Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Prev Med. 2021 Oct 20;62(2):183–192. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2021.07.016

Storybooks About Healthy Beverage Consumption: Effects in an Online Randomized Experiment With Parents

Anna H Grummon 1,2, Rebeccah L Sokol 3, Dina Goodman 4, Christina A Hecht 5, Meg Salvia 6, Aviva A Musicus 7, Anisha I Patel 8,9
PMCID: PMC8748291  NIHMSID: NIHMS1750108  PMID: 34688521

Abstract

Introduction:

Parents spend substantial time reading to their children, making storybooks a promising but understudied avenue for motivating parents to serve their children healthier beverages. This study examines parents’ reactions to messages promoting healthy beverage consumption embedded in a children’s storybook.

Methods:

In 2020, a total of 2,164 demographically diverse parents of children aged 6 months to 5 years participated in an online survey. Participants were randomized to view control messages (school readiness) or 1 of 3 beverage message topics (sugary drink discouragement, water encouragement, or combined discouragement and encouragement) presented as pages from the storybook Potter the Otter. Survey items assessed parents’ reactions to the messages and their perceptions, beliefs, and intentions regarding sugary drinks and water. Data were analyzed in 2021.

Results:

Compared with control messages, exposure to the beverage messages led to higher discouragement from serving children sugary drinks and higher encouragement to serve children more water (p-values <0.001). The beverage messages also elicited more thinking about beverages’ health effects and led to stronger perceptions that sugary drinks are unhealthy (p-values <0.001). Moreover, the beverage messages led to higher intentions to limit serving children sugary drinks and higher intentions to serve children more water (p-values ≤0.02). Parents’ reactions to the beverage messages did not differ by most demographic characteristics. Few differences in outcomes were observed among the 3 beverage message topics.

Conclusions:

Embedding beverage messages in storybooks is a promising, scalable strategy for motivating parents from diverse backgrounds to serve children more water and fewer sugary drinks.

INTRODUCTION

Establishing healthy beverage consumption patterns in early childhood (age 0–5 years) remains a critical public health goal.1 Consumption of sugary drinks and juice in childhood contributes to weight gain, obesity, type 2 diabetes, and dental caries.24 Conversely, water consumption may help limit excess weight gain and these poor health outcomes.1,5 On a typical day, however, 47% of U.S. children aged 2–5 years consume sugary drinks and 22% do not consume any water.6 Sugary drink consumption is even higher among children whose parents have lower incomes or identify as Black or Latinx,7 contributing to preventable health inequities.8,9 Scalable strategies are needed to reduce sugary drink consumption and promote water consumption among young children.

Parents play an important role in influencing what young children eat and drink10 and are therefore an important group to reach with healthy beverage interventions. Moreover, healthy eating and obesity prevention interventions may be most successful when they engage parents and children simultaneously.1113 One promising but understudied avenue for such interventions is children’s storybooks. As an “education entertainment” intervention, storybooks can use relatable characters, entertaining narratives, and child-friendly language to promote healthy behaviors.14 The average U.S. child aged 2–4 years spends 29 minutes/day reading or being read to,15 providing ample opportunity for storybooks to reach families with messages encouraging healthy beverage consumption. Research suggests that communicating with parents about foods and beverages through warning labels,16 TV advertisements,17 and text messages18 can change their knowledge, intentions, and behaviors regarding the foods and beverages they serve their children. If storybooks exert similar influence, they would represent a highly scalable strategy for motivating parents to serve their children healthier beverages. However, the potential for storybooks to influence parents’ beverage-related behaviors remains unknown.

Uncertainty also remains about how to design storybooks and other beverage messaging interventions to maximize their effectiveness. Some prior interventions have focused only on discouraging sugary drink consumption, others have focused only on encouraging water consumption, and others combine messages that discourage sugary drinks with messages that encourage water.1921 One hypothesis is that interventions focused on promoting water consumption would be unlikely to impact sugary drink consumption, and vice versa. Alternatively, messaging about one behavior could affect related behaviors if consumers interpret the message to imply something about the related behavior. For example, consumers might understand that a message promoting water consumption implies that sugary drink consumption is unhealthy.22 Understanding which message topics are most promising could inform interventions, but limited experimental research has compared message topics.19

The primary objective of this study is to evaluate parents’ reactions to messages about healthy beverages presented in a children’s storybook. Analyses also assess whether parents’ message reactions differ by demographic characteristics relevant for health disparities (e.g., race/ethnicity, income). Finally, analyses compare parents’ reactions to messages that only discourage sugary drinks, only encourage water, or combined discouragement and encouragement messages.

