Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2021 Dec 20;16(12):e0261570. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0261570

Inter-individual differences in pain anticipation and pain perception in migraine: Neural correlates of migraine frequency and cortisol-to-dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEA-S) ratio

Gyöngyi Kökönyei 1,2,3,*, Attila Galambos 2,4, Natália Kocsel 2, Edina Szabó 2,5, Andrea Edit Édes 1, Kinga Gecse 1,3, Dániel Baksa 1,3, Dorottya Pap 3, Lajos R Kozák 6, György Bagdy 3,7, Gabriella Juhász 1,3
Editor: Mukesh Dhamala8
PMCID: PMC8687546  PMID: 34929017

Abstract

Previous studies targeting inter-individual differences in pain processing in migraine mainly focused on the perception of pain. Our main aim was to disentangle pain anticipation and perception using a classical fear conditioning task, and investigate how migraine frequency and pre-scan cortisol-to-dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEA-S) ratio as an index of neurobiological stress response would relate to neural activation in these two phases. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) data of 23 participants (18 females; mean age: 27.61± 5.36) with episodic migraine without aura were analysed. We found that migraine frequency was significantly associated with pain anticipation in brain regions comprising the midcingulate and caudate, whereas pre-scan cortisol-to DHEA-S ratio was related to pain perception in the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA). Both results suggest exaggerated preparatory responses to pain or more general to stressors, which may contribute to the allostatic load caused by stressors and migraine attacks on the brain.

Introduction

Migraine, as one of the most debilitating diseases [1], is considered to be a complex neurological disorder characterized by subtle changes in functioning of the brain which is related to variations in the structure and connectivity of specific brain areas [24]. Altered functioning of the brain is not limited to migraine attacks (ictal phase) but is present between-attacks (interictal phase) as well. For instance, in fMRI studies, applying acute painful stimuli, migraineurs compared to control subjects had altered pain-induced activation in a broad range of cortical and subcortical structures, including the somatosensory cortices [5, 6], cingulate [5, 7], insula [8], dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [5, 8], brainstem [9, 10], cerebellum [10], the striatum [11], and the temporal lobe [12].

However, there is accumulating evidence that there are inter-individual differences in pain processing in migraine. Migraine frequency is an important clinical variable. For instance, a recent systematic review [13] shows that headache frequency is an important risk factor for migraine chronification in episodic migraine. With increasing frequency of attacks and years with migraine, the reorganization of grey [14] and white matter [15] and functional connectivity [11] is more and more likely constituting an allostatic load in the long term, since every migraine attack can be considered as a hit on the brain [16]. Thus, it can be considered as a proxy of allostatic load of migraine. In line with this, we can expect that more changes in pain processing occur with more frequent migraine attacks or more years with migraine. Previous results support this notion: for instance, a positive relationship between migraine frequency and activation in several brain areas including the cingulate, DLPFC, fusiform gyrus, precentral gyrus, hippocampus to painful heat stimuli has been detected in Schwedt et al’s study [5]. Mathur and colleagues [8] found a positive relationship between activation in bilateral posterior insula to thermal noxious stimuli and migraine frequency, albeit on a lower threshold. BOLD response in the pons to trigeminal painful stimulation correlated positively with attack frequency [6]. Duration of migraine (years with migraine) showed a positive association with increased activation in the fusiform gyrus [5], cerebellum [17], and superior temporal gyrus [8].

It is important to note that pain perception is influenced by expectations and anticipation [18, 19] as illustrious placebo and nocebo studies in this field have showed [20]. Furthermore, in conditioning paradigms, subjective perception and neural response to noxious stimulus are enhanced if it is preceded by cues predicting pain [18], which is in line with the predictive coding model of brain functioning [21]. This means that our pain perception is not simply based on upcoming and ascending sensory input but our previous experiences, and hence related expectations also shape our perception [19]. Taking into account that cues predicting subsequent pain are salient and appraised as threats [22] that help to initiate appropriate actions or adjust behaviour accordingly, it is unsurprising that neural response to pain anticipation have been demonstrated to be widespread, including, for instance, the thalamus, caudate, anterior cingulate cortex, midcingulate and insula [23].

Some evidence suggests that pain anticipation-related neural response in chronic pain compared to pain-free subjects, is enhanced (e.g. in fibromyalgia [24]), however, data about pain anticipation in migraine are scarce, so as its relationship with migraine frequency. For instance, in a placebo study pain anticipation in migraine, compared to control subjects, was associated with enhanced neural response in some regions of the primary visual cortex [25]. Results showing impaired habituation to repeated (and predicted) contact-heat pain in migraine also support the notion that anticipatory processes could be enhanced [26]. In the context of migraine, as attacks become recurrent, pain and the related negative emotional, cognitive or social consequences become more salient, thus any cues associated with pain (as an important symptom of a migraine attack) may have a stronger impact. It is therefore plausible to suggest that there is an association between migraine frequency and the heightened pain anticipation in migraine.

It should be also noted, that not just waiting for pain (or a pain task) could be a stressful situation, but the fMRI procedure itself [27], especially if it is the first time exposure to the fMRI scanner environment [28]. In line with this, experienced stress before the scan indexed by pre-scan cortisol was associated with enhanced emotional processing of fearful faces relative to neutral ones in limbic structures [29]. However, dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) or its sulphated metabolite (dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate; DHEA-S), precursors for adrogens, released during acute stress [30, 31] may counteract the effects of cortisol [32], supported by findings showing that acute administration of DHEA decreased the cortisol level [33]. DHEA and DHEA-S (collectively referred here to DHEA(S)) are also associated with better performance under stressful condition [34]. Besides, accumulating evidence suggests that DHEA(S) is associated with neural processing of emotional stimuli [35]. For instance, exogenous DHEA administration, relative to placebo, reduced the activity of amygdala and hippocampus, and enhanced the activity of medial prefrontal cortex (or more precisely, the rostral ACC) during a task requiring implicit emotional processing and regulation [36].

What is important, based on the opposing effects of cortisol and DHEA(S), that cortisol-to-DHEA(S) ratio has been suggested to be more informative than the two parameters alone [37]. For instance, higher cortisol-to-DHEAS ratio has been found to be associated with all-cause mortality in a prospective cohort [38], and with shorter time till reoccurrence of major depressive episode in a subsample of participants with major depressive disorder in a prospective study [39]. Findings of Grillon and colleagues [40] on the relationship between cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio and heightened fear potentiated startle in an aversive conditioning task lend support for the notion that cortisol-to-DHEA(S) ratio might be associated with emotion/threat processing, as one of the mechanisms by which complex pattern of adrenocortical activity exerts its effect on health. Thus, it is reasonable to believe that inter-individual differences in pre-scan ratio of cortisol-to-DHEA(S) will be related to pain processing.

The present study therefore aimed to investigate neural response to pain predictive cues and painful stimulus in episodic migraine. More specifically, we were interested in how inter-individual differences in migraine frequency would be related to anticipation and processing of experimentally induced pain between migraine attacks (interictal state). In light of previous studies on pain processing [5, 6, 8, 17], we expected that migraine frequency would be related to pain perception. We further hypothesized that neural anticipatory response to pain predicting cues would also be related to this clinical variable. In the context of pre-scan stress, we considered that cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio reflects inter-individual differences in stress responsivity to a new and potentially stressful situation, and assumed that this ratio, as an index of neuroendocrine stress reaction, would influence functional activation to pain anticipation and pain perception. In the context of migraine, we supposed that allostatic load exerted by increased anticipatory neuroendocrine stress reaction could contribute to the adverse effects of everyday stressors, resulting changes in brain states. Based on previous evidence on the role of medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in stress [41], emotion [42] and pain regulation [43], we expected that higher pre-scan cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio reflecting increased neuroendocrine stress reaction would enhance pain anticipation and/or perception in mPFC.

Methods

Participants

Right-handed subjects aged between 18–38 years were recruited via newspapers, headache clinics and university advertisements. Inclusion criteria were having episodic migraine without aura diagnosed by a headache specialist using the International Classification of Headache Disorders-III criteria (ICHD-III, beta version; Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society [44]. The exclusion criteria were the followings: any current or past serious medical, neurological (except migraine), Axis I psychiatric disorders and psychotropic medication use. Participants were screened using the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview [45] by trained researchers. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. They were invited to the scan if they did not have migraine attacks 48 h prior to the scan, and their data was analysed if they did not have migraine attacks during 24 h follow-up after the scan. They refrained from taking any analgesics 48 h before the scan session and did not take any prophylactic medication during the last three months. Participants were asked to refrain from alcohol for at least 24 hours and from caffeine for at least 4 hours before the scanning session.

Of the 28 participants (22 females, mean age = 27.46, SD = 4.89, age range: 20 to 37 years) taking part in the pain task, five subjects were excluded: three by technical reasons (missing images/volumes originated from technical problems or incomplete voxel level information), and two subjects did not see any correspondence between visual signals and electric stimuli according to the post-task interview (see below). Thus, finally data of 23 subjects (18 females) with migraine without aura (mean age = 27.61, SD = 5.36) were analysed. Mean body mass index (BMI) was 21.52 (SD = 3.72) kg/m2, and all participants had a normal BMI (except one who was obese, his BMI was: 32.9). Nine women were in the follicular phase, and 9 were in the luteal phase. Six women reported using hormonal contraception.

The study was approved by the Scientific and Research Ethics Committee of the Medical Research Council (Hungary, number: 23609-1/2011-EKU, 23421-1/2015-EKU) and written informed consents were received from all subjects in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Blood sample collection

Blood samples were collected from participants to 3 ml K3EDTA tubes. The samples were immediately centrifuged, then plasma samples were frozen at -80°C (-112°F) until the assay.

Plasma cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate (DHEA-S) levels have been measured by competitive ELISA kits from NovaTec Immunodiagnostica GmbH (Dietzenbach, Germany) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Plasma samples were thawed only once. Inter- and intra-assay variance, expressed as coefficient of variation (CV) were: Cortisol: 11.0% and 5.1%; DHEA-S: 10.4% and 7.9%, respectively. Cortisol antibody shows 46.2% cross reaction with prednisolone and DHEA-S antibody shows cross reaction with DHEA (100%) and androstenedione (59%).

DHEA-S was measured in μg/ml and cortisol was measured in ng/ml, their ratio (cortisol-to-DHEA-S) was calculated in μg/ml, and its log-transformed value was used in the analyses based on the recommendation of Sollberger and Ehlert [46]. For descriptive purposes, raw cortisol and DHEA-S values are reported in Table 1. The raw values were unrelated (rho = 0.07, p>0.05).

Table 1. Experimental design.

Number of trials Cue Stimulus
10 Pain cue Painful (VAS = 7)–Pain
5 Pain cue Non-painful (VAS = 3)–Omitted pain
15 No pain cue Non-painful (VAS = 3)–Touch

VAS Visual Analog Scale. Fifteen trials for pain cue and 15 trials for no pain cue.

fMRI scans were administered between 4:00pm and 8:00pm in order to avoid the impact of circadian variations in cortisol [47]. Level of DHEA-S as an important marker of adrenal androgen synthesis also shows a diurnal decline [48], therefore all the participants were scanned in the same time interval.

Self-report measures

Migraine-related clinical variables: Besides demographic data, all participants were asked to provide information on 1) age at migraine onset; 2) years with migraine (duration of the disease); 3) migraine frequency (average number of migraine per month).

Anxiety: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI [49]) was used to measure trait- and state-like anxiety symptoms by 20 items. Items are answered on a 4-point Likert-type scale (1 = almost never to 4 = almost always). The internal consistency of both scales was excellent (Cronbach’s alpha: 0.84 and 0.93, respectively).

Psychological task

Two electronic stimuli delivered by Digitimer boxes (Digitimer DS7A, Digitimer Ltd, Welywyn Garden City, UK) were used during the fMRI session that were applied to the dorsum of subject’s right hand. The intensity of stimulation was individually set before the session using a staircase protocol. Participants rated the stimuli in a 10-point visual analogue scale (VAS). When the stimulus was rated as 3 (VAS = 3, currents ranging from 0.16 to 0.78 mA) it was set as a non-painful one. Electric shock was augmented to painful but a tolerable stimulus, rated as 7 (VAS = 7, currents ranging from 0.29 to 1.7 mA). Each shock lasts for 2-ms as in Spoormaker and colleagues study [50]. E-Prime script (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA) controlled the input of Digitimer boxes.

Participants laid still during the scanning session and they had to pay attention to the screen where they could see two different shapes that would be followed by the stimuli. They were not informed about the cue-stimulus relationship to prevent expectation formation based on verbal suggestion. In total, 30 trials in two scanning runs composed the experimental paradigm. The green triangle (15 trials) was always followed by a non-painful stimulus, meanwhile the red square (15 trials) was followed by a painful stimulus. Five trials were exceptional; in these cases, the pain cue was followed by a non-painful stimulus (VAS rating = 3) (omitted pain trials) (see Table 1), as in our previous study [51]).

Participants had to report in a post-task interview, if they found any correspondence between cues and subsequent stimuli during the task. During the inter-trial interval, participants saw only a black screen lasting 30 s. In the task, each visual signal was preceded by a white fixation cross for 1 s (see Fig 1). The pseudorandomized visual stimuli duration was ranged from 6 to 12 s (average = 9.3 s).

Fig 1. Design of the pain task.

Fig 1

fMRI acquisition

In this study, a whole-brain functional MRI data acquisition was carried out at a 3T MRI scanner (Achieva 3T, Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands) using a BOLD-sensitive T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence (repetition time [TR] = 2,500 ms, echo time [TE] = 30 ms, field of view [FOV] = 240 × 240 mm2) with 3 mm × 3 mm in-plane resolution and contiguous 3-mm slices providing whole-brain coverage. A series of high-resolution anatomical images were also obtained with a T1-weighted 3D TFE sequence with 1 × 1 × 1 mm resolution.

Statistical analysis of self-report data

To analyse demographic and self-report data, t-tests and correlation analyses were used with two-tailed p<0.05 threshold in SPSS version 25.0 (IBMSPSS, IBMCorp, Armonk, NY, USA).

fMRI data analysis

Regarding preprocessing and first-level model we followed the same procedure described in our previous study [51].

Preprocessing

Imaging data analysis was carried out using the Statistical Parametrical Mapping (SPM12) analysis software package (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, Institute of Neurology, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm12/) implemented in Matlab 2015b (Math Works, Natick, MA, USA). Preprocessing of the functional images contained realignment, co-registration to the structural image, segmentation, normalization in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, and spatial smoothing with an 8-mm full width half-maximum Gaussian kernel. In the last step of pre-processing, quality of images was visually inspected.

First-level model

First level fMRI models included the fixation cross, each of the two cues (pain cue and no-pain cue) and the three outcomes (touch, pain, omitted pain; see Table 5). The effects of low-frequency physiological noise were removed using high-pass temporal filtering with a cut-off of 128 s. An autoregressive AR (1) model was used to serial correlations in data series. We identified the motion outliers (threshold of global signal > 3 SD and motion > 1mm) with the Artifact Detection Tools (ART www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect/) and along with the six motion parameters they were used as regressors of no interest in the first level fMRI model.

Table 5. Relationship between migraine frequency (average number of migraine attacks per month) and pain anticipation.
Contrast Cluster size (voxels) Region Br Side Peak T-value MNI coordinates
x y z
Pain cue–No pain cue 586 caudate R 5.16 15 17 2
midcingulate 24 R 4.84 3 17 32
caudate L 4.27 21 17 17

Note: Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05. R: right; L: left; NA: coordinates are not in the AAL.

First-level analysis was performed on each participant to determine the significant BOLD signal responses to anticipation of pain (pain cue vs. no pain cue) and to perception of pain (painful stimuli (VAS = 7) vs. non-painful one (VAS = 3).

Second level analysis

The above described contrast maps were then entered into the second-level analyses. First, one sample t-tests were performed to examine anticipation-related activations in the whole brain. Another whole-brain activation analysis was conducted to identify activations in the pain perception period.

In the second step, migraine frequency as a clinical variable was used as a covariate in the analyses to reveal the associations between BOLD responses to pain cue and painful stimuli. Based on previous studies (see the introduction) we tested whether migraine frequency correlated with BOLD signal in the pain perception period, and then we explored its relationship with pain anticipation (see introduction) using whole-brain regression analyses. Then we investigated whether the log transformed cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio associated with BOLD response in the medial PFC, using the mask of de la Vega and co-workers [52] to test our a-priori hypothesis. We lacked cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio for one subject who was excluded from this analysis.

Considering group-level whole-brain analysis, we used a nonparametric, permutation-based approach (SnPM13; http://www.nisox.org/Software/SnPM13/) with 5000 permutations and without variance smoothing in order to control for false-positives due to multiple testing [53]. Whole-brain analyses were carried out at a p<0.001 uncorrected level and cluster-level family-wise error-corrected pFWE <0.05 values were reported as significant. Activated clusters were identified anatomically using the automated anatomical labelling atlas [54]. For ROI analyses, we performed small volume correction (SVC) analyses in SPM using voxel-wise threshold p<0.05, FWE corrected.

Trait anxiety has been demonstrated to be associated with anticipation of pain [55], and state anxiety seems to affect perceived pain intensity [56] and sensitivity [57] to experimental pain, therefore we also checked whether results changed if trait or state anxiety was added as a covariate.

We used a gray matter mask in our analyses, the template we used was provided by the Brain Imaging Centre, Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University: https://digital.lib.washington.edu/researchworks/handle/1773/33312).

Statistical maps were visualized on the MNI 152 template brain provided in MRIcroGL (http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricrogl/) [58]

Results

Clinical characteristics and demographic data of participants along with descriptive data of psychological scales are shown in Table 2. Trait and state anxiety correlated positively (rho = 0.49, p<0.05), but anxiety scores were unrelated to age, education level and clinical characteristics (correlational coefficients were between -0.12 and 0.15, p>0.05). Endocrine parameters, i.e. raw cortisol and DHEAS were not related to age, anxiety scores, clinical variables, menstrual phase and hormonal contraception taking. Raw cortisol and cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio correlated negatively with BMI (rho = -0.43 p<0.05 and rho = -0.55 p<0.01, respectively). Correlational results are reported in the S1 and S2 Tables.

Table 2. Demographic data and clinical characteristics of participants (N = 23).

Frequency (%) or Mean (SD) Range
Women 18 (78.3%)
Age 27.61 (5.36) 20–37
Highest education
    High school 11 (47.8%)
    Graduate degree 12 (52.2%)
Age at migraine onset 15.59 (7.16) 6–29
Number of years with migraine 12.02 (7.97) 2–30
Migraine frequency/month 2.35 (2.40) 1–12
State anxiety 34.26 (8.59) 23–67
Trait anxiety 39.04 (7.87) 27–54
Cortisol (ng/ml) 136.33 (90.00) 8.52–319.11
DHEA-S (μg/ml) 1.92 (1.01) 0.41–5.22
Cortisol/DHEA-S (μg/ml) 0.09 (0.9) 0.003–0.412

Note. DHEAS: dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate. Cortisol, DHEAS and cortisol-to-DHEAS ratio for one subject were missing.

fMRI results

Task-related activations

Pain anticipation. Increased activation to pain cues versus no pain cues was elicited in bilateral occipital cortex (calcarine, lingual gyrus), bilateral middle cingulate gyrus and right supplementary motor area (see Table 3 and Fig 2), while decreased activation in inferior occipital gyrus and superior parietal lobule was detected.

Table 3. Activation changes during pain anticipation (N = 23).
Contrast Cluster size (voxels) Region Br Side Peak T-value MNI coordinates
x y z
Pain cue > No pain cue 246 Calcarine L 5.68 -9 -88 11
Lingual gyrus L 5.28 -9 -76 -1
Calcarine 18 R 5.12 6 -85 11
87 Midcingulate R 4.23 12 14 41
Midcingulate 24 L 4.09 -6 2 44
SMA 6 R 4.04 6 2 56
No pain cue > Pain cue 75 Inferior occipital gyrus L 5.22 -42 -73 -7
Inferior occipital gyrus L 5.03 -42 -61 -13
83 Superior parietal lobule R 5.13 33 -61 53
Superior occipital gyrus R 4.43 27 -61 32

Note: cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R: right; L: left; SMA: Supplementary motor area.

Fig 2. Significant activations to pain anticipation (pain cue vs. no pain cue—red) and perception (painful vs. non-painful stimulus—yellow).

Fig 2

Significant activated clusters are shown at pFWE < 0.05. Coordinates are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. Statistical maps were visualized on the MNI 152 template brain provided in MRIcroGL (http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricrogl/) [58].

Results did not change substantially when gender, trait or state anxiety were controlled for (except in one case: when gender was controlled for, the analysis for the no pain cue vs. pain cue contrast resulted only one significant cluster in the inferior occipital gyrus (k = 69); these results are reported in the S3S5 Tables).

Pain perception. Contrasting painful (VAS = 7) versus non-painful stimuli (VAS = 3) led to widespread activation in brain areas including the cerebellum, vermis, insula, postcentral gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, Rolandic operculum and Heschl gyrus (see Table 4 and Fig 2). These result did not change substantially when state anxiety or gender was entered as a control variable in the analysis (see S6 and S7 Tables). The other contrast (non-painful stimuli (VAS = 3) vs. painful stimuli (VAS = 7)) did not produce any significant voxels.

Table 4. Activation changes to painful and non-painful stimulation (N = 23).
Contrast Cluster size (voxels) Region Side Peak T-value MNI coordinates
x y z
Pain > No pain 1138 Vermis 8 L 7.00 -3 -64 -28
Cerebellum_6 R 6.03 33 -49 -31
Cerebellum_6 R 6.00 30 -55 -25
206 Supramarginal gyrus L 5.59 -63 -28 29
Postcentral gyrus L 5.50 -57 -19 17
Insula L 4.94 -39 -16 8
366 Middle temporal gyrus R 5.37 45 -43 -1
Rolandic operculum R 5.01 60 -1 5
Heschl gyrus R 4.84 42 -22 11

Note: cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05. R: right; L: left.

Regression analyses with migraine frequency and cortisol/DHEAS ratio. Migraine frequency was correlated with activation in the caudate, and midcingulate to pain cue vs. no-pain cue (see Table 5 and Fig 3), but was unrelated to pain perception (this analysis did not produce any significant voxels).

Fig 3. The association between pain anticipation and migraine frequency.

Fig 3

Significantly activated cluster (caudate on the left and midcingulate on the right) is shown at pFWE < 0.05. Statistical map was visualized on the MNI 152 template brain provided in MRIcroGL (http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricrogl/) [58]. Coordinates are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.

Using the mask of mPFC by de la Vega and coworkers [52] we tested whether pre-scan cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio related to pain anticipation and perception. We found that cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio was associated with activation in the right supplementary motor area (peak MNI coordinates x/y/z = 3/14/50; t(1, 20) = 5.32 SVC, pFWE = 0.041) to painful vs. non-painful stimuli positively, but was unrelated to pain anticipation (this analysis did not produce any significant voxels). Since the cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio correlated negatively with BMI, we repeated our analyses while controlling for BMI, but the results were unchanged.

Discussion

Previous fMRI studies on pain processing in migraine focused primarily on pain perception, demonstrating an altered response to acute painful stimuli and its relationship with some clinical variables (e.g. migraine frequency). In our study we aimed to disentangle anticipation and perception processes of painful stimuli and examine how inter-individual differences in migraine frequency and pre-scan neurobiological stress response would relate to these two phases.

Repeated (or more frequent) attacks have been considered as a potent migraine-related factor leading to allostatic load [16, 59]. Regarding pre-scan neurobiological stress response, the cortisol-to-DHEAS ratio to the first time exposure to fMRI scanner environment was calculated. We considered that this ratio reflects inter-individual differences in stress responsivity to a new and potentially stressful situation. Thus, the neural correlates of both indices might help to elucidate how or why allostatic load induced by repeated attacks or stressors contribute to changes in brain states. Using a classical fear conditioning task, we found that migraine frequency was associated with enhanced pain anticipation, whereas pre-scan neurobiological stress response was related to pain perception. We start our discussion with pain perception since most of the studies investigated this process.

Pain perception

Generally, several parts of the pain processing brain network [60] were activated. For instance, we found that the perception of painful stimuli vs. non-painful one was associated with widespread activation in the posterior part of the cerebellum and vermis, with peaks in the cerebellar lobule VI and posterior vermis (VIII lobule) in episodic migraine without aura. This elevated activity is in line with previous studies demonstrating cerebellar activation to painful stimuli [12, 61]. More specifically, functional connectivity of the cerebellum with the brainstem and higher cortical areas such as the insula during painful stimuli [61] suggest a modulatory role of cerebellum in pain processing [12]. In addition, overlapping activation in the cerebellum (including the lobule VI, Crus I and VIIb) to noxious heat and aversive pictures was found, leading to the notion that the cerebellum may play a role in encoding aversive stimuli in general [12]. In the context of migraine without aura, cerebellar activity to trigeminal nociceptive stimulation [6] along with microstructural alterations in the posterior part of the cerebellum [10, 62] supports the notion that cerebellum may contribute to the pathophysiology of migraine.

Next to cerebellar activity, we also found increased activity in two clusters: one cluster involving the left posterior insula, postcentral gyrus (including the primary somatosensory cortex, S1 Table) and supramarginal gyrus, and the other involving the Rolandic operculum, middle temporal gyrus and Heschl gyrus. According to a recent meta-analysis by Xu and colleagues [63] the insula, supramarginal gyrus and Rolandic operculum–along with the thalamus, secondary somatosensory cortex and MCC–are consistently activated to painful stimuli irrespective of the experimental paradigm used in healthy participants.

In our study, the insular activity to painful stimuli was located in the posterior subdivision which is considered to reflect the sensory-discriminative properties of pain [64]. The functional and structural connectivity of posterior insula (PI) with secondary somatosensory cortex further supports its involvement in sensory-discriminative processing [65]. Regarding the timing of pain response in the insula, it occurs first in the PI [66], then the anterior parts are also activated [66, 67] which is considered to play a role in the encoding of the emotional aspects of pain. In the present study we applied a short-duration pain and did not assess subjective experience of the painful stimuli on a trial-by-trial basis. This may explain why we did not find any activation in the anterior insula [68].

Due to the lack of a control group, we could only investigate within-group activations to painful stimulation, but the activated areas in our study have relevance in between-group comparisons as well, as previous studies show. Among female migraineurs without aura, altered structural and functional properties of the PI has been documented [69]. For instance, altered tactile sensory processing indexed by reduced habituation in posterior insula along with enhanced activation in brainstem to repeated blocks of innocuous ophthalmic nerve somatosensory stimulation was found in episodic migraine [70]. As a hub region of the brain, insula plays a role in information processing derived from the external or internal milieu, thus it has been proposed as one of the key regions in migraine pathophysiology [2] since altered sensory processing has been commonly reported in migraine [4].

Altered structural and functional properties of primary somatosensory cortex has also been detected in migraine [71]. Comparing groups, activation of S1 Table to noxious thermal stimuli was higher among high-frequency migraine compared to low-frequency migraine [11]. In addition, activation in S1 Table to fearful vs. neutral face also showed a positive relationship with migraine frequency [72]. Interestingly, in both mentioned studies the activation corresponded with head/face area of the sensory homunculus.

Contrary to previous findings, in our study migraine frequency did not relate to the perception of painful (threatening) stimulus. It might be due to methodological issues, since with our task we could disentangle anticipation and perception periods, while tasks in previous studies were not designed such a way.

Pain perception and pre-scan stress

Increased anabolic imbalance reflected in cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio to a new stressor may reflect individual vulnerability, therefore we expected that inter-individual differences in pre-scan stress reflected in higher cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio would be associated with the perception of painful stimuli. We restricted our analysis to the mPFC since it is a common site for stress [41], emotion [42] and pain regulation [43]. We found that pre-scan cortisol-to-DHEAS ratio was associated with activation in the right supplementary motor area to painful stimulus vs. non-painful one. Thus, generally, our finding supports the notion that cortisol-to-DHEA(S) ratio might be related to threat processing.

Activation of SMA has been demonstrated in many pain studies [68, 73], and although it is traditionally a motor area, its engagement in pain processing is often interpreted as reflecting motor/behaviour planning in response to pain [74]. Based on this, our result might indicate that those with higher cortisol-to-DHEA-S ratio are more ready to produce behavioural responses to painful stimuli (e.g. escape from the stimuli). The site of the activation corresponds with pre-SMA [75], which is presumably involved not just in motor responses, but in more complex processes, such as response inhibition [76] as well. Thus alternatively, our results might suggest that those who experience higher pre-scan stress indicated by higher cortisol-to-DHEA(S) ratio have to employ more resources to inhibit (automatic) motor responses to painful stimuli.

We did not find any significant associations between state anxiety and pre-scan cortisol level, similar to previous studies [77]. However, the relationship between them may be dependent on the type of the stressor. For instance, cortisol level before a sport competition and state anxiety correlated positively [78].

Pain anticipation

Given its aversive and salient nature, pain elicits fear, as do pain predictive cues [79] along with widespread brain activation [23]. However, previous studies on pain processing in migraine have rarely investigated anticipation processes. In the present study, the activation of occipital–visual–areas corresponds with our previous result using the same task [51], and might be due to the long duration of anticipatory cues [80].

