Abstract
Children with and without dyslexia differ in their behavioral responses to visual information, particularly when required to pool dynamic signals over space and time. Importantly, multiple processes contribute to behavioral responses. Here we investigated which processing stages are affected in children with dyslexia when performing visual motion processing tasks, by combining two methods that are sensitive to the dynamic processes leading to responses. We used a diffusion model which decomposes response time and accuracy into distinct cognitive constructs, and high-density EEG. Fifty children with dyslexia (24 male) and 50 typically developing children (28 male) 6-14 years of age judged the direction of motion as quickly and accurately as possible in two global motion tasks (motion coherence and direction integration), which varied in their requirements for noise exclusion. Following our preregistered analyses, we fitted hierarchical Bayesian diffusion models to the data, blinded to group membership. Unblinding revealed reduced evidence accumulation in children with dyslexia compared with typical children for both tasks. Additionally, we identified a response-locked EEG component which was maximal over centro-parietal electrodes which indicated a neural correlate of reduced drift rate in dyslexia in the motion coherence task, thereby linking brain and behavior. We suggest that children with dyslexia tend to be slower to extract sensory evidence from global motion displays, regardless of whether noise exclusion is required, thus furthering our understanding of atypical perceptual decision-making processes in dyslexia.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Reduced sensitivity to visual information has been reported in dyslexia, with a lively debate about whether these differences causally contribute to reading difficulties. In this large preregistered study with a blind modeling approach, we combine state-of-the art methods in both computational modeling and EEG analysis to pinpoint the stages of processing that are atypical in children with dyslexia in two visual motion tasks that vary in their requirement for noise exclusion. We find reduced evidence accumulation in children with dyslexia across both tasks, and identify a neural marker, allowing us to link brain and behavior. We show that children with dyslexia exhibit general difficulties with extracting sensory evidence from global motion displays, not just in tasks that require noise exclusion.
Keywords: decision-making, development, diffusion model, EEG, motion perception, perception
Introduction
It has long been suspected that visual processing relates to the reading difficulties characterizing developmental dyslexia (e.g., Hinshelwood, 1896; Lovegrove et al., 1980). One visual function that develops atypically in those with dyslexia is visual motion processing: an important ability contributing to scene segmentation, depth perception and object recognition (Braddick et al., 2003). Difficulties in global motion tasks requiring integration over space and time have been widely reported in dyslexia (Benassi et al., 2010). Typically, participants are required to detect or discriminate coherently moving signal dots among randomly moving noise dots (Newsome and Paré, 1988). In this “motion coherence” task, dyslexic individuals tend to have elevated psychophysical thresholds, requiring higher proportions of signal dots to perform at the same level of accuracy as those without dyslexia (Benassi et al., 2010). The nature of the relationship is still being debated, with some researchers proposing a causal relationship between motion sensitivity and reading ability (Boets et al., 2011; Gori et al., 2016; but see Olulade et al., 2013; Goswami, 2015; Joo et al., 2017; Piotrowska and Willis, 2019).
Atypical global motion processing in dyslexia may reflect reduced sensitivity to rapid temporal information originating from deficiencies in the magnocellular system (Livingstone et al., 1991; Stein and Walsh, 1997; Stein, 2001, 2019) or related dorsal stream (Hansen et al., 2001; Braddick et al., 2003), which are particularly specialized for motion perception (Livingstone and Hubel, 1988). Alternative accounts suggest that dyslexic individuals have difficulty filtering out the randomly moving noise dots in motion coherence tasks (“noise exclusion”) (Sperling et al., 2006; Conlon et al., 2012) or difficulties integrating over space and time (Raymond and Sorensen, 1998; Hill and Raymond, 2002; Benassi et al., 2010).
Despite focusing on the sensory parameters of visual motion stimuli, these accounts give little consideration to the dynamic processes leading to atypical behavioral responses in dyslexia, and particularly, whether decision-making processes are affected. Here we explicitly modeled the decision-making process using a popular cognitive model of accuracy and response time (RT): the diffusion model (Stone, 1960; Ratcliff, 1978; Evans and Wagenmakers, 2020). The decision is modeled as a noisy evidence accumulation process from a starting point toward one of two decision bounds (Fig. 1). This modeling approach will help identify the locus of atypical processing in dyslexia, with two further advantages: (1) the resulting parameters may be more sensitive to group differences than accuracy or RT alone (Stafford et al., 2020); and (2) the parameters relate well to neural measures (Kelly and O'Connell, 2013; Turner et al., 2015; Manning et al., 2021a). Accordingly, we combined the diffusion model with a neural measure sensitive to the dynamic processes contributing to behavioral responses (EEG), bridging brain and behavior.
The diffusion model was recently applied to motion coherence performance in children with varying reading abilities (O'Brien and Yeatman, 2021). Poorer reading was related to lower drift rates, wider decision bounds, and more intra-individual variability in starting point and nondecision time. Therefore, poor readers accumulated motion evidence more slowly and responded more cautiously than good readers.
Here, we used diffusion models to identify the processing stages affected in children with dyslexia across two global motion tasks. The first task was a standard motion coherence task (comparable to O'Brien and Yeatman, 2021). The second task was a direction integration task not used before with dyslexic individuals, whereby dot directions are sampled from a Gaussian distribution, with difficulty manipulated via the SD of the distribution. In this task, the optimal strategy is to average over all dots, with no noise exclusion requirement. The reason for presenting both tasks to children with dyslexia was to determine whether differences in model parameters are found for both motion tasks, suggesting a general motion-processing deficit (i.e., magnocellular/dorsal deficit) (Stein, 2001; Braddick et al., 2003), or whether differences in model parameters are found particularly for the motion coherence task, reflecting noise exclusion difficulties (Sperling et al., 2006; Conlon et al., 2012).
Materials and Methods
Preregistration
We preregistered our inclusion criteria and analysis plan before completing data collection and before commencing analyses (https://osf.io/enkwm). When analyzing the data, we used a blind modeling approach to ensure that modeling decisions were not biased by our hypotheses. Our preregistered primary research questions and hypotheses were as follows:
1. Do children with dyslexia have reduced drift rates in a motion coherence task compared with typically developing children?
We hypothesized that children with dyslexia would have reduced drift rates in the motion coherence task compared with typically developing children, in line with the results of O'Brien and Yeatman (2021) and reports of reduced motion coherence sensitivity in dyslexic individuals (Benassi et al., 2010).
2. Do children with dyslexia have reduced drift rates in a direction integration task compared with typically developing children?
If children with dyslexia show difficulties with all global motion tasks (in line with impaired magnocellular/dorsal stream functioning) (Stein, 2001; Braddick et al., 2003), then we would expect children with dyslexia to have a reduced drift rate in this task as well. Instead, if the performance of children with dyslexia in a motion coherence task is limited solely by difficulties with noise exclusion (Sperling et al., 2006; Conlon et al., 2012), we would expect to see no difference between children with and without dyslexia in this task, as it does not require segregating signal dots from randomly moving noise dots.
3. Do children with dyslexia show increased boundary separation?
We hypothesized that children with dyslexia would have wider boundary separation compared with typically developing children in both tasks, following O'Brien and Yeatman (2021).
4. Do children with dyslexia show increased nondecision time?
We hypothesized no group differences in overall nondecision time in either task, following O'Brien and Yeatman (2021).
Participants
We collected data from 50 children with dyslexia and 60 typically developing children who met our inclusion criteria. Specifically, participants were required to be 6-14 years (inclusive) of age, have verbal and/or performance IQ (PIQ) scores >70 (measured using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence, Ed 2 [WASI-2]) (Wechsler, 2011), and to have normal or corrected-to-normal acuity, as measured using a Snellen acuity chart (with binocular acuities of 6/9 or better for children 6-8 years of age and 6/6 or better for children 9-14 years of age). Children in the dyslexia group were required to have a dyslexia diagnosis (or be in the process of obtaining one, n = 1), and to have a reading and spelling composite score of ≤89, which was computed by averaging the standard scores for the spelling subtest of the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT-III) (Wechsler, 2017) and the Phonological Decoding Efficiency subtest of the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE-2) (Torgesen et al., 2012). A cutoff of 89 was chosen to correspond to 1.5 SDs below the mean of typically developing children in a similar study (Snowling et al., 2019a,b). Children in the typically developing group were required to have composite scores >89 and to have no diagnosed developmental conditions. Datasets from an additional 4 typically developing children were excluded because of poor visual acuity (n = 1), having a composite score of ≤89 (n = 2), or failing to pass criterion on the task (n = 1), and datasets from an additional 11 children with dyslexia were excluded because of poor visual acuity (n = 2) or having a composite score >89 (n = 9).
We then selected 50 typically developing children to best match the children with dyslexia in terms of age and PIQ using the R MatchIt package (Ho et al., 2011), so that the final dataset included 50 children with dyslexia (24 male) and 50 typically developing children (28 male). As shown in Table 1, the children with dyslexia had slightly higher ages and lower IQ values on average than the typically developing children. EEG data were collected during task performance in 47 typically developing and 44 children with dyslexia (although EEG data were available only in the motion coherence task for one child with dyslexia and one typically developing child). The EEG data from these participants were included in a paper investigating responses locked to the onset of coherent motion in typically developing children and children with autism or dyslexia (Toffoli et al., 2021), and the larger group of 60 typically developing children were used to form the comparison group in an autism study (Manning et al., 2021b).
Table 1.
Typically developing (n = 50) | Dyslexia (n = 50) | |
---|---|---|
Age | 10.65 (2.34); 6.55-14.98 | 11.08 (1.87); 7.81-14.53 |
PIQb | 109.26 (11.53); 81-145 | 99.40 (15.29); 72-141 |
Verbal IQ | 110.60 (8.42); 95-127 | 98.56 (10.60); 77-118 |
Full-scale IQ | 111.36 (9.02); 89-132 | 98.70 (12.85); 75-132 |
TOWRE-2 PDE | 111.18 (16.53); 81-153 | 79.16 (9.45); 51-99 |
WIAT-Spelling | 105.74 (10.21); 80-127 | 77.86 (7.96); 58-99 |
Composite score | 108.46 (12.15); 89.5-138.0 | 78.51 (7.46); 54.5-89.0 |
aData are mean (SD); range.
bPIQ, performance IQ.
Apparatus
The tasks were presented on a Dell Precision M3800 laptop (2048 × 1152 pixels, 60 Hz) using the Psychophysics Toolbox for MATLAB (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997; Kleiner et al., 2007). EEG signals were collected using 128-channel Hydrocel Geodesic Sensor Nets connected to Net Amps 300 (Electrical Geodesics) and NetStation 4.5 software. A photodiode attached to the monitor independently verified stimulus presentation timing. Participants used a Cedrus RB-540 response box (Cedrus).
Stimuli
Stimuli were 100 white, randomly positioned dots (diameter 0.19°) moving at 6°/s within a square aperture (10° × 10°) on a black background, with a limited lifetime of 400 ms. Each trial had a fixation period, a random motion period, a stimulus period, and an offset period, with a red fixation square (0.24° × 0.24°) presented throughout (Fig. 2). By presenting random (incoherent) motion before the stimulus period, we could dissociate evoked responses to directional motion from pattern- and motion-onset evoked potentials. The start of the stimulus period was highlighted to participants with an auditory tone. In the motion coherence task, directional motion (leftward or rightward) was introduced in a proportion of “signal” dots, while the remainder of the dots continued to move in random directions. In the direction integration task, the directions of dots in the stimulus phase were distributed according to a Gaussian distribution with a mean leftward or rightward direction. The fixation period, random motion period, and offset period had jittered durations within a fixed range, while the stimulus period was presented until a response or 2500 ms had elapsed. The offset period continued the directional motion to temporally separate motion offset from the response.
Experimental task procedure
Children completed motion coherence and direction integration tasks within child-friendly games (based on Manning et al., 2019, 2021a). Using animations, participants were told that fireflies were escaping from their viewing boxes, and they were asked to tell the zookeeper which way the fireflies were escaping. There were 10 “levels” of the game. Levels 1-5 corresponded to one task (either motion coherence or direction integration), and Levels 6-10 corresponded to the other task, with the order of tasks being counterbalanced across participants. Levels 1 and 6 were practice phases, and the remaining four levels for each task were experimental blocks. In the motion coherence task, difficulty was manipulated by varying the proportion of coherently moving dots; and in the direction integration task, difficulty was manipulated by varying the SD of the Gaussian distribution from which the dot directions were sampled.
In the practice phases, four demonstration trials were presented with no random motion phase and an unlimited stimulus phase, so that the experimenter could explain the task. Participants reported stimulus direction using a response box. The first two demonstration trials were “easy” (100% coherence or 1° SD), and the last two were more difficult (75% and 50% coherence, or 10° and 25° SDs). Following the demonstration trials, there were up to 20 criterion trials with a coherence of 95% or an SD of 5°. These trials introduced the random motion phase. Participants were told that the fireflies would be going “all over the place” at first, and that they must wait for an alarm (auditory beep) before deciding which way the fireflies were escaping. A time limit was enforced, with visual feedback presented on the screen if participants did not respond within 2500 ms (“Timeout! Try to be quicker next time!”). Feedback on accuracy was given for responses made within the time limit (“That was correct!”, or “It was the other way that time”). When participants met a criterion of four consecutive correct responses, no more criterion trials were presented. Next, there were eight practice trials of increasing difficulty (motion coherence task: 80%, 70%, 60%, 50%, 40%, 30%, 20%, 10%; direction integration task: 5°, 10°, 15°, 20°, 30°, 40°, 50°, 60°) with feedback as before. Level 1 was repeated for one typically developing child and 2 children with dyslexia who did not meet the criterion of four consecutive correct responses on the first attempt, but passed on the second attempt.
Levels 2-5 and 7-10 each contained 38 trials, with 9 repetitions of each of two difficulty levels (motion coherence task: 30%, 75%; direction integration task: 70°, 30° SD), for each motion direction (leftward, rightward), and an additional 2 catch trials presenting 100% coherent (0° SD) motion. The experimental phase for each task therefore consisted of 152 trials. No trial-by-trial feedback was presented during the experimental phase, apart from a “timeout” message if no response was made within 2500 ms after stimulus onset. At the end of each level, participants were given points for their speed and accuracy in the preceding block (computed by (1/median RT) × the number of correct responses × 2, rounded to the nearest integer). If participants obtained a score <10, a score of 10 points was given to maintain motivation. Trials were presented automatically, although the experimenter could pause and resume trial presentation if necessary. The experimental code can be found here: https://osf.io/fkjt6/.
General procedure
The procedure was approved by the Central University Research Ethics Committee at the University of Oxford. Parents provided written informed consent, and children gave verbal or written assent. All children took part at the University of Oxford apart from one child with dyslexia who was seen at school without EEG. During the experimental tasks, participants sat 80 cm away from the computer screen in a dimly lit room. For children who participated with EEG, we fitted the net before the experiment and ensured that electrode impedances were <50 kΩ. EEG data were acquired at a sampling rate of 500 Hz with a vertex reference electrode.
Children were closely monitored by an experimenter sitting beside them. The experimenter provided general encouragement and task reminders, pausing before the start of a trial if needed (e.g., to remind the child to keep still). Children had short breaks at the end of each “level” and a longer break at the end of the first task (at the end of “level 5”). During the longer break, electrode impedances were reassessed for children wearing EEG nets. Children marked their progress through the levels using a stamper on a record card. The children also completed a Snellen acuity test, the WASI-2, the TOWRE-2, and the spelling subtest of the WIAT-III. The whole session took no longer than 2 h, and children were given a gift voucher to thank them for their time.
Diffusion model analysis
Initially, a blinded analysis was conducted to ensure that modeling decisions were made without being biased by the hypotheses under test. The first author (C.M.) prepared a blinded dataset in which group membership was randomly permuted (see also Dutilh et al., 2017) and one of the authors (N.J.E.) ran diffusion model analysis on this blinded dataset.
Before modeling, trials with RTs <200 ms were removed (corresponding to 0.20% of trials in the typical group and 0.24% of trials in the dyslexia group). Trials without a response (i.e., no response made within the 2500 ms deadline) were modeled as nonterminating accumulation trajectories, with the probability of a nonresponse occurring being the survivor function for the model at the time of the 2500 ms deadline (Ulrich and Miller, 1994; Evans et al., 2018; Howard et al., 2020). These trials accounted for 1.02% of the data in the typical group and 1.26% of the data in the dyslexia group. We fit the data from each task with hierarchical, Bayesian diffusion models with five parameters: (1) average drift rate across difficulty levels v.mean, (2) boundary separation a, (3) nondecision time ter, (4) difference in mean drift rate between difficulty levels v.diff, and (5) starting point z. The stochastic noise within the model (s) was fixed at 0.1 to solve a scaling problem within the model, as per convention (Ratcliff, 1978). There were three hyperparameters for each parameter reflecting the mean (μ) and SD (σ) across the two groups and the difference between groups (δ). Importantly, this parameterization allowed us to explicitly set priors on the differences between groups, which was the key effect of interest within the current study. More specifically, the priors were as follows:
Data level:
Parameters:
Hyperparameters:
where y reflects the data, and subscripts p and i reflect the participant and difficulty level, respectively. The priors for the μ and σ parameters were based on those used in previous studies implementing hierarchical diffusion models (e.g., Evans and Brown, 2017; Evans and Hawkins, 2019; Evans et al., 2019), and the priors for the δ parameters were based on the “moderately informative priors” used for the differences between conditions in Evans (2019). We used a differential evolution Markov chain Monte Carlo algorithm (Ter Braak, 2006; Turner et al., 2013b) to sample from the posterior with 15 interacting chains, each with 4000 iterations, the first 1500 of which were discarded as burn-in. We also implemented a migration algorithm (see Turner et al., 2013b), where chains were randomly migrated every 14 iterations between iterations 500 and 1100. We calculated Bayes factors (BFs) through the Savage-Dickey ratio. Where we found evidence of group differences, we established the population effect size by dividing the posterior of the group difference (δ) by the posterior of the population SD (σ).
As shown in Table 1, the children with dyslexia were on average slightly older and of lower IQ than the typically developing children. As preregistered, the first author (C.M.) ran a default Bayesian t test using the BayesFactor R package (Morey and Rouder, 2018) which revealed weak, inconclusive evidence for the absence of group differences in age (BF in support of group differences = 0.33) (Jeffreys, 1961). As we know that diffusion model parameters change with age (Manning et al., 2021a), and as we could not conclusively rule out group differences in age, we also ran models which partialled out the effects of age from all of the parameters (using the residuals from the line of best fit between age and each of the parameters), in addition to our standard models. In our preregistered analysis plan, we decided not to control for PIQ as it may relate to both group membership and decision-making in cognitively relevant ways (Dennis et al., 2009). The analysis files were posted on the Open Science Framework before unblinding (https://osf.io/nvwf7/), at which point all models were rerun on the unblinded dataset with correct group membership.
EEG analysis for joint modeling
We ran exploratory analysis on the unblinded dataset to investigate links between drift rate and EEG activity. EEG data were bandpass filtered between 0.3 and 40 Hz in NetStation and then exported for further processing in MATLAB using EEGLAB functions (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). We downsampled each participant's data to 250 Hz and selected only the data between the first fixation onset and the last offset period. We then bandpass-filtered between 0.3 and 40 Hz (because of insufficient attenuation of low frequencies by NetStation filters) (Manning et al., 2019) and used EEGLAB's “clean_artifacts” function to remove bad channels, identify data segments with SDs over 15 and correct them using artifact subspace reconstruction (Chang et al., 2018). Missing channels were then interpolated. We then ran independent components analysis on 3000 ms epochs starting at fixation onset using an Infomax algorithm and subtracted ocular components from the continuous data. Finally, we average rereferenced the data. In line with the behavioral analyses, we excluded triggers for response events made <200 ms or >2500 ms after stimulus onset.
Following previous work, we used a data-driven component decomposition technique to identify spatiotemporally reliable patterns of activity across trials, which has the effect of maximizing signal-to-noise ratio (reliable components analysis) (Dmochowski et al., 2012; Dmochowski and Norcia, 2015; Manning et al., 2019, 2021a). To do this, we epoched each participant's preprocessed continuous data from −600 to 200 ms around each response, and we baselined the data to the last 100 ms of the random motion period. We submitted the baselined epochs for participants in both groups to reliable components analysis for each task separately. The forward-model projections of the weights for the most reliable component for each task (which explained 28.7% and 27.1% of the reliability in the motion coherence and direction integration tasks, respectively) are shown in Figure 3. This component resembled the most reliable component found in our previous work (Manning et al., 2021a), which in turn resembles the centro-parietal positivity (O'Connell et al., 2012; Kelly and O'Connell, 2013). Buildup of activity in this component has been linked to drift rate in typically developing children (Manning et al., 2021a). To investigate links with drift rate in the current dataset, we projected each participant's continuous data through the spatial weights for this component to yield a single component waveform for each participant for each task.
In our paradigm, stimulus-locked and response-locked activity overlaps temporally, with the degree of overlap relating to the participant's reaction time. Importantly, the extent of overlap could vary between groups and/or conditions (Ehinger and Dimigen, 2019). Thus, to obtain an EEG measure for inclusion in our model that reflects the decision-making process as purely as possible, and fully separate the contributions of stimulus-locked and response-locked activity, we used a linear deconvolution method to unmix overlapping stimulus-locked and response-locked activity in our component waveform using the Unfold toolbox (Ehinger and Dimigen, 2019). We modeled the continuous waveform for each participant by selecting a time window of −1000 to 1000 ms around each stimulus event or response event. We specified a design matrix with predictors for each difficulty level (difficult, easy) for each event type (stimulus, response). We then time-expanded the design matrix by adding a predictor for each time point sampled (i.e., every 4 ms from −1000 to 1000 ms) for each event type. The reason for this “time-expansion” is that each regressor in the resulting design matrix models the evoked response (either stimulus-locked or response-locked) at a particular point in time (Smith and Kutas, 2015; Ehinger and Dimigen, 2019); this is equivalent to the “finite impulse response” approach to analysis of fMRI timeseries (Henson et al., 2001). The predictors are therefore simply “boxcar” functions at each point in time, rather than information relating to the stimulus display. Having constructed the design matrix, we identified segments with amplitudes >±250 μV using a sliding 2000 ms segment in 100 ms steps, and excluded these segments from the design matrix (motion coherence task: mean 2.36% of data for each participant, range: 0 to 19.50%; direction integration task: mean 2.19% of the data for each participant, range: 0 to 21.17%). We then fit the deconvolution model resulting in regression weights (betas) for each of the 2 event types, 2 difficulty levels, and 500 time points, which we used to construct regression waveforms (see Figs. 4 and 5). Comparing the left and middle columns of Figures 4 and 5 shows that deconvolution led to reduced amplitudes (which is expected as the nondeconvolved waveform contains a mix of overlapping stimulus-locked and response-locked activity).
The nondeconvolved waveforms showed amplitude differences between difficult and easy levels (Figs. 4 and 5, left column), as to be expected for an EEG measure which reflects the decision-making process. However, these differences across difficulty levels were not evident in the deconvolved waveforms (Figs. 4 and 5, middle column). The fact that the difference between difficulty levels changed as a result of deconvolution could suggest that the overlap between stimulus- and response-locked activity differs between difficulty levels, because of different RT distributions in each difficulty level. However, we found a difficulty level difference in the nondeconvolved waveforms even when matching the RT distributions for the easy and difficult levels, so that difficulty level differences could not be purely attributed to different RT distributions. We therefore suspected that the β estimates may be noisy and that the deconvolution technique was overfitting the noise. Therefore, in the final step where we selected EEG measures for inclusion in the diffusion model, we reran the deconvolution model using a regularization method which penalizes the squared magnitude of the regression coefficients (ridge regression) (Kristensen et al., 2017) to minimize noise. Using this approach retained the difficulty level differences while minimizing the noise in the waveforms (Figs. 4 and 5, right column). Specifically, we found the best regularization parameter for each participant using cross-validation, and then took the mode across all participants and constrained the regularization parameter to ensure that differences in regularization did not contribute to group differences in resulting waveforms. The modal parameter value was 10 for the motion coherence task (5.5 and 10 for the typically developing children and children with dyslexia, separately) and 5 for the direction integration task (5 and 4.5 for the typically developing children and children with dyslexia, separately). We then fit a regression slope to each participant's average deconvolved waveform for each difficulty level between −200 and 0 ms around the time of the response to obtain a slope measure which we entered into the diffusion model and related to drift rate.
To assess the relationship between drift rate and the EEG component discussed above, we used a joint modeling approach (Turner et al., 2013a, 2015, 2016; Evans et al., 2018; Knowles et al., 2019). Specifically, we estimated additional hyperparameters for the correlation between the v.mean parameter and the average of the EEG measure (slope of centro-parietal component activity between −200 and 0 ms before response) over difficulty levels (EEG.mean), and between the v.diff parameter and the difference in the EEG measure between difficulty levels (EEG.diff). Specifically, this meant that the structure of the original hierarchical model (with age partialled out) was only different for the drift rate parameter, which was now a bivariate normal with the EEG measure as follows:
where ρ refers to the correlation between drift rate and the EEG measure. We again used a differential evolution Markov chain Monte Carlo algorithm with 15 interacting chains to sample from the posterior of the joint model, although because of the greater computational burden of the model, we used 3000 iterations, of which the first 1000 were discarded as burn-in and no migration algorithm was implemented. Furthermore, we estimated two different variants of this joint model: one where the correlations were constrained to be the same across groups, which would allow for the estimation of more precise posteriors because of the limited sample size, and another less constrained version where the correlations were estimated separately for each group.
Data and code availability
Analysis scripts are available at https://osf.io/nvwf7/. Data and output files are available on the United Kingdom Data Service at http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-855236.
Results
Diffusion modeling of behavioral data
Figure 6 summarizes the accuracy and RT data subjected to diffusion modeling. This figure shows that the children with dyslexia had slightly slower median RTs compared with typically developing children, on average, and were slightly less accurate in the direction integration task, particularly on the difficult trials. However, there was substantial overlap between the groups with considerable variability within each group. These behavioral data were well fit by our diffusion models, as shown by the cumulative density functions in Figure 7. All chains were well converged, as reflected by Gelman-Rubin diagnostic values (Gelman and Rubin, 1992) close to 1 (mean = 1.00, range = 1.00-1.07).
Figure 8 shows the prior and posterior distributions for the group-level parameters that reflect the difference between groups for each of the five parameters (v.mean, a, ter, v.diff, β), along with BFs. BFs >1 reflect more evidence for the alternative hypothesis of group differences compared with the null hypothesis, whereas BFs <1 reflect relatively more evidence for the null hypothesis than the alternative hypothesis. We use the heuristic that BFs between 1/3 and 3 constitute only weak, inconclusive evidence (Jeffreys, 1961).
In support of our first hypothesis, children with dyslexia had reduced drift rates in the motion coherence task compared with typically developing children, as shown by the leftward shift in the posterior distribution of v. mean in Figure 8. When age was partialled out, there was moderate evidence in favor of group differences (BF = 4.57, population effect size mean = –0.18, 95% CI: [–0.40, 0.02]). The evidence was weaker when age was not partialled out (BF = 1.75). Interestingly, the same pattern was found in support of our second hypothesis, with children with dyslexia also showing reduced drift rates in the direction integration task compared with typically developing children. Again, there was moderate evidence for group differences when age was controlled for (BF = 4.28, population effect size mean = –0.21, 95% CI: [–0.45, 0.02]), but weak evidence when age was not controlled for (BF = 1.71).
Our third hypothesis was that children with dyslexia would show increased boundary separation. Although children with dyslexia did have slightly higher boundary separation compared with typically developing children (indicated by a small rightward shift in the posterior distribution of a in Fig. 8), particularly in the motion coherence task, the evidence remained inconclusive, even when controlling for age. Our final hypothesis was that there would be no group differences in nondecision time (ter) in either task. Figure 8 shows little difference between the groups in this parameter, but the BFs are close to 1, suggesting inconclusive evidence. Therefore, more data would be required to make firm conclusions regarding these hypotheses.
These preregistered analyses did not control for PIQ because it could be meaningfully related to both decision-making parameters and group membership, and investigating its contribution to both was beyond the scope of our multilevel modeling approach. However, as there was an indication of a relationship between PIQ and drift rate (Fig. 9), and as both PIQ and drift rate differed between the groups, we investigated these links further with an exploratory analysis, which partialled out the effects of both age and PIQ (Fig. 10). In brief, BFs of 2.3 and 2.38 in the two tasks continue to provide weak evidence for group differences in mean drift rate when both age and PIQ are controlled for.
Joint modeling of EEG and behavioral data
Figure 11 shows the distribution of slope measures that were extracted from each participant's deconvolved (with regularization) response-locked waveform, which were used in joint modeling to explore links between EEG and model parameters. While there was considerable between-participants variability, the children with dyslexia had shallower slopes than the typical children, on average. A Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVA in JASP (JASP Team, 2020) showed that, in the motion coherence task, the best model of EEG slope measures included the within-participants factor of difficulty level, the between-participants factor of group, and an interaction term. When averaging across models, there was strong evidence for including a main effect of group (BFincl = 14.70) and a group by difficulty level interaction (BFincl = 4.65). Yet in the direction integration task, the best model of EEG slope measures included only the within-participants factor of difficulty, with inconclusive evidence for including a main effect of group (BFincl = 0.70) or a group by difficulty level interaction (BFincl = 0.49). Therefore, it seems that the buildup of activity in the centro-parietal component is clearly reduced in children with dyslexia in the motion coherence task, but the reduction is not compelling in the direction integration task.
Next, we established whether this EEG measure was related to drift rate across the whole sample, estimating a single correlation for both groups, with the effects of age partialled out. For both tasks, the EEG measure was positively related to the mean drift rate across difficulty levels, although the evidence was only weak in the case of the direction integration task (motion coherence: posterior mean r = 0.44, 95% credible intervals (CI) = [0.26, 0.6], BF = 8869.49; direction integration: posterior mean r = 0.25, CI = [0.03, 0.45], BF = 1.65). The posterior means were in the direction of a positive relationship between the difference in EEG measure and the difference in drift rate between difficulty levels, although the evidence was inconclusive with relatively more evidence for the null hypothesis (motion coherence: posterior mean r = 0.22, CI = [–0.02, 0.44], BF = 0.73; direction integration: posterior mean r = 0.17, CI = [–0.08, 0.4], BF = 0.43; for scatterplots, see Fig. 12).
Next, we fit joint models in which we estimated a separate correlation coefficient between drift rate and the EEG measure for the children with dyslexia and typical children (Fig. 13). Our intention was not to explicitly test for differences in correlations between groups, but rather to see if the previous findings seem to hold for each group; any separation between the groups below is intended to merely describe our estimated posterior distributions. A positive correlation can be seen for both groups in the motion coherence task for the mean drift rate across difficulty levels (typical: posterior mean r = 0.41, CI = [0.13, 0.63], BF = 7.45; dyslexia: posterior mean r = 0.43, CI = [0.15, 0.64], BF = 12.75). The posterior means were in the direction of a positive relationship for the difference in drift rate between difficulty levels, but the evidence was inconclusive with relatively more evidence for the null hypothesis (typical: posterior mean r = 0.18, CI = [–0.2, 0.51], BF = 0.39; dyslexia: posterior mean r = 0.20, CI = [–0.12, 0.49], BF = 0.46). The strength of correlations was weaker in the direction integration task, particularly for the typical children, for whom the BFs suggested moderate evidence for no relationship (mean drift rate across difficulty levels: posterior mean r = 0.10, CI = [–0.22, 0.4], BF = 0.29; difference between difficulty levels: posterior mean r = 0.04, CI = [–0.31, 0.38], BF = 0.24). The strength of the correlations in children with dyslexia was slightly stronger than in the typical children, with the mean drift rate across difficulty levels showing weak evidence for a relationship, although the difference in drift rate between difficulty levels showed weak evidence for no relationship (mean drift rate across difficulty levels: posterior mean r = 0.34, CI = [0.04, 0.58], BF = 2.59; difference between difficulty levels: posterior mean r = 0.24, CI = [–0.09, 0.53], BF = 0.61).
Discussion
We analyzed the performance of children with dyslexia and typical children in two global motion tasks using diffusion modeling, to identify the processing stages that are altered in dyslexia. In both the motion coherence and direction integration tasks, children with dyslexia accumulated sensory evidence more slowly than typical children, on average, once controlling for age. Moreover, we found a neural correlate of this evidence accumulation process that was attenuated in dyslexia in the motion coherence task, thus linking brain and behavioral measures with a latent model parameter.
The finding of reduced evidence accumulation for children with dyslexia during the motion coherence task echoes O'Brien and Yeatman (2021) and helps to explain previous reports of elevated motion coherence thresholds in dyslexia (Benassi et al., 2010). Importantly, the current study goes further by showing that reduced evidence accumulation is also found in a direction integration task that does not require segregating signal dots from noise dots. This result suggests that dyslexic individuals have general difficulties with extracting global motion information, rather than solely difficulties with noise exclusion (in contrast to Sperling et al., 2006; Conlon et al., 2012), in line with reports of atypical performance in an illusory motion task without noise exclusion requirements (Gori et al., 2015, 2016). These general difficulties could reflect reduced temporal and/or spatial integration of motion signals (Raymond and Sorensen, 1998; Hill and Raymond, 2002; Benassi et al., 2010). This conclusion does not negate the possibility that dyslexic individuals face additional difficulties when segregating signal from noise, as we suggested based on stimulus-locked analyses using a similar dataset (Toffoli et al., 2021).
By supplementing our diffusion modeling analysis with EEG, we identified a neural index of reduced evidence accumulation in dyslexia. Specifically, we used a data-driven component decomposition technique to find a centro-parietal component previously linked to decision-making (O'Connell et al., 2012; Kelly and O'Connell, 2013; Manning et al., 2021a), and then “unmixed” overlapping stimulus- and response-locked activity. In the motion coherence task, we found that children with dyslexia showed a shallower buildup in the response-locked centro-parietal component compared with typical children, and the gradient of the buildup was positively correlated with drift rate in the joint model. While the EEG analysis was exploratory, the results are consistent with an earlier study of typically developing children (Manning et al., 2021a) and follow our hypothesized pattern (https://osf.io/enkwm). Similarly, Stefanac et al. (2021) reported reduced centro-parietal buildup in children with dyslexia compared with chronological and reading age-matched controls. Yet, in our direction integration task, we found no compelling evidence for reduced centro-parietal buildup in children with dyslexia, and the evidence for a relationship between this EEG measure and drift rate was weaker. This suggests that the magnitude of the centro-parietal positivity and its association with drift rate may be group- and task-dependent, to some extent (see also Lui et al., 2021).
Alongside reductions in drift rate, we hypothesized that children with dyslexia would show wider boundary separation compared with typically developing children, reflecting more cautious responses, and no differences in nondecision time. We found some evidence for increased boundary separation in children with dyslexia in the motion coherence task, but this was inconclusive. There was also inconclusive evidence for group differences in nondecision time. These results are not at odds with O'Brien and Yeatman (2021) but suggest that more data are required to reach a firm conclusion regarding these parameters. Seemingly, any group differences in these parameters are more subtle than group differences in drift rate. We note that the inferential method used by O'Brien and Yeatman (2021) differed from our own: while they also fit a hierarchical Bayesian model, they then extracted point estimates of diffusion model parameters for each individual to draw statistical inferences. Importantly, this means that O'Brien and Yeatman (2021) ignored the uncertainty in the individual-level parameters, which can inflate the evidence in favor of the winning model (Boehm et al., 2018; Evans and Wagenmakers, 2019).
Together with the results from stimulus-locked analyses using a similar dataset (Toffoli et al., 2021), our results suggest that early sensory encoding of motion information is not altered in children with dyslexia. While differences in drift rate cannot completely tease apart sensory and decision-making processes, in the current study we found no evidence of group differences in nondecision time, a measure that includes the time taken for sensory encoding. Moreover, Toffoli et al. (2021) showed that early peaks reflecting motion-specific processing were similar in children with dyslexia and typically developing children, with differences arising only after ∼430 ms following stimulus onset, specifically in the motion coherence task. The current analyses suggest that differences in dyslexia arise because of the efficiency with which evidence is extracted from global motion stimuli and integrated toward a decision bound, which is often attributed to parietal areas (Shadlen and Newsome, 1996, 2001; Hanks et al., 2006; de Lafuente et al., 2015). Without a comparable form task, it is unclear from the current study whether reduced evidence accumulation is restricted to tasks that tax the dorsal stream. However, we suggest that, within the magnocellular/dorsal stream, early sensory processing is unaffected in dyslexia with group differences emerging only at later processing stages, including those involved in decision-making. While this conclusion contrasts studies indicating early alterations of the magnocellular pathway in dyslexia (Livingstone et al., 1991; Stein and Walsh, 1997; Stein, 2001, 2019; Giraldo-Chica et al., 2015; Perani et al., 2021), the global motion tasks used in the current study are not ideally placed to isolate magnocellular processes (Skottun and Skoyles, 2006, 2008; Skottun, 2011, 2016). Future work will be required to determine how specific reduced evidence accumulation in dyslexia is to visual motion processing. Slower responses have been reported in dyslexia for other tasks (Nicolson and Fawcett, 1994; Catts et al., 2002), which could reflect pervasive reduced evidence accumulation, and reduced global integrative processes have been reported in static tasks in children with dyslexia (Franceschini et al., 2017a). However, slowed responses could arise for different reasons (e.g., increased nondecision time, wider boundary separation), so diffusion model decompositions on various tasks are required.
A number of future research directions emerge. What cognitive skills other than magnocellular/dorsal stream processing contribute to reduced drift rate in dyslexia? General processing speed is a unique predictor of word reading and comprehension (Christopher et al., 2012); and RAN is a recognized independent contributor to variation in reading ability, complementing phonological skills (e.g., O'Brien and Yeatman, 2021). Future work will need to establish the extent to which reduced processing speed and slower RAN associate with reduced drift rate in dyslexia. Additionally, PIQ varied across our two groups and was associated with drift rate. Exploratory models revealed that, even when controlling for both age and PIQ, there was still relatively more evidence for group differences in drift rate than no group differences. Yet the evidence was weaker than in models controlling only for age. Importantly, partialling out differences in PIQ could remove some of the variance related to the group differences we are interested in, as atypical development could lead to both dyslexia and reduced IQ (Dennis et al., 2009). Indeed, PIQ has been shown to strongly predict reading skills, independently of phonological skills (O'Brien and Yeatman, 2021). Future work will need to investigate the contribution of processing speed and PIQ to decision-making across the spectrum of reading abilities. Future research will also be required to explain the considerable between-participants variability in model and EEG parameters in children with and without dyslexia.
By combining diffusion modeling and EEG measures that are sensitive to the multiple processes contributing to motion perception, we have uncovered differences between children with dyslexia and typically developing children that could not be observed in behavioral responses alone. Moreover, diffusion modeling allows motion sensitivity to be measured without confounding speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Given that reduced behavioral sensitivity to motion has been reported in a range of other disorders (Braddick et al., 2003; Chen et al., 2003; McKendrick and Badcock, 2004), we suggest that diffusion modeling may provide a useful framework to identify convergence and divergence across different conditions, with implications for understanding the development of these conditions and their relationship to other cognitive processes.
Future work should establish whether differences in evidence accumulation of motion information contribute causally to the reading difficulties experienced by children with dyslexia. Some studies have suggested a causal relationship between motion perception and reading difficulties (e.g., Kevan and Pammer, 2009; Boets et al., 2011; Qian and Bi, 2015; Gori et al., 2016; Lawton, 2016; Ebrahimi et al., 2019), so it would be interesting to know whether evidence accumulation processes can be trained to improve reading ability. In support of this possibility, action video game training has been shown to improve motion perception by acting on the evidence accumulation phase (Green et al., 2010) and action video game training has also been linked to improved reading skills in children with dyslexia (Franceschini et al., 2013, 2017b; Bertoni et al., 2019, 2021; Franceschini and Bertoni, 2019). Such causal links will need to be investigated in future work using training or intervention designs.
Footnotes
The work was supported by Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship Grant 204685/Z/16/Z to C.M.; and James S. McDonnell Foundation Understanding Human Cognition Scholar Award to G.S. N.J.E. was supported by Australian Research Council Discovery Early Career Researcher Award DE200101130. We thank Dorothy Bishop for providing funding for research assistance. A CC BY or equivalent license is applied to the author accepted manuscript arising from this submission in accordance with the Wellcome Trust grant's open access conditions. We thank the participants and families who took part; the schools and organizations who kindly advertised the study; Irina Lepadatu, the Oxford Babylab, and Dhea Bengardi for help with recruitment; and Helena Wood, Lisa Toffoli, Madeleine Mills, Amber Heaton, and Kate Seaborne for help with data collection and data entry.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Benassi M, Simonelli L, Giovagnoli S, Bolzani R (2010) Coherence motion perception in developmental dyslexia: a meta-analysis of behavioural studies. Dyslexia 16:341–357. 10.1002/dys.412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertoni S, Franceschini S, Ronconi L, Gori S, Facoetti A (2019) Is excessive visual crowding causally linked to developmental dyslexia? Neuropsychologia 130:107–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertoni S, Franceschini S, Puccio G, Mancarella M, Gori S, Facoetti A (2021) Action video games enhance attentional control and phonological decoding in children with developmental dyslexia. Brain Sci 11:171. 10.3390/brainsci11020171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boehm U, Marsman M, Matzke D, Wagenmakers EJ (2018) On the importance of avoiding shortcuts in applying cognitive models to hierarchical data. Behav Res Methods 50:1614–1631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boets B, Vandermosten M, Cornelissen P, Wouters J, Ghesquière P (2011) Coherent motion sensitivity and reading development in the transition from prereading to reading stage. Child Dev 82:854–869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braddick O, Atkinson J, Wattam-Bell J (2003) Normal and anomalous development of visual motion processing: motion coherence and 'dorsal-stream vulnerability.' Neuropsychologia 41:1769–1784. 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00178-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catts HW, Gillispie M, Leonard LB, Kail RV, Miller CA (2002) The role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. J Learn Disabil 35:510–525. 10.1177/00222194020350060301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang CY, Hsu SH, Pion-Tonachini L, Jung TP (2018) Evaluation of artifact subspace reconstruction for automatic EEG artifact removal. 40th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC) 1242-1245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen Y, Nakayama K, Levy D, Matthysse S, Holzman P (2003) Processing of global, but not local, motion direction is deficient in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 61:215–227. 10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00222-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christopher ME, Miyake A, Keenan JM, Pennington B, DeFries JC, Wadsworth SJ, Willcutt E, Olson RK (2012) Predicting word reading and comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: a latent variable analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen 141:470–488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conlon EG, Lilleskaret G, Wright CM, Power GF (2012) The influence of contrast on coherent motion processing in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 50:1672–1681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Lafuente V, Jazayeri M, Shadlen MN (2015) Representation of accumulating evidence for a decision in two parietal areas. J Neurosci 35:4306–4318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including Independent Component Analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Francis DJ, Cirino PT, Schachar R, Barnes MA, Fletcher JM (2009) Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 15:331–343. 10.1017/S1355617709090481 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dmochowski JP, Norcia AM (2015) Cortical components of reaction-time during perceptual decisions in humans. PLoS One 10:e0143339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dmochowski JP, Sajda P, Dias J, Parra LC (2012) Correlated components of ongoing EEG point to emotionally laden attention–a possible marker of engagement? Front Hum Neurosci, 6:112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dutilh G, Vandekerckhove J, Ly A, Matzke D, Pedroni A, Frey R, Rieskamp J, Wagenmakers EJ (2017) A test of the diffusion model explanation for the worst performance rule using preregistration and blinding. Atten Percept Psychophys 79:713–725. 10.3758/s13414-017-1304-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebrahimi L, Pouretemad H, Khatibi A, Stein J (2019) Magnocellular based visual motion training improves reading in Persian. Sci Rep 9:1–10. 10.1038/s41598-018-37753-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehinger BV, Dimigen O (2019) Unfold: an integrated toolbox for overlap correction, non-linear modelling, and regression-based EEG analysis. PeerJ 7:e7838. 10.7717/peerj.7838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans NJ (2019) Assessing the practical differences between model selection methods in inferences about choice response time tasks. Psychon Bull Rev 26:1070–1098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans NJ, Brown SD (2017) People adopt optimal policies in simple decision-making, after practice and guidance. Psychon Bull Rev 24:597–606. 10.3758/s13423-016-1135-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans NJ, Hawkins GE (2019) When humans behave like monkeys: feedback delays and extensive practice increase the efficiency of speeded decisions. Cognition 184:11–18. 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans NJ, Steyvers M, Brown SD (2018) Modeling the covariance structure of complex datasets using cognitive models: an application to individual differences and the heritability of cognitive ability. Cogn Sci 42:1925–1944. 10.1111/cogs.12627 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans NJ, Wagenmakers EJ (2019) Theoretically meaningful models can answer clinically relevant questions. Brain 142:1172–1175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans NJ, Wagenmakers EJ (2020) Evidence accumulation models: current limitations and future directions. TQMP 16:73–90. 10.20982/tqmp.16.2.p073 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Evans NJ, Bennett AJ, Brown SD (2019) Optimal or not; depends on the task. Psychon Bull Rev 26:1027–1034. 10.3758/s13423-018-1536-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franceschini S, Bertoni S (2019) Improving action video games abilities increases the phonological decoding speed and phonological short-term memory in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 130:100–106. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.10.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franceschini S, Gori S, Ruffino M, Viola S, Molteni M, Facoetti A (2013) Action video games make dyslexic children read better. Curr Biol 23:462–466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franceschini S, Bertoni S, Gianesini T, Gori S, Facoetti A (2017a) A different vision of dyslexia: local precedence on global perception. Sci Rep 7:17462. 10.1038/s41598-017-17626-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franceschini S, Trevisan P, Ronconi L, Bertoni S, Colmar S, Double K, Facoetti A, Gori S (2017b) Action video games improve reading abilities and visual-to-auditory attentional shifting in English-speaking children with dyslexia. Sci Rep 7:1–12. 10.1038/s41598-017-05826-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelman A, Rubin DB (1992) Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Statist Sci 7:457–511. 10.1214/ss/1177011136 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Giraldo-Chica M, Hegarty JP II, Schneider KA (2015) Morphological differences in the lateral geniculate nucleus associated with dyslexia. Neuroimage Clin 7:830–836. 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.03.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gori S, Mascheretti S, Giora E, Ronconi L, Ruffino M, Quadrelli E, Facoetti A, Marino C (2015) The DCDC2 Intron 2 deletion impairs illusory motion perception unveiling the selective role of magnocellular-dorsal stream in reading (dis)ability. Cereb Cortex 25:1685–1695. 10.1093/cercor/bhu234 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gori S, Seitz AR, Ronconi L, Franceschini S, Facoetti A (2016) Multiple causal links between magnocellular–dorsal pathway deficit and developmental dyslexia. Cereb Cortex 26:4356–4369. 10.1093/cercor/bhv206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami U (2015) Sensory theories of developmental dyslexia: three challenges for research. Nat Rev Neurosci 16:43–54. 10.1038/nrn3836 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green CS, Pouget A, Bavelier D (2010) Improved probabilistic inference as a general learning mechanism with action video games. Curr Biol 20:1573–1579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanks TD, Ditterich J, Shadlen MN (2006) Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task. Nat Neurosci 9:682–689. 10.1038/nn1683 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansen PC, Stein JF, Orde SR, Winter JL, Talcott JB (2001) Are dyslexics' visual deficits limited to measures of dorsal stream function? Neuroreport 12:1527–1530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henson R, Rugg MD, Friston KJ (2001) The choice of basis functions in event-related fMRI. Neuroimage 13:149. 10.1016/S1053-8119(01)91492-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hill GT, Raymond JE (2002) Deficits of motion transparency perception in adult developmental dyslexics with normal unidirectional motion sensitivity. Vision Res 42: 1195–1203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinshelwood J (1896) A case of dyslexia: a peculiar form of word-blindness. Lancet 148:1451–1454. 10.1016/S0140-6736(01)60603-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ho DE, Imai K, King G, Stuart EA (2011) MatchIt: nonparametric preprocessing for parametric causal inference. J Stat Soft 42:1–28. 10.18637/jss.v042.i08 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Howard ZL, Evans NJ, Innes RJ, Brown SD, Eidels A (2020) How is multi-tasking different from increased difficulty? Psychon Bull Rev 27:937–951. 10.3758/s13423-020-01741-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- JASP Team. (2020) JASP (version 0.14.1). [Google Scholar]
- Jeffreys H (1961) Theory of probability. Oxford: Oxford UP. [Google Scholar]
- Joo SJ, Donnelly PM, Yeatman JD (2017) The causal relationship between dyslexia and motion perception reconsidered. Sci Rep 7:1–7. 10.1038/s41598-017-04471-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly SP, O'Connell RG (2013) Internal and external influences on the rate of sensory evidence accumulation in the human brain. J Neurosci 33:19434–19441. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3355-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kevan A, Pammer K (2009) Predicting early reading skills from pre-reading measures of dorsal stream functioning. Neuropsychologia 47:3174–3181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D (2007) What's new in Psychtoolbox-3?. Perception, 36(ECVP Abstract Supplement), 14. [Google Scholar]
- Knowles JP, Evans NJ, Burke D (2019) Some evidence for an association between early life adversity and decision urgency. Front Psychol 10:243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kristensen E, Guerin-Dugué A, Rivet B (2017) Regularization and a general linear model for event-related potential estimation. Behav Res 49:2255–2274. 10.3758/s13428-017-0856-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawton T (2016) Improving dorsal stream function in dyslexics by training figure/ground motion discrimination improves attention, reading fluency, and working memory. Front Hum Neurosci 10:397. 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Livingstone M, Hubel D (1988) Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science 240:740–749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Livingstone MS, Rosen GD, Drislane FW, Galaburda AM (1991) Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci 88:7943–7947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lovegrove WJ, Bowling A, Badcock D, Blackwood M (1980) Specific reading disability: differences in contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency. Science 210:439–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lui KK, Nunez MD, Cassidy JM, Vandekerckhove J, Cramer SC, Srinivasan R (2021) Timing of readiness potentials reflects a decision-making process in the human brain. Comput Brain Behav 4:264–283. 10.1007/s42113-020-00097-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning C, Kaneshiro B, Kohler PJ, Duta M, Scerif G, Norcia AM (2019) Neural dynamics underlying coherent motion perception in children and adults. Dev Cogn Neurosci 38:100670. 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100670 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning C, Wagenmakers EJ, Norcia AM, Scerif G, Boehm U (2021a) Perceptual decision-making in children: age-related differences and EEG correlates. Comput Brain Behav 4:53–69. 10.1007/s42113-020-00087-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning C, Hassall CD, Hunt LT, Norcia AM, Wagenmakers EJ, Evans NJ, Scerif G (2021b) Behavioural and neural indices of perceptual decision-making in autistic children during visual motion tasks. PsyArXiv. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKendrick AM, Badcock DR (2004) Motion processing deficits in migraine. Cephalalgia 24:363–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morey RD, Rouder JN (2018) BayesFactor: computation of Bayes Factors for Common Designs. R package version 0.9.12-4.2. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=BayesFactor.
- Newsome WT, Paré EB (1988) A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). J Neurosci 8:2201–2211. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-06-02201.1988 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ (1994) Reaction times and dyslexia. Q J Exp Psychol A 47:29–48. 10.1080/14640749408401142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Brien G, Yeatman J (2021) Bridging sensory and language theories of dyslexia: toward a multifactorial model. Dev Sci 24:e13039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connell RG, Dockree PM, Kelly SP (2012) A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans. Nat Neurosci 15:1729–1737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olulade OA, Napoliello EM, Eden GF (2013) Abnormal visual motion processing is not a cause of dyslexia. Neuron 79:180–190. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perani D, Scifo P, Cicchini GM, Della Rosa P, Banfi C, Mascheretti S (2021) White matter deficits correlate with visual motion perception impairments in dyslexic carriers of the DCDC2 genetic risk variant. Exp Brain Res 239:2725–2740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piotrowska B, Willis A (2019) Beyond the global motion deficit hypothesis of developmental dyslexia: a cross-sectional study of visual, cognitive, and socio-economic factors influencing reading ability in children. Vision Res 159:48–60. 10.1016/j.visres.2019.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qian Y, Bi HY (2015) The effect of magnocellular-based visual-motor intervention on Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Front Psychol 6:1529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ratcliff R (1978) A theory of memory retrieval. Psychol Rev 85:59–108. 10.1037/0033-295X.85.2.59 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Raymond JE, Sorensen RE (1998) Visual motion perception in children with dyslexia: normal detection but abnormal integration. Vis Cogn 5:389–404. 10.1080/713756790 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Shadlen MN, Newsome WT (1996) Motion perception: seeing and deciding. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:628–633. 10.1073/pnas.93.2.628 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadlen MN, Newsome WT (2001) Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 86:1916–1936. 10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.1916 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skottun BC (2011) On the use of visual motion perception to assess magnocellular integrity. J Integr Neurosci 10:15–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skottun BC (2016) A few remarks on the utility of visual motion perception to assess the integrity of the magnocellular system or the dorsal stream. Cortex 79:155–158. 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skottun BC, Skoyles JR (2006) Is coherent motion an appropriate test for magnocellular sensitivity? Brain Cogn 61:172–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skottun BC, Skoyles JR (2008) Coherent motion, magnocellular sensitivity and the causation of dyslexia. Int J Neurosci 118:185–190. 10.1080/00207450601041872 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith NJ, Kutas M (2015) Regression-based estimation of ERP waveforms: I. The rERP framework. Psychophysiology 52:157–168. 10.1111/psyp.12317 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling MJ, Nash HM, Gooch DC, Hayiou-Thomas ME, Hulme C, Wellcome Language and Reading Project Team (2019a) Developmental outcomes for children at high risk of dyslexia and children with developmental language disorder. Child Dev 90:e548–e564. 10.1111/cdev.13216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling MJ, Hayiou-Thomas ME, Nash HM, Hulme C (2019b) Dyslexia and developmental language disorder: comorbid disorders with distinct effects on reading comprehension. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 61:672–680. 10.1111/jcpp.13140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sperling AJ, Lu ZL, Manis FR, Seidenberg MS (2006) Motion-perception deficits and reading impairment: it's the noise, not the motion. Psychol Sci 17:1047–1053. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01825.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stafford T, Pirrone A, Croucher M, Krystalli A (2020) Quantifying the benefits of using decision models with response time and accuracy data. Behav Res Methods 52:2142–2155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stefanac NR, Zhou SH, Spencer-Smith MM, O'Connell R, Bellgrove MA (2021) A neural index of inefficient evidence accumulation in dyslexia underlying slow perceptual decision making. Cortex 142:122–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein J (2001) The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 7:12–36. 10.1002/dys.186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein J (2019) The current status of the magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 130:66–77. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein J, Walsh V (1997) To see but not to read: the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends Neurosci 20:147–152. 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)01005-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stone M (1960) Models for choice-reaction time. Psychometrika 25:251–260. 10.1007/BF02289729 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ter Braak CJ (2006) A Markov Chain Monte Carlo version of the genetic algorithm Differential Evolution: easy Bayesian computing for real parameter spaces. Stat Comput 16:239–249. 10.1007/s11222-006-8769-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Toffoli L, Scerif G, Snowling MJ, Norcia A, Manning C (2021) Global motion evoked potentials in autistic and dyslexic children: a cross-syndrome approach. Cortex 143:109–126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA (2012) Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE-2), Ed 2. Austin: Pro-Ed. [Google Scholar]
- Turner BM, Forstmann BU, Wagenmakers EJ, Brown SD, Sederberg PB, Steyvers M (2013a) A Bayesian framework for simultaneously modeling neural and behavioral data. Neuroimage 72:193–206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner BM, Sederberg PB, Brown SD, Steyvers M (2013b) A method for efficiently sampling from distributions with correlated dimensions. Psychol Methods 18:368–384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner BM, Van Maanen L, Forstmann BU (2015) Informing cognitive abstractions through neuroimaging: the neural drift diffusion model. Psychol Rev 122:312–336. 10.1037/a0038894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner BM, Rodriguez CA, Norcia AM, McClure SM, Steyvers M (2016) Why more is better: simultaneous modeling of EEG, fMRI, and behavioral data. Neuroimage 128:96–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulrich R, Miller J (1994) Effects of truncation on reaction time analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen 123:34–80. 10.1037/0096-3445.123.1.34 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D (2011) WASI-II: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Ed 2. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D (2017) Wechsler Individual Achievement Test – Third UK Edition: Pearson, Inc. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Analysis scripts are available at https://osf.io/nvwf7/. Data and output files are available on the United Kingdom Data Service at http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-855236.