METHODS

Prior to data collection, the analysis plan and hypotheses were pre-registered on AsPredicted.org (https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=dn8b5v).

Study Sample

In May 2020, a convenience sample of 2,164 adults was recruited through Dynata, a survey research firm. Participants were eligible if they lived in the U.S., were aged ≥18 years, and were a parent or other caregiver (hereafter “parent”) of ≥1 child aged 6 months to 5 years. Online convenience samples can yield generalizable findings for experiments such as the one used in this study.23 Because beverage consumption habits in young children vary by race/ethnicity,7 this study sought to recruit a diverse sample such that ≥25% of parents would identify as Latinx and ≥25% would identify as Black (not mutually exclusive). Participants who completed the study received previously agreed upon incentives from Dynata in the form of reward points redeemable for gift cards, charitable contributions, or products. The Harvard Longwood Campus IRB approved the study.

Participants provided informed consent and completed an online survey programmed in Qualtrics. The survey asked participants to think about their child aged 6 months to 5 years. To provide an even distribution of ages, participants who had multiple children in this age range were instructed to think about the child who had the most recent birthday. Participants first indicated how often they served this child different beverages,24 then viewed 8 messages. To provide realism, messages were presented as pages from Potter the Otter,25 a children’s book series covering health and educational topics developed by FIRST 5 Santa Clara County, a nonprofit organization supporting healthy development of children prenatally through age 5 years.25 This study adapted messages and images from the Potter the Otter series because it is targeted to young children and includes a book focused specifically on healthy beverages.

The survey software randomly assigned participants with a simple allocation ratio to a control condition or to 1 of 4 beverage message conditions, each focusing on a different topic: sugary drink discouragement, water encouragement, combined discouragement and encouragement messages that alternated these message topics, or combined discouragement and encouragement messages that integrated these message topics. The control condition presented school readiness messages that did not discuss beverages or health, using text and imagery adapted from the book Potter Gets Ready for Kindergarten. The sugary drink discouragement condition presented text and imagery from the book Potter the Otter: A Tale About Water modified to only discourage sugary drinks without mentioning or showing water. Similarly, the water encouragement condition presented messages from Potter the Otter: A Tale About Water modified to only encourage water without mentioning or showing sugary drinks. The combined discouragement and encouragement condition that alternated topics showed pages from the discouragement and encouragement conditions in a randomly alternating order. Finally, the combined discouragement and encouragement condition that integrated topics presented unmodified messages and imagery from Potter the Otter: A Tale About Water, which includes messages discouraging sugary drinks and encouraging water on most pages. A graphic designer prepared the messages to ensure optimal online viewing. Figure 1 depicts example pages from each condition and Appendix Exhibit 1 shows all stimuli.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Examples of stimuli used in randomized experiment of beverage messages embedded in a children’s storybook.

Notes: Participants in the combined discouragement and encouragement, alternating messages condition, saw messages from the sugary drink discouragement condition and water encouragement condition in a randomly alternating order.

Participants were instructed to imagine they were reading their assigned messages aloud to their child. After viewing the messages, participants responded to survey questions as described in the Measures section. Participants could choose to take the survey in English or Spanish. A professional translation company translated study materials from English to Spanish.

Measures

The co-primary outcomes were perceived message effectiveness for discouraging parents from serving sugary drinks to their child (“discouragement”) and perceived message effectiveness for encouraging parents to serve water to their child (“encouragement”). Prior research has found that perceived message effectiveness is predictive of messages’ actual effects on intentions and behavior across a variety of behaviors, including smoking,2628 vaping,29,30 alcohol consumption,31 marijuana use,32 dental flossing,31 and sugary drink consumption.33 Thus, perceived effectiveness has predictive value as to messages’ potential to elicit behavior change. Discouragement and encouragement were each assessed with 3 items adapted from the previously-validated UNC Perceived Message Effectiveness Scale34 (e.g., The messages in the book discouraged me from wanting to serve my child soda, juice, juice pouches, or punch). Items used 5-point response scales coded as 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Both scales showed acceptable reliability (Cronbach’s α: discouragement=0.81, encouragement=0.87).