In addition, contrasting pain cues versus no pain cues, we found activations in the midcingulate and SMA. Activation on both areas has been detected in the anticipation phase of pain [81], and in one study activation in SMA, for instance, correlated with the expected pain intensity positively [82]. In addition, a recent meta-analytic study found that MCC is part of the core network involved in aversive anticipation [80]. Increased reactivity in the MCC to aversive anticipatory cues in fMRI studies may reflect that this area has a role in preparing for the aversive stimulation and/or initiating avoidance or defensive reactions [83]. However, this activation is presumably not specific to pain or pain cues [84, 85], as the MCC, especially its anterior part, is involved in orienting to the emotional salience of information. In line with this, cortical hyperresponsivity towards sensory information in migraine [4] might be associated with greater anticipatory responses in the MCC. Though we could not test this directly, since we did not have a control group. However, it is worth mentioning that functional connectivity of the anterior MCC was associated with clinical variables–such as attack frequency, disease duration and pain intensity–in a previous study in migraine [86].

Amplified anticipation response is not rare in pain conditions (e.g. in fibromyalgia), or other neurological diseases (e.g. Parkinson disease [87]). Our findings highlight that investigating anticipation processes, and hence expectations, can be fruitful in migraine as well.

Pain anticipation and migraine frequency

We found that migraine frequency was associated with increased neural response to pain anticipation in a relatively large cluster involving the MCC along with the caudate. As mentioned in the previous section, increased reactivity in the MCC to aversive anticipatory cues in fMRI studies may reflect that this area has a role in preparing for the aversive stimulation and/or initiating avoidance or defensive reactions [83], but it also has a role in evaluating the salience of stimuli. Based on functional connectivity studies, the ventral caudate is also considered to be involved in affective processing [88]. Our results correspond with this notion, since anticipation of noxious stimuli (i.e. pain) has an indisputable biological significance, allowing preparation in responses to upcoming noxious stimuli [89].

It is worth mentioning, that the role of basal ganglia in pathophysiology of migraine has been proposed [90], supported by functional and structural alterations among migraineurs [11]. However, it is difficult to determine whether it is a correlate for progression of the illness, or it is a pre-morbid vulnerability factor.

Based on our findings, we may conclude that more frequent attacks are accompanied with increased preparatory responses to painful—or in more general threatening—stimuli, establishing another path for repeated headaches and stressors to exert their effects on brain in migraine.

Limitation

There are some limitations to our study. First, we did not have a control group, so we could not test between-group differences. However, our main aim was to investigate how inter-individual differences in migraine frequency and pre-scan stress are associated with pain anticipation and perception. Regarding migraine-related variables, we chose migraine frequency, since it has been associated with brain alteration [11, 14, 15] and chronification of episodic migraine [13]. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that migraine-related variables other than migraine frequency had an association with anticipatory processes. For instance, previous findings showed that monetary loss anticipation was associated with the time of the last attack [91]. In addition, migraine cycle can also alter pain processing. For instance, brainstem activation was modulated by migraine cycle, resulting activation differences in inter-ictal, pre-ictal and post-ictal groups [9]. In our study, we scanned our participants in the inter-ictal phase, thus subsequent studies should provide additional data on pain anticipation across migraine cycle.

We did not measure cutaneous allodynia reflecting central sensitization processes, which may have important implications for pain processing. For instance, migraine-related allodynia is associated with resting state functional connectivity of areas involved in descending pain modulation (e.g. PAG) [92]. Interestingly, Schwedt and colleagues [93] found that based on structural brain characteristics, there are two subgroups of patients with migraine, differing on allodynia, duration of disease and disability scores, pointing out that identification of subgroups on meaningful characteristics is warranted to provide more tailored therapies or interventions.

We used electrical stimulation to trigger pain, which activates not just nociceptive, but non-nociceptive afferents as well. But according to a recent EEG study, similarly to laser evoked stimulation, electric simulation is also suitable to investigate expectation-related pain processing [94]. However, future studies using painful stimulus in different pain modalities as well as non-painful aversive stimuli will help to characterize anticipatory processes in migraine. Regarding our pain task, other methodological issues could be also relevant. For instance, unlike many studies [18, 95] we did not ask our participants to rate the stimuli delivered to their hand, and a long period of rest was used, which might affect our results. Due to the task parameters (i.e. pain stimuli occurred immediately after the offset of cues), we could not test how predictions (anticipatory cues) affect pain perception. Though it is known that threat prediction can bias pain perception [18, 19], it would be interesting to see whether migraineurs are more biased towards prediction than towards nociceptive inputs.

Detection of mismatch between the anticipated/expected and experienced stimuli generates prediction error signals that makes possible to the precision of our expectation based on the incoming signals/inputs. In the context of pain, the activity of anterior insula, especially its ventral part [96], using computational modelling, was best explained if the prediction error component was included in the model [97]. In tasks using omitted pain vs. pain contrast representing receiving lower-than-expected pain, recruitment of the ACC, especially its rostral part was found [98]. Though we had five trials in which pain cue was followed by non-painful stimulus, called omitted pain trials, we did not analyse omitted pain vs. pain contrast, since we think that this contrast would deserve between-group comparison.

We did not have the possibility to see how cortisol, DHEA-S or their ratio changed from pre-scan to post-scan, however it would have been informative even for pain processing. For instance, in one study, those who had the largest reactive cortisol response to the pain task rated the pain less unpleasant, and showed smaller activity in several areas including the posterior insula, primary somatosensory cortex, anterior MCC, and nucleus accumbens [99].

Last, but not least our sample size may limit the generalizability of our findings, and might contribute to the lack of common brain activations to pain stimulus in the thalamus and ACC [63, 100].

Conclusion

Based on our finding that migraine frequency is related to enhanced anticipation in areas involved in preparatory response, we may conclude that this is another path through which repeated headaches and/or stressors may create an allostatic load on the brain and the body in migraine. Additionally, expectations about upcoming migraine attacks, or generally, stressors in daily life, may have important implications for quality of life in migraine. In line with this, further studies should focus on characterizing anticipatory processes and expectations in migraine with different methods, and their possible involvement in chronification of migraine with prospective studies. Our results may also highlight that decreasing migraine frequency either pharmacologically or with other methods (e.g. with psychological treatment [101]) could be important to minimize allostatic load resulting from frequent attacks and related phenomena. Importantly, our neuroimaging findings on pre-scan biological stress response and pain perception also underlines preparatory responses in migraine. Overall, exaggerated preparedness on a biological and/or a behavioural level may contribute to the allostatic load [102] caused by stressors and migraine attacks on the brain in migraine [16].

Supporting information

S1 Table. Spearman correlations of anxiety scores with age, education level and clinical characteristics (N = 23).

*p<0.05; 1point biserial correlation.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Spearman correlations of pre-scan raw cortisol, DHEAS and cortisol-to-DHEAS with age, anxiety scores, clinical variables, menstrual phase and hormonal contraception taking (N = 23).

*p<0.05; 1point biserial correlation.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Activation changes during pain anticipation if state anxiety is controlled for (N = 23).

Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left; SMA, Supplementary motor area.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Activation changes during pain anticipation if trait anxiety is controlled for (N = 23).

Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left; SMA, Supplementary motor area.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Activation changes during pain anticipation if gender is controlled for (N = 23).

Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left; SMA, Supplementary motor area.

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Activation changes to painful and non-painful stimulation if state anxiety was controlled for (N = 23).

Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left.

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Activation changes to painful and non-painful stimulation if gender was controlled for (N = 23).

Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left.

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Activation changes during pain anticipation controlled for gender, state and trait anxiety (N = 23).

Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left; SMA, Supplementary motor area.

(DOCX)

S9 Table. Activation changes to painful and non-painful stimulation controlled for gender, state and trait anxiety (N = 23).

Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The fMRI study was conducted as a groupwork at MR Research Center, Semmelweis University. The authors thank Ádám György Békésy-Szabó, Krisztina Oláh Koósné, István Kóbor, Márk Folyovich, Krisztina Kovács, Zita Nagy, Zsuzsanna Tóth, Terézia Zsombók, Máté Magyar and Éva Csépány for their contribution and József Edőcs for the assistance of programming Digitimer boxes.

Data Availability

All data underlying the findings described in the paper, i.e. self-report data, raw cortisol and DHEA-S data along with main fMRI contrast maps, are fully available at https://osf.io/g9vph/. Only raw imaging dataset cannot be shared publicly, because at the time our study started, there was no information on open access data availability in the consent forms (the study was approved by the Scientific and Research Ethics Committee of the Medical Research Council (Hungary)), therefore authors are not allowed to share raw imaging data publicly, since participants were not able to accept or refuse their assent to share imaging data in an open access repository. However, raw imaging data are available from the corresponding author (Gyöngyi Kökönyei, kokonyei.gyongyi@ppk.elte.hu) or from the Department of Pharmacodynamics, Faculty of Pharmacy, Semmelweis University (titkarsag.gyhat@pharma.semmelweis-univ.hu) on reasonable request.

