Abstract
Behavioral measurements in mice are critical tools used to evaluate the effects of interventions. Whilst mice are nocturnal animals, many studies conduct behavioral tests during the day. To better understand the effects of diurnal rhythm on mouse behaviors, we compared the results from behavioral tests conducted in the active and inactive phases. C57BL/6 mice were used in this study; we focus on sensorimotor performance, anxiety, learning and memory. Overall, our results show mice exhibit slightly higher cutaneous sensitivity, better long-term contextual memory, and a greater active avoidance escape response during the active phase. We did not observe significant differences in motor coordination, anxiety, or spatial learning and memory. Furthermore, apart from the elevated-O-maze, there was no remarkable sex effect among these tests. This study provides information on the effects of different diurnal phases on types of behavior and demonstrates the importance of the circadian cycle on learning and memory. Although we did not detect differences in anxiety and spatial learning/memory, diurnal rhythm may interact with other factors to influence these behaviors.
Subject terms: Circadian rhythms and sleep, Emotion, Learning and memory, Behavioural methods
Introduction
Mouse behavioral testing is an important methodology for assessing the impact of pharmaceutical interventions and genetic engineering. Mouse models are widely used for scientific investigation and are particularly prevalent in neuroscience. Among all the mouse strains, C57BL/6 is one of the most popular with neuroscientists to study brain functions. From previous high-throughput phenotyping data, C57BL/6 has been shown to be less anxious compared to other inbred strain of mice in behavioral tests. This feature makes it a more suitable strain for behavioral assays1.
However, mice are nocturnal animals; and their active phase is during the night. Given that most scientists work during daytime, many studies are conducted during the natural sleep phase of mice. There are two issues here. First, sleep disruption can alter emotional responses and cognition2,3. Second, many physiological processes including hormone secretions, such as corticosterone, and gene expression, which influence behavioral performance, are regulated by the circadian clock4,5. However, it is uncommon for research institutes to maintain mice under a reverse light system in animal houses. Thus, many behavioral assays are conducted during their inactive phase when mice are normally asleep. If mice perform quite differently during the active phase compared to the inactive phase, then this may partially explain some contradictory results between different published studies. Numerous studies have demonstrated the importance of circadian clocks on mood disorders and learning6,7. So far, there are a limited number of published documents investigating the anxiety and learning performance at different times of the day8–10. In this study, we aimed to investigate whether mice performed differently in behavioral tests during their active and inactive phases. Specifically, we focused on hippocampal-related anxiety and learning memory, which have been studied extensively. The behaviors we assayed include motor coordination, cutaneous sensitivity, anxiety, learning and memory. Despite some slight differences detected in a few tests, overall our results demonstrate that there are no dramatic discrepancies in behavioral performance conducted during active and inactive phases.
Results
To understand the effects of circadian phase on mice behavior, 24 mice were divided into two groups when 4 week-old (active phase: mice were maintained in reverse light; inactive phase: mice were maintained in normal light). All mice (8 week-old) during the active and inactive phases were subjected to a battery of behavioral tests (initial test), including: rotarod, von Frey test, open field, elevated-O-maze, light–dark box, water maze, T-maze, contextual fear conditioning, and active avoidance. To further confirm the results at a different age, all the behaviors were repeated again three months later (retest, 20 week-old) (Fig. 1A). Behavioral testing was started two hours after lights on/off; Zeitgeber Time (ZT) 14 for the active group and Zeitgeber Time (ZT) 2 for the inactive group. Ten minutes before the test, tested cages were relocated from animal room to the waiting hallway (red dim light, 15 lx) to minimize the effect of light (Fig. 1B). Tests were then conducted under the following light intensities: open field (400 lx), elevated-O-maze (350 lx), light–dark box (440 lx), water maze (440 lx), T-maze (400 lx), contextual fear conditioning (inside the dark box), and active avoidance (inside the dark box).
Effects of diurnal phase on motor coordination and sensory stimulation
To test the influence of the light–dark cycle on motor coordination and sensory stimuli, mice were subjected to the rotarod and von Frey tests. Our results show that there are no differences on the latency to fall in the rotarod test between inactive and active groups in the initial test (p = 0.54, t(22) = 0.61), nor the retest three months later (p = 0.94, t(22) = 0.07) (Fig. 1C). Next, to assess sensitivity to sensory stimuli, all mice were subjected to the von Frey test; a mechanical stimulation by a filament to hind paws. Mice tested in the inactive phase are slightly less sensitive in the initial test (p = 0.049, F(1,44) = 4.06). For the retest three months later, the results are similar to the initial test, mice tested in inactive phase are still less sensitive than tested in active phase (p = 0.0085, F(1,44) = 7.59) (Fig. 1D). These results suggest that the performance of motor coordination and balance is not influenced by diurnal activity, but cutaneous sensitivity is more responsive in the active phase.
Effects of diurnal phase on anxiety tests
To better understand the effects of diurnal rhythm on anxiety, we chose three common tests associated with anxiety like behavior: the open field test, elevated-O-maze, and light–dark box. All mice were subjected to these three tests. Our results show that there were no significant differences detected between inactive and active groups in the open field test in the initial test (travel distance, p = 0.25, t(22) = 1.17; time in center zone, p = 0.09, t(22) = 1.72), nor the retest three months later (travel distance, p = 0.89, t(22) = 0.13; time in center zone, p = 0.57, t(22) = 0.56) (Fig. 2A). For the elevated-O-maze test, there were no significant difference detected in the initial test (travel distance, p = 0.244, t(22) = 1.19; during in the open arms, p = 0.11, t(22) = 1.64). However, mice tested in inactive period showed higher travel distance (p = 0.01, t(22) = 2.62) and exhibited a trend to stay longer in the open arms (p = 0.06, t(22) = 1.94) in the retest (Fig. 2B). Next, we subjected all the mice to the light–dark box test, in the initial test there were no differences in time spent in the light compartment: p = 0.75, t(22) = 0.31). Three months later, all mice were subjected to the tests again. Mice tested in the inactive period spent more time in the light compartment (p = 0.036, t(22) = 2.23) (Fig. 2C). The results show that there were no remarkable differences observed in anxiety tests while testing mice during active or inactive periods in the first test. However, three months later upon retesting, we did observe that mice exhibited less travel distance in the elevated-O-maze and spent less time in the light compartment.
Effects of diurnal phase on spatial learning and memory
It is unclear whether a time-of-day may influence the performance of spatial learning and memory. To investigate the effects of diurnal rhythm on spatial learning and memory, the water maze was used. All mice were subjected to the water maze for 4 days to examine acquisition. The results show there were no differences in escape latency (p = 0.24, F(1,22) = 1.45) and travel distance (p = 0.85, F(1,22) = 0.03) in the initial test, nor the retest three months later (escape latency, p = 0.53, F(1,22) = 0.39; travel distance, p = 0.65, F(1,22) = 0.2) (Fig. 3A). Seven days after the last training, the escape platform was removed, and mice were subjected to the water maze, and spatial memory was evaluated. There were no significant differences detected between the active group and inactive group for the initial test (time spent in target zone, p = 0.82, t(22) = 0.22; platform crosses, p = 0.06, t(22) = 1.9), nor the retest three months later (time spent in target zone, p = 0.21, t(22) = 1.28; platform crosses, p = 0.22, t(22) = 1.25) (Fig. 3B). These data suggest that there are no obvious differences detected in spatial learning and memory between mice tested in active or inactive period.
Effects of diurnal phase on T-maze, contextual fear, and active avoidance, and the expression of clock genes
Next, to better assess whether the circadian period affects other types of cognitive behavior, we conducted T-maze, contextual fear, and active avoidance tests. For T-maze alternation, the results show there were no differences in percentage of correct choices in the initial test (p = 0.79), nor the second test three months later (p = 0.8) (Fig. 4A). For contextual fear conditioning, there were no differences detected in freezing time during the pre-shock session (p = 0.07, t(22) = 1.88), and there was no difference in freezing time detected in the test session 24 h after the shock (p = 0.15, t(22) = 1.47) (Fig. 4B). Three months later, all mice were subjected to the footshock chamber again. Mice tested in the active phase exhibited an increase in freezing time during the pre-shock session (p = 0.007, t(22) = 2.94), but no differences during the test session (p = 0.21, t(22) = 1.27), suggesting mice tested in the active phase show better long-term memory of the footshock chamber (Fig. 4B). For active avoidance, mice tested in the active period exhibited higher escape success rate than those tested in the inactive phase (p = 0.0028, t(22) = 3.36), suggesting that mice learn active avoidance better during the active phase. Three months later all the mice were subjected to active avoidance testing again, and the results show there was no significant difference (p = 0.25, t(22) = 1.15) (Fig. 4C). Clock genes have been extensively studied and show circadian expression in the brain11,12. To further confirm physiological gene expression pattern of active and inactive periods in these mice, three days after the last behavioral test, we harvested hippocampal tissue for clock gene expression four hours after lights on/off. Our results show that hippocampal tissue harvested in the active period (Zeitgeber Time 16, ZT16) exhibit higher expression of Per1 (p = 0.0001, t(8) = 6.74) and Per2 (p = 0.0013, t(9) = 4.57), and lower expression of Bmal1 (p = 0.0049, t(9) = 3.69) compared to tissue harvested during the inactive phase (Zeitgeber Time 4, ZT4) (Fig. 4D). These results confirm the mice had differential gene expression between active and inactive phase.
Sex differences in behaviors
In order to determine whether differences in sex could be contributing to our results, for each measure we compared a model in which phase predicted the behavior to a model that in addition contained sex as a predictor. We applied a Bonferroni 5% corrected threshold of 0.00156 to take into account the fact that we ran 32 tests (0.05/32 = 0.00156). Our figures show results for males and females where males are colored blue and females colored red. We found two behavioral results exceeded the 5% threshold; time spent in open arms (p = 0.00017) and travel distance (p = 0.0012) of elevated–O-maze at 20 weeks in our analysis. However, the small sample size and consequent low power means we cannot exclude the presence of a sex difference. Table 1 provides a summary of the sex difference analyses.
Table 1.
Test | Time | p.sex | t.sex | Time | p.sex | t.sex |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Rotarod (latency to fall) | 8-week-old | 0.88405 | 0.14762 | 20-week-old | 0.05798 | − 2.00536 |
Von Frey (left) | 0.08414 | 1.81309 | 0.71014 | 0.37675 | ||
Von Frey (right) | 0.10251 | 1.70728 | 0.00439 | 3.19124 | ||
Open field (travel distance) | 0.29786 | − 1.06750 | 0.16461 | − 1.43998 | ||
Open field (time in center zone) | 0.49743 | 0.69052 | 0.21310 | − 1.28407 | ||
Elevated-O-maze (travel distance) | 0.84313 | − 0.20035 | 0.00127† | − 3.71962 | ||
Elevated-O-maze (time in open arms) | 0.01552 | − 2.63370 | 0.00017† | − 4.55817 | ||
Light–dark box (time in light box) | 0.61658 | 0.50824 | 0.97763 | − 0.02837 | ||
T-maze (correct choice) | 0.31119 | − 1.03775 | 0.52166 | 0.65170 | ||
Water maze acquisition (escape latency) | 0.33181 | 0.99343 | 0.53841 | − 0.62545 | ||
Water maze acquisition (travel distance) | 0.06541 | 1.94401 | 0.62695 | − 0.49325 | ||
Water maze memory (time in target zone) | 0.23214 | − 1.23043 | 0.90232 | − 0.12422 | ||
Water maze memory (platform crosses) | 0.31547 | − 1.02837 | 0.16005 | 1.45646 | ||
Active avoidance (percentage of avoidance) | 0.38343 | − 0.89023 | 0.39436 | 0.86959 | ||
Contextual fear (pre-shock) | 0.01358 | − 2.69419 | 0.77097 | 0.29489 | ||
Contextual fear (test) | 0.90799 | 0.11697 | 0.01173 | − 2.76031 |
The table shows the behavioral tests used for each experiment. The t-statistic (t.sex) and the associated p-value (p.sex) are provided for the effect of sex (n = 12 in each sex) on the phenotype.
†p < 0.00156 (Bonferroni corrected threshold of 0.05/32 as the 5% significance threshold).
Discussion
Mouse behavior is an important tool used to evaluate the effects of genes or experimental interventions in research. Mice are nocturnal animals, and few animal houses maintain mice under a reverse light system. In this study, we set out to investigate how marked differences are when behavioral tests are conducted during the different phases. At the beginning of the experiment, we assessed motor and sensory function. Our results show there is no significant difference in motor coordination, but mice tested in the active phase exhibit slightly greater cutaneous sensitivity. Indeed, previous studies have shown that the circadian rhythm influences the responses of mice in the von Frey test13.
Sleep is crucial for maintaining a normal emotional response14, and circadian period mutants have been shown to have altered anxiety in mice15. However, few studies explore anxiety across time of day16. Previous studies provide evidence that the expression of suprachiasmatic nucleus circadian oscillatory protein (SCOP) in the basolateral amygdala plays a key role in generating circadian rhythmicity in anxiety-like behavior17. Other studies show melatonin is involved in the anxiety response of diurnal and nocturnal species18. One previous study showed that mice tested at night or day performed differently in the light–dark box test, but displayed no significant difference detected in the open field19. In this study, we used three of the most commonly used tests for anxiety. In the initial test we did not observe remarkable differences between active and inactive groups. To further confirm these results, all mice were retested three months later. However, mice tested in active phase exhibited less travel distance in the elevated-O-maze, and spent less time in the light compartment in the light–dark box test. It is unclear if the difference at 20 weeks is because there is an age-dependent time of day effect or if there is a memory-dependent time of day effect. Whilst the initial exposure may have measured anxiety-like behavior, it may be the case that a subsequent exposure reflects a learned fear response in a mouse that remembers the original test.
The Morris water maze is one of the most popular tests to assess the performance of spatial learning and memory. Earlier studies in rats demonstrated that there is a modulating effect of diurnal rhythm on performance in the water maze9. However, our results show mice performed similarly in spatial learning and memory regardless of the phase in which they were tested. To better assess the effects of diurnal rhythm on learning and memory, we further investigated different types of learning and memory. For contextual fear conditioning, previous studies show that mice acquired conditioning faster in the day than in the night20. From our results, although mice tested in the active phase exhibit similar freezing time 24 h later, these mice showed higher freezing percentage in the pre-shock phase, 3 months later, indicating that mice tested in the active phase have more enduring memory for the footshock chamber. For the active avoidance test, we found mice tested in the active phase show higher escape success rate in the initial test. We did not detect differences in escape response in the retest experiment. One of the reasons for the lack of difference is that the mice show high avoidance responses in both groups, even though the retest was conducted three months after the initial test. Our data suggests that mice perform better in long-term contextual memory when trained in active phase, but not for spatial learning and memory.
In conclusion, we use two groups of mice to assess whether diurnal rhythm has a significant impact on sensorimotor, anxiety, learning and memory. First, we did not find a sex effect on most behaviors, but female mice featured lower anxiety in the elevated-O-maze test. Indeed, a large cohort study shows very limited evidence for sex differences21,22. Second, mice tested in active phase exhibited slightly more cutaneous sensitivity and better contextual memory and active avoidance escape response, but no difference in spatial learning memory and anxiety. However, behavioral tests, inevitably, will affect the biological rhythm, and the sleep of mice. The experimental results may be different if the mice tested in active phase were only exposed to red dim light. One thing to be noted, in this study we only tested mouse behaviors in different phases, without any intervention or genetic mutation involved. It is very likely that circadian rhythm will interact with the genes or treatment and potentially affect anxiety, learning or memory. Behavioral studies involving in genes manipulation or drug treatment, still need to take the diurnal rhythm into account.
Methods
Experiments were performed on sex balanced cohorts of 8-week-old C57BL/6. Founders were ordered from National Laboratory Animal Center, Taiwan. After one generation of breeding, 4-week-old mice after weaning immediately were allocated in normal or reverse light animal room. Twenty-four mice were used in this experiment (n = 12 in each group; 6 males, 6 