2020). The features of light-search share many of the hallmark traits observed in well-established invertebrate models of turn bias that have been instrumental for characterizing mechanisms that regulate inter-individual variation (Ayroles et al., 2015; Buchanan et al., 2015; Akhund-Zade et al., 2019). Moreover, our work has shown that neurons in the habenula and rostral posterior tuberculum (PT) are essential for maintaining zebrafish turn bias (Horstick et al., 2020). Therefore, larval zebrafish is a potentially powerful vertebrate model to determine how inter-individual variation is imposed in the vertebrate brain. What remains lacking is a rigorous analysis of turn bias variation in the population and the identification of external and internal factors modulating inter-individual turn bias differences.
Here, we capitalize on the larval zebrafish turning bias to characterize environmental factors and signaling pathways that modulate inter-individual variation. Previous work identified a persistent left/right turn bias maintained by a habenula-rostral PT circuit and Notch associated signaling pathways (Horstick et al., 2020). However, locomotor features or factors instructing turn direction phenotypes was unexplored. Here we develop a multiplex recording pipeline and a new metric, bias ratio, which permit turn bias recording in a medium-throughput manner and rigorous unbiased analysis of inter-individual variation. Previous work used metrics that weighted behavior on a single trial to categorize turning type (Horstick et al., 2020), which these metrics are potent indicators of bias, yet can easily compound error over serial testing that is typically required to study probabilistic behavior like turning bias. Using our new testing pipeline, we first characterized turning types, finding previously described left and right turning types and a previously undescribed unbiased turning type in a wildtype strain. We further establish that turning types are distinguishable by unique path trajectory features. Second, we determined that temperature selectively impacts inter-individual variation and rostral PT neurons, establishing a tentative mechanism for temperature dependent regulation of inter-individual variation. Last, we investigate molecular pathways, demonstrating a direct role for Notch signaling using pharmacological inhibition. We establish levels of Notch inhibition that disrupts habenula development and bias, yet well-established Notch mechanisms such as neuronal proliferation or morphological development are unaffected. By testing a mutant associated with Notch signaling, gsx2, we implicate that brain regions beyond that previously described circuit could be important for developing variation in a vertebrate. This work develops zebrafish search behavior as a model for inter-individual variation and reveals how environmental and molecular cues impact specific neural substrates to generate distinct behavior types in a vertebrate.
Materials and Methods
Animal Husbandry
All experiments were approved by the West Virginia University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Zebrafish (Danio rerio) Tübingen long-fin (TL) wildtype strain was used in all experiments and used as the genetic background to maintain transgenic and mutant lines. Experiments were conducted during the first 7 days post fertilization (dpf), which is before sex determination. Larval rearing conditions were 28°C, 14/10 h light-dark cycle, in E3h media (5 mM NaCl, 0.17 mM KCl, 0.33 mM CaCl2, 0.33 mM MgSO4, and 1 mM HEPES, pH 7.3), and at a stocking density of 40 embryos per 30 mL E3h, unless stated otherwise. Social environment: To test the effect of social interaction, we raised larvae under two different social conditions: 20 larvae in a 6 cm petri dish or a single larva per 6cm dish. Social or isolation rearing started at 5–8 h post fertilization (hpf) and continued until testing at 6 dpf. Temperature: To test the impact of temperature on the development of turn bias, larvae were raised 1–4 dpf at either 24, 28, or 32°C. At 4 dpf, all groups were moved to 28°C until testing at 6 dpf. To determine if a specific development period was sensitive to elevated temperature, separate groups of larvae were raised at 32°C from either 31–55 hpf or 55–79 hpf, after which they were returned to standard rearing temperature and tested at 6 dpf. Salinity: The impact of increased salinity was tested over 4 salt concentrations (1, 2, and 5 ppt – parts per thousand) and standard E3h (∼0.5 ppt) as a control. Larvae were reared in variable salinities from 1 to 4 dpf, and behavior tested at 6 dpf. An elevated salinity stock of E3h was made by adding 9.5g NaCl (Sigma) to standard E3h, creating a 10 ppt stock, which was diluted for working concentrations with standard E3h media. Environmental enrichment: Enriched environments were created by adhering mixed size and color (predominately red, blue, gray, and white colors) LEGO® blocks onto the bottom of a 10 cm petri dish. Previously, LEGO® blocks have been used to stimulate novel object recognition and interaction in larval zebrafish (Bruzzone et al., 2020). In addition, 5–8 plastic aquarium leaves were included to float on the surface. Last, dishes were positioned on platforms with mixed white and black shape substrates. A total of four enriched environments were created with variable LEGO® colors and sized blocks, and larvae were rotated daily between enriched environments. As controls, larvae were raised in plain 10 cm dishes placed on either a solid white substrate. For experiments, larvae were maintained in enriched or control dishes from 1 dpf until behavior testing. Shaking: We tested the impact of environmental instability on motor bias by continuously shaking larvae from 1 to 4 dpf. At 1 dpf, embryos were placed in a 75 cm2 cell culture flask (Sigma) with approximately 80 mL E3h. Flasks were propped at 30 degrees on a Stovall Belly Dancer orbital rotator, set to 70 rpm. At 4 dpf, larvae were removed from culture dishes and raised under standard conditions prior to testing at 6–7 dpf.
Transgenic lines used were enhancer trap Tg(y279-Gal4) (Marquart et al., 2015) and Tg(UAS:Kaede)s1999t (Davison et al., 2007). Mutant line used was gsx2Δ13a (Coltogirone et al., 2021).
Behavior Tracking and Analysis
Behavioral experiments were performed on 6–7 dpf larvae, except as noted. All experiments were recorded using infrared illumination (940 nm, CMVision Supplies), a μEye IDS1545LM-M CMOS camera (1st Vision) fitted with a 12 mm lens, and a long-pass 780 nm filter (Thorlabs, MVL12WA and FGL780, respectively). Visible illumination was provided by a white light LED (Thorlabs) positioned above the larvae, adjusted to 40–50 μW/cm2 (International Light Technologies, ILT2400 Radiometer with SED033 detector). Testing conditions were maintained between 26 and 28°C for all behavioral recording, and all larvae adapted to the recording room conditions for 20 min before recording under matched illumination to recording rigs. Custom DAQtimer software was used to control lighting, camera recording parameters, and real-time tracking as previously described (Yokogawa et al., 2012; Horstick et al., 2017). The camera field of view was set to record four 10 cm dishes simultaneously with one larva per dish for multiplex recordings. A total of four recording rigs were used. Path trajectories of individual larvae are recorded over 30-s recording intervals at 10 fps and analyzed using five measures: net turn angle (NTA), total turning angle (TTA), match index (MI), bias ratio (BR), and performance index (PI) (see Table 1 for metric reference). A minimum of 100 points were required to be included in the analysis. NTA is the summation of leftward and rightward angular displacement (−leftward, +rightward) over the recording interval, whereas TTA is the sum of absolute values of all angular displacement. MI measures the proportion of events in a series going in the same direction. Leftward and rightward trials are scored as 0 or 1, and MI is the percent of events matching the direction of the first trial in a testing series. For example, a MI = 1 is all events are in the same direction as the first trial, whereas 0.33 is a third of the events matching the first trial. For MI analysis, individuals missing the first dark trial were excluded from analysis. BR is a proportion of directional turning compared to total turning, calculated by dividing NTA by TTA, e.g., −1 represents that all directional movement in a single trial occurred in a leftward direction, while −0.5 indicates that 50% of all turning was in a net leftward direction (e.g., −200 degrees NTA out of 400 TTA). PI was calculated by averaging binary bias ratios, with leftward trials scored as 0 and rightward 1. Where noted on figures, bias ratios were weighted by the proportion of larvae within a PI group in order to demonstrate changes in the number of larvae within a performance group. In all analyses that required a PI for categorizing larvae, all individuals that had missing trials were excluded. This criteria was necessary to ensure rigorous categorization. For gsx2 experiments, larvae were housed individually following behavior testing for post hoc genotyping. Genotyping was performed as previously described (Coltogirone et al., 2021). In brief, genotypes were confirmed using PCR spanning the deletion: gsx2 (primers: 5′TGCGTATCCTCACACATCCA, 5′TGTCCAGGGTGCGCTAAC; 134 bp wildtype, 121 bp mutant, and 134/121 bp heterozygous). Previous reports describe that gsx2 mutants have reduced swim bladder inflation (Coltogirone et al., 2021), which was minimized by raising larvae in shallow water dishes. Only larvae with normal swim bladder inflation and balance were used for experiments.
TABLE 1.
Behavior metrics | Name | Measure |
NTA | Net turn angle | Net sum of leftward and rightward angular movement |
TTA | Total turn angle | Absolute sum of all angular movement |
BR | Bias ratio | NTA/TTA ratio. Proportion of directional movement |
MI | Match index | Proportion of trials matching direction of first trial |
PI | Performance index | Average of binarized turn directions (0 = left; 1 = right) |
| ||
Behavior assays | Name | Measure |
| ||
4× | NA | Paired 30 s Light ON and OFF recordings, repeated 4× |
8× | NA | Paired 30 s Light ON and OFF recordings, repeated 8× |
q4× | Quad 4× | Four repeated ‘4×’ recordings separated by 10 min |
The 4× recording assay was performed by recording larval path trajectories over four recording intervals, each composed of 30 s baseline recordings, immediately followed by 30 s recording following the loss of visible illumination. Each recording interval was separated by 3 min of baseline illumination. The 8× recording was performed in a similar format, including four additional light ON/OFF recording intervals performed in series. The quad 4× (q4×) assay is identical to the 4×, except that the 4× recording interval is repeated four times, separated by 10 min baseline illumination (see Table 1 for assay reference). A 4× recording strategy was used to test the developmental onset of turn bias. Individual larvae were first tested at 3 dpf, and were separately raised in 6-well plates and retested daily through 6 dpf. For analysis, larvae were grouped as left or right biased based on BR (average BR+, right bias; −, left bias) at 6 dpf when turn bias is well-established (Horstick et al., 2020). To ensure rigorous categorization, larvae with ambiguous responses at 6 dpf (BR between −0.1 and 0.1) were removed.
Pharmacology
Notch signaling was inhibited using the Υ-secretase inhibitor LY411575 (Sigma, SML050). A 10 mM stock of LY411575 was prepared in DMSO and diluted to working concentrations with a final volume of 0.08–0.1% DMSO for all trials. To test Notch inhibition on turn bias, mid-gastrulation (6–8 hpf) groups of embryos were treated with 0.05, 0.1, 0.15, 0.2, 0.25, 1 or 10 μM LY411575 until 4 dpf; the drug was replaced daily. At 4 dpf, LY411575 was removed and larvae placed in fresh E3h until behavior testing at 6 dpf. Phenotypic categorization was performed at 3 dpf. Individuals were scored as normal (visually no abnormal tail curvature, edema, reduced/decreased swim bladder size, necrosis, or overt abnormal swimming), mild (abnormal touch responsiveness), moderate (tail curvature), or severe (gross developmental defects, necrosis). Only normal larvae were used for behavioral testing. Vehicle controls were 0.08–0.1% DMSO treated.
Labeling and Imaging
Immunohistochemistry
To assay neuronal proliferation, we labeled with anti-HuC/D (Elav protein) (Invitrogen A21271). Control (0.08% DMSO) and LY411575 groups (100 nM and 8 μM) were prepared as described above. At 24 hpf, embryos were fixed overnight using 4% paraformaldehyde in 1× PBS at 4°C. Washes were performed with 1× PBS containing 0.1% TritonX-100. We used primary antibody mouse anti-HuC/D (1:500, Invitrogen, 16A11). Secondary detection was performed with goat anti-mouse IgG1 Alexa 488 (1:500, Invitrogen, A32723). To analyze images, signal intensity of a 56 μm × 6 μm (W × H) region spanning a lateral to midline hemi-section of the anterior spinal cord was recorded using ImageJ. Three sections were measured per larva, averaged and standardized for comparison between groups.
Fluorescent in situ Hybridization
To determine the levels of Notch signaling we examined transcript levels of her12 (Jacobs and Huang, 2019). Hybridization chain reaction (Molecular Instruments) probes and labeling technology was used to detect her12 transcripts. Her12 mRNA sequence (NM_205619) was provided to Molecular Instruments to design a custom gene-specific her12 probe detection set. LY411575 and control larvae were treated as described above. At 30 hpf, larvae were fixed overnight using 4% paraformaldehyde in 1× PBS at 4°C. Fixed larvae were washed in 1× PBS containing 0.1% Tween20 and labeled following Molecular Instruments HCR RNA-Fish protocol for whole-mount zebrafish embryos (Schwarzkopf et al., 2021). All images were collected using the same parameters. For analysis, the percent area of her12 expression was quantified within the spinal cord using ImageJ.
Neuron Temperature Sensitivity
Rostral PT and habenula image stacks were captured and neurons counted in max projections using ImageJ. All imaging was performed using larvae from Tg(y279-Gal4)/Tg(UAS:kaede) carrier in-crosses. At 1 dpf, larvae were screened for Kaede and reared at elevated temperatures as described above. Larvae were moved to standard raising conditions at 4 dpf, and live-imaged at 6 dpf. Larvae were anesthetized using MS-222 (Sigma) and mounted in 2% low melting temp agar. To determine if a specific developmental time period was crucial, larvae were similarly prepared and analyzed, yet only raised at elevated temperature during either 31–55 hpf or 55–79 hpf intervals. Controls were raised at standard rearing temperatures.
Neuron Sensitivity to Notch Inhibition
Using Tg(y279-Gal4)/Tg(UAS:kaede) carrier in-crosses we performed LY411575 as described above, except treatments ended at 3 dpf when both the habenula and PT could be observed, while attempting to minimize severe morphological phenotypes and death at higher concentrations. We treated embryos at concentrations of 0.1 and 1 μM with a vehicle control. All treatments had a final DMSO concentration of 0.01%. Imaging the habenula and PT was performed as above and neuron counts performed using max projections in ImageJ. Counts were only performed on groups were habenula and PT neurons could be reliable identified. For counting neuron numbers, the larger habenula was classified as the ‘left’ habenula regardless of hemisphere. Habenula were classified as symmetric if the left to right neuron ratio was less than 2.
Imaging
All imaging was performed on an Olympus Fluoview FV1000. For live imaging, larvae were anesthetized in a low dose of MS222 (Sigma) and embedded in 2% low melting temp agar. Fixed samples were transferred into 70% glycerol/30% 1× PBS and slide-mounted for imaging.
Statistical Analysis
Analysis was performed in R R Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing, 2020), R ggplot2 package (Wickham, 2016) (R Core Team) and Prism (GraphPad). All statistical comparisons were two-sided, unless noted otherwise. Standard error of the mean (±SEM) was used for all experiments, except MAD analysis which display 95% confidence intervals. Cohen D was calculated in R using package effsize. For all experiments, data was collected from a minimum of three independent clutches. Normality was tested using the Shapiro–Wilks test. Normally distributed data was compared using either 1 or 2-way t-tests. Non-normal data was analyzed using a Mann–Whitney U test or Wilcoxon signed-rank test for 2 or 1-way tests, respectively. To perform multiple comparisons, ANOVAs were performed in GraphPad and multiple comparisons adjusted using a Bonferroni correction. Boxplots show median and quartiles with outliers identified beyond 2.7 standard deviations from the mean.
Permutation and bootstrapping was performed using “sample” R function without and with replacement, respectively. For permutation experiments, bias ratios values were randomized across all individuals in a dataset. Randomization was performed only within the same trial, e.g., reshuffling of bias ratios within the first light off trial. Permutations were simulated 1,000 times and average bias ratios and MAD values calculated using custom R code, and used to plot permutated probability density curves and MAD values. Probability density plots and area under the curve measurements were performed using custom R code. For area under the curve analysis, ±0.3 tails were chosen for comparison, which are approximately two standard deviations from the population average. To generate error bars for MAD analysis, average bias ratios were bootstrapped (1,000 bootstrap replicates) with resampling. For each resampled dataset a MAD was calculated and MAD values across all resampled datasets used to calculate a 95% confidence interval applied as an error bar. A 1-way comparison was used to calculate significance for all simulated dataset comparisons. To generate a p-value, the number of resampled dataset MAD values were totaled that fall within or exceed the 95% confidence interval of the comparison group, and this total was divided by 1,000 to produce a final p-value. This represents the fraction of simulated experimental groups that fall within a range that supports a null hypothesis of no difference between groups. For example, 600 bootstrapped datasets from a simulated control that fall within or exceed the confidence interval of an experimental group yields p = 0.60, implicating that 60% of simulated datasets do not support the statistical difference between compared groups. Direction of comparison is noted in the legend for each dataset.
Results
Turning Behavior During Light Search Shows High Inter-Individual Variation
We developed a multiplexed strategy to record path trajectories to assess inter-individual variation during larval zebrafish light search behavior (Figure 1A). Previously, the stereotypic turning was described using a large recording arena (14,400 mm2) to record single larva (Horstick et al., 2017). Larvae are recorded in 100 mm diameter dishes (7,854 mm2) for our multiplexed strategy, and robust circling is observed following light extinction (Figure 1B). To characterize individual motor biases, we initially recorded larval path trajectories over a series of four intervals of paired 30-s baseline illumination and 30 s following the loss of illumination, with each of these recording pairs separated by 3 min of illumination to restore baseline behavior (Horstick et al., 2020), which we refer to as 4× recording (Figure 1C). This recording yields four paired light on and off events per individual. We recorded responses from 374 individuals, representing 1,496 paired baseline and dark responses. The presence of motor bias was previously described using a match index (MI) – the percent of turning trials in which turning direction was the same as the first dark trial (Horstick et al., 2020). Here we confirm previous findings showing a significant MI increase following the loss of illumination (Wilcoxon matched-pairs test, p < 0.001), showing the number of individuals recorded can be upscaled via multiplexing (Supplementary Figure 1A). Overall, our current approach for multiplexed recording recapitulates previous findings. These data show that our multiplexed strategy provides medium-throughput recording, allowing a rigorous analysis of larval zebrafish inter-individual variation.
We calculated a bias ratio by dividing net turning angle (NTA) by total turning angle (TTA – absolute sum of all angular displacement) for each baseline, and dark trial recorded to examine the spectrum of wildtype larvae inter-individual variation during search behavior (Supplementary Figure 1B). This metric provides the proportion of same-direction turning within a continuous numerical range bounded by −1 and 1, representing all directional movement in a leftward or rightward direction, respectively. The average bias ratio across the entire population during baseline illumination and light-search did not significantly vary from zero showing no population bias [one-sample t-test against 0, baseline: t(373) = 0.007842, p = 0.9937; dark: t(373) = 0.1696, p = 0.89] (Supplementary Figure 1C). Despite similar population-level bias ratios between baseline and dark, significant variation is observed in the dark that is not observed during baseline (Figures 1D–F). Using a probability density curve, where the area under the curve represents the proportion of individuals in the population, we find that during dark turning, 12.38% of the population displayed a robust sustained turning bias over 4 trials (bias ratio <−0.7 = 6.41%, left bias; >0.7 = 5.97%, right bias) (Figure 1D). Conversely, 1.72% of baseline events displayed sustained directional turning (Figure 1E). The distribution of bias ratios shows that, following light extinction, a significantly greater number of individuals utilize sustained same-direction turning [χ2(1) = 51.02, p < 0.0001]. To determine whether these distributions were the product of chance, we simulated ‘randomized’ baseline and dark datasets by resampling bias ratios (1,000 resamples) within each trial (Figures D,E, dotted line). Following randomizing, 2.35% of the simulated dark responses maintained strong directional turning, similar to that observed during baseline. A previous study used mean absolute deviation (MAD) as a metric to quantify variation in a population; a higher MAD represents increased variation across individuals in the population (Buchanan et al., 2015). Here, MAD was calculated for baseline, dark, and simulated datasets. As MAD was generated from the whole population, average bias ratios were bootstrapped (1,000 boots) to generate 95% confidence intervals for statistical comparison. MAD is 44.10% (p < 0.001) and 15.79% (p < 0.001) reduced in baseline or in randomized dark groups compared to light-search dark responses, respectively (Figure 1F), whereas no difference was observed between baseline MAD and randomized baseline responses (Figure 1F, yellow bar). These findings show that turn bias during light search behavior shows significant variation beyond what is expected by chance or while larvae navigate in an illuminated environment.
Our analysis, along with findings from previous reports, illustrates robust left and right turners, or turning types, within the population. However, the distribution of bias ratios from 4× recordings shows that over 14% of the population exhibits an average bias ratio consistent with no sustained turn direction (−0.1 < BR < 0.1) (see Figure 1D). These individuals could represent either a stable unbiased population or endogenous behavioral fluctuation. To evaluate whether unbiased individuals are a sustained turning type in the population, in addition to left/right biased turners, we created a performance index (PI) by transforming all individual trials to either 0 or 1 for overall leftward or rightward preference per trial, respectively. From these binary values, we created a transition index for the first and second set of responses from the 4× dataset, i.e., left (LL = 0), right (RR = 1), or random (LR; RL = 0.5) responses that can be compared between the first and last response pairs. Using the transition pair PI, we assessed the frequency of turn direction change or conservation (Figures 1G,H). During dark trials, 36% of all transitions showed sustained turn direction (left = 17%, right = 19%; average PI = 0 or 1), whereas during baseline illumination 12% of larvae sustained turn direction [χ2(1) = 54.545, p < 0.0001]. Conversely, 21 and 35% of transitions yielded sustained random behavior between dark and baseline recording conditions, respectively (for example, LR to RL or RR to LL; average PI = 0.5) [χ2(1) = 8.615, p = 0.0033] (Figures 1G,H). Interestingly, during light-search initially random response pairs transitioned to directional (RR or LL) responses 22% of the time, yielding partial turn bias (average PI 0.75 or 0.25).
To confirm our observations persisted over longer timescales, we ran an additional 8× experiment, testing 189 larvae as before, with four additional light ON/OFF intervals in series. From this extended testing condition, we observed conserved trends demonstrating significant inter-individual variation in turning bias during light-search, yet not during baseline illumination (Supplementary Figures 1D–F). Neither 4× or 8× recording showed a change in TTA over time, establishing overall behavior is not disrupted by our assays (Supplementary Figures 1G,H). As 4× and 8× experiments were broadly consistent, we focused on the 4× recording strategy for ongoing investigations. Our data show that wildtype larvae exhibit significant inter-individual variation in turn bias during light-search, greater than that expected by chance, with a subset of individuals potentially exhibiting a previously unexplored unbiased turning type.
Multiple Stable Turning Types Exist With Distinct Locomotor Features
Characterizing changes in locomotor parameters in zebrafish has been a powerful strategy to develop etiological and mechanistic models (Burgess and Granato, 2007; Horstick et al., 2013; Chen and Engert, 2014; Dunn et al., 2016). Therefore, we next aimed to establish what underlying locomotor changes account for unbiased and biased motor types. We hypothesized that three possible modes could generate unbiased behavior: (1) normal turning with high rates of direction switching across trials, (2) reduced same-direction turning within single trials, or (3) weak photo-responsiveness and, therefore, low total turning. To differentiate between these hypotheses, we categorized all larvae based on average PI across all four trials, generating five categories. Across PI groups, we compared the absolute average bias ratio to determine if the magnitude of directional turning changed based on PI. During light search the average bias ratio magnitude significantly changed based on PI [1-way ANOVA F(4,352) = 10.43, p < 0.0001], where partial and unbiased PI groups showed less overall directional turning (Figure 2A). No difference was observed between strong left and right biased turners [PI = 0, 0.603 ± 0.022; PI = 1, 0.58 ± 0.021: t(352) = 0.7811 adjusted p > 0.9999]. Consistent with earlier observations, no differences were observed across PI groups during baseline [1-way ANOVA F(4,352) = 2.087, p = 0.082], consistent with an absence of turn individuality (Supplementary Figure 2A). Moreover, there was no significant change in TTA during dark turning [1-way ANOVA F(4,352) = 1.263, p = 0.28] across all PI groups (Supplementary Figure 2B). As all PI groups exhibited normal levels of total turning, this ruled out variable photo responsiveness as the basis of different turning types. Unexpectedly, partially biased populations (0.25, 0.75 PI) showed a similar average bias ratio as unbiased larvae (Figure 2A). To explain this observation, we reasoned that bias ratio magnitude might vary depending on whether an individual trial matches or opposes the overall larva turning type. For example, for 0.25 PI larvae, leftward matched direction bias ratios compared to rightward opposed direction trials. We analyzed all individual trials between all performance groups to explore this idea, sorting trials into matched or opposing based on the average PI for each individual. Perfect performance trials (0,1) were categorized as all matched, whereas unbiased trials (0.5) as all unmatched. Left and right direction bias ratios did not vary in these groups; therefore, we combined these groups to simplify comparison (Supplementary Figure 2C). A significant effect was observed across groups [1-way ANOVA F(3,1408) = 27.93, p < 0.0001], with trials opposed to overall PI direction showing lower overall bias ratio strength (Figure 2B, magenta lines). These data suggest that the basis of unbiased motor types is due to a lower bias ratio or less persistent same-direction turning, yet not a loss of overall turning. Interestingly, we noted that matched bias ratios were reduced in partially matched trials compared to events in the fully matched group [match 0.594 ± 0.013: partial match 0.514 ± 0.015: t(1408) = 4.046 adjusted p = 0.003] (black line), implicating that the underlying differences between biased and unbiased larvae may be graded.
In order to confirm rigorously the three motor types, we performed a quad 4× assay (q4×), using the standard 4× assay repeated four times, with each recording sequence separated by 10 min of baseline illumination (Supplementary Figure 2D). We recorded 114 larvae, and consistent with our previous measures, individuals showed significant inter-individual turn bias variation during light-search (±0.3 probability density tails: 7.34% dark; 0.00037%, randomized dark), and sustained left, right, or unbiased locomotor preferences (Supplementary Figure 2E). The cumulative summation of bias ratios provided a qualitative measure of turn performance over time (Figure 2C). From this analysis, we noted that some larvae initially categorized as strong or unbiased turners, seemingly switched over time. Therefore, we next aimed to utilize the q4× analysis to quantify bias determination accuracy by comparing the first 4× PI to overall q4× performance. We equally divided the 0 to 1 PI scale for classifying left, right, or unbiased behavior (left ≤ 0.33; unbiased 0.33 < 0.66; right ≥ 0.66) (Figure 2D). Of the larvae that show an initial strong or partial bias during the first 4× interval, 2/96 (2.08%) reverse bias direction during the q4× assay, and 27/96 (28.13%) of these individuals ultimately switch to an unbiased response after serial q4× testing. However, switching is primarily observed in larvae showing an initially partial bias, as the larvae that displayed an initially strong bias (0, 1 PI) in the q4× assay, 50/59 (84.75%) maintained a left or rightward turning type. Interestingly, at the population level, the 9/114 (7.89%) of unbiased individuals initially categorized with a strong bias was comparable to that expected by random chance, i.e., the same 6.25% likelihood of flipping 4 heads with a coin [χ2(1) = 0.609, p = 0.435]. As expected, classifying unbiased larvae was less accurate, yet a single 4× trial accurately represented 10/18 (55.56%) of individuals. Altogether, the q4× testing strategy confirms our earlier findings and demonstrates the veracity of our recording strategies to detect specific turning types.
As the q4× assay allowed for a rigorous confirmation of turning type, we next wanted to determine whether left, right, or unbiased turning types exhibited unique path trajectory characteristics. A PI was calculated from all 16 trials in the q4× assay for each individual and categorized as left, unbiased, or right type. For each turning type, we examined fractal dimension (F.D.) and displacement (displ) as measures of local search behavior (Tremblay et al., 2007; Horstick et al., 2017). Comparison across all three motor types yielded no differences in the tested motor parameters [main effect due to turn type 2-way ANOVA displ: F(2,222) = 0.42, p = 0.66; F.D: F(2,222) = 2.12, p = 0.12], yet the expected changes in behavior following light extinction were observed [main effect due to illumination 2-way ANOVA displ: F(1,222) = 604, p < 0.0001; F.D: F(1,222) = 643, p < 0.0001] (Figures 2E,F). Interestingly, upon closer inspection, we did notice a small yet significant change in F.D. between left and right turning groups during dark trials [left 1.240 ± 0.012; right 1.200 ± 0.014: t(222) = 2.974, adjusted p = 0.0489, effect size d = 0.63]. This effect was specific, and not observed during baseline [left F.D. 1.021 ± 0.003; right F.D. 1.021 ± 0.004: t(222) = 0.059, adjusted p > 0.9999] or for displacement. These results show that the difference of left and right turning type also generate mild changes to search pattern behavior, yet not motor trajectories during baseline movement.
Development of Inter-Individual Variation Is Sensitive to Specific Environmental Factors
Many instances of motor and behavioral biases show limited heritability (Collins, 1969; Buchanan et al., 2015; Linneweber et al., 2020). This observation suggests that inter-individual variation is, at least in part, modulated through individual experience with environmental factors. Indeed, previous studies show that social experience and environmental enrichment modify inter-individual variation of specific behaviors (Freund et al., 2015; Akhund-Zade et al., 2019; Versace et al., 2020; Zocher et al., 2020). As larval zebrafish turning bias is not heritable (Horstick et al., 2020), we reasoned that the environment might contribute to overall inter-individual variation or the generation of specific turning types. To test this hypothesis, we first established that turn bias appears at 4 dpf (Supplementary Figures 3A,B). Therefore, larvae were exposed to changes in the environment from 1 through either 4 or 7 dpf, dependent on the tested factor. The four parameters we screened were social experience, environmental enrichment, temperature, and salinity (Figure 3A). Social interaction and environmental enrichment were selected because each has been shown to modulate inter-individual variation (Akhund-Zade et al., 2019; Versace et al., 2020). For social interaction, larvae are raised in isolation or groups. For enrichment, we generated two environments: (1) an enriched environment where a petri dish was fitted with internal surfaces, diverse color, hiding spots, water surface cover, and dynamic substrate pattern, and (2) an empty petri dish with a uniform white bottom as a control. In addition, we also tested the impact of etiologically relevant temperature (24 or 32°C) and salinity [0.5–5 parts per thousand (ppt)] variations during early development compared to standard rearing parameters (Engeszer et al., 2007; Sundin et al., 2019). Thus, our parameters test factors that generated inter-individual variation in other models and abiotic environmental fluctuations that larvae could encounter in a native habitat.
To determine if any of the tested parameters altered turning type development or magnitude of inter-individual variation, we looked at the average population bias ratio and MAD, respectively (Figures 3B,C) (Supplementary Figure 3C). Interestingly, the elevated temperature during early development caused a significant population shift from random [high temp 0.094 ± 0.044: one-sample t-test against 0, t(112) = 2.157, p = 0.033], implicating a population-level rightward bias, whereas no significant changes were observed in other temperature conditions or any other tested environmental parameter (Figure 3B). Conversely, the magnitude of turn bias variation during light-search was only reduced by low-temperature rearing, yet unaffected by other testing conditions (Figure 3C). To confirm the observed temperature-dependent changes, we examined the bias ratio per PI, weighted for the number of individuals per PI group. We observed that temperature imposed a significant effect on turn bias persistence [main effect of temperature 2-way ANOVA F(2,364) = 9.275, p = 0.0001] (Figure 3D). Indeed, the tested high temperature resulted in a significant depression of leftward turning [within PI group comparison t(364) = 3.031, adjusted p = 0.0078; red line] and increase in rightward turning [0.75 PI t(364) = 2.904, adjusted p = 0.012; red line]. Conversely, low temperature depressed turn bias performance in the population (Figure 3D, blue lines). These results suggest a specific temperature-mediated change. However, in larval zebrafish, fluctuating temperature is a stressor, and elevation of stress signaling has been shown to attenuate visual bias in chickens (Rogers and Deng, 2005; Long et al., 2012; Haesemeyer et al., 2018). Therefore, we tested the effect of shaking on turn bias which is a potent stressor for larval zebrafish (Eto et al., 2014; Castillo-Ramírez et al., 2019; Apaydin et al., 2020). Sustained shaking during early development resulted in no population or turn bias magnitude changes (Figures 3E–G). Moreover, external temperature impacts the rate of zebrafish development, and based on previous studies, our conditions would lead to an estimated ±13 h shift in development (Kimmel et al., 1995). We show that our temperature assay results in a change in hatching, a developmental marker, yet no gross changes in morphology or survival (Supplementary Figures 3D–F). These data illustrate that etiologically relevant temperature fluctuations differentially and specifically affect inter-individual turn bias variation.
Elevated Temperature Impacts Rostral Posterior Tuberculum Specification
A basic circuit involving the rostral posterior tuberculum (PT) and dorsal habenula (dHb) neurons has previously been described for zebrafish turn bias (Horstick et al., 2020). However, in wildtype larvae, no hemispheric differences in these neurons were found to account for left or right turning preference (Horstick et al., 2020). Because we found that elevated temperature disrupted left and right turning balance, we next wanted to determine if elevated temperature caused changes in neurons necessary for turn bias. We reasoned our environmental variables could alter neuronal development, as bias maintaining PT neurons are present as early as 2 dpf (Horstick et al., 2020), and dHb differentiation begins on 1 dpf (Gamse et al., 2003; Amo et al., 2010). First, we wanted to identify if a specific period during early development was sensitive to increased temperature. We found that elevated temperature during either 31–55 hpf or 55–79 hpf intervals did not recapitulate the population shift observed during the 1–4 dpf exposure (Supplementary Figure 4); therefore, we selected the full testing duration for further investigation. To visualize key dHb and PT neurons, we used the enhancer trap line y279:Gal4, which labels both populations of neurons (Horstick et al., 2020) (Figure 4A). In zebrafish, the left dHb is considerably larger than the right dHb (Gamse et al., 2003; Roussigné et al., 2009). We found that elevated temperature did not alter the habenula, and typical left/right asymmetry was observed [2-way ANOVA: interaction between temperature and hemisphere F(1,56) = 0.070, p = 0.79; effect of hemisphere F(1,56) = 101.2, p < 0.0001) (Figures 4B,E]. No hemispheric differences [main effect of hemisphere 2-way ANOVA F(1,56) = 0.493, p = 0.49] were observed in the number of y279 positive PT neurons (Figure 4C). Therefore, we combined PT measures from both hemispheres. Interestingly, from these combined pools, y279 positive neurons in the PT were reduced after exposure to elevated etiological temperature during early development [high temperature 17.64 ± 0.885; normal temperature 27.59 ± 1.172: 2-tail t-test t(58) = 6.625, p < 0.0001] (Figures 4D,F and Supplementary Figures 4C–F), establishing a potential neuronal basis for how high temperature during development modifies turn bias inter-individual variation.
Motor Individuality Is Sensitive to Gene Signaling Associated With Neuronal Proliferation
Studies from C. elegans (Bertrand et al., 2011) and Drosophila (Linneweber et al., 2020) demonstrate that Notch signaling can generate functional asymmetries in the brain that drive unique individual behavioral responses. Established zebrafish mutant lines mindbomb (mib) and mosaic eyes (moe), E3 ubiquitin ligase and Epb41l5 adapter, respectively, do not directly disrupt the Notch cascade, yet impair Notch signaling (Itoh et al., 2003; Ohata et al., 2011; Matsuda et al., 2016). Indeed, haploinsufficiency in these lines abrogates zebrafish turn bias, suggesting sensitivity to the levels of Notch signaling (Horstick et al., 2020). One of the canonical roles of Notch during early development is the regulation of neuronal proliferation (Appel et al., 2001; Mizutani et al., 2007; Yoon et al., 2008). Therefore, we next aimed to elucidate if turn bias is (1) sensitive to direct Notch antagonism in a dose-dependent manner and (2) if partial Notch inhibition impairs neuronal proliferation.
To disrupt Notch signaling, we used the specific Υ-secretase inhibitor LY411575, which blocks the activation of the Notch signaling cascade (Geling et al., 2002; Fauq et al., 2007). Previous reports show that treatment with micromolar concentrations of LY411575 starting at mid-gastrulation results in a near-total loss of Notch signaling, which largely recapitulates the mindbomb mutant (Jacobs and Huang, 2019; Sharma et al., 2019). Therefore, we used 10 μM as a maximum dose and positive control for inhibitor efficacy across trials. To identify a level of Notch inhibition that could impair turning bias, we LY411575-treated larvae from mid-gastrulation to 4 dpf over 7 concentrations ranging from 50 nM to 10 μM and scored phenotypes at 3 dpf (Figure 5A). Developmental exposure of LY411575 up to 100 nM left most larvae morphologically normal, which we used as a maximum dose to test the impact on turn bias. Notch inhibition resulted in a significant change in TTA following the loss of illumination [1-way ANOVA F(2,152) = 4.614, p = 0.011], causing an increase in overall turning at 100 nM inhibitor treatment compared to controls [vehicle 1175.95 ± 53.34, 100 nM 1411.39 ± 66.50: t(152) = 2.786, adjusted p = 0.018] (Supplementary Figure 5). Whereas turn bias performance was reduced by Notch inhibition [main effect due to treatment 2-way ANOVA F(2,144) = 8.995, p = 0.0002], with 100 nM inhibitor nullifying bias ratio strength differences due to PI, which was not observed at lower inhibitor concentrations (Figures 5B,C). In addition, 100 nM but not 50 nM treatment reduced overall inter-individual turn bias variation in the population (Figure 5D). This data suggests that a critical threshold of Notch signaling is required for generating variation in turn bias and overall performance, which is lower than levels necessary for normal gross morphological development. To identify a potential neuronal basis for the loss of bias following Notch inhibition, we LY411575-treated y279:Gal4 embryos to quantify transgene positive dHb and rostral PT neurons, focusing on the inhibitor concentration that specifically impairs behavior yet not morphological development. Interestingly, we found that the levels of inhibition that abrogate bias also disrupts typical dHb hemispheric asymmetry, producing an increase in reversed and symmetric habenular phenotypes (Figure 5E). Similarly, we observed an increase in the smaller ‘right’ dHb neuron number [vehicle 4.25 ± 1.21, 100 nM 10.22 ± 1.64: t(15) = 2.870, p = 0.012], consistent with increased habenular symmetry (Figures 5F,H,I). Conversely, the rostral PT was unaffected (Figures 5G–I).
To confirm that LY411575 exposure impaired Notch signaling, we examined her12 expression, a downstream target of the Notch signaling cascade that is robustly expressed in the spinal cord, providing an unambiguous region to quantify expression changes (Jacobs and Huang, 2019). Exposure to micromolar inhibitor concentrations resulted in a near-total absence of her12 expression, consistent with previous reports (Jacobs and Huang, 2019). The her12 expression was, however, observed in the spinal cord of the 100 nM group at an intensity indistinguishable from controls (Figures 5E,L).
A canonical and conserved role for Notch during early development is regulating neuronal proliferation and maintaining progenitor pools, and the loss of Notch leads to increased proliferation (Appel et al., 2001; Cheng et al., 2004; Sharma et al., 2019). Therefore, we next wanted to determine whether the level of Notch inhibition that impairs turn bias individuality also disrupts proliferation. During zebrafish embryonic development, proliferative neurons are readily visualized in the anterior hindbrain using Elav (HuC/D) protein expression as a marker (Kim et al., 1996; Sharma et al., 2019). These proliferative neuron pools expand following high levels of Notch inhibition or in the mindbomb mutant background (Itoh et al., 2003; Sharma et al., 2019). Consistent to our observation with her12 and PT neuron counts, partial pharmacological Notch inhibition (100 nM drug) induced no change in actively proliferating neurons, yet positive controls (8 μM) displayed robust expansion of proliferating neurons (Figures 5K,M).
Notch signaling is ubiquitous in the larval zebrafish nervous system (Tallafuss et al., 2009; Banote et al., 2016; Kumar et al., 2017), and pharmacological inhibition is not specific. Consequentially, we next aimed to determine whether proliferative pathways in restricted areas of the brain may also contribute to turn bias. Genomic screen homeobox transcription factors (Gsx1 and 2, formerly Gsh1 and 2) are affecters of the Notch signaling pathway in mouse, and Gsx2 maintains neural progenitor pools in the developing telencephalon (Wang et al., 2009; Pei et al., 2011; Roychoudhury et al., 2020). In larval zebrafish, gsx2 is predominantly expressed in the pallium, preoptic area, hypothalamus, and hindbrain, with an established putative null TALEN deletion mutant line (Coltogirone et al., 2021). As gsx2 mutants show no gross morphological abnormalities during larval stages, we used these lines to test turn bias. Heterozygous and mutant gsx2 larvae displayed reduced inter-individual variation and a shift toward less persistent turn bias (Figure 6A). The loss of persistent same-direction turning was similarly observed using match index (MI), an analogous metric (Figure 6B). Yet, TTA during light-search was not significantly changed across genotypes [1-way ANOVA F(2,187) = 2.730, p = 0.068], suggesting the loss of same-direction turning is not due to reduced light-driven behavior (Figure 6C). Thus, our analysis implies that broad and local haploinsufficient changes in Notch signaling and Gsx2 contribute to inter-individual variation in turn bias behavior, independent of canonical roles in proliferation.
Discussion
Here we reveal that during light-search initiated by the loss of illumination, larval zebrafish exhibit significant inter-individual variation in turn bias, a handed-like behavior. Based on our newly developed assays, we were further able to show mild changes in search behavior correlated with left and right turning types. However, the impact of turning on search motor patterns was specific, as we found no evidence of individual motor changes during baseline illumination, consistent with previous studies (Horstick et al., 2020). We demonstrated a turn bias spectrum across the population which shows the previously described left/right turning types (Horstick et al., 2020). In addition, our analysis revealed a consistently unbiased turning type, supported by multiple independent recording strategies (4×, 8×, and q4×). Furthermore, we show that temperature changes during early development result in sustained changes in inter-individual variation. Finally, we tested how signaling pathways associated with neuronal proliferation affected turn bias development, using either pharmacological inhibition of Notch signaling or a presumable null Gsx2 mutant. Notch and Gsx2 represent canonical broad and regional regulators of proliferation, respectively. Interestingly, turn bias attenuation is observed with partial Notch inhibition and in gsx2 heterozygotes, suggesting dose-dependent sensitivity. Despite a well-established role for Notch in cell proliferation, the inhibitor concentrations that selectively impairs turn bias did not result in observable changes in proliferation, at least early in development (see Figure 5). Our findings confirm that three turning types can be categorically defined, are modulated by specific etiological relevant environmental cues, and are sensitive to internal proliferative associated signaling pathways. One potential caveat is that zebrafish strains are not isogenic and maintain some genetic heterogeneity (Butler et al., 2015), potentially contributing to inter-individual differences. Nevertheless, our work develops larval zebrafish as a powerful model to identify mechanisms generating inter-individual variation in vertebrates.
Determination of Bias
Our findings suggest a ‘hemispheric noise’ model where turn bias and inter-individual variation is modulated by conflicting brain hemisphere signals in turn bias driving neurons (Figure 7). We elucidated that change in bias ratios strength distinguishes unbiased versus biased larvae. Moreover, we establish that this change is not a result of a loss of photo-responsiveness in unbiased individuals (total turning, see Supplementary Figure 1); rather a failure to navigate in a single direction during light-search consistently. This observation supports the conclusion that unbiased individuals are not a subset with impaired photo-responsiveness, but a distinct behavioral motor profile during search behavior. Supporting this model, when we quantify the strength of individual trials, the bias ratios exhibit a step-wise decrease, i.e., PI 1 < 0.75 < 0.5, suggesting accumulating inter-hemispheric noise that degrades overall individual bias persistence. Corroborating this model, previous studies showing that unilateral ablation of rostral PT neurons, which are required for turn bias in larval zebrafish, increases turning strength in the direction ipsilateral to the intact neurons, indicating ablation removes conflicting input from the contralateral hemisphere (Horstick et al., 2020). In pigeons, a classic model for hemispheric specialization and individual variation (Güntürkün et al., 1998; Freund et al., 2016), increased conflict between hemispheres exacerbates visual task latency (Manns and Römling, 2012). Therefore, variable balance in hemispheric signaling may be a conserved mechanism in generating inter-individual variation (Chen-Bee and Frostig, 1996; Linneweber et al., 2020). Inter-hemispheric communication is vital for the function of the visual system (Bui Quoc et al., 2012; Chaumillon et al., 2018), including photo-driven behavior in larval zebrafish (Gebhardt et al., 2019). The counter hypothesis is a ‘switching model’ where unbiased larvae would display vigorous directional turning, yet in randomly selected directions over sequential trials. This model is consistent with a ‘winner take all’ circuit function (Fernandes et al., 2021). Indeed, within the primary visual processing center in zebrafish, the optic tectum, neurons operate in a winner take all style during visually guided behavior (Fernandes et al., 2021). However, turn bias is driven by the loss of visual cues that activate rostral PT neurons, which do not project to the tectum (Horstick et al., 2020), implicating that even though turn bias is visually evoked, the mechanism is likely independent of a tectal winner take all mechanism. Despite the neurons maintaining zebrafish turn bias being identified, the underlying mechanism imposing a specific turning type remains unknown (Horstick et al., 2020). Our analysis suggests a model of competitive inter-hemispheric communication modulating the magnitude of inter-individual turn bias variation that is further adjusted by fluctuating and specific variables in the internal and external environment.
Regulation of Individuality
The mechanisms driving unique individual behavioral responses based on sex or sensory context are well described (Asahina et al., 2014; Lewis et al., 2015; Yapici et al., 2016; Marquart et al., 2019; Ishii et al., 2020; Nelson et al., 2020). However, why individuals in a population will show variable response types to a consistent stimulus is poorly understood, especially in vertebrates. One of our goals was to determine what internal and external elements may modulate turn bias variation as a basis to explain how different response types potentially arise. To test external environmental factors, we selected environmental enrichment, social experience, temperature, and salinity. One hypothesis for how factors like enrichment or social experience influence inter-individual variation is through micro-environmental interactions that create unique individual experiences (Kain et al., 2015). However, our data indicate that these interactions do not influence turn bias variation in zebrafish. One possible explanation is that during early development, 1–3 days post-fertilization, larvae are primarily inactive and only begin actively exploring around 4 days post fertilization (Colwill and Creton, 2011; Lambert et al., 2012). Conversely, responsiveness to conspecifics is not observed until 3 weeks (Dreosti et al., 2015; Larsch and Baier, 2018). As bias is established by 4 dpf, the underlying mechanisms may no longer be malleable to environmental experiences beyond this developmental interval.
We also tested temperature and salinity, emphasizing etiological ranges that zebrafish could experience in their native environments (Engeszer et al., 2007; Sundin et al., 2019). Salinity and temperature are critical environmental determinants and have been shown to drive evolutionary changes in stickleback populations (Gibbons et al., 2017). However, we found that only raising larvae at varying temperatures resulted in modifications to inter-individual variation. We show that temperature-dependent effects are not a generic thermal stress response. Etiological increases in temperature have been shown to attenuate turn bias in adult reef fish, implicating a potentially broader thermal sensitivity in bias establishing mechanisms (Domenici et al., 2014). Our analysis establishes that early developmental exposure to etiological temperature fluctuation results in sustained and specific turn bias changes.
Intriguingly, the specification of habenular hemispheric asymmetry is sensitive to the rate of development (Aizawa et al., 2007), and developmental rate is temperature sensitive (Kimmel et al., 1995). This observation could provide a potential mechanism for thermal driven changes in turn bias. However, our testing conditions produced no gross change in the habenula nuclei morphology. This observation, however, does not exclude functional or subcellular changes. Surprisingly, we observe a bilateral reduction in Tg(y279) positive PT neurons, which are essential for maintaining turn bias, in the elevated temperature experiments. A primary function of the PT is to integrate diverse sensory inputs (Striedter, 1991; Derjean et al., 2010; Yaeger et al., 2014). However, thermosensitivity of the PT neurons has not been previously described, and we believe this is a novel observation. Future studies identifying the genetic basis of the Tg(y279) enhancer trap, which is currently unknown, will be instrumental in elucidating how temperature impacts PT neuron specification and inter-individual variation. The specific abrogation of leftward turning types in increased temperature conditions provides a powerful model to interrogate underlying neural changes in a vertebrate brain associated with individual behavioral patterns.
Last, we wanted to identify molecular signaling pathways regulating turn bias. Biased turning in larvae is largely lost in heterozygotes of mutant lines associated with Notch signaling, yet the impact of direct Notch inhibition was unexplored (Horstick et al., 2020). In Drosophila and C. elegans, Notch signaling is essential for establishing individual visual navigational strategies and asymmetric chemosensory neuron identities, respectively (Bertrand et al., 2011; Linneweber et al., 2020). Thus, work from several species implicates Notch as a driver of variation at behavioral and neuronal levels. Indeed, we show that partial Notch disruption, using a specific pharmacological inhibitor, disrupts biased turning in larval zebrafish, yet not the ability to respond to illumination changes, establishing a direct role of Notch signaling for turn bias, which is independent of gross morphological development. Despite the established role of Notch in neural proliferation, we found no significant change in proliferative neurons, her12 expression, or number of PT neurons at the dosages used for behavioral studies. However, we did observe disruption to the typical left/right hemispheric asymmetry of the habenula, observing an increase in reversed or symmetric habenula. Interestingly, similar disruption to habenula symmetry is observed in mib zebrafish which have severely reduced Notch signaling (Aizawa et al., 2007). Our results, show a novel Notch dose-sensitivity for habenula asymmetry development, which may be a potential neural basis for the absence of biased behavior following low levels of Notch inhibition. Since Notch signaling is essential for diverse cellular functions, and the precise downstream signaling mechanisms are highly sensitive to the strength of Notch signaling (De Smedt et al., 2005; Shen et al., 2021), the low inhibitor concentrations used may be sub-threshold for disturbing the spatial-temporal patterns of her12 and HuC/D tested here. In addition, the downstream effects of Notch are dependent on the cellular micro-environments, determined by the local co-expression of Notch receptors, ligands, and auxiliary proteins (Demehri et al., 2009; Bertrand et al., 2011; LaFoya et al., 2016). Therefore, the levels of Notch reduction that impair turn bias, but not morphology, may not be sufficient to alter Notch associated mechanisms impacting proliferation. Nevertheless, subtle changes in Notch could lead to changes in cellular micro-environments, thereby altering downstream signaling cascades, and ultimately impacting turn bias maintaining neurons. Notch haploinsufficiency is known to generate a myriad of defects and disease states, including vasculature defects, seizure, autism, and brain malformations, demonstrating that reduced Notch signaling can disrupt biological functions (Krebs et al., 2004; Connor et al., 2016; Fischer-Zirnsak et al., 2019; Blackwood et al., 2020). However, the pharmacological inhibition used in our current study is not regionally specific. Therefore, we also tested an established zebrafish gsx2 mutant line, and gsx2 is predominately expressed in subsets of hypothalamic, preoptic area, pallium, and hindbrain neurons (Coltogirone et al., 2021). The reduction in turn bias in gsx2 heterozygotes and mutants suggests that turn bias variation is sensitive to local changes in brain regions where gsx2 is expressed, independent of the previously described rostral PT and habenula (Horstick et al., 2020). As the levels of Notch that reduce turn bias do not impact proliferation, it seems possible that Notch and Gsx2 modulate turn bias by independent mechanisms. Our current analysis identifies two conserved molecular signaling and transcriptional control mechanisms, Notch and Gsx2, and novel neuroanatomical substrates as important for generating variation in turn bias.
Function of Turn Bias and Inter-Individual Variation
Behavioral variation is observed in diverse species and behavioral modalities (Byrne et al., 2004; Elnitsky and Claussen, 2006; Cauchard et al., 2013; Horváth et al., 2020). In zebrafish, even complex neuromodulatory processes such as startle habituation display inter-individual variation with distinguishable ‘habituation types’ (Pantoja et al., 2016, 2020). Yet, the general question remains, “why do specific behavioral modalities manifest inter-individual differences?” Considering a simple form of inter-individual variation, such as turn bias, may offer insights to these questions. Zebrafish are active hunters during larval stages and predatory success depends on visual input, thus establishing a potent drive to remain in illuminated areas (Gahtan et al., 2005; Filosa et al., 2016; Muto et al., 2017). Following the loss of light and of overt navigation cues, larvae initiate a local light-search, where individual turn bias is triggered, causing looping trajectories (Horstick et al., 2017). Looping search trajectories are observed in various species in the absence of clear navigational cues, suggesting an efficient systematic strategy (Collins et al., 1994; Conradt et al., 2000; Zadicario et al., 2005). However, even seemingly optimal behaviors may not be advantageous in all contexts (Simons, 2011). Variation in turning types may ensure individuals across the population possess strategies to mitigate erratic environmental challenges, a form of bet-hedging (Simons, 2011; Kain et al., 2015). Similarly, behavioral variation adds unpredictability to a population. Predictable behavioral patterns can be exploited by predators (Catania, 2009, 2010). For aquatic species, this may be advantageous as some heron species, a predator of small fish, use a canopy hunting strategy, covering the water surface with their wings and blocking light (Kushlan, 1976). Prey populations with unpredictable responses would potentially provide a more challenging target (Humphries and Driver, 1970). Even though larval fish may not be the intended target of heron canopy hunting, larval behavioral patterns may persist over their lifespan. Indeed, adult zebrafish display a persistent turn direction preference (Fontana et al., 2019), although the correlation to larval turn bias is currently unexplored. Ultimately, the etiological purpose for turn bias variation is most likely a combination of multiple explanations, including bet-hedging, generating unpredictability, and genetically encoded sources of variation.
Data Availability Statement
Inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author for R scripts and datasets generated in this study.
Ethics Statement
The animal study was reviewed and approved by West Virginia University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Author Contributions
EH conceived the experiments. EH and JH wrote the manuscript and analyzed data. SL wrote custom R scripts for data analysis. SB provided gsx2 lines and associated analysis. JH, MW, JS, HC, HP, LB, and CS performed experiments. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
We thank Alexandra Schmidt and Rebecca Coltogirone from the Bergeron lab for help with imaging and fluorescent in situ and Erik Duboué and Andrew Dacks for helpful comments on the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by West Virginia University and Department of Biology startup funds to EH and SB, Research and Scholarship Advancement award to EH, and Program to Stimulate Competitive Research funds provided to EH. MW was supported by the Ruby Distinguished Doctoral Fellowship. HP was supported by NICHD R15HD101974 award to SB. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2021.777778/full#supplementary-material
References
- Aizawa H., Goto M., Sato T., Okamoto H. (2007). Temporally Regulated Asymmetric Neurogenesis Causes Left-Right Difference in the Zebrafish Habenular Structures. Dev. Cell 12 87–98. 10.1016/j.devcel.2006.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akhund-Zade J., Ho S., O’Leary C., de Bivort B. (2019). The effect of environmental enrichment on behavioral variability depends on genotype, behavior, and type of enrichment. J. Exp. Biol. 222(Pt 19):jeb202234. 10.1242/jeb.202234 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amo R., Aizawa H., Takahoko M., Kobayashi M., Takahashi R., Aoki T., et al. (2010). Identification of the Zebrafish ventral habenula as a homolog of the mammalian lateral habenula. J. Neurosci. 30 1566–1574. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3690-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrew R. J., Dharmaretnam M., Gyori B., Miklósi A., Watkins J. A. S., Sovrano V. A. (2009). Precise endogenous control of involvement of right and left visual structures in assessment by zebrafish. Behav. Brain Res. 196 99–105. 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.07.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apaydin D. C., Jaramillo P. A. M., Corradi L., Cosco F., Rathjen F. G., Kammertoens T., et al. (2020). Early-Life stress regulates cardiac development through an IL-4-glucocorticoid signaling balance. Cell Rep. 33:108404. 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Appel B., Givan L. A., Eisen J. S. (2001). Delta-Notch signaling and lateral inhibition in zebrafish spinal cord development. BMC Dev. Biol. 1:13. 10.1186/1471-213X-1-13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asahina K., Watanabe K., Duistermars B. J., Hoopfer E., González C. R., Eyjólfsdóttir E. A., et al. (2014). Tachykinin-expressing neurons control male-specific aggressive arousal in drosophila. Cell 156 221–235. 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ayroles J. F., Buchanan S. M., O’Leary C., Skutt-Kakaria K., Grenier J. K., Clark A. G., et al. (2015). Behavioral idiosyncrasy reveals genetic control of phenotypic variability. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112 6706–6711. 10.1073/pnas.1503830112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banote R. K., Edling M., Eliassen F., Kettunen P., Zetterberg H., Abramsson A. (2016). β-Amyloid precursor protein-b is essential for Mauthner cell development in the zebrafish in a Notch-dependent manner. Dev. Biol. 413 26–38. 10.1016/j.ydbio.2016.03.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell W. J., Cathy T., Roggero R. J., Kipp L. R., Tobin T. R. (1985). Sucrose-stimulated searching behaviour of Drosophila melanogaster in a uniform habitat: modulation by period of deprivation. Anim. Behav. 33 436–448. 10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80068-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bertrand V., Bisso P., Poole R. J., Hobert O. (2011). Notch-dependent induction of left/right asymmetry in C. elegans interneurons and motoneurons. Curr. Biol. 21 1225–1231. 10.1016/j.cub.2011.06.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bierbach D., Laskowski K. L., Wolf M. (2017). Behavioural individuality in clonal fish arises despite near-identical rearing conditions. Nat. Commun. 8:15361. 10.1038/ncomms15361 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bisazza A., Pignatti R., Vallortigara G. (1997). Detour tests reveal task- and stimulus-specific behavioural lateralization in mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki). Behav. Brain Res. 89 237–242. 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00061-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackwood C. A., Bailetti A., Nandi S., Gridley T., Hébert J. M. (2020). Notch Dosage: jagged1 haploinsufficiency is associated with reduced neuronal division and disruption of periglomerular interneurons in mice. Front. Cell Dev. Biol. 8:113. 10.3389/fcell.2020.00113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown C., Magat M. (2011). The evolution of lateralized foot use in parrots: a phylogenetic approach. Behav. Ecol. 22 1201–1208. 10.1093/beheco/arr114 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bruzzone M., Gatto E., Lucon Xiccato T., Dalla Valle L., Fontana C. M., Meneghetti G., et al. (2020). Measuring recognition memory in zebrafish larvae: issues and limitations. PeerJ 8:e8890. 10.7717/peerj.8890 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buchanan S. M., Kain J. S., de Bivort B. L. (2015). Neuronal control of locomotor handedness in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112 6700–6705. 10.1073/pnas.1500804112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bui Quoc E., Ribot J., Quenech’Du N., Doutremer S., Lebas N., Grantyn A., et al. (2012). Asymmetrical interhemispheric connections develop in cat visual cortex after early unilateral convergent strabismus: anatomy, physiology, and mechanisms. Front. Neuroanat. 5:68. 10.3389/fnana.2011.00068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bulman-Fleming M. B., Bryden M. P., Rogers T. T. (1997). Mouse paw preference: effects of variations in testing protocol. Behav. Brain Res. 86 79–87. 10.1016/s0166-4328(96)02249-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess H. A., Granato M. (2007). Sensorimotor gating in larval zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 27 4984–4994. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0615-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler M. G., Iben J. R., Marsden K. C., Epstein J. A., Granato M., Weinstein B. M. (2015). SNPfisher: tools for probing genetic variation in laboratory-reared zebrafish. Development 142 1542–1552. 10.1242/dev.118786 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne R. A., Kuba M. J., Meisel D. V. (2004). Lateralized eye use in Octopus vulgaris shows antisymmetrical distribution. Anim. Behav. 68 1107–1114. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.027 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Casey M. B., Karpinski S. (1999). The development of postnatal turning bias is influenced by prenatal visual experience in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Psychol. Rec. 49 67–74. 10.1007/BF03395307 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Castillo-Ramírez L. A., Ryu S., De Marco R. J. (2019). Active behaviour during early development shapes glucocorticoid reactivity. Sci. Rep. 9:12796. 10.1038/s41598-019-49388-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catania K. C. (2009). Tentacled snakes turn C-starts to their advantage and predict future prey behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106 11183–11187. 10.1073/pnas.0905183106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catania K. C. (2010). Born knowing: tentacled snakes innately predict future prey behavior. PLoS One 5:e10953. 10.1371/journal.pone.0010953 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cauchard L., Boogert N. J., Lefebvre L., Dubois F., Doligez B. (2013). Problem-solving performance is correlated with reproductive success in a wild bird population. Anim. Behav. 85 19–26. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chaumillon R., Blouin J., Guillaume A. (2018). Interhemispheric transfer time asymmetry of visual information depends on eye dominance: an electrophysiological study. Front. Neurosci. 12:72. 10.3389/fnins.2018.00072 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen X., Engert F. (2014). Navigational strategies underlying phototaxis in larval zebrafish. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:39. 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen-Bee C. H., Frostig R. D. (1996). Variability and interhemispheric asymmetry of single-whisker functional representations in rat barrel cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 76 884–894. 10.1152/jn.1996.76.2.884 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng Y.-C., Amoyel M., Qiu X., Jiang Y.-J., Xu Q., Wilkinson D. G. (2004). Notch activation regulates the segregation and differentiation of rhombomere boundary cells in the zebrafish hindbrain. Dev. Cell 6 539–550. 10.1016/s1534-5807(04)00097-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chu O., Abeare C. A., Bondy M. A. (2012). Inconsistent vs consistent right-handers’ performance on an episodic memory task: evidence from the California Verbal Learning Test. Laterality 17 306–317. 10.1080/1357650X.2011.568490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins R. D., Gargesh R. N., Maltby A. D., Roggero R. J., Tourtellot M. K., Bell W. J. (1994). Innate control of local search behaviour in the house fly, Musca domestica. Physiol. Entomol. 19 165–172. 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1994.tb01039.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Collins R. L. (1969). On the inheritance of handedness: II. Selection for sinistrality in mice. J. Heredity 60 117–119. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a107951 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coltogirone R. A., Sherfinski E. I., Dobler Z. A., Peterson S. N., Andlinger A. R., Fadel L. C., et al. (2021). Gsx2 but not Gsx1 is necessary for early forebrain patterning and long-term survival in zebrafish. Biorxiv [preprint] 10.1101/2021.09.13.460150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colwill R. M., Creton R. (2011). Locomotor behaviors in zebrafish (Danio rerio) larvae. Behav. Processes 86 222–229. 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connor S. A., Ammendrup-Johnsen I., Chan A. W., Kishimoto Y., Murayama C., Kurihara N., et al. (2016). Altered cortical dynamics and cognitive function upon haploinsufficiency of the autism-linked excitatory synaptic suppressor MDGA2. Neuron 91 1052–1068. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.08.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conradt L., Bodsworth E. J., Roper T. J., Thomas C. D. (2000). Non-random dispersal in the butterfly Maniola jurtina: implications for metapopulation models. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B: Biol. Sci. 267 1505–1510. 10.1098/rspb.2000.1171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuellar-Partida G., Tung J. Y., Eriksson N., Albrecht E., Aliev F., Andreassen O. A., et al. (2020). Genome-wide association study identifies 48 common genetic variants associated with handedness. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5 59–70. 10.1038/s41562-020-00956-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davison J. M., Akitake C. M., Goll M. G., Rhee J. M., Gosse N., Baier H., et al. (2007). Transactivation from Gal4-VP16 transgenic insertions for tissue-specific cell labeling and ablation in zebrafish. Dev. Biol. 304 811–824. 10.1016/j.ydbio.2007.01.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Santi A., Sovrano V. A., Bisazza A., Vallortigara G. (2001). Mosquitofish display differential left- and right-eye use during mirror image scrutiny and predator inspection responses. Anim. Behav. 61 305–310. 10.1006/anbe.2000.1566 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- De Smedt M., Hoebeke I., Reynvoet K., Leclercq G., Plum J. (2005). Different thresholds of Notch signaling bias human precursor cells toward B-, NK-, monocytic/dendritic-, or T-cell lineage in thymus microenvironment. Blood 106 3498–3506. 10.1182/blood-2005-02-0496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demehri S., Turkoz A., Kopan R. (2009). Epidermal Notch1 loss promotes skin tumorigenesis by impacting the stromal microenvironment. Cancer Cell 16 55–66. 10.1016/j.ccr.2009.05.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derjean D., Moussaddy A., Atallah E., St-Pierre M., Auclair F., Chang S., et al. (2010). A novel neural substrate for the transformation of olfactory inputs into motor output. PLoS Biol. 8:e1000567. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000567 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dingemanse N. J., Both C., Drent P. J., Tinbergen J. M. (2004). Fitness consequences of avian personalities in a fluctuating environment. Proc. Biol. Sci. 271 847–852. 10.1098/rspb.2004.2680 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domenici P., Allan B. J. M., Watson S.-A., McCormick M. I., Munday P. L. (2014). Shifting from right to left: the combined effect of elevated CO2 and temperature on behavioural lateralization in a coral reef fish. PLoS One 9:e87969. 10.1371/journal.pone.0087969 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dreosti E., Lopes G., Kampff A. R., Wilson S. W. (2015). Development of social behavior in young zebrafish. Front. Neural. Circuits 9:39. 10.3389/fncir.2015.00039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dreosti E., Vendrell Llopis N., Carl M., Yaksi E., Wilson S. W. (2014). Left-right asymmetry is required for the habenulae to respond to both visual and olfactory stimuli. Curr. Biol. 24 440–445. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn T. W., Mu Y., Narayan S., Randlett O., Naumann E. A., Yang C.-T., et al. (2016). Brain-wide mapping of neural activity controlling zebrafish exploratory locomotion. Elife 5:e12741. 10.7554/eLife.12741 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elnitsky M. A., Claussen D. L. (2006). The effects of temperature and inter-individual variation on the locomotor performance of juvenile turtles. J. Comp. Physiol. B 176 497–504. 10.1007/s00360-006-0071-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engeszer R. E., Patterson L. B., Rao A. A., Parichy D. M. (2007). Zebrafish in the wild: a review of natural history and new notes from the field. Zebrafish 4 21–40. 10.1089/zeb.2006.9997 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eto K., Mazilu-Brown J. K., Henderson-MacLennan N., Dipple K. M., McCabe E. R. B. (2014). Development of catecholamine and cortisol stress responses in zebrafish. Mol. Genet. Metab. Rep. 1 373–377. 10.1016/j.ymgmr.2014.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fauq A. H., Simpson K., Maharvi G. M., Golde T., Das P. (2007). A multigram chemical synthesis of the gamma-secretase inhibitor LY411575 and its diastereoisomers. Bioorg. Med. Chem. Lett. 17 6392–6395. 10.1016/j.bmcl.2007.07.062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandes A. M., Mearns D. S., Donovan J. C., Larsch J., Helmbrecht T. O., Kölsch Y., et al. (2021). Neural circuitry for stimulus selection in the zebrafish visual system. Neuron 109 805.e6–822.e6. 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Filosa A., Barker A. J., Dal Maschio M., Baier H. (2016). Feeding state modulates behavioral choice and processing of prey stimuli in the Zebrafish tectum. Neuron 90 596–608. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer-Zirnsak B., Segebrecht L., Schubach M., Charles P., Alderman E., Brown K., et al. (2019). Haploinsufficiency of the notch ligand DLL1 causes variable neurodevelopmental disorders. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 105 631–639. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2019.07.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fontana B. D., Cleal M., Clay J. M., Parker M. O. (2019). Zebrafish (Danio rerio) behavioral laterality predicts increased short-term avoidance memory but not stress-reactivity responses. Anim. Cogn. 22 1051–1061. 10.1007/s10071-019-01296-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund J., Brandmaier A. M., Lewejohann L., Kirste I., Kritzler M., Krüger A., et al. (2013). Emergence of individuality in genetically identical mice. Science 340 756–759. 10.1126/science.1235294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund J., Brandmaier A. M., Lewejohann L., Kirste I., Kritzler M., Krüger A., et al. (2015). Association between exploratory activity and social individuality in genetically identical mice living in the same enriched environment. Neuroscience 309 140–152. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.05.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund N., Valencia-Alfonso C. E., Kirsch J., Brodmann K., Manns M., Güntürkün O. (2016). Asymmetric top-down modulation of ascending visual pathways in pigeons. Neuropsychologia 83 37–47. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.08.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gahtan E., Tanger P., Baier H. (2005). Visual prey capture in larval zebrafish is controlled by identified reticulospinal neurons downstream of the tectum. J. Neurosci. 25 9294–9303. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2678-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gamse J. T., Kuan Y.-S., Macurak M., Brösamle C., Thisse B., Thisse C., et al. (2005). Directional asymmetry of the zebrafish epithalamus guides dorsoventral innervation of the midbrain target. Development 132 4869–4881. 10.1242/dev.02046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gamse J. T., Thisse C., Thisse B., Halpern M. E. (2003). The parapineal mediates left-right asymmetry in the zebrafish diencephalon. Development 130 1059–1068. 10.1242/dev.00270 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gebhardt C., Auer T. O., Henriques P. M., Rajan G., Duroure K., Bianco I. H., et al. (2019). An interhemispheric neural circuit allowing binocular integration in the optic tectum. Nat. Commun. 10:5471. 10.1038/s41467-019-13484-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geling A., Steiner H., Willem M., Bally-Cuif L., Haass C. (2002). A γ-secretase inhibitor blocks Notch signaling in vivo and causes a severe neurogenic phenotype in zebrafish. EMBO Rep. 3 688–694. 10.1093/embo-reports/kvf124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbons T. C., Rudman S. M., Schulte P. M. (2017). Low temperature and low salinity drive putatively adaptive growth differences in populations of threespine stickleback. Sci. Rep. 7:16766. 10.1038/s41598-017-16919-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giljov A., Karenina K., Malashichev Y. (2013). Forelimb preferences in quadrupedal marsupials and their implications for laterality evolution in mammals. BMC Evol. Biol. 13:61. 10.1186/1471-2148-13-61 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray J. M., Hill J. J., Bargmann C. I. (2005). A circuit for navigation in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102 3184–3191. 10.1073/pnas.0409009101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Güntürkün O., Hellmann B., Melsbach G., Prior H. (1998). Asymmetries of representation in the visual system of pigeons. Neuroreport 9 4127–4130. 10.1097/00001756-199812210-00023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haesemeyer M., Robson D. N., Li J. M., Schier A. F., Engert F. (2018). A brain-wide circuit model of heat-evoked swimming behavior in larval zebrafish. Neuron 98 817.e6–831.e6. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hager T., Jansen R. F., Pieneman A. W., Manivannan S. N., Golani I., van der Sluis S., et al. (2014). Display of individuality in avoidance behavior and risk assessment of inbred mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:314. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00314 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hills T., Brockie P. J., Maricq A. V. (2004). Dopamine and glutamate control area-restricted search behavior in caenorhabditis elegans. J. Neurosci. 24 1217–1225. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1569-03.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hills T. T., Kalff C., Wiener J. M. (2013). Adaptive lévy processes and area-restricted search in human foraging. PLoS One 8:e60488. 10.1371/journal.pone.0060488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horstick E. J., Bayleyen Y., Burgess H. A. (2020). Molecular and cellular determinants of motor asymmetry in zebrafish. Nat. Commun. 11:1170. 10.1038/s41467-020-14965-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horstick E. J., Bayleyen Y., Sinclair J. L., Burgess H. A. (2017). Search strategy is regulated by somatostatin signaling and deep brain photoreceptors in zebrafish. BMC Biol. 15:4. 10.1186/s12915-016-0346-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horstick E. J., Linsley J. W., Dowling J. J., Hauser M. A., McDonald K. K., Ashley-Koch A., et al. (2013). Stac3 is a component of the excitation-contraction coupling machinery and mutated in Native American myopathy. Nat. Commun. 4:1952. 10.1038/ncomms2952 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horváth G., Jiménez-Robles O., Martín J., López P., De la Riva I., Herczeg G. (2020). Linking behavioral thermoregulation, boldness, and individual state in male Carpetan rock lizards. Ecol. Evol. 10 10230–10241. 10.1002/ece3.6685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humphries D. A., Driver P. M. (1970). Protean defence by prey animals. Oecologia 5 285–302. 10.1007/BF00815496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ishii K., Wohl M., DeSouza A., Asahina K. (2020). Sex-determining genes distinctly regulate courtship capability and target preference via sexually dimorphic neurons. Elife 9:e52701. 10.7554/eLife.52701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itoh M., Kim C.-H., Palardy G., Oda T., Jiang Y.-J., Maust D., et al. (2003). Mind bomb is a ubiquitin ligase that is essential for efficient activation of Notch signaling by Delta. Dev. Cell 4 67–82. 10.1016/s1534-5807(02)00409-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izvekov E. I., Nepomnyashchikh V. A., Medyantseva E. N., Chebotareva Y. V., Izyumov Y. G. (2012). Selection of direction of movement and bilateral morphological asymmetry in young roach (Rutilus rutilus). Biol. Bull. Rev. 2 364–370. 10.1134/S2079086412040044 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs C. T., Huang P. (2019). Notch signalling maintains Hedgehog responsiveness via a Gli-dependent mechanism during spinal cord patterning in zebrafish. eLife 8:e49252. 10.7554/eLife.49252 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jäncke L., Steinmetz H. (1995). Hand motor performance and degree of asymmetry in monozygotic twins. Cortex 31 779–785. 10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80028-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kain J. S., Stokes C., de Bivort B. L. (2012). Phototactic personality in fruit flies and its suppression by serotonin and white. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109 19834–19839. 10.1073/pnas.1211988109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kain J. S., Zhang S., Akhund-Zade J., Samuel A. D. T., Klein M., de Bivort B. L. (2015). Variability in thermal and phototactic preferences in Drosophila may reflect an adaptive bet-hedging strategy. Evolution 69 3171–3185. 10.1111/evo.12813 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim C. H., Ueshima E., Muraoka O., Tanaka H., Yeo S. Y., Huh T. L., et al. (1996). Zebrafish elav/HuC homologue as a very early neuronal marker. Neurosci. Lett. 216 109–112. 10.1016/0304-3940(96)13021-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimmel C. B., Ballard W. W., Kimmel S. R., Ullmann B., Schilling T. F. (1995). Stages of embryonic development of the zebrafish. Dev. Dyn. 203 253–310. 10.1002/aja.1002030302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein S., Pasquaretta C., Barron A. B., Devaud J.-M., Lihoreau M. (2017). Inter-individual variability in the foraging behaviour of traplining bumblebees. Sci. Rep. 7:4561. 10.1038/s41598-017-04919-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Körholz J. C., Zocher S., Grzyb A. N., Morisse B., Poetzsch A., Ehret F., et al. (2018). Selective increases in inter-individual variability in response to environmental enrichment in female mice. eLife 7:e35690. 10.7554/eLife.35690 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krebs L. T., Shutter J. R., Tanigaki K., Honjo T., Stark K. L., Gridley T. (2004). Haploinsufficient lethality and formation of arteriovenous malformations in Notch pathway mutants. Genes Dev. 18 2469–2473. 10.1101/gad.1239204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A., Huh T.-L., Choe J., Rhee M. (2017). Rnf152 is essential for NeuroD expression and delta-notch signaling in the zebrafish embryos. Mol. Cells 40 945–953. 10.14348/molcells.2017.0216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kushlan J. A. (1976). Feeding behavior of North Americans herons. Auk 93 86–94. 10.1371/journal.pntd.0001452 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaFoya B., Munroe J. A., Mia M. M., Detweiler M. A., Crow J. J., Wood T., et al. (2016). Notch: a multi-functional integrating system of microenvironmental signals. Dev. Biol. 418 227–241. 10.1016/j.ydbio.2016.08.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambert A. M., Bonkowsky J. L., Masino M. A. (2012). The conserved dopaminergic diencephalospinal tract mediates vertebrate locomotor development in zebrafish larvae. J. Neurosci. 32 13488–13500. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1638-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsch J., Baier H. (2018). Biological motion as an innate perceptual mechanism driving social affiliation. Curr. Biol. 28 3523.e4–3532.e4. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.09.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis L. P. C., Siju K. P., Aso Y., Friedrich A. B., Bulteel A. J. B., Rubin G. M., et al. (2015). A higher brain circuit for immediate integration of conflicting sensory information in drosophila. Curr. Biol. 25 2203–2214. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.07.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linneweber G. A., Andriatsilavo M., Dutta S. B., Bengochea M., Hellbruegge L., Liu G., et al. (2020). A neurodevelopmental origin of behavioral individuality in the Drosophila visual system. Science 367 1112–1119. 10.1126/science.aaw7182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long Y., Li L., Li Q., He X., Cui Z. (2012). Transcriptomic characterization of temperature stress responses in larval zebrafish. PLoS One 7:e37209. 10.1371/journal.pone.0037209 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manns M., Otto T., Salm L. (2021). Pigeons show how meta-control enables decision-making in an ambiguous world. Sci. Rep. 11:3838. 10.1038/s41598-021-83406-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manns M., Römling J. (2012). The impact of asymmetrical light input on cerebral hemispheric specialization and interhemispheric cooperation. Nat. Commun. 3:696. 10.1038/ncomms1699 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marquart G. D., Tabor K. M., Bergeron S. A., Briggman K. L., Burgess H. A. (2019). Prepontine non-giant neurons drive flexible escape behavior in zebrafish. PLoS Biol. 17:e3000480. 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000480 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marquart G. D., Tabor K. M., Brown M., Strykowski J. L., Varshney G. K., LaFave M. C., et al. (2015). A 3D searchable database of transgenic zebrafish Gal4 and cre lines for functional neuroanatomy studies. Front. Neural Circuits 9:78. 10.3389/fncir.2015.00078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsuda M., Rand K., Palardy G., Shimizu N., Ikeda H., Dalle Nogare D., et al. (2016). Epb41l5 competes with Delta as a substrate for Mib1 to coordinate specification and differentiation of neurons. Development 143 3085–3096. 10.1242/dev.138743 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyashita T., Palmer A. R. (2014). Handed behavior in hagfish–an ancient vertebrate lineage–and a survey of lateralized behaviors in other invertebrate chordates and elongate vertebrates. Biol. Bull. 226 111–120. 10.1086/BBLv226n2p111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mizutani K., Yoon K., Dang L., Tokunaga A., Gaiano N. (2007). Differential Notch signalling distinguishes neural stem cells from intermediate progenitors. Nature 449 351–355. 10.1038/nature06090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muto A., Lal P., Ailani D., Abe G., Itoh M., Kawakami K. (2017). Activation of the hypothalamic feeding centre upon visual prey detection. Nat. Commun. 8:15029. 10.1038/ncomms15029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson J. C., Witze E., Ma Z., Ciocco F., Frerotte A., Randlett O., et al. (2020). Acute regulation of habituation learning via posttranslational palmitoylation. Curr. Biol. 30 2729.e4–2738.e4. 10.1016/j.cub.2020.05.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohata S., Aoki R., Kinoshita S., Yamaguchi M., Tsuruoka-Kinoshita S., Tanaka H., et al. (2011). Dual roles of Notch in regulation of apically restricted mitosis and apicobasal polarity of neuroepithelial cells. Neuron 69 215–230. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.12.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pantoja C., Hoagland A., Carroll E. C., Karalis V., Conner A., Isacoff E. Y. (2016). Neuromodulatory regulation of behavioral individuality in zebrafish. Neuron 91 587–601. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.06.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pantoja C., Larsch J., Laurell E., Marquart G., Kunst M., Baier H. (2020). Rapid effects of selection on brain-wide activity and behavior. Curr. Biol. 30 3647.e3–3656.e3. 10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pei Z., Wang B., Chen G., Nagao M., Nakafuku M., Campbell K. (2011). Homeobox genes Gsx1 and Gsx2 differentially regulate telencephalic progenitor maturation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108 1675–1680. 10.1073/pnas.1008824108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (2020). Vienna: R Core Team. [Google Scholar]
- Rogers L. J. (1982). Light experience and asymmetry of brain function in chickens. Nature 297 223–225. 10.1038/297223a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers L. J., Deng C. (2005). Corticosterone treatment of the chick embryo affects light-stimulated development of the thalamofugal visual pathway. Behav. Brain Res. 159 63–71. 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roussigné M., Bianco I. H., Wilson S. W., Blader P. (2009). Nodal signalling imposes left-right asymmetry upon neurogenesis in the habenular nuclei. Development 136 1549–1557. 10.1242/dev.034793 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roychoudhury K., Salomone J., Qin S., Cain B., Adam M., Potter S. S., et al. (2020). Physical interactions between Gsx2 and Ascl1 balance progenitor expansion versus neurogenesis in the mouse lateral ganglionic eminence. Development 147:dev185348. 10.1242/dev.185348 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiffner I., Srinivasan M. V. (2013). Behavioural lateralization in Budgerigars varies with the task and the individual. PLoS One 8:e82670. 10.1371/journal.pone.0082670 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzkopf M., Liu M. C., Schulte S. J., Ives R., Husain N., Choi H. M. T., et al. (2021). Hybridization chain reaction enables a unified approach to multiplexed, quantitative, high-resolution immunohistochemistry and in situ hybridization. bioRxiv [preprint] 2021.06.02.446311. 10.1101/2021.06.02.446311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharma P., Saraswathy V. M., Xiang L., Fürthauer M. (2019). Notch-mediated inhibition of neurogenesis is required for zebrafish spinal cord morphogenesis. Sci. Rep. 9:9958. 10.1038/s41598-019-46067-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen W., Huang J., Wang Y. (2021). Biological significance of NOTCH signaling strength. Front. Cell Dev. Biol. 9:652273. 10.3389/fcell.2021.652273 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons A. M. (2011). Modes of response to environmental change and the elusive empirical evidence for bet hedging. Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 278 1601–1609. 10.1098/rspb.2011.0176 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Souman J. L., Frissen I., Sreenivasa M. N., Ernst M. O. (2009). Walking straight into circles. Curr. Biol. 19 1538–1542. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sovrano V. A. (2004). Visual lateralization in response to familiar and unfamiliar stimuli in fish. Behav. Brain Res. 152 385–391. 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.10.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stern S., Kirst C., Bargmann C. I. (2017). Neuromodulatory control of long-term behavioral patterns and individuality across development. Cell 171 1649.e10–1662.e10. 10.1016/j.cell.2017.10.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Striedter G. F. (1991). Auditory, electrosensory, and mechanosensory lateral line pathways through the forebrain in channel catfishes. J. Comparat. Neurol. 312 311–331. 10.1002/cne.903120213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sundin J., Morgan R., Finnøen M. H., Dey A., Sarkar K., Jutfelt F. (2019). On the observation of wild zebrafish (Danio rerio) in India. Zebrafish 16 546–553. 10.1089/zeb.2019.1778 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallafuss A., Trepman A., Eisen J. S. (2009). DeltaA mRNA and protein distribution in the zebrafish nervous system. Dev. Dyn. 238 3226–3236. 10.1002/dvdy.22136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tremblay Y., Roberts A. J., Costa D. P. (2007). Fractal landscape method: an alternative approach to measuring area-restricted searching behavior. J. Exp. Biol. 210 935–945. 10.1242/jeb.02710 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Versace E., Caffini M., Werkhoven Z., de Bivort B. L. (2020). Individual, but not population asymmetries, are modulated by social environment and genotype in Drosophila melanogaster. Sci. Rep. 10:4480. 10.1038/s41598-020-61410-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt G., Huber M., Thiemann M., Boogaart G., Schmitz O. J., Schubart C. D. (2008). Production of different phenotypes from the same genotype in the same environment by developmental variation. J. Exp. Biol. 211 510–523. 10.1242/jeb.008755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang B., Waclaw R. R., Allen Z. J., Guillemot F., Campbell K. (2009). Ascl1 is a required downstream effector of Gsx gene function in the embryonic mouse telencephalon. Neural Dev. 4:5. 10.1186/1749-8104-4-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson N. V., Kimura D. (1989). Right-hand superiority for throwing but not for intercepting. Neuropsychologia 27 1399–1414. 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90133-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- Yaeger C., Ros A. M., Cross V., DeAngelis R. S., Stobaugh D. J., Rhodes J. S. (2014). Blockade of arginine vasotocin signaling reduces aggressive behavior and c-Fos expression in the preoptic area and periventricular nucleus of the posterior tuberculum in male Amphiprion ocellaris. Neuroscience 267 205–218. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.02.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yapici N., Cohn R., Schusterreiter C., Ruta V., Vosshall L. B. (2016). A Taste circuit that regulates ingestion by integrating food and hunger signals. Cell 165 715–729. 10.1016/j.cell.2016.02.061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yokogawa T., Hannan M. C., Burgess H. A. (2012). The dorsal raphe modulates sensory responsiveness during arousal in zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 32 15205–15215. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1019-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoon K.-J., Koo B.-K., Im S.-K., Jeong H.-W., Ghim J., Kwon M.-C., et al. (2008). Mind bomb 1-expressing intermediate progenitors generate notch signaling to maintain radial glial cells. Neuron 58 519–531. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zadicario P., Avni R., Zadicario E., Eilam D. (2005). ‘Looping’-an exploration mechanism in a dark open field. Behav. Brain Res. 159 27–36. 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.09.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zocher S., Schilling S., Grzyb A. N., Adusumilli V. S., Bogado Lopes J., Günther S., et al. (2020). Early-life environmental enrichment generates persistent individualized behavior in mice. Sci. Adv. 6:eabb1478. 10.1126/sciadv.abb1478 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author for R scripts and datasets generated in this study.