Funding Statement

The study was supported by the MTA-SE-NAP B Genetic Brain Imaging Migraine Research Group, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Semmelweis University (Grant No. KTIA_NAP_13-2-2015-0001) to GJ; Hungarian Brain Research Programe (Grant No. 2017-1.2.1-NKP-2017-00002) to GJ and the Hungarian Academy of Sciences (MTA-SE Neuropsychopharmacology and Neurochemistry Research Group) To GB. The preparation of this work was supported by the Thematic Excellence Programme (Tématerületi Kiválósági Program, 2020-4.1.1.-TKP2020) of the Ministry for Innovation and Technology in Hungary, within the framework of the Neurology and Translational Biotechnology thematic programmes of the Semmelweis University. LRK was supported by the Bolyai Research Fellowship Program of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences. The preparation of this article for GK was supported by the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office (FK128614). GJ was supported by the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office, Hungary (2019-2.1.7-ERA-NET-2020-00005), under the frame of ERA PerMed (ERAPERMED2019-108). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Steiner TJ, Stovner LJ, Vos T, Jensen R, Katsarava Z. Migraine is first cause of disability in under 50s: will health politicians now take notice? Journal of Headache and Pain. 2018;19. doi: 10.1186/s10194-018-0846-2 WOS:000426377300001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Borsook D, Veggeberg R, Erpelding N, Borra R, Linnman C, Burstein R, et al. The Insula: A "Hub of Activity" in Migraine. Neuroscientist. 2016;22(6):632–52. doi: 10.1177/1073858415601369 WOS:000387313300011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Coppola G, Parisi V, Di Renzo A, Pierelli F. Cortical pain processing in migraine. Journal of Neural Transmission. 2020;127(4):551–66. doi: 10.1007/s00702-019-02089-7 WOS:000489289000002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Schwedt TJ, Chong CD. Functional imaging and migraine: new connections? Current Opinion in Neurology. 2015;28(3):265–70. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000194 WOS:000354067500010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Schwedt TJ, Chong CD, Chiang CC, Baxter L, Schlaggar BL, Dodick DW. Enhanced pain-induced activity of pain-processing regions in a case-control study of episodic migraine. Cephalalgia. 2014;34(12):947–58. Epub 2014/03/15. doi: 10.1177/0333102414526069 [pii] ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4163130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Russo A, Tessitore A, Esposito F, Marcuccio L, Giordano A, Conforti R, et al. Pain processing in patients with migraine: an event-related fMRI study during trigeminal nociceptive stimulation. Journal of Neurology. 2012;259(9):1903–12. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6438-1 WOS:000308327300018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Aderjan D, Stankewitz A, May A. Neuronal mechanisms during repetitive trigemino-nociceptive stimulation in migraine patients. Pain. 2010;151(1):97–103. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.06.024 WOS:000281733300018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mathur VA, Moayedi M, Keaser ML, Khan SA, Hubbard CS, Goyal M, et al. High Frequency Migraine Is Associated with Lower Acute Pain Sensitivity and Abnormal Insula Activity Related to Migraine Pain Intensity, Attack Frequency, and Pain Catastrophizing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2016;10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00010 WOS:000384353500002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Marciszewski KK, Meylakh N, Di Pietro F, Mills EP, Macefield VG, Macey PM, et al. Changes in Brainstem Pain Modulation Circuitry Function over the Migraine Cycle. Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38(49):10479–88. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1088-18.2018 WOS:000452156200008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mehnert J, May A. Functional and structural alterations in the migraine cerebellum. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism. 2019;39(4):730–9. doi: 10.1177/0271678X17722109 WOS:000463032400012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Maleki N, Becerra L, Nutile L, Pendse G, Brawn J, Bigal M, et al. Migraine attacks the Basal Ganglia. Molecular Pain. 2011;7. doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-7-7 WOS:000295833000001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Moulton EA, Elman I, Pendse G, Schmahmann J, Becerra L, Borsook D. Aversion-related circuitry in the cerebellum: responses to noxious heat and unpleasant images. J Neurosci. 2011;31(10):3795–804. Epub 2011/03/11. 31/10/3795 [pii] 3679178 [pii] doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6709-10.2011 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3063442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Buse DC, Greisman JD, Baigi K, Lipton RB. Migraine Progression: A Systematic Review. Headache. 2019;59(3):306–38. doi: 10.1111/head.13459 WOS:305890908200003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Jia ZH, Yu SY. Grey matter alterations in migraine: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroimage-Clinical. 2017;14:130–40. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.019 WOS:000405984300014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Yu ZB, Lv YB, Song LH, Liu DH, Huang XL, Hu XY, et al. Functional Connectivity Differences in the Insular Sub-regions in Migraine without Aura: A Resting-State Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Front Behav Neurosci. 2017;11:124. Epub 2017/07/14. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00124 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5487515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Borsook D, Maleki N, Becerra L, McEwen B. Understanding Migraine through the Lens of Maladaptive Stress Responses: A Model Disease of Allostatic Load. Neuron. 2012;73(2):219–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.001 WOS:000299761600005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Russo A, Tessitore A, Silvestro M, Di Nardo F, Trojsi F, Del Santo T, et al. Advanced visual network and cerebellar hyperresponsiveness to trigeminal nociception in migraine with aura. Journal of Headache and Pain. 2019;20. doi: 10.1186/s10194-019-1002-3 WOS:000466972700001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Atlas LY, Bolger N, Lindquist MA, Wager TD. Brain Mediators of Predictive Cue Effects on Perceived Pain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(39):12964–77. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0057-10.2010 WOS:000282571800008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Jepma M, Koban L, van Doorn J, Jones M, Wager TD. Behavioural and neural evidence for self-reinforcing expectancy effects on pain. Nature Human Behaviour. 2018;2(11):838–55. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0455-8 WOS:000449539900015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Atlas LY, Wager TD. How expectations shape pain. Neuroscience Letters. 2012;520(2):140–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.039 WOS:000306162800003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Barrett LF, Simmons WK. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015;16(7):419–29. doi: 10.1038/nrn3950 WOS:000356585900009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Brown CA, Seymour B, Boyle Y, El-Deredy W, Jones AKP. Modulation of pain ratings by expectation and uncertainty: Behavioral characteristics and anticipatory neural correlates. Pain. 2008;135(3):240–50. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.05.022 WOS:000254912700006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Palermo S, Benedetti F, Costa T, Amanzio M. Pain Anticipation: An Activation Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis of Brain Imaging Studies. Human Brain Mapping. 2015;36(5):1648–61. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22727 WOS:000353068300002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Brown CA, El-Deredy W, Jones AKP. When the brain expects pain: common neural responses to pain anticipation are related to clinical pain and distress in fibromyalgia and osteoarthritis. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;39(4):663–72. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12420 WOS:242195878500014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Linnman C, Catana C, Petkov MP, Chonde DB, Becerra L, Hooker J, et al. Molecular and functional PET-fMRI measures of placebo analgesia in episodic migraine: Preliminary findings. Neuroimage-Clinical. 2018;17:680–90. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.11.011 WOS:000426180300072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Lev R, Granovsky Y, Yarnitsky D. Enhanced Pain Expectation in Migraine: EEG-Based Evidence for Impaired Prefrontal Function. Headache. 2013;53(7):1054–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2012.02297.x WOS:000321959600003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Gossett EW, Wheelock MD, Goodman AM, Orem TR, Harnett NG, Wood KH, et al. Anticipatory stress associated with functional magnetic resonance imaging: Implications for psychosocial stress research. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2018;125:35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.02.005 WOS:000428606300005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Peters S, Cleare AJ, Papadopoulos A, Fu CHY. Cortisol responses to serial MRI scans in healthy adults and in depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36(5):737–41. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.10.009 WOS:000291190400014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Weldon AL, Hagan M, Van Meter A, Jacobs RH, Kassel MT, Hazlett KE, et al. Stress Response to the Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Environment in Healthy Adults Relates to the Degree of Limbic Reactivity during Emotion Processing. Neuropsychobiology. 2015;71(2):85–96. doi: 10.1159/000369027 WOS:000353721600004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Morgan CA, Rasmusson A, Pietrzak RH, Coric V, Southwick SM. Relationships Among Plasma Dehydroepiandrosterone and Dehydroepiandrosterone Sulfate, Cortisol, Symptoms of Dissociation, and Objective Performance in Humans Exposed to Underwater Navigation Stress. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66(4):334–40. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.004 WOS:000268840200007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Jiang XL, Zhong W, An HY, Fu MY, Chen YY, Zhang ZG, et al. Attenuated DHEA and DHEA-S response to acute psychosocial stress in individuals with depressive disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2017;215:118–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.03.013 WOS:283196883300017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Maninger N, Wolkowitz OM, Reus VI, Epel ES, Mellon SH. Neurobiological and neuropsychiatric effects of dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and DHEA sulfate (DHEAS). Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2009;30(1):65–91. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.11.002 WOS:000262775800005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wolf OT, Koster B, Kirschbaum C, Pietrowsky R, Kern W, Hellhammer DH, et al. A single administration of dehydroepiandrosterone does not enhance memory performance in young healthy adults, but immediately reduces cortisol levels. Biological Psychiatry. 1997;42(9):845–8. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00323-5 WOS:A1997YC16100013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Morgan CA, Southwick S, Hazlett G, Rasmusson A, Hoyt G, Zimolo Z, et al. Relationships among plasma dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate and cortisol levels, symptoms of dissociation, and objective performance in humans exposed to acute stress. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61(8):819–25. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.819 WOS:000223140400007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.do Vale S, Escera C. Dehydroepiandrosterone and Dehydroepiandrosterone-Sulfate and Emotional Processing. Dehydroepiandrosterone. 2018;108:413–41. doi: 10.1016/bs.vh.2018.01.022 WOS:000452381500017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Sripada RK, Marx CE, King AP, Rajaram N, Garfinkel SN, Abelson JL, et al. DHEA Enhances Emotion Regulation Neurocircuits and Modulates Memory for Emotional Stimuli. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38(9):1798–807. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.79 WOS:000321823400020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kamin HS, Kertes DA. Cortisol and DHEA in development and psychopathology. Hormones and Behavior. 2017;89:69–85. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.11.018 WOS:000398007000009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Phillips AC, Carroll D, Gale CR, Lord JM, Arlt W, Batty GD. Cortisol, DHEA sulphate, their ratio, and all-cause and cause-specific mortality in the Vietnam Experience Study. European Journal of Endocrinology. 2010;163(2):285–92. doi: 10.1530/EJE-10-0299 WOS:204981394800015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Mocking RJT, Pellikaan CM, Lok A, Assies J, Ruhe HG, Koeter MW, et al. DHEAS and cortisol/DHEAS-ratio in recurrent depression: State, or trait predicting 10-year recurrence? Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;59:91–101. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.05.006 WOS:000358099100009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Grillon C, Pine DS, Baas JMP, Lawley M, Ellis V, Charney DS. Cortisol and DHEA-S are associated with startle potentiation during aversive conditioning in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2006;186(3):434–41. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0124-2 WOS:000237857000017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.McKlveen JM, Myers B, Herman JP. The medial prefrontal cortex: coordinator of autonomic, neuroendocrine and behavioural responses to stress. J Neuroendocrinol. 2015;27(6):446–56. Epub 2015/03/05. doi: 10.1111/jne.12272 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4580281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15(2):85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004 WOS:000287295500005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ong W-Y, Stohler CS, Herr DR. Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Pain Processing. Molecular Neurobiology. 2019;56(2):1137–66. doi: 10.1007/s12035-018-1130-9 Ong2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.(IHS) HCCotIHS. The international classification of headache disorders, 3rd edition (beta version). Cephalalgia; 2013. p. 629–808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1998;59:22–33. WOS:000077584300005. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sollberger S, Ehlert U. How to use and interpret hormone ratios. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;63:385–97. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.09.031 WOS:000367422400049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Weitzman ED, Fukushima D, Nogeire C, Roffwarg H, Gallagher TF, Hellman L. Twenty-four hour pattern of the episodic secretion of cortisol in normal subjects. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1971;33(1):14–22. Epub 1971/07/01. doi: 10.1210/jcem-33-1-14 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Laudenslager ML, Calderone J, Philips S, Natvig C, Carlson NE. Diurnal patterns of salivary cortisol and DHEA using a novel collection device: Electronic monitoring confirms accurate recording of collection time using this device. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(9):1596–606. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.01.006 WOS:000325188300014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg P, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Spoormaker VI, Andrade KC, Schroter MS, Sturm A, Goya-Maldonado R, Samann PG, et al. The neural correlates of negative prediction error signaling in human fear conditioning. Neuroimage. 2011;54(3):2250–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.042 WOS:000286302000047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kokonyei G, Galambos A, Edes AE, Kocsel N, Szabo E, Pap D, et al. Anticipation and violated expectation of pain are influenced by trait rumination: An fMRI study. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2019;19(1):56–72. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-0644-y WOS:000456550100003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.de la Vega A, Chang LJ, Banich MT, Wager TD, Yarkoni T. Large-Scale Meta-Analysis of Human Medial Frontal Cortex Reveals Tripartite Functional Organization. J Neurosci. 2016;36(24):6553–62. Epub 2016/06/17. 36/24/6553 [pii] 4402–15 [pii] doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4402-15.2016 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5015787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2016;113(28):7900–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113 WOS:000379694100060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15(1):273–89. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978 WOS:000173174900027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Grupe DW, Nitschke JB. Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14(7):488–501. doi: 10.1038/nrn3524 WOS:000321044900011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Tang J, Gibson SJ. A psychophysical evaluation of the relationship between trait anxiety, pain perception, and induced state anxiety. Journal of Pain. 2005;6(9):612–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.03.009 WOS:000232068000006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kaunisto MA, Jokela R, Tallgren M, Kambur O, Tikkanen E, Tasmuth T, et al. Pain in 1,000 Women Treated for Breast Cancer A Prospective Study of Pain Sensitivity and Postoperative Pain. Anesthesiology. 2013;119(6):1410–21. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000012 WOS:000330528300021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Rorden C, Brett M. Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behav Neurol. 2000;12(4):191–200. Epub 2001/09/25. doi: 10.1155/2000/421719 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Maleki N, Becerra L, Borsook D. Migraine: Maladaptive Brain Responses to Stress. Headache. 2012;52:102–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2012.02241.x WOS:000309447000011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Atlas LY, Lindquist MA, Bolger N, Wager TD. Brain mediators of the effects of noxious heat on pain. Pain. 2014;155(8):1632–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.05.015 WOS:000340309800027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Mehnert J, Schulte L, Timmann D, May A. Activity and connectivity of the cerebellum in trigeminal nociception. Neuroimage. 2017;150:112–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.023 WOS:000399855800010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Qin ZX, He XW, Zhang JL, Xu S, Li GF, Su JJ, et al. Structural changes of cerebellum and brainstem in migraine without aura. Journal of Headache and Pain. 2019;20(1). doi: 10.1186/s10194-019-1045-5 WOS:000483542300001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Xu A, Larsen B, Baller EB, Scott JC, Sharma V, Adebimpe A, et al. Convergent neural representations of experimentally-induced acute pain in healthy volunteers: A large-scale fMRI meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2020;112:300–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.004 WOS:000531016100019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Albanese MC, Duerden EG, Rainville P, Duncan GH. Memory traces of pain in human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(17):4612–20. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0695-07.2007 WOS:000246293900014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Wiech K, Jbabdi S, Lin CS, Andersson J, Tracey I. Differential structural and resting state connectivity between insular subdivisions and other pain-related brain regions. Pain. 2014;155(10):2047–55. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.07.009 WOS:000343831300022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Frot M, Faillenot I, Mauguiere F. Processing of Nociceptive Input From Posterior to Anterior Insula in Humans. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35(11):5486–99. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22565 WOS:000344176100012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Bastuji H, Frot M, Perchet C, Hagiwara K, Garcia-Larrea L. Convergence of sensory and limbic noxious input into the anterior insula and the emergence of pain from nociception. Scientific Reports. 2018;8. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18329-3 WOS:000443802200021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Wilcox CE, Mayer AR, Teshiba TM, Ling J, Smith BW, Wilcox GL, et al. The Subjective Experience of Pain: An FMRI Study of Percept-Related Models and Functional Connectivity. Pain Medicine. 2015;16(11):2121–33. doi: 10.1111/pme.12785 WOS:000368296600009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Zhang JL, Su JJ, Wang MX, Zhao Y, Zhang QT, Yao Q, et al. The Posterior Insula Shows Disrupted Brain Functional Connectivity in Female Migraineurs Without Aura Based on Brainnetome Atlas. Scientific Reports. 2017;7. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-00035-9 WOS:000417025400049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Lee J, Lin RL, Garcia RG, Kim J, Kim H, Loggia ML, et al. Reduced insula habituation associated with amplification of trigeminal brainstem input in migraine. Cephalalgia. 2017;37(11):1026–38. Epub 2016/08/16. 0333102416665223 [pii] doi: 10.1177/0333102416665223 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Maleki N, Becerra L, Brawn J, Bigal M, Burstein R, Borsook D. Concurrent functional and structural cortical alterations in migraine. Cephalalgia. 2012;32(8):607–20. Epub 2012/05/25. 0333102412445622 [pii] doi: 10.1177/0333102412445622 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3846436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Szabo E, Galambos A, Kocsel N, Edes AE, Pap D, Zsombok T, et al. Association between migraine frequency and neural response to emotional faces: An fMRI study. Neuroimage-Clinical. 2019;22. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101790 WOS:000470123000054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Wager TD, Atlas LY, Lindquist MA, Roy M, Woo CW, Kross E. An fMRI-Based Neurologic Signature of Physical Pain. New England Journal of Medicine. 2013;368(15):1388–97. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471 WOS:000317333600006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Misra G, Coombes SA. Neuroimaging Evidence of Motor Control and Pain Processing in the Human Midcingulate Cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25(7):1906–19. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu001 WOS:000359319700021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Ruan JH, Bludau S, Palomero-Gallagher N, Caspers S, Mohlberg H, Eickhoff SB, et al. Cytoarchitecture, probability maps, and functions of the human supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Brain Structure & Function. 2018;223(9):4169–86. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1738-6 WOS:000451061700016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Obeso I, Robles N, Marron EM, Redolar-Ripoll D. Dissociating the role of the pre-SMA in response inhibition and switching: a combined online and offline TMS approach. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00007 WOS:000317904600001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Leininger S, Skeel R. Cortisol and Self-report Measures of Anxiety as Predictors of Neuropsychological Performance. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2012;27(3):318–28. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acs035 WOS:000303158400007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Cintineo HP, Arent SM. Anticipatory Salivary Cortisol and State Anxiety Before Competition Predict Match Outcome in Division I Collegiate Wrestlers. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research. 2019;33(11):2905–8. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000003376 WOS:000505958500002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Berns GS, Chappelow J, Cekic M, Zink CF, Pagnoni G, Martin-Skurski ME. Neurobiological substrates of dread. Science. 2006;312(5774):754–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1123721 WOS:000237312100053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Andrzejewski JA, Greenberg T, Carlson JM. Neural correlates of aversive anticipation: An activation likelihood estimate meta-analysis across multiple sensory modalities. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2019;19(6):1379–90. doi: 10.3758/s13415-019-00747-7 WOS:000511620000003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Seidel EM, Pfabigan DM, Hahn A, Sladky R, Grahl A, Paul K, et al. Uncertainty During Pain Anticipation: The Adaptive Value of Preparatory Processes. Human Brain Mapping. 2015;36(2):744–55. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22661 WOS:000348378800026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Koyama T, McHaffie JG, Laurienti PJ, Coghill RC. The subjective experience of pain: Where expectations become reality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102(36):12950–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0408576102 WOS:000231716700055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Vogt BA. Midcingulate cortex: Structure, connections, homologies, functions and diseases. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy. 2016;74:28–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2016.01.010 WOS:000378448900006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Shackman AJ, Salomons TV, Slagter HA, Fox AS, Winter JJ, Davidson RJ. The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12(3):154–67. doi: 10.1038/nrn2994 WOS:000287474900003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Silvestrini N, Chen JI, Piche M, Roy M, Vachon-Presseau E, Woo CW, et al. Distinct fMRI patterns colocalized in the cingulate cortex underlie the after-effects of cognitive control on pain. Neuroimage. 2020;217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116898 WOS:000542369500001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Hubbard C, Khan S, Keaser M, Goyal M, Seminowicz D. Gray matter abnormalities in migraine patients associated with disease chronicity, attack frequency, and intensity of-migraine pain. Journal of Pain. 2014;15(4):S29–S. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2014.01.121 WOS:000334648700115. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Martin SL, Jones AKP, Brown CA, Kobylecki C, Silverdale MA. A neurophysiological investigation of anticipation to pain in Parkinson’s disease. Eur J Neurosci. 2020;51(2):611–27. Epub 2019/08/26. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14559 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Huang HQ, Nguyen PT, Schwab NA, Tanner JJ, Price CC, Ding MZ. Mapping Dorsal and Ventral Caudate in Older Adults: Method and Validation. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2017;9. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00009 WOS:000398234700002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Meulders A. Fear in the context of pain: Lessons learned from 100 years of fear conditioning research. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2020;131. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2020.103635 WOS:000541091400005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Borsook D, Upadhyay J, Chudler EH, Becerra L. A key role of the basal ganglia in pain and analgesia—insights gained through human functional imaging. Molecular Pain. 2010;6. doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-6-27 WOS:000279283700001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Kocsel N, Galambos A, Szabo E, Edes AE, Magyar M, Zsombok T, et al. Altered neural activity to monetary reward/loss processing in episodic migraine. Scientific Reports. 2019;9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-36956-2 WOS:000462864700026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Schwedt TJ, Larson-Prior L, Coalson RS, Nolan T, Mar S, Ances BM, et al. Allodynia and Descending Pain Modulation in Migraine: A Resting State Functional Connectivity Analysis. Pain Medicine. 2014;15(1):154–65. doi: 10.1111/pme.12267 WOS:000329756400015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Schwedt TJ, Si B, Li J, Wu T, Chong CD. Migraine Subclassification via a Data-Driven Automated Approach Using Multimodality Factor Mixture Modeling of Brain Structure Measurements. Headache. 2017;57(7):1051–64. doi: 10.1111/head.13121 WOS:000406886400004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Hird EJ, Jones AKP, Talmi D, El-Deredy W. A comparison between the neural correlates of laser and electric pain stimulation and their modulation by expectation. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2018;293:117–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.09.011 WOS:000418211200014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Wiech K, Lin CS, Brodersen KH, Bingel U, Ploner M, Tracey I. Anterior Insula Integrates Information about Salience into Perceptual Decisions about Pain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(48):16324–31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2087-10.2010 WOS:000284999900026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Fazeli S, Buchel C. Pain-Related Expectation and Prediction Error Signals in the Anterior Insula Are Not Related to Aversiveness. Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38(29):6461–74. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0671-18.2018 WOS:000439698400005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Geuter S, Boll S, Eippert F, Buchel C. Functional dissociation of stimulus intensity encoding and predictive coding of pain in the insula. Elife. 2017;6. doi: 10.7554/eLife.24770 WOS:000403413100001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Shih YW, Tsai HY, Lin FS, Lin YH, Chiang CY, Lu ZL, et al. Effects of Positive and Negative Expectations on Human Pain Perception Engage Separate But Interrelated and Dependently Regulated Cerebral Mechanisms. Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;39(7):1261–74. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2154-18.2018 WOS:000458499400011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Vachon-Presseau E, Martel MO, Roy M, Caron E, Albouy G, Marin MF, et al. Acute Stress Contributes to Individual Differences in Pain and Pain-Related Brain Activity in Healthy and Chronic Pain Patients. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(16):6826–33. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4584-12.2013 WOS:000317723000014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Jensen KB, Regenbogen C, Ohse MC, Frasnelli J, Freiherr J, Lundstrom JN. Brain activations during pain: a neuroimaging meta-analysis of patients with pain and healthy controls. Pain. 2016;157(6):1279–86. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000517 WOS:000378876300014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Zsombok T, Juhasz G, Budavari A, Vitrai J, Bagdy G. Effect of autogenic training on drug consumption in patients with primary headache: an 8-month follow-up study. Headache. 2003;43(3):251–7. Epub 2003/02/27. hed03049 [pii] doi: 10.1046/j.1526-4610.2003.03049.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease—Allostasis and allostatic load. Neuroimmunomodulation: Molecular Aspects, Integrative Systems, and Clinical Advances. 1998;840:33–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x WOS:000074444500004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Mukesh Dhamala

6 Oct 2021

PONE-D-21-27604Inter-individual differences in pain anticipation and pain perception in migraine: neural correlates of migraine frequency and cortisol-to-dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEA-S) ratioPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Gyöngyi Kökönyei,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Nov 20, 2021. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Mukesh Dhamala, Ph. D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. 

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially identifying or sensitive patient information) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. Please see http://www.bmj.com/content/340/bmj.c181.long for guidelines on how to de-identify and prepare clinical data for publication. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

3.  Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

Additional Editor Comments:

As you can see, the manuscript was reviewed by one expert and was assessed for a minor revision. We ask the authors to address the critiques of the reviewer and revise the manuscript accordingly.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Summary: In this manuscript, authors used fear conditioning fMRI task to study the association of migraine frequency and neurobiological stress response with neural activation in two specific phases i.e., pain anticipation and pain perception. Authors found that migraine frequency was associated with pain anticipation in a cluster comprising the midcingulate and caudate, whereas pre-scan neurobiological stress response was related to pain perception in the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA). Overall, this is an interesting study with very nicely written Introduction and Discussion sections, detailed Methods, and well-organized Results. Biggest limitation is the small sample size of only 23 participants. I have few major and mostly minor concerns and suggestions related to this manuscript as pointed below:

Abstract:

- It would make more sense to define DHEA-S as an index of neurobiological stress response at the start of the abstract when it was first mentioned rather in the results.

- It’s not clear what do the authors mean by “one cluster” comprising the midcingulate and caudate – given that midcingulate is a cortical whereas caudate is a sub-cortical region. It’s not clear how both these regions lead to one comprised cluster?

- “Pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) region” should be changed to just “pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA)”.

- Last sentence needs revision – “migraine attacks on the brain in migraine” is confusing.

Keywords:

- I would avoid using the keywords that are already present in the title. Instead, authors should use other keywords to broaden the search criteria of their paper.

Introduction:

- Introduction is very well written with detailed overview of literature and very clear hypotheses and predictions.

Methods:

- Line 158: “Right-handed subjects with migraine between 18–38 years” needs revision. It appears the subjects had migraine between 18 and 38, rather I would suggest mentioning sample size, mean and SD of age, age range, and male/female details here – for the full initial sample.

- Line 167: “three by technical reasons” should be “three due to technical reasons”, and any brief details about the technical reasons would be helpful.

- Line 171: Authors mentioned that participants did not have migraine attacks 48 h prior and 24 h after the scan. Was this by chance or this was one of the inclusion criteria, and for 24 h after the scan, did the authors follow-up with participants? These details are missing here.

- Lines 296-297: “Whole-brain analysis…” needs revision.

Results:

- Line 351: It’s confusing when authors mention three clusters but then report eight clusters. Similarly, lines 364, 369, 422-425, and Table 5. This issue needs to be fixed throughout the manuscript.

Discussion: Very detailed and nicely written.

General comments/questions and suggestions:

- Why did the authors prefer to report Spearman correlations rather than Pearson’s?

- Did the authors perform quality check on outliers while performing correlation analysis?

- Authors report supplementary data with state anxiety, trait anxiety, and gender as controlled variables but individually. Why didn’t author use all these three variables together as a set of covariates instead of using those one-by-one?

- Suggestion for authors for their future work is to use this data and explore if structural data (e.g., morphometry parameters such as cortical/sub-cortical volume) and brain connectivity measures (e.g., directed functional and effective) have any association with migraine frequency, stress response parameters and other behavioral data, if available, from this project.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Dec 20;16(12):e0261570. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0261570.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


28 Nov 2021

Dear Dr. Mukesh Dhamala,

We thank you that you are willing to consider a revision. We would like to take the opportunity and resubmit the revised version of our manuscript, entitled “Inter-individual differences in pain anticipation and pain perception in migraine: neural correlates of migraine frequency and cortisol-to-dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEA-S) ratio”.

Regarding data availability, we would like to complete our previous statement. We can share the self-report data and raw cortisol and DHEA-S values along with main main fMRI contrast maps, however raw imaging data cannot be shared publicly, but request can be sent to our department as well. “Self-report data, raw cortisol and DHEA-S data along with main fMRI contrast maps are available at https://osf.io/g9vph/. Raw imaging dataset cannot be shared publicly, because at the time our study started, there was no information on open access data availability in the consent forms (the study was approved by the Scientific and Research Ethics Committee of the Medical Research Council (Hungary)), therefore authors are not allowed to share raw imaging data publicly, since participants were not able to accept or refuse their assent to share imaging data in an open access repository. However, raw imaging data are available from the corresponding author (Gyöngyi Kökönyei, kokonyei.gyongyi@ppk.elte.hu) or from the Department of Pharmacodynamics, Faculty of Pharmacy, Semmelweis University (titkarsag.gyhat@pharma.semmelweis-univ.hu) on reasonable request.”

We were asked to review our reference list. It is complete and correct.

We thank the reviewer for their feedback, comments and suggestions. Please find below our detailed answers to the reviewer’s comments (we attached our responses in a separate file as well). In the revised manuscript the original text is presented in black, modifications are indicated by tracked changes mode in MS Word. We hope that we have managed to address each of the reviewer’s concerns.

Sincerely,

the authors

Reviewer #1: Summary: In this manuscript, authors used fear conditioning fMRI task to study the association of migraine frequency and neurobiological stress response with neural activation in two specific phases i.e., pain anticipation and pain perception. Authors found that migraine frequency was associated with pain anticipation in a cluster comprising the midcingulate and caudate, whereas pre-scan neurobiological stress response was related to pain perception in the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA). Overall, this is an interesting study with very nicely written Introduction and Discussion sections, detailed Methods, and well-organized Results. Biggest limitation is the small sample size of only 23 participants. I have few major and mostly minor concerns and suggestions related to this manuscript as pointed below:

Abstract:

- It would make more sense to define DHEA-S as an index of neurobiological stress response at the start of the abstract when it was first mentioned rather in the results.

#Response: Thank you for taking the time to review our manuscript! We changed the abstract accordingly. In the text: “Previous studies targeting inter-individual differences in pain processing in migraine mainly focused on the perception of pain. Our main aim was to disentangle pain anticipation and perception using a classical fear conditioning task, and investigate how migraine frequency and pre-scan cortisol-to-dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEA-S) ratio as an index of neurobiological stress response would relate to neural activation in these two phases.”

- It’s not clear what do the authors mean by “one cluster” comprising the midcingulate and caudate – given that midcingulate is a cortical whereas caudate is a sub-cortical region. It’s not clear how both these regions lead to one comprised cluster?

#Response: Using cluster-extent based thresholding can results clusters that cover multiple anatomical brain regions. It is the case for our analysis when we tested correlates of migraine frequency in pain anticipation.

We set primary threshold to p<0.001 as suggested by Woo et al. (2014, Neuroimage) and used non-parametric, permutation-based approach (SnPM13; http://www.nisox.org/Software/SnPM13/) with 5000 permutations and without variance smoothing in order to control for false-positives due to multiple testing, and cluster-level family-wise error-corrected pFWE <0.05 values were reported as significant.

According to Woo and colleagues (2014) a primary threshold of p<0.001 is considered to “be sufficient to identify regions that are localized enough to be anatomically interpretable in many studies”, and especially recommended for studies with low or moderate sample size (N<50). In addition, we chose non-parametric, permutation-based approach to determine the minimum cluster-extend threshold to prevent that false positives are greatly inflated. Thus, we think that the mentioned cluster though cover regions that are spatially distinct is interpretable.

However, to avoid misunderstanding we rephrased this sentence in the abstract:

“We found that migraine frequency was significantly associated with pain anticipation in brain regions comprising the midcingulate and caudate, whereas pre-scan cortisol-to DHEA-S ratio was related to pain perception in the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA).”

- “Pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) region” should be changed to just “pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA)”.

#Response: Thank you, we changed the abstract accordingly.

- Last sentence needs revision – “migraine attacks on the brain in migraine” is confusing.

#response: We deleted “in migraine” from the sentence.

Keywords:

- I would avoid using the keywords that are already present in the title. Instead, authors should use other keywords to broaden the search criteria of their paper.

#Response: Thank you very much for the suggestion, we changed our keywords to the following: headache; fMRI; stress response; pre-SMA; caudate

Introduction:

- Introduction is very well written with detailed overview of literature and very clear hypotheses and predictions.

#Response: Thank you very much for the feedback.

Methods:

- Line 158: “Right-handed subjects with migraine between 18–38 years” needs revision. It appears the subjects had migraine between 18 and 38, rather I would suggest mentioning sample size, mean and SD of age, age range, and male/female details here – for the full initial sample.

#Response: Thank you, we re-wrote the first sentence of the Participants section: “Right-handed subjects aged between 18-38 years were recruited …”

In addition, we added all the important details for the full initial sample. In the text: “Of the 28 participants (22 females, mean age=27.46 , SD=4.89, age range: 20 to 37 years) taking part in the pain task, … “

- Line 167: “three by technical reasons” should be “three due to technical reasons”, and any brief details about the technical reasons would be helpful.

#Response: We corrected the sentence, and added details about the technical reasons. In the text: “…five subjects were excluded: three due to technical reasons (missing images/volumes originated from technical problems or incomplete voxel level information), and two subjects did not see any correspondence between visual signals and electric stimuli according to the post-task interview (see below).”

- Line 171: Authors mentioned that participants did not have migraine attacks 48 h prior and 24 h after the scan. Was this by chance or this was one of the inclusion criteria, and for 24 h after the scan, did the authors follow-up with participants? These details are missing here.

#Response: It was an inclusion criteria not to have migraine attacks 48 h prior the scan, or more precisely, we scheduled the scans accordingly, and the data were analysed if participants did not have attacks 24 h after the scan, thus we followed-up our participants. To make it clear, we moved up these details to the paragraph about inclusion criteria and added these pieces of information to the text.

In the text: “They were invited to the scan if they did not have migraine attacks 48 h prior to the scan, and their data was analysed if they did not have migraine attacks during the 24 h follow up after the scan. They refrained from taking any analgesics 48 h before the scan session and did not take any prophylactic medication during the last three months. Participants were asked to refrain from alcohol for at least 24 hours and from caffeine for at least 4 hours before the scanning session.”

- Lines 296-297: “Whole-brain analysis…” needs revision.

#Response: Thank you, we corrected it.

Results:

- Line 351: It’s confusing when authors mention three clusters but then report eight clusters. Similarly, lines 364, 369, 422-425, and Table 5. This issue needs to be fixed throughout the manuscript.

#Response: The text can be confusing, however it is correct. We identified only 3 statistically significant clusters, but all the clusters contained more regions (see our explanation above). For instance in Table 5 three clusters with 8 regions are reported. However, to avoid misunderstanding we rephrased this sentence:

“Contrasting painful (VAS=7) versus non-painful stimuli (VAS=3) led to widespread activation in brain areas including the cerebellum, vermis, insula, postcentral gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, Rolandic operculum and Heschl gyrus (see Table 4 and Figure 2)."

Discussion: Very detailed and nicely written.

#Response: Thank you very much for the feedback.

General comments/questions and suggestions:

- Why did the authors prefer to report Spearman correlations rather than Pearson’s?

#Response: Since some of the variables (e.g. state anxiety) were not normally distributed, therefore we decided to use Spearman correlations.

- Did the authors perform quality check on outliers while performing correlation analysis?

#Response: thank you for the question. When we calculated correlational coefficients we checked whether there are any outliers using Simple Scatterplot in SPSS, and if any outliers were detected we checked whether results changed after excluding them. We did not find substantial differences in these cases, so we came to the conclusion that outliers were not influential.

- Authors report supplementary data with state anxiety, trait anxiety, and gender as controlled variables but individually. Why didn’t author use all these three variables together as a set of covariates instead of using those one-by-one?

#Response: We decided to use them one-by-one in our analyses in the first place since our sample size was small. However, based on your comment, we checked whether results would change if all the three variables were used as a set of covariates. You can see the results below which were unchanged. We added these tables to the Supporting information file. (Please see S8 and S9 Tables, and we also added these tables to the Response to Reviewers file).

- Suggestion for authors for their future work is to use this data and explore if structural data (e.g., morphometry parameters such as cortical/sub-cortical volume) and brain connectivity measures (e.g., directed functional and effective) have any association with migraine frequency, stress response parameters and other behavioral data, if available, from this project.

#Response: Thank you very much for the suggestion, we are currently working on additional analyses as you also suggested.

We also thank you for the constructive critics and the useful suggestions.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response_to_Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Mukesh Dhamala

6 Dec 2021

Inter-individual differences in pain anticipation and pain perception in migraine: neural correlates of migraine frequency and cortisol-to-dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEA-S) ratio

PONE-D-21-27604R1

Dear Dr. Kökönyei,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Mukesh Dhamala, Ph. D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Acceptance letter

Mukesh Dhamala

10 Dec 2021

PONE-D-21-27604R1

Inter-individual differences in pain anticipation and pain perception in migraine: neural correlates of migraine frequency and cortisol-to-dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEA-S) ratio

Dear Dr. Kökönyei:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Mukesh Dhamala

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Spearman correlations of anxiety scores with age, education level and clinical characteristics (N = 23).

    *p<0.05; 1point biserial correlation.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. Spearman correlations of pre-scan raw cortisol, DHEAS and cortisol-to-DHEAS with age, anxiety scores, clinical variables, menstrual phase and hormonal contraception taking (N = 23).

    *p<0.05; 1point biserial correlation.

    (DOCX)

    S3 Table. Activation changes during pain anticipation if state anxiety is controlled for (N = 23).

    Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left; SMA, Supplementary motor area.

    (DOCX)

    S4 Table. Activation changes during pain anticipation if trait anxiety is controlled for (N = 23).

    Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left; SMA, Supplementary motor area.

    (DOCX)

    S5 Table. Activation changes during pain anticipation if gender is controlled for (N = 23).

    Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left; SMA, Supplementary motor area.

    (DOCX)

    S6 Table. Activation changes to painful and non-painful stimulation if state anxiety was controlled for (N = 23).

    Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left.

    (DOCX)

    S7 Table. Activation changes to painful and non-painful stimulation if gender was controlled for (N = 23).

    Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left.

    (DOCX)

    S8 Table. Activation changes during pain anticipation controlled for gender, state and trait anxiety (N = 23).

    Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left; SMA, Supplementary motor area.

    (DOCX)

    S9 Table. Activation changes to painful and non-painful stimulation controlled for gender, state and trait anxiety (N = 23).

    Cluster-level familywise error rate of p<0.05; R, right; L, left.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response_to_Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All data underlying the findings described in the paper, i.e. self-report data, raw cortisol and DHEA-S data along with main fMRI contrast maps, are fully available at https://osf.io/g9vph/. Only raw imaging dataset cannot be shared publicly, because at the time our study started, there was no information on open access data availability in the consent forms (the study was approved by the Scientific and Research Ethics Committee of the Medical Research Council (Hungary)), therefore authors are not allowed to share raw imaging data publicly, since participants were not able to accept or refuse their assent to share imaging data in an open access repository. However, raw imaging data are available from the corresponding author (Gyöngyi Kökönyei, kokonyei.gyongyi@ppk.elte.hu) or from the Department of Pharmacodynamics, Faculty of Pharmacy, Semmelweis University (titkarsag.gyhat@pharma.semmelweis-univ.hu) on reasonable request.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES