Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Apr 24.
Published in final edited form as: Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021 Jan 4;31(4):615–624. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01705-2

Dietary Inflammatory Index of mothers during pregnancy and Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder symptoms in the child at preschool age: A prospective investigation in the INMA and RHEA cohorts

N Lertxundi 1,2, A Molinuevo 3, D Valvi 4, A Gorostiaga 1,2, N Balluerka 1,2, N Shivappa 5,6, JR Hebert 5,6, EM Navarrete-Muñoz 7,8,9, J Vioque 7,8, A Tardon 10, M Vrijheid 7,11,12, T Roumeliotaki 13, K Koutra 13, L Chatzi 14, J Ibarluzea 1,2,3,7
PMCID: PMC8713648  NIHMSID: NIHMS1759220  PMID: 33398651

Abstract

Background/Aim:

Inflammation provides a substrate for mechanisms that underlie the association of maternal diet during pregnancy with Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms in childhood. However, no previous study has quantified the proinflammatory potential of maternal diet as a risk factor for ADHD. Thus, we evaluated the association of maternal dietary inflammatory index (DII®) scores during pregnancy with ADHD symptoms in 4 year-old children born in two Mediterranean regions.

Methods:

We analyzed data from two population-based birth cohort studies – INMA (Environment and Childhood) four subcohorts in Spain (N=2,097), and RHEA study in Crete (Greece) (N=444). The DII score of maternal diet was calculated based on validated food frequency questionnaires completed during pregnancy (12th and/or 32nd week of gestation). ADHD symptoms were assessed by ADHD-DSM-IV in INMA cohort and by ADHDT test in RHEA cohort, with questionnaires filled-out by teachers and parents, respectively. The associations between maternal DII and ADHD symptoms were analysed using multivariable-adjusted zero-inflated negative binomial regression models in each cohort study separately. Meta-analysis was conducted to combine data across the cohorts for fitting within one model.

Results:

The DII was significantly higher in RHEA (RHEA=2.09 [1.94, 2.24]) in comparison to INMA subcohorts (Asturias=−1.52 [−1.67,−1.38]; Gipuzkoa=−1.48 [−1.64,−1.33]; Sabadell=−0.95 [−1.07,−0.83]; Valencia= −0.76 [−0.90,−0.62]). Statistically significant reduced risk of inattention symptomatology (OR = 0.86; CI 95% = 0.77–0.96), hyperactivity symptomatology (OR = 0.82; CI 95% = 0.72–0.92) and total ADHD symptomatology (OR = 0.82; CI 95%= −0.72–0.93) were observed with increased maternal DII in boys. No statistically significant associations were observed in girls between maternal DII and inattention, hyperactivity and total ADHD symptomatology.

Conclusions:

We found reduced risk of ADHD symptomatology with increased DII only in boys. This relationship requires further exploration in other settings.

Keywords: dietary inflammatory index, ADHD, maternal diet, prenatal environment, European birth cohorts

INTRODUCTION

In the last 20 years childhood neuropsychiatric disorders have increased significantly (Atladottir et al., 2015), with important consequences for the affected child and their families, and for the public health system. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most frequent childhood-onset neuropsychiatric condition, with an estimated worldwide prevalence of approximately 5% in school-aged children (Polanczyk et al., 2007), and males manifest more symptoms than females (Gershon and Gershon, 2002).

The etiology of ADHD probably is multifactorial, reflecting a complex interplay between genetic, lifestyle, nutritional, psychosocial and environmental factors (Cortese et al., 2011, Spencer et al., 2007). Growing evidence suggests that exposure to adverse environmental and psychological conditions, especially when exposure occurs in intrauterine life may increase the risk of developing ADHD in childhood (Buss et al., 2012). Many pre and perinatal environmental or reproductive factors have been identified as risk factors, such as maternal smoking, alcohol and substance misuse, maternal stress or low birth weight and prematurity (Thapar et al., 2012). Exposure to environmental toxins such as lead may also increase the risk of ADHD (Park et al., 2016), and of other adverse psychosocial outcomes (Rutter et al., 2007; Thapar et al., 2013).

Maternal overweight and obesity before pregnancy are known risk factors for ADHD in the child (Andersen, 2018; Pugh et al., 2016; Rodriguez et al., 2010). More recent evidence further supports that nutrition during pregnancy is crucial for child neurodevelopment (De Capo, 2019). Previous studies have indicated that maternal dietary patterns and the consumption of specific food groups or nutrients during pregnancy are associated with cognitive and behavioral outcomes (Anjos et al., 2013). Many dietary components associated with child neurodevelopment have a known pro-inflammatory potential (i.e., fat products) and anti-inflammatory potential (i.e., fermented products) (Bordoni et al., 2015); however, the joint contribution of diet-related inflammation with child neurodevelopmental outcomes has not been evaluated previously. This is an important evidence gap, as increased inflammation is thought to underlie, at least in part, the associations between maternal diet and overweight/obesity and ADHD (Bilbo and Tsang, 2010; Chen et al., 2014; Edlow, 2017; Rodriguez et al., 2008; Rodriguez, 2010). Obese pregnant women have higher levels of circulating pro-inflammatory cytokines and both placental and intrauterine inflammation are related to altered fetal cytokine expression, fetal neuronal damage, and changes in neonatal brain gene expression (Elovitz et al., 2011).

The dietary inflammatory index (DII®) was developed to capture the inflammatory potential of diets based on known associations of specific food groups and nutrients with serum inflammatory markers (Shivappa, Steck, Hurley, Hussey and Hebert, 2014). The DII therefore offers a unique opportunity to assess the overall association of a maternal proinflammatory diet with child health outcomes. Some outcomes have been studied, such as cortical bone outcomes (Coheley, Shivappa, Hebert and Lewis, 2019), anthropometric indices (Aslani eta al., 2019), or muscle mass and strength (Amakye et al., 2018); however, child neurodevelopment outcomes have not been previously examined. Given the pro-inflammatory potential of suspected nutrients, and evidence supporting a role of inflammation in the pathogenesis of ADHD, we hypothesised that a more pro-inflammatory diet during the mother’s pregnancy will increase ADHD symptomatology in her offspring, especially in boys. Thus, we evaluated the association between the DII scores of mothers’ diets during pregnancy and ADHD symptoms in their children at the age of 4 years in two Mediterranean regions (Spain and Greece).

METHODS

Study participants

INMA

The population-based birth cohort study INMA (INfancia y Medio Ambiente - Environment and Childhood) recruited pregnant women from four Spanish regions (Asturias, Gipuzkoa, Sabadell and Valencia) between November 2003 and February 2008 (Guxens et al., 2012). Pregnant women were enrolled during the 1st trimester of pregnancy at primary health care centers or public hospitals, depending on the region, if they fulfilled these inclusion criteria: (i) age ≥16 years, (ii) singleton pregnancy, (iii) no assisted conception, (iv) intention to deliver at the reference hospital, and (v) no communication problems. Written informed consent was signed by the mothers and the study was approved by the Hospital Ethics Committees in the participating regions and hospitals. Out of a total of 2644 mothers who were recruited in the first trimester of pregnancy, 2,097 children were included in the present analysis after exclusion of neuropsychological tests of uncertain quality, due to less than optimal cooperation or poor quality test and basic pathologies.

RHEA

The RHEA Study in Crete (Greece) (Chatzi et al., 2017) enrolled pregnant women during the 1st trimester of pregnancy at hospitals or private clinics if they were: (i) resident in the study areas, (ii) ≥16 years of age, and (iii) without communication handicap. The study was approved by the ethical committee of the University Hospital in Heraklion, Crete (Greece) and all participants provided written, informed consent. A total of 1606 mothers were originally enrolled and a subgroup of 612 children performed the neuropsychological development test at age 4 years. After excluding data from women who did not have exposure information, had a specific pathology, or a low-quality test, a total of 444 children were included in the analysis.

Dietary Inflammatory Index

DII scores were calculated based on dietary data (Shivappa, Steck, Hurley, Hussey and Hebert, 2014) collected in the first and third trimesters of pregnancy (12th & 32nd week of gestation) in the INMA cohorts and in the 2nd trimester of pregnancy (14th-18th week of gestation) in the RHEA cohort using a validated food frequency questionnaire (Vioque et al., 2013; Chatzi et al., 2011; Fernandez-Barrés et a., 2016). Thirty-two nutrients were accounted for the DII calculation in the RHEA and INMA cohort including both pro-inflammatory factors (such as alcohol, carbohydrate and saturated fats) and anti-inflammatory factors (such as certain vitamins, fiber and green tea). We excluded oregano intake for the DII calculation in the INMA cohort, and caffeine intake for the DII calculation in the RHEA cohort because this information had not been collected in the respective FFQs.

The method of calculation is described in detail elsewhere (Shivappa, Steck, Hurley, Hussey and Hebert, 2014). Briefly, the scoring algorithm was based on extensive review of the literature published from 1950 to 2010focused on the effect of diet on six inflammatory biomarkers (IL-1β, IL-4, IL-6, IL-10, TNF-α, and CRP). A total of 45 food parameters, including macronutrients and micronutrients, were scored according to whether they increased (+1), decreased (−1), or had no effect (0) on these inflammatory biomarkers. These inflammatory effect scores were weighted based on study design. To avoid the arbitrariness resulting from simply using raw consumption amounts, intakes of foods and nutrition were standardized to a representative range of dietary intake based on actual human consumption in 11 populations living in different countries across the world that provided an estimate of a mean and standard deviation for each parameter. These values were converted to a proportion (with values from 0 to 1). Each proportion was doubled, and then 1 was subtracted to achieve a symmetrical distribution around a mean of ≈0. For each individual food parameter, this score was then multiplied by the respective food parameter effect score, derived from the literature review, in order to obtain a food parameter-specific DII score. All of the food parameter-specific DII scores were then summed to create the overall DII score for every participant in the study, DII= b1*n1+b2*n2........... b32*n32, where b refers to the literature-derived inflammatory effects score for each of the evaluable food parameters and n refers to the food parameter-specific centered percentiles, which were derived from the dietary data from the mother. For comparative purposes between the two cohort studies, we used the average mean of the first and third pregnancy trimester DIIs in INMA as a better proxy of maternal diet throughout pregnancy. Energy adjusted-DII (E-DII) was calculated by converting all nutrients from the studies and the global database to per 1000 kcal and then calculating the DII.

ADHD symptoms

ADHD symptoms were assessed by ADHD-DSM-IV (18 items) in INMA cohort and by Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Test (ADHDT) (36 items) in RHEA cohort at around 4.5 years of age, with questionnaires filled-out by teachers and mothers, respectively. The ADHD-DSM-IV symptom data were gathered using the Diagnostic Criteria for ADHD of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (ADHD-DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). We used the DSM-IV symptom checklist for ADHD as a rating scale without further modifications, found to be psychometrically useful in a prior validation study comparing reports of parents and teachers (Amador-Campos, 2006). It is an 18-item checklist designed to identify inattention (1–9), and hyperactivity–impulsivity (10–18) symptoms in children. Each ADHD symptom is rated on a four-point Likert scale (0=never/rarely, 1=sometimes, 2=often, or 3=very often). ADHDT (Gilliam, 1995) is a 36-item questionnaire designed to identify and evaluate ADHD symptoms in ages 3–23 years according to the DSM-IV criteria for ADHD. It contains three subscales: hyperactivity (13 items), impulsivity (10 items) and inattention (13 items). All 36 items are summed to generate an index for total ADHD difficulties. The ADHDT has been adapted in the Greek population (Maniadaki and Kakouros, 2002). In this case each item is rated on a three-point Likert scale (0=not a problem, 1=mild problem, or 2= severe problem).

For all tests, higher scores indicate more ADHD symptoms. The continuous measure of ADHD symptoms was calculated as the sum of the score of each item. This measure ranges from 0 to 54 in the case of ADHD-DSM-IV (INMA cohort) and from 0 to 72 in ADHDT (RHEA cohort) test. The categorical measure of ADHD symptoms was calculated recoding the list of symptoms as 0 (symptom absent when the response is 0 or 1) or 1 (symptom present when the response is 2 or 3) (Gomez, 2007). Children rated with six or more symptoms of each dimension were classified as inattentive, hyperactive or inattentive and hyperactive taking into account the criteria of Diagnostic Criteria for ADHD of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (ADHD-DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).

Covariates

Information related to sociodemographic and anthropometric characteristics, diet, and lifestyle was collected through questionnaires administered during pregnancy and later, when children were around 14 months and 4 years old in INMA and 9–12 months and 4 years old in RHEA. All questionnaires and measurements were conducted face-to-face by trained interviewers. Child’s sex (male or female), weeks at birth (preterm or not), cesarean (yes/no), gestational age and pre-pregnancy body mass index (BMI) calculated in kg/m2 and classified as <25 (normal), 25–29.9 (overweight) and ≥30 (obesity). Child BMI z-scores specific for age and sex were calculated using the WHO growth standards, and a BMI z-scores above the 85th percentile, were afterwards classified as overweight (including obesity) (de Onis et al., 2007). Information about passive smoking at home at 4 years (yes/no), duration of breastfeeding and working status at 4 years (yes/no), were collected in a subsequent interview when infants were around 14 months and 4 years old in INMA and 9–12 months and 4,5 years old in RHEA. Information on maternal age at delivery and education (low/medium/high), maternal working status (yes/no) during pregnancy and at child age 4 years, maternal origin (from country cohort/other) and residence (rural/urban), maternal smoking during pregnancy (yes/no), parity, BMI, pre-pregnancy diabetes (yes/no), gestational diabetes and hypertension (yes/no) was obtained through questionnaires administered during pregnancy or 4 years and from medical records.

Statistical analysis

As preliminary analyses, we compared socio-demographic characteristics of the mother and child across the cohorts of INMA (Asturias, Gipuzkoa, Sabadell and Valencia) and RHEA. The difference in the mean of inattention, hyperactivity, impulsivity and ADHD symptoms according the mother and child characteristics were analysed using the Student t test and one way ANOVA.

Due to the excess of zeros values in symptoms, the association between maternal DII and the symptoms was analysed using multivariable-adjusted zero-inflated negative binomial regression models (Vilor-Tejedor et al., 2016). Separate models were created for each cohort (INMA subcohorts and RHEA) due to the measurement differences in the test administered for the evaluation of ADHD symptomatology. We adjusted the models for those variables with p-value ≤ 0.20 in the bivariate analysis. These variables were maintained in the final fully adjusted models if they changed the magnitude of the DII-ADHD effect estimate by more than 10%. The final fully adjusted models in both INMA and RHEA cohorts were adjusted by child variables such as sex and age at the moment of the test, and mother’s variables: pre-pregnancy BMI, age at delivery, working status, educational level, parity and smoking during pregnancy. All models were adjusted also by the interaction between sex and DII. We used meta-analysis to obtain combined estimates for the study areas. Statistical analyses were conducted using the Statistical Package R version 3.5.1.

RESULTS

The sample consisted of data from 2541 children and their mothers, encompassing 2,097 mother child pairs from the INMA cohort (Asturias=531; Gipuzkoa=377; Sabadell=631; Valencia=558) and 444 from the RHEA cohort. Descriptive DII data in each cohort and the comparison between them are presented in Supplementary Figure 1. The DII was significantly higher in RHEA (RHEA=2.09 [1.94, 2.24]) in comparison to INMA subcohorts (Asturias=−1.52 [−1.67,−1.38]; Gipuzkoa=−1.48 [−1.64,−1.33]; Sabadell=−0.95 [−1.07,−0.83]; Valencia= −0.76 [−0.90,−0.62]).

Descriptive statistics and comparative analysis of child and maternal characteristics in each cohort are presented in Table 1. In the bivariate analysis, scores for several behaviors were significantly related to child and mother characteristics; boys presented with higher symptomatology of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity in both the INMA and RHEA cohorts. In both cohorts children with normal BMI (based on WHO criteria) presented with fewer symptoms in total ADHD compared to overweight and obese children, and children exposed to passive smoking at home had more total ADHD symptoms. Children with more than 3.43 months breastfeeding duration presented with fewer symptoms of hyperactivity, impulsivity and total symptomatology in the RHEA cohort. High maternal education and working status during pregnancy (yes) were associated with fewer symptoms in both cohorts.

Table 1.

Mean and standard deviation for ADHD symptoms, child and maternal characteristics in all cohorts

N(%)
mean(sd)
All cohorts
N=2541
Asturias
N=531
Gipuzkoa
N=377
Sabadell
N=631
Valencia
N=558
RHEA
N=444
p.overall

ADHD outcomes
Inattention1 - 0.97(1.86) 0.38(1.21) 0.65 (1.67) 0.82(1.91) - <0.001
Hyperactivity1 - 0.93 (2.00) 0.34(1.12) 0.68 (1.59) 0.80(1.77) - <0.001
ADHD1 - 1.90(3.36) 0.72 (2.07) 1.33 (2.85) 1.62(3.23) - <0.001
Inattention2 - - - - - 3.80 (4.47) -
Hyperactivity2 - - - - - 5.63 (5.37) -
ADHD2 - - - - - 14.7(12.5) -

Child Characteristics
Sex Boy 1311 (51.6%) 259 (48.8%) 185 (49.1%) 323 (51.2%) 299 (53.6%) 245 (55.2%) 0.210
Girl 1230 (48.4%) 272 (51.2%) 192 (50.9%) 308 (48.8%) 259 (46.4%) 199 (44.8%)
Age (in the moment of the test) 4.39 (0.20) 4.42 (0.20) 4.47(0.10) 4.46 (0.18) 4.32 (0.13) 4.26 (0.26) <0.001
BMI at 4 years based on IOTF criteria Normal 2014 (79.7%) 379 (72.6%) 288 (77.0%) 537(85.1%) 452 (81.1%) 358 (80.6%) <0.001
Overweight 349 (13.8%) 77(14.8%) 77 (20.6%) 63 (9.98%) 72(12.9%) 60 (13.5%)
Obese 165 (6.53%) 66(12.6%) 9 (2.41%) 31 (4.91%) 33 (5.92%) 26 (5.86%)
Gestational age 39.3 (1.66) 39.3 (1.64) 39.7(1.48) 39.7(1.39) 39.5 (1.73) 38.2 (1.59) <0.001

Maternal Characteristics
Mother DII −0.58 (2.04) −1.52 (1.70) −1.48 (1.53) −0.95 (1.54) −0.76 (1.67) 2.09 (1.58) <0.001
Mother origin Spain 2397 (94.4%) 515 (97.0%) 372 (98.7%) 571 (90.6%) 520 (93.2%) 419 (94.6%) <0.001
Other 142 (5.59%) 16(3.01%) 5(1.33%) 59 (9.37%) 38 (6.81%) 24 (5.42%)
Residence Rural 150 (6.01%) 7(1.32%) 37 (9.81%) 0 (0.00%) 27 (4.84%) 79 (19.8%) <0.001
Urban 2344 (94.0%) 524 (98.7%) 340 (90.2%) 630 (100%) 531 (95.2%) 319 (80.2%)
Maternal education Low 468(18.5%) 91 (17.1%) 43 (11.5%) 131 (20.9%) 135 (24.2%) 68(15.5%) <0.001
Medium 1115 (44.1%) 234 (44.1%) 130 (34.8%) 280 (44.7%) 251 (45.0%) 220 (50.0%)
High 946(37.4%) 206(38.8%) 201 (53.7%) 215 (34.3%) 172 (30.8%) 152(34.5%)
Maternal smoking during pregnancy No 1817 (73.2%) 364 (71.2%) 288 (79.8%) 460 (74.0%) 345 (61.8%) 360 (83.5%) <0.001
Yes 666 (26.8%) 147 (28.8%) 73 (20.2%) 162 (26.0%) 213 (38.2%) 71 (16.5%)
Working status during pregnancy No 499 (20.0%) 130 (25.4%) 42 (11.6%) 57(9.16%) 84(15.1%) 186(42.3%) <0.001
Yes 1996 (80.0%) 382 (74.6%) 321 (88.4%) 565 (90.8%) 474 (84.9%) 254 (57.7%)
Parity Primiparous 1419 (56.3%) 330 (62.1%) 206 (54.6%) 365 (58.1%) 330 (59.1%) 188 (44.0%)
Multiparous 1102 (43.7%) 201 (37.9%) 171 (45.4%) 263 (41.9%) 228 (40.9%) 239 (56.0%)
<0.001
BMI Underweight 145 (5.76%) 30 (5.75%) 16 (4.28%) 30 (4.75%) 34 (6.20%) 35 (7.88%)
Normal 1861 (73.9%) 349 (66.9%) 272 (72.7%) 507 (80.3%) 409 (74.6%) 324 (73.0%)
Overweight 349 (13.9%) 77 (14.8%) 77 (20.6%) 63 (9.98%) 72(13.1%) 60 (13.5%)
Obese 164(6.51%) 66 (12.6%) 9 (2.41%) 31 (4.91%) 33 (6.02%) 25 (5.63%)
Pre pregnancy diabetes No 2516 (99.4%) 529 (99.6%) 376 (99.7%) 630 (100%) 555 (99.5%) 426 (97.7%) <0.001
Yes 16 (0.63%) 2 (0.38%) 1 (0.27%) 0 (0.00%) 3 (0.54%) 10 (2.29%)
Gestational diabetes No 2055 (94.5%) 508 (95.7%) 346 (97.2%) 302 (94.4%) 525 (94.1%) 374 (91.4%)
Yes 119 (5.47%) 23 (4.33%) 10 (2.81%) 18 (5.62%) 33 (5.91%) 35 (8.56%)
BF duration 3.30(2.88) 2.61 (2.47) 3.54 (2.39) 3.30 (2.28) 3.01 (2.43) 4.25 (4.37) <0.001
Age at delivery 31.9(4.16) 32.9 (3.95) 32.8 (3.19) 31.7 (3.99) 31.8 (4.02) 30.1 (4.87) <0.001
1

ADHD-DSM-IV test for INMA cohorts (Asturias, Sabadell, Gipuzkoa and Valencia)

2

ADHDT Test for Rhea cohort

Figure 1 shows the results of the meta-analysis of multivariable-adjusted zero-inflated negative binomial regression models of DII and Inattention, Hyperactivity and total ADHD in boys. After adjusting models by childreńs age at the time of the test, region, pre-pregnancy BMI, maternal age at delivery, working status during pregnancy, educational level, parity and smoking during pregnancy, statistically significant inverse associations were observed between maternal DII and inattention symptomatology (OR = 0.86; 95% CI = 0.77–0.96), maternal DII and hyperactivity symptomatology (OR = 0.82; 95% CI = 0.72–0.92) and maternal DII and total ADHD symptomatology (OR = 0.82; 95% CI = 0.72–0.93) in boys.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Meta- analysis of multivariable-adjusted zero-inflated negative binomial regression models of DII and Inattention, Hyperactivity and total ADHD in boys (Asturias=259; Gipuzkoa=185; Sabadell=323; Valencia=299; RHEA=245)

Figure 2 shows the results of the meta-analysis of multivariable-adjusted zero-inflated negative binomial regression models of DII and Inattention, Hyperactivity and total ADHD in girls. No statistically significant associations were observed between maternal DII and inattention symptomatology (OR CI 95% 1.04 (0.88–1.19)), maternal DII and hyperactivity symptomatology (OR CI 95% 0.89 (0.76–1.02)) and maternal DII and total ADHD symptomatology (OR CI 95% 0.89 (0.76–1.02)).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Meta- analysis of multivariable-adjusted zero-inflated negative binomial regression models of DII and Inattention, Hyperactivity and total ADHD in girls (Asturias=272; Gipuzkoa=192; Sabadell=308; Valencia=259; RHEA=199)

DISCUSSION

The present study assessed the association between maternal DII and ADHD symptomatology in 2541 children and their mothers from the Spanish INMA cohort and the Greek RHEA cohort. Significant differences were seen between the Spanish and Greek cohorts suggesting a more pro-inflammatory diet of mothers in Greece comparing to mothers in Spain. A higher proportion of mothers with high adherence to the Mediterranean diet was observed in RHEA cohort in a previous study (Chatzi et al., 2013), indicating differences in the diet of Greek and Spanish pregnant women.

We found statistically significant inverse associations between maternal DII and inattention, hyperactivity and total ADHD symptomatology only in boys. These results suggest that a higher DII during pregnancy could have a protective effect for ADHD symptomatology, which is inconsistent with our primary hypothesis and requires further exploration in other settings.

Taking into consideration evidence that supports the pro-inflammatory potential of diet and its effect on the pathogenesis of ADHD, we expected that higher levels of maternal DII would have a positive relation with ADHD symptomatology in children. Recent studies in some other children outcomes, have observed positive associations between DII and anthropometric indices such as children BMI, waist circumference, hip circumference and parental BMI (Aslani et al., 2019), inverse association between DII and skeletal muscle mass in boys (Amakye et al., 2018), or increased risk for wheeze in adults and children with allergic (atopic) wheeze (Han et al., 2019). A potential limitation of the current state of literature is that most previous findings have been reported from cross-sectional studies of adults and it is yet unclear to what extent maternal DII over the pregnancy period may be associated with health outcomes in the child at later ages. In our study, we examined for the first time, associations with child ADHD symptomatology and we observed associations suggesting that a more pro-inflammatory diet may be linked to lower risk for ADHD symptoms in boys. It is yet unclear whether this association may be indeed causal. However, one possible explanation for our findings may be that within an overall Mediterranean diet pattern, that characterizes our two study regions and it is generally considered as a healthy diet, the pro-inflammatory potential captured by dietary inflammatory indexes may not necessarily be associated with detrimental health effects. Given the smaller sample size of the RHEA cohort, it is not clear whether the absence of significant associations were due to limited power, the generally higher DII range observed in the RHEA cohort or other methodological limitations (e.g., differences in ADHD assessment tests or FFQs).

The main strengths of this study include the use of the previously validated DII that permits to evaluating the association of the overall anti- or pro-inflammatory diet potential on health outcomes, and. Also, data were derived from the large sample size and analysis of two large Mediterranean-based cohorts. Another strength is the use of standardized and validated questionnaires for assessment of ADHD symptoms in children in both INMA and RHEA cohorts. However, in spite of the fact that both cohorts used standardized, validated and instruments based on DSM IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994, 2000), there are differences in the distribution of the items in each dimension of the test and this has been a limitation in our study. In INMA cohort ADHD symptoms were assessed by ADHD-DSM-IV (18 items) and in RHEA cohort by Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Test (ADHDT) (36 items). In ADHD-DSM-IV there are 9 items for inattention, and another 9 items for hyperactivity–impulsivity, and for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Test (ADHDT), there are 13 items for Hyperactivity, 10 for Impulsivity and 13 items for inattention. This is the reason why we could not perform pooled-analysed analysis of the INMA and RHEA cohorts, thus precluding that may have permitted to provide even more precise effect estimates, especially in regard to interactions.

In conclusion, we found sex-specific inverse association between maternal DII and ADHD symptomatology, with associations seen only in boys, and going to the opposite direction to what was expected. More specifically, we found that higher maternal DII during pregnancy, suggesting an overall more pro-inflammatory diet, may have a protective effect on the behavioral symptoms in boys. Further research is needed to replicate the findings from this study and clarify the potential impact of anti- and pro-inflammatory dietary patterns on child health.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Figure

Acknowledgements:

The authors would particularly like to thank all the cohort participants for their generous collaboration. A full roster of the INMA Project Investigators can be found at http://www.proyectoinma.org/presentacioninma/listadoinvestigadores/en_listadonvestigadores.html[V1] and of the Rhea project Investigators at http://www.rhea.gr/en/about-rhea/the-rhea-team/.

Funding:

This study was funded by grants from Spanish Ministry of Health-Instituto de Salud Carlos III (Red INMA G03/176, CB06/02/0041, FIS-PI041436, FIS-PI081151, FIS-PI042018, FISPI09/02311, FIS-PI06/0867, FIS-PS09/00090, FIS-FEDER PI11/1007 FIS-FEDER 03/1615, 04/1509, 04/1112, 04/1931, 05/1079, 05/1052, 06/1213, 07/0314, and 09/02647), Generalitat de Catalunya-CIRIT 1999SGR 00241, Conselleria de Sanitat Generalitat Valenciana, FISS-FEDER PI13/02429, FISS-FEDER PI18/00909, Universidad de Oviedo, Obra Social Cajastur-LIBERBANK, Department of Health of the Basque Government (2005111093 and 2009111069), the Provincial Government of Gipuzkoa (DFG06/004 and DFG08/001), Fundación Roger Torné, and Alicia Koplowitz Foundation 2017.

Drs. Valvi and Chatzi are currently receiving funding from the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (R21 ES029328, R21 ES028903).

Footnotes

Conflicts of interest:

Disclosure: Dr. James R. Hébert owns controlling interest in Connecting Health Innovations LLC (CHI), a company planning to license the right to his invention of the dietary inflammatory index (DII) from the University of South Carolina in order to develop computer and smart phone applications for patient counselling and dietary intervention in clinical settings. Dr. Nitin Shivappa is an employee of CHI.

All other authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval:

The INMA Project was approved by the Hospital Ethics Committees in the participating regions and hospitals. The RHEA study was approved by the ethical committee of the University Hospital in Heraklion, Crete (Greece).

References

  1. Amador-Campos JA, Forns-Santacana M, Guàrdia-Olmos J, Peró-Cebollero M (2006). DSM-IV attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms: agreement between informants in prevalence and factor structure at different ages. J Psychopathol Behav Assess, 28, 23–32. [Google Scholar]
  2. Amakye WK, Zhang Z, Wei Y, Shivappa N, Hebert JR, Wang J, Su Y, Mao L (2018). The relationship between dietary inflammatory index (DII) and muscle mass and strength in Chinese children aged 6–9 years. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr, 27, 1315–1324. 10.6133/apjcn.201811_27(6).0019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  4. Andersen CH,Thomsen PH, Nohr EA, Lemcke S (2018). Maternal body mass index before pregnancy as a risk factor for ADHD and autism in children, Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 27, 139–148. 10.1007/s00787-017-1027-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Anjos T, Altmäe S, Emmett P, Tiemeier H, Closa-Monasterolo R, Luque V, Wiseman S, Pérez-García M, Lattka E, Demmelmair H, Egan B, Straub N, Szajewska H, Evans J, Horton C, Paus T, Isaacs E, van Klinken JW, Koletzko B, Campoy C (2013). Nutrition and neurodevelopment in children: focus on NUTRIMENTHE project, Eur J Nutr, 52, 1825–1842. 10.1007/s00394-013-0560-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Aslani Z, Qorbani M, Hébert JR, Shivappa N, Motlagh ME, Asayesh H, Mahdavi-Gorabi A, Kelishadi R (2019). Association of Dietary Inflammatory Index with anthropometric indices in children and adolescents: the weight disorder survey of the Childhood and Adolescence Surveillance and Prevention of Adult Non-communicable Disease (CASPIAN)-IV study. Br J Nutr, 121, 340–350. 10.1017/S0007114518003240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Atladottir HO, Gyllenberg D, Langridge A, Sandin S, Hansen SN, Leonard H, Gissler M, Reichenberg A, Schendel DE, Bourke J, Hultman CM, Grice DE, Buxbaum JD, Parner ET (2015). The increasing prevalence of reported diagnoses of childhood psychiatric disorders: a descriptive multinational comparison. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 24, 173–183. 10.1007/s00787-014-0553-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bilbo SD and Tsang V (2010). Enduring consequences of maternal obesity for brain inflammation and behavior of offspring, The FASEB Journal, 24, 2104–2115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bordoni A, Danesi F, Dardevet D, Dupont D, Fernandez AS, Gille D, Nunes dos Santos C, Pinto P, Re R, Rémond D, Shahar DR and Vergères G (2017). Dairy products and inflammation: A review of the clinical evidence. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition, 57:12, 2497–2525. 10.1080/10408398.2014.967385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buss C, Entringer S, Poggi Davis E, Hobel CJ, Swanson JM, Wadhwa PD, Sandman CA (2012). Impaired Executive Function Mediates the Association between Maternal Pre-Pregnancy Body Mass Index and Child ADHD Symptoms, Plos One, 7, e37758. 10.1371/journal.pone.0037758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chatzi L, Melaki V, Sarri K, et al. (2011). Dietary patterns during pregnancy and the risk of postpartum depression: the mother–child ‘Rhea’ cohort in Crete, Greece. Public Health Nutr, 14, 1663–1670. 10.1017/S1368980010003629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chatzi L, Garcia R, Roumeliotaki T, BAsterrechea M, Begiristain H, Iñiguez C, Vioque J, kogevinas M, and Sunyer J (2013). Mediterranean diet adherence during pregnancy and risk of wheeze and eczema in the first year of life: INMA (Spain) and RHEA (Greece) mother–child cohort studies. British Journal of Nutrition, 110, 2058–2068. 10.1017/S0007114513001426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chatzi L, Leventakou V, Vafeiadi M, Koutra K, Roumeliotaki T, Chalkiadaki G, Karachaliou M, Daraki V, Kyriklaki A, Kampouri M, Fthenou E, Sarri K, Vassilaki M, Fasoulaki M, Bitsios P, Koutis A, Stephanou EG., Kogevinas M (2017). Cohort Profile: The Mother-Child Cohort in Crete, Greece (Rhea Study). International Journal of Epidemiology, 46, 1392–1393. 10.1093/ije/dyx084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chen Q, Sjolander A, Langstrom N, Rodriguez A, Serlachius E, D’Onofrio BM, et al. (2014). Maternal pre-pregnancy body mass index and offspring attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a population-based cohort study usinga sibling-comparison design. Int. J. Epidemiol,43, 83–90. 10.1093/ije/dyt152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Coheley LM, Shivappa N, Hebert JR, Lewis RD (2019). Dietary inflammatory index® and cortical bone outcomes in healthy adolescent children. Osteoporos Int. 10.1007/s00198-019-04946-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cortese S, Vincenzi B (2011). Obesity and ADHD: Clinical and Neurobiological Implications. In: Stanford C, Tannock R (eds) Behavioral Neuroscience of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Its Treatment. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, vol 9. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. DeCapo M, Thompson JR, Dunn G, and Sullivan EL (2019). Perinatal Nutrition and Programmed Risk for Neuropsychiatric Disorders: A Focus on Animal Models. Biological Psychiatry, 85, 122–134. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. De Onis M, Onyango AW, Borghi E, Siyam A, Nishida C, and Siekmann J (2007). Development of a WHO growth reference for school-aged children and adolescents. Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 85, 660–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Edlow AG (2017). Maternal obesity and neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders in offspring. Prenat Diagn, 37,95–110. 10.1002/pd.4932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Elovitz MA, Brown AG, Breen K, et al. (2011). Intrauterine inflammation, insufficient to induce parturition, still evokes fetal and neonatal brain injury. Int J Dev Neurosci, 29, 663–671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fernández-Barrés S, Romaguera D, Valvi D, Martínez D, Vioque J, Navarrete-Muñoz EM, Amiano P, Gonzalez-Palacios S, Guxens M, Pereda E, Riaño I, Tardón A, Iñiguez C, Arija V, Sunyer J, Vrijheid M (2016). Mediterranean dietary pattern in pregnant women and offspring risk of overweight and abdominal obesity in early childhood: the INMA birth cohort study. Pediatric Obesity, 11(6), 491–499. https://doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gershon J, & Gershon J (2002). A Meta-Analytic Review of Gender Differences in ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 5(3), 143–154. https://doi: 10.1177/108705470200500302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gilliam JE (1995). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Test, Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. [Google Scholar]
  24. Gomez R (2007). Testing gender differential item functioning for ordinal and binary scored parent rated ADHD symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 733–742. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Guxens M, Ballester F, Espada M, et al. (2012). Cohort Profile: the INMA--INfancia y Medio Ambiente-(Environment and Childhood) Project. Int J Epidemiol, 41, 930–940. 10.1093/ije/dyr054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Han YY, Forno E, Shivappa N, Wirth MD, Hébert JR, Celedón JC (2018).The Dietary Inflammatory Index and Current Wheeze Among Children and Adults in the United States. J Allergy Clin Immunol Pract, 6, 834–841. 10.1016/j.jaip.2017.12.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Maniadaki K, Kakouros E, 2002. Translation and adaptation of the Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Test (ADHDT; Giliam, 1995). In: Stalikas A, Triliva S, Roussi P (Eds.), The psychometric Instruments in Greece. Ellinika Grammata, Athens, pp. 102–103. [Google Scholar]
  28. Park JH, Seo JH, Hong YS, Kim YM, Kang JW, Yoo JH, Chueh HW, Lee JH, Kwak MJ, Kim J, Woo HD, Kim DW, Bang YR, Choe BM (2016). Blood lead concentrations and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Korean children: a hospital-based case control study, BMC Pediatrics 16, 156, 10.1186/s12887-016-0696-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Polanczyk G, Rohde LA (2007). Epidemiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Curr Opin Psychiatry, 20, 386–392. 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3281568d7a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Pugh SJ, Hutcheon JA, Richardson GA, Brooks MM, Himes KP, Day NL, Bodnar LM (2016). Gestational weight gain, prepregnancy body mass index and offspring attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and behaviour at age 10. BJOG, 123, 2094–2103. 10.1111/1471-0528.13909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Rodriguez A, Miettunen J, Henriksen TB, Olsen J, Obel C, Taanila A, Ebeling H, Linnet KM, Moilanen I, Jarvelin MR (2008). Maternal adiposity prior to pregnancy is associated with ADHD symptoms in offspring: evidence from three prospective pregnancy cohorts. Int J Obes 32, 550–557. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Rodriguez A (2010) Maternal pre-pregnancy obesity and risk for inattention and negative emotionality in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 51, 134–143. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02133.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rutter M, Beckett C, Castle J, Colvert E, Kreppner J, Mehta M, Stevens S & Sonuga-Barke E (2007). Effects of profound early institutional deprivation: An overview of findings from a UK longitudinal study of Romanian adoptees. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 332–350. [Google Scholar]
  34. Shivappa N, Steck SE, Hurley TG, Hussey JR, Hebert JR (2014). Designing and developing a literature-derived population-based dietary inflammatory index. Public Health Nutr, 17(6), 1689–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Mick E (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: diagnosis, lifespan, comorbidities, and neurobiology. J Pediatr Psychol, 32, 631–642. 10.1093/jpepsy/jsm005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Steck S, Shivappa N, Tabung FE, Harmon B, Wirth M, Hurley T, Hebert J (2014). The Dietary Inflammatory Index: A New Tool for Assessing Diet Quality Based on Inflammatory Potential. The Digest, 49, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  37. Thapar A, Cooper M, Eyre O, Langley K (2013). Practitioner Review: What have we learnt about the causes of ADHD? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 3–16. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02611.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Thapar A, Cooper M, Jefferies R, Stergiakouli E (2012). What causes attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Archives Disease in Childhood, 97, 260–265. 10.1136/archdischild-2011-300482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Vilor-Tejedor N, Alemany S, Forns J, Cáceres A, Murcia M, Maciá D, Pujol J, Sunyer J, and González JR (2016). Assessment of Susceptibility Risk Factors for ADHD in Imaging Genetic Studies. Journal of Attention Disorders,1–11. 10.1177/1087054716664408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Vioque J, Navarrete-Muñoz EM, Gimenez-Monzó D, García-de-la-Hera M, Granado F, Young IS, Ramón R, Ballester F, Murcia M, Rebagliato M, Iñiguez C (2013). Reproducibility and validity of a food frequency questionnaire among pregnant women in a Mediterranean area. Nutrition Journal, 12:26. 10.1186/1475-2891-12-26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Figure

RESOURCES