The survey also assessed outcomes from the Message Impact Framework and Tobacco Warnings Model35,36 (hereafter “Message Impact Framework”) because a randomized trial found that these constructs mediated the effect of sugary drink messages on beverage purchase behaviors,37 meaning that these constructs hold predictive value as to messages’ ability to change behavior. Outcomes from this model were: thinking about the health effects of beverages children drink, negative emotional reactions to the messages, parents’ intentions to limit serving their child sugary drinks, and parents’ intentions to increase serving their child water. Finally, the survey assessed positive emotional reactions to the messages, beverage healthfulness perceptions, parents’ injunctive social norms about serving beverages to their child, and expectations about the consequences of drinking sugary drinks and water. These outcomes used 5- or 7-point response scales coded as 1 (low) to 5 or 7 (high). Survey items appear in Appendix Exhibit 2.

Statistical Analysis

Power analyses indicated that the target enrollment of approximately 2,100 participants would provide >90% power to detect a small effect (d=0.2) of any beverage message compared with control, assuming α=0.05. The analytic sample included 2,164 participants (Appendix Figure 1).

Analyses used ordinary least squares regression to examine the impact of exposure to any message about beverages (compared with control) on the co-primary outcomes, discouragement from serving sugary drinks and encouragement to serve water. These analyses collapsed across the 4 beverage message conditions and did not control for participants characteristics, which were balanced across study groups. Analyses used the same approach to examine the impact of the beverage messages on constructs from the Message Impact Framework and on other outcomes.

To examine whether the effect of beverage messages on the co-primary outcomes varied by demographic characteristics, analyses regressed the outcomes on participant characteristics, an indicator for treatment arm (any beverage message versus control), and the interaction between participant characteristics and treatment arm. Analyses used separate models for each co-primary outcome and each characteristic and examined 9 potential moderators: child’s age, frequency of serving water, frequency of serving sugary drinks, education, race, Latinx ethnicity, language of survey administration, participation in the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program (SNAP) or the Special Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) in the past 12 months, and household income. Analyses calculated treatment effects at each level of the moderator.

Secondary analyses assessed whether the 3 beverage message topics (sugary drink discouragement, water encouragement, or combined discouragement and encouragement) differed from one another in their impacts on the co-primary outcomes. Analyses regressed the outcomes on indicator variables for each message topic (excluding the control as the ref) and used Wald tests to compare the impact of each beverage message topic (versus control) to one another. Combined discouragement and encouragement messages that alternated topics performed similarly to those that integrated topics (p-values >0.36 for differences between these conditions on co-primary outcomes), so analyses collapsed across these conditions for simplicity.

To facilitate comparisons across outcomes measured on different scales, all dependent variables were standardized prior to running analyses. Results report unstandardized regression coefficients (Bs). Analyses were conducted in Stata MP, version 16.1 in 2021.

RESULTS

Participants’ average age was 30.2 (SD=9.3) years. Children’s ages were approximately evenly distributed across the age categories. About half (49%) of the sample identified as Latinx. Additionally, 44% identified as Black or African American and 32% as White (regardless of ethnicity). Participants’ characteristics did not differ by study condition (p>0.07 for all) (Table 1).

Table 1.

Participant Characteristics, n=2,164 Parents of Children Ages 6 Months to 5 Years

Characteristic Message condition
Control n=426 Sugary drink discouragement n=430 Water encouragement n=416 Combined discouragement and encouragement, alternating messages n=457 Combined discouragement and encouragement, integrated messages n=435

N % N % N % N % N %
Child characteristics
 Age
  6–11 months 71 17% 82 19% 70 17% 75 16% 47 11%
  12–23 months 71 17% 44 10% 53 13% 50 11% 60 14%
  2 years 73 17% 80 19% 80 19% 82 18% 77 18%
  3 years 76 18% 78 18% 71 17% 81 18% 84 19%
  4 years 66 15% 68 16% 73 18% 73 16% 76 17%
  5 years 69 16% 78 18% 69 17% 96 21% 91 21%
Parent characteristics
 Age
  18–24 years 110 26% 123 30% 131 32% 130 29% 116 27%
  25–34 years 185 44% 185 45% 166 41% 189 42% 198 47%
  35–44 years 103 25% 93 22% 83 20% 91 20% 93 22%
  ≥45 years 18 4% 14 3% 28 7% 36 8% 18 4%
 Gender
  Man 134 32% 111 26% 112 27% 130 29% 112 26%
  Woman 273 65% 304 71% 290 70% 314 69% 309 71%
  Transgender, nonbinary, or another gender identity 16 4% 13 3% 13 3% 12 3% 14 3%
 Relationship to child
  Parent 347 82% 355 83% 344 83% 363 79% 367 84%
  Other caregivera 77 18% 74 17% 72 17% 94 21% 68 16%
 Educational attainment
  Less than high school or GED 28 7% 25 6% 20 5% 28 6% 22 5%
  High school diploma or GED 81 19% 78 18% 92 22% 93 20% 91 21%
  Some college 61 14% 93 22% 88 21% 89 20% 80 18%
  2-year college degree or higher 253 60% 232 54% 214 52% 246 54% 242 56%
 Race
  White 139 33% 136 32% 124 30% 155 34% 146 34%
  Black or African American 198 47% 189 44% 185 45% 196 43% 189 44%
  American Indian or Alaska Native 18 4% 12 3% 18 4% 11 2% 18 4%
  Asian or Pacific Islander 14 3% 13 3% 12 3% 17 4% 10 2%
  Other or Multiracial 54 13% 78 18% 76 18% 75 17% 71 16%
  Latino(a) ethnicity 207 49% 209 49% 209 50% 226 50% 214 49%
 Language selected for survey administration
  English 379 89% 384 89% 373 90% 402 88% 395 91%
  Spanish 47 11% 46 11% 43 10% 55 12% 40 9%
 Participation in SNAP or WIC in past 12 months 217 51% 256 60% 244 59% 254 56% 243 56%
 Household size
  1 56 13% 38 9% 35 8% 37 8% 33 8%
  2 80 19% 92 21% 92 22% 90 20% 80 18%
  3 110 26% 100 23% 108 26% 134 29% 118 27%
  4 or more 180 42% 200 47% 181 44% 196 43% 204 47%
 Annual household income
  Less than $25,000 102 24% 119 28% 107 26% 117 26% 115 27%
  $25,000–49,999 107 25% 90 21% 103 25% 114 25% 111 26%
  $50,000–74,999 61 14% 76 18% 73 18% 74 16% 72 17%
  $75,000–99,999 60 14% 53 12% 52 13% 67 15% 51 12%
  $100,000 or more 94 22% 91 21% 80 19% 85 19% 84 19%
 Frequency of serving water to child
  Never or <1 time per week 32 8% 28 7% 21 5% 27 6% 20 5%
  1–6 times per week 139 33% 161 38% 145 35% 162 36% 143 33%
  1– <2 times per day 35 8% 31 7% 40 10% 41 9% 40 9%
  ≥2 times a day 218 51% 207 48% 208 50% 221 49% 230 53%
 Frequency of serving sugary drinks to child
  Never or <1 time per week 29 7% 25 6% 33 8% 36 8% 38 9%
  1–6 times per week 156 37% 155 36% 166 40% 169 37% 152 35%
  1– <2 times per Day 109 26% 116 27% 99 24% 119 26% 104 24%
  ≥2 times a day 132 31% 134 31% 118 28% 133 29% 141 32%

Notes: Missing data ranged from 0 to 2.5%. Participant characteristics did not differ by experimental condition. Participant characteristics did not differ by study condition (all ps>0.07).

a

Other types of caregivers included grandparents, aunts, uncles, and legal guardians.

GED, General Education Development test; SNAP, Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program; WIC, Special Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program for Women, Infants, and Children.

Participants exposed to the beverage messages reported higher discouragement from serving their children sugary drinks than parents exposed to the control messages (B=0.57, p<0.001) (Table 2). Likewise, the beverage messages led to higher encouragement to serve their children water compared with control (B=0.60, p<0.001).

Table 2.

Impacts of Storybooks About Healthy Beverage Consumption, n=2,164 Parents of Children Aged 6 Months–5 Years

Outcome (response scale) Beverage messages Mean (SD) Control messages Mean (SD) Impact of beverage messages B 95% CI
Co-primary outcomes
 Discouragement from serving sugary drinks (1–5) 3.7 (1.0) 3.1 (1.2) 0.57 (0.47, 0.67)
 Encouragement to serve water (1–5) 3.9 (1.1) 3.2 (1.2) 0.60 (0.50, 0.70)
Message Impact Framework outcomes
 Thinking about beverages’ health effects (1–5) 3.7 (1.2) 2.9 (1.5) 0.63 (0.53, 0.74)
 Negative emotions (1–5) 2.4 (1.2) 2.1 (1.2) 0.20 (0.09, 0.30)
 Intentions to limit serving sugary drinks (1–7) 4.6 (1.8) 4.3 (1.7) 0.13 (0.02, 0.23)
 Intentions to increase serving water (1–7) 5.8 (1.5) 5.6 (1.6) 0.13 (0.02, 0.23)
Other outcomes
 Positive emotions (1–5) 3.5 (1.1) 3.4 (1.1) 0.09 (−0.02, 0.19)
 Perceptions that sugary drinks are unhealthy (1–7) 4.8 (1.5) 4.4 (1.5) 0.26 (0.15, 0.36)
 Perceptions that water is healthy (1–7) 6.3 (1.3) 6.3 (1.3) 0.03 (−0.07, 0.14)
 Social norms to limit sugary drinks (1–5) 3.5 (1.0) 3.5 (1.1) 0.02 (−0.08, 0.13)
 Social norms to increase water (1–5) 3.9 (1.0) 3.9 (1.1) 0.04 (−0.07, 0.14)
 Beliefs that sugary drinks lead to negative outcomes (1–7) 4.7 (1.4) 4.5 (1.3) 0.14 (0.04, 0.25)
 Beliefs that water leads to positive outcomes (1–7) 5.7 (1.3) 5.5 (1.2) 0.13 (0.02, 0.23)

Note: Table shows means and SDs for each outcome for participants exposed to the beverage messages (collapsed across all beverage message topics), means and SDs for each outcome for participants exposed the control messages, and impacts of beverage messages compared to the control condition. Impacts are shown as unstandardized regression coefficients (Bs) from regressions of standardized dependent variables. All response scales are Likert-type scales coded as 1 (low) to 5 or 7 (high). Boldface indicates statistically significant impact of beverage messages compared to the control condition, p<0.05.

Exposure to beverage messages also influenced constructs from the Message Impact Framework. Beverage messages elicited more thinking about beverages’ health effects (B=0.63, p<0.001) (Table 2) and more negative emotional reactions (B=0.20, p<0.001) compared with control messages. Beverage messages also led to higher intentions to limit serving sugary drinks (B=0.13, p=0.02) and higher intentions to increase serving water (B=0.13, p=0.02).

Additionally, beverage messages led parents to hold stronger perceptions that sugary drinks are unhealthy (B=0.26, p<0.001), stronger beliefs that drinking sugary drinks leads to negative outcomes for their child (B=0.14, p=0.008), and stronger beliefs that drinking water leads to positive outcomes for their child (B=0.13, p=0.02). By contrast, no differences were observed between beverage messages and the control for positive emotions, perceptions that water is healthy, social norms to limit serving sugary drinks to their child, or social norms to increase serving water to their child (p>0.11 for all).

In moderation analyses, the impact of beverage messages on discouragement from serving sugary drinks was stronger among parents of children aged 1–5 years compared with parents of children aged 6–11 months (p=0.005 for interaction) (Appendix Table 1). Additionally, the effect of beverage messages on discouragement was stronger among participants who reported serving water more frequently than those who reported doing so less frequently (p<0.001 for interaction) and among parents who reported not participating in SNAP or WIC compared with those who reported participating in the prior 12 months (p<0.001 for interaction). The impact of beverage messages on discouragement did not differ by any of the other participant characteristics (p>0.05 for all).

A similar pattern of moderation emerged for beverage messages’ impacts on encouragement to serve water. Beverage messages had a larger effect on encouragement among parents who reported serving their child water more often (p<0.001 for interaction) and among parents not participating in SNAP or WIC (p=0.02 for interaction) (Appendix Table 2). The impact of beverage messages on encouragement did not differ by any other characteristics (p>0.05 for all).

The 3 beverage message topics (sugary drink discouragement, water encouragement, and combined discouragement and encouragement) did not differ from one another in their effects on discouragement (Bs=0.51–0.61; p>0.09 for all comparisons of coefficients) (Figure 2) or encouragement (Bs=0.53–0.63; p>0.09 for all comparisons of coefficients). Beverage message topics also did not differ in their influence on most constructs from the Message Impact Framework. One exception was that water encouragement messages had a smaller effect on negative emotions (B=0.06) compared with both the sugary drink discouragement messages (B=0.29, p=0.001 for comparison of coefficients) and the combined discouragement and encouragement messages (B=0.22, p=0.006 for comparison of coefficients). No consistent pattern emerged in comparisons of the different beverage message topics’ effects on other outcomes (Appendix Table 3).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Impact of beverage messages compared with control messages, by beverage message topic, 2,164 parents of children aged 6 months to 5 years.

Notes: Figure shows impacts of water encouragement messages, sugary drink discouragement messages, and combined discouragement and encouragement messages, compared with the control messages. Impacts are shown as unstandardized regression coefficients (Bs) from regressions of standardized dependent variables. Boldface indicates a statistically significant impact of the message topic compared to the control condition, p<0.05. *indicates message topics differed from one another in pairwise comparisons of coefficients, p<0.01.

DISCUSSION

Young children spend approximately half an hour each day reading or being read to,15 making children’s books a highly scalable avenue for reaching both parents and children with health messages. In this experimental study in a large, sociodemographically diverse sample of U.S. parents, messages about beverages embedded in children’s books influenced parents’ perceptions, beliefs, and intentions about the beverages they serve their children. Specifically, parents perceived beverage messages as likely to discourage them from serving their children sugary drinks and likely to encourage them to serve their children water. The beverage messages also elicited beneficial changes in thinking about health effects, negative emotions, and behavioral intentions. Evidence indicates that changes in perceived message effectiveness,2633 thinking about health effects,35,37 emotions,35,37 and intentions38 are associated with actual behavior change. Thus, these findings offer preliminary support for using children’s books as a strategy for motivating parents of young children to serve healthier beverages.

Beverage messages elicited similar impacts regardless of participants’ income, education, race, ethnicity, language of survey administration, and frequency of serving sugary drinks. These results suggest that storybooks about beverages are a promising strategy for reaching parents from diverse backgrounds. One exception was that beverage messages did not lead to higher discouragement from serving sugary drinks among parents of children aged 6–11 months, despite beneficial impacts among parents of older children. This result could be because infants are less likely to consume sugary drinks than older children,6 making sugary drink messages less salient for parents of infants. Additionally, beverage messages had a smaller impact on both discouragement and encouragement for parents who reported serving their children water less often, perhaps because of defense processing39 or concerns about water safety. Finally, although the beverage messages’ impacts did not differ by income, the messages had a smaller impact for SNAP and WIC participants compared with non-participants. This finding may reflect that SNAP and WIC participants often receive nutrition education via those programs, making the beverage messages less novel. Although this study found few differences in beverage messages’ effects by demographics, prior research indicates that children’s time spent reading or being read to varies by characteristics such as income and education.40 Thus, for storybook interventions to have equitable impact, additional interventions may be needed to support all parents’ ability to read regularly to their children.

Interventions to encourage healthier beverage consumption can focus on promoting water, discouraging sugary drinks, or both. Limited research has assessed which of these approaches is most promising.19 This study found some evidence that messages about one type of beverage spilled over to also influence perceptions and intentions about the other type of beverage. For example, messages that focused only on encouraging water elicited encouragement for serving children water and discouragement from serving children sugary drinks, and vice versa. These results offer preliminary evidence that beverage messages could elicit similarly beneficial reactions from parents regardless of the message topic. However, other studies have found that water-focused interventions do not consistently reduce sugary drink consumption or purchases.19 Studies using factorial designs in larger samples will help clarify the independent and joint effects of water promotion and sugary drink reduction messages.

Limitations

Strengths of this study include the randomized design, large and diverse sample, and realistic stimuli adapted from an existing children’s book. There were several limitations. First, although research indicates that perceived effectiveness, thinking about health effects, negative emotions, and intentions have predictive value for identifying messages likely to change a variety of personal health behaviors,2633,35,37,38 studies have not evaluated whether these constructs predict parents’ behaviors toward their children. Future studies examining parents’ behaviors are therefore warranted.41 Second, this study was conducted online, and generalizability to populations lacking Internet access or computer literacy remains to be established. Third, sample size needs were estimated to detect effects of any beverage message compared with control and analyses may have been underpowered to detect differences between beverage message topics. Fourth, outcomes were self-reported, and results could be driven by social desirability bias. However, the survey was anonymous, which should reduce such bias.42,43

CONCLUSIONS

Embedding messages in children’s storybooks holds promise for encouraging parents from diverse backgrounds to serve their children more water and fewer sugary drinks. Future studies should assess storybook interventions’ impacts on parents’ and children’s health behaviors and outcomes.

Supplementary Material

1

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Research reported in this manuscript was supported by FIRST 5 Santa Clara County. DG was supported by T32 HL 098048. MGS was supported by T32 DK 007703. AAM was supported by T32 CA 057711-27. The funders had no role in the management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; or decision to submit the manuscript for publication. The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Footnotes

The contents of this article have not been previously published elsewhere.

No financial disclosures were reported by the authors of this paper.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Healthy Eating Research. Healthy Beverage Consumption in Early Childhood. Healthy Eating Research. https://healthyeatingresearch.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/09/HER-HealthyBeverage-ConsensusStatement.pdf. Published September 2019. Accessed September 9, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bleich SN, Vercammen KA. The negative impact of sugar-sweetened beverages on children’s health: an update of the literature. BMC Obes. 2018;5:6. 10.1186/s40608-017-0178-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Malik V, Pan A, Willett WC, Hu FB. Sugar-sweetened beverages and weight gain in children and adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;98(4):1084–1102. 10.3945/ajcn.113.058362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Auerbach BJ, Wolf FM, Hikida A, et al. Fruit juice and change in BMI: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2017;139(4):e20162454. 10.1542/peds.2016-2454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Muckelbauer R, Libuda L, Clausen K, Toschke AM, Reinehr T, Kersting M. Promotion and provision of drinking water in schools for overweight prevention: randomized, controlled cluster trial. Pediatrics. 2009;123(4):e661–e667. 10.1542/peds.2008-2186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kay MC, Welker EB, Jacquier EF, Story MT. Beverage consumption patterns among infants and young children (0–47.9 months): data from the Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study, 2016. Nutrients. 2018;10(7):825. 10.3390/nu10070825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bleich SN, Vercammen KA, Koma JW, Li Z. Trends in beverage consumption among children and adults, 2003–2014. Obesity. 2018;26(2):432–441. 10.1002/oby.22056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Crall JJ, Vujicic M. Children’s oral health: progress, policy development, and priorities for continued improvement: study examines improvements in American children’s oral health and oral health care that stem from major federal and state initiatives, and persistent disparities. Health Aff. 2020;39(10):1762–1769. 10.1377/hlthaff.2020.00799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Skinner AC, Ravanbakht SN, Skelton JA, Perrin EM, Armstrong SC. Prevalence of obesity and severe obesity in U.S. children, 1999–2016. Pediatrics. 2018;141(3):e20173459. 10.1542/peds.2017-3459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Savage JS, Fisher JO, Birch LL. Parental influence on eating behavior: conception to adolescence. J Law Med Ethics. 2007;35(1):22–34. 10.1111/j.1748-720X.2007.00111.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Epstein LH, Paluch RA, Roemmich JN, Beecher MD. Family-based obesity treatment, then and now: twenty-five years of pediatric obesity treatment. Health Psychol. 2007;26(4):381–391. 10.1037/0278-6133.26.4.381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Matwiejczyk L, Mehta K, Scott J, Tonkin E, Coveney J. Characteristics of effective interventions promoting healthy eating for pre-schoolers in childcare settings: an umbrella review. Nutrients. 2018;10(3):293. 10.3390/nu10030293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Rasmussen EE, Keene JR, Berke CK, Densley RL, Loof T. Explaining parental coviewing: the role of social facilitation and arousal. Commun Monogr. 2017;84(3):365–384. 10.1080/03637751.2016.1259532. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Moyer‐Gusé E Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion: explaining the persuasive effects of entertainment–education messages. Commun Theory. 2008;18(3):407–425. 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00328.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Common Sense Media. Children, Teens, and Reading. https://d2e111jq13me73.cloudfront.net/sites/default/files/research/csm-childrenteensandreading-2014_0.pdf. Published May 12, 2014. Accessed October 2, 2019.
  • 16.Roberto CA, Wong D, Musicus A, Hammond D. The influence of sugar-sweetened beverage health warning labels on parents’ choices. Pediatrics. 2016;137(2):e20153185. 10.1542/peds.2015-3185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Grunert K, Bolton L, Raats M. Processing and acting upon nutrition labeling on food: the state of knowledge and new directions for transformative consumer research. In: Mick D, Pettigrew J, Ozanne J, Pechmann C, eds. Transformative Consumer Research for Personal and Collective Well-Being. Routledge; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Nezami B, Ward D, Lytle L, Ennett S, Tate D. A mHealth randomized controlled trial to reduce sugar‐sweetened beverage intake in preschool‐aged children. Pediatr Obes. 2018;13(11):668–676. 10.1111/ijpo.12258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Dibay Moghadam S, Krieger JW, Louden DKN. A systematic review of the effectiveness of promoting water intake to reduce sugar‐sweetened beverage consumption. Obes Sci Pract. 2020;6(3):229–246. 10.1002/osp4.397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Vargas‐Garcia E, Evans C, Prestwich A, Sykes‐Muskett B, Hooson J, Cade J. Interventions to reduce consumption of sugar‐sweetened beverages or increase water intake: evidence from a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Obes Rev. 2017;18(11):1350–1363. 10.1111/obr.12580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cradock AL, Poole MK, Agnew KE, et al. A systematic review of strategies to increase drinking‐water access and consumption among 0‐to 5‐year‐olds. Obes Rev. 2019;20(9):1262–1286. 10.1111/obr.12833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Franken S, Smit CR, Buijzen M. Promoting water consumption on a Caribbean island: an intervention using children’s social networks at schools. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(4):713. 10.3390/ijerph15040713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Jeong M, Zhang D, Morgan J, et al. Similarities and differences in tobacco control research findings from convenience and probability samples. Ann Behav Med. 2018;53(5):476–485. 10.1093/abm/kay059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lora KR, Davy B, Hedrick V, Ferris AM, Anderson MP, Wakefield D. Assessing initial validity and reliability of a beverage intake questionnaire in Hispanic preschool-aged children. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2016;116(12):1951–1960. 10.1016/j.jand.2016.06.376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Potter The Otter Book Shop. Potter The Otter. https://www.pottertheotter.com/. Published 2020. Accessed February 10, 2021.
  • 26.Bigsby E, Cappella JN, Seitz HH. Efficiently and effectively evaluating public service announcements: additional evidence for the utility of perceived effectiveness. Commun Monogr. 2013;80(1):1–23. 10.1080/03637751.2012.739706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Baig SA, Noar SM, Gottfredson NC, Lazard AJ, Ribisl KM, Brewer NT. Message perceptions and effects perceptions as proxies for behavioral impact in the context of anti-smoking messages. Prev Med Rep. 2021;23:101434. 10.1016/j.pmedr.2021.101434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Noar SM, Barker J, Bell T, Yzer M. Does perceived message effectiveness predict the actual effectiveness of tobacco education messages? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Commun. 2018;35(2):148–157. 10.1080/10410236.2018.1547675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Noar SM, Rohde JA, Prentice-Dunn H, Kresovich A, Hall MG, Brewer NT. Evaluating the actual and perceived effectiveness of e-cigarette prevention advertisements among adolescents. Addict Behav. 2020;109:106473. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rohde JA, Noar SM, Prentice-Dunn H, Kresovich A, Hall MG. Comparison of message and effects perceptions for The Real Cost e-cigarette prevention ads. Health Commun. 2021;36(10):1222–1230. 10.1080/10410236.2020.1749353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Dillard JP, Shen L, Vail RG. Does perceived message effectiveness cause persuasion or vice versa? 17 consistent answers. Hum Commun Res. 2007;33(4):467–488. 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2007.00308.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Alvaro EM, Crano WD, Siegel JT, Hohman Z, Johnson I, Nakawaki B. Adolescents’ attitudes toward antimarijuana ads, usage intentions, and actual marijuana usage. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(4):1027. 10.1037/a0031960. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dillard JP, Li SS, Cannava K. Talking about sugar-sweetened beverages: causes, processes, and consequences of campaign-induced interpersonal communication. Health Commun. Published online October 30, 2020:1–11. 10.1080/10410236.2020.1838107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Baig SA, Noar SM, Gottfredson NC, Boynton MH, Ribisl KM, Brewer NT. UNC Perceived Message Effectiveness: validation of a brief scale. Ann Behav Med. 2019;53(8):732–742. 10.1093/abm/kay080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Brewer N, Parada H Jr., Hall M, Boynton M, Noar S, Ribisl K. Understanding why pictorial cigarette pack warnings increase quit attempts. Ann Behav Med. 2019;53(3):232–243. 10.1093/abm/kay032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Noar S, Hall M, Francis D, Ribisl K, Pepper J, Brewer N. Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tob Control. 2015;25:341–354. 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-051978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Grummon AH, Brewer NT. Health warnings and beverage purchase behavior: mediators of impact. Ann Behav Med. 2020;54(9):691–702. 10.1093/abm/kaaa011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Webb TL, Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(2):249–268. 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Brehm SS, Brehm JW. Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control. Academic Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Rideout V The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Kids Age Zero to Eight. Common Sense Media. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/the-common-sense-census-media-use-by-kids-age-zero-to-eight-2017. Published 2017. Accessed November 27, 2018.
  • 41.Larsen JK, Hermans RC, Sleddens EF, et al. How to bridge the intention-behavior gap in food parenting: automatic constructs and underlying techniques. Appetite. 2018;123:191–200. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.12.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Grimm P Social desirability bias. In: Kamakura W, ed. Wiley International Encyclopedia of Marketing. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2011. 10.1002/9781444316568.wiem02057. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Dodou D, de Winter JC. Social desirability is the same in offline, online, and paper surveys: a meta-analysis. Comput Hum Behav. 2014;36:487–495. 10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES