Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Jun 15.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Brain Sci. 2021 Dec 15;44:e192. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X2100114X

Ratio-based Perceptual Foundations for Rational Numbers, and Perhaps Whole Numbers, too?

Edward M Hubbard 1, Percival G Matthews 2
PMCID: PMC8736706  NIHMSID: NIHMS1768195  PMID: 34907871

Abstract

Clarke and Beck suggest that the Ratio Processing System (RPS) may be a component of the ANS, which they suggest represents rational numbers. We argue that available evidence is inconsistent with their account and advocate for a two-systems view. This implies that there may be many access points for numerical cognition - and that privileging the ANS may be a mistake.


We applaud Clarke and Beck’s (C&B’s) compelling use of analogies with other domains of perception to defend the notion that the ANS truly represents numbers. We were also gratified to see that they arrived at a similar conclusion to that proposed by our own work, one “closely related to a suggestion from the developmental and educational psychology literatures according to which there is a “ratio processing system” (RPS)”. We have previously advanced the view that humans and other animals possess a perceptual system for representing ratio magnitudes, and that they therefore represent rational numbers, rather than being limited to representing purely integers (e.g., Lewis et al. 2015; Matthews et al. 2016, see also Jacob et al., 2012). C&B go on to suggest that it isn’t always clear whether the RPS is a separate system from the ANS or a component of it, deciding in favor of the latter. They further argue that “the hypothesis that the RPS represents rational numbers is not always clearly distinguished from the conjecture that it represents real numbers more generally” (p. 37). In this commentary, we focus on these points in light of the current empirical record.

Our view is informed by our prior findings that the RPS is operative in multiple visual formats – extending beyond the discrete dot arrays that have typically been the focus of ANS research. We showed that children and adults can also compare ratios made of lines, circles, and irregular blobs (e.g., Park et al., 2020; Binzak et al., submitted; see also Bonn & Cantlon, 2017). Because ratio perception has been demonstrated using various continuous stimuli not typically considered the province of the ANS, we argue 1) that the RPS cannot be a component of the ANS, and 2) that perceiving numerical ratios may be every bit as fundamental as perceiving exact number (or numerosities). A corollary to this position is that one route to whole number representations might be an emergent property of ratio perception (i.e., when the denominator is 1).

Although these issues must ultimately be settled empirically, in the spirit of C&B, we think an analogy from brightness perception illustrates the plausibility of our argument that the ANS and RPS are two systems. Although individual photoreceptors signal absolute light levels, much of the perceptual system is tuned to relative (ratio) brightnesses of different portions of surfaces, such as when identifying edges in a scene or perceiving shades of gray in black and white images. This system yields the same percept even under a 1000-fold difference in absolute light levels, such as when moving from indoors to outside under bright sunlight. That is, the visual system computes relative ​brightness as its primary perceptual feature (for a review, see Gilchrist, 2013) and either normalizes or discounts absolute illumination. In parallel, intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells signal absolute illumination and feed into systems that regulate the pupillary reflex and circadian rhythms (e.g., Yamakawa et al., 2019). By analogy, the RPS could be specialized for perception of relative quantity (numerosity), whereas the ANS is specialized for perception of absolute numerosity. Furthermore, as with brightness perception, absolute number may be calculated less frequently and relative number perception may be the predominant mode of perception.

In line with the two-systems view, findings from our labs further suggest that the RPS is not a component of the ANS. For instance, in prior studies, we showed that the predictive power of the RPS was independent of ANS acuity, which contributed almost no explanatory power to the models (Matthews et al., 2016; Park & Matthews, in press). Moreover, if the ANS and RPS constitute a single system, we would predict that long-term ANS training--which successfully transferred across visual field locations--should also transfer across tasks to the RPS, but this was not observed (Cochrane et al., 2019). Moving forward, it would be interesting to test whether RPS training transfers to ANS tasks. Most importantly, Park et al. (2020) carried out a battery of tasks using different stimulus formats (e.g., circles, dots, lines) where participants compared simple stimuli (ANS style) and ratio stimuli (RPS style). They showed that performance was driven more by task similarity (ANS vs. RPS) than by stimulus format (circles, dots, lines).

Despite our preferred take, however, this clearly remains an open question. For instance, in a recent computational modelling study, we trained a deep convolutional neural network (DCNN) to compare non-symbolic numbers, either as simple dot arrays or as ratios composed of two dot arrays (Chuang et al., 2020). Analysis of the hidden unit responses suggested that RPS representations might emerge from tuned (ANS style) units.

More research is necessary for the final adjudication. That said, C&B have done the entire field a service by highlighting that the ANS might be only one component of a multifaceted number sense that integrates various cues and generates various usable outputs from those cues. In highlighting the importance of ratios, C&B underscore that Weber-guided systems can compute not only integers, but also rational numbers. This implies that there may be many access points for numerical cognition - and that privileging the ANS may be a mistake.

As for what type of numbers might be represented by a perceptual number sense, we concur with C&B that the type of number represented may be limited by the nature and precision of the inputs of the perceptual system. The RPS can presumably represent the entire set of x/y for all x and y which a given input system can represent. Thus, if the RPS is truly limited to discrete inputs, then the number sense would include only the rationals. However, if it is more continuous in character, then it could include the reals.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Comparison stimuli used by Park et al., 2020 organized by task type (simple vs ratio comparison) and by format (dots, lines, blob, and circles).

FUNDING STATEMENT

This research was supported by grants from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01 HD088585) to E.M.H. and P.G.M. a core grant (U54-HD090256–01) to the Waisman Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center, and by the National Science Foundation (NSF REAL 1420211).

Footnotes

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST. None

Contributor Information

Edward M. Hubbard, University of Wisconsin-Madison

Percival G. Matthews, University of Wisconsin-Madison

References

  1. Binzak JV, Matthews PG & Hubbard EM (submitted). Holistic magnitude processing of symbolic fractions and nonsymbolic ratios
  2. Bonn CD, & Cantlon JF (2017). Spontaneous, modality-general abstraction of a ratio scale. Cognition, 169, 36–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Chuang YS, Hubbard EM, & Austerweil JL (2020). The “Fraction Sense” Emerges from a Deep Convolutional Neural Network Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1207–1213). Toronto, Canada: Cognitive Science Society [Google Scholar]
  4. Cochrane A, Cui L, Hubbard EM & Green CS (2019). “Approximate number system” training: A perceptual learning approach. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 81(3):621–636. 10.3758/s13414-018-01636-w [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Gilchrist AL (Ed.). (2013). Lightness, Brightness and Transparency Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  6. Jacob SN, Vallentin D, & Nieder A (2012). Relating magnitudes: the brain’s code for proportions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 157–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Lewis MR, Matthews PG & Hubbard EM (2015). Neurocognitive Architectures and the Nonsymbolic Foundations of Fractions Understanding. In Berch DB, Geary DC, and Koepke KM (Eds.) Development of Mathematical Cognition-Neural Substrates and Genetic Influences (p. 141–160) Elsevier. ISBN: 978–0128018712. [Google Scholar]
  8. Matthews PM, Lewis MR & Hubbard EM (2016). Individual differences in nonsymbolic ratio processing predict symbolic math performance. Psychological Science, 27(2):191–202. 10.1177/0956797615617799 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Park Y & Matthews PG (in press). Revisiting and refining relations between nonsymbolic ratio processing and symbolic math achievement. Journal of Numerical Cognition
  10. Park Y, Viegut AA, & Matthews PG (2020). More than the sum of its parts: Exploring the development of ratio magnitude vs. simple magnitude perception. Developmental Science, e13043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  11. Yamakawa M, Tsujimura Si. & Okajima K (2019). A quantitative analysis of the contribution of melanopsin to brightness perception. Scientific Reports, 9, 7568. 10.1038/s41598-019-44035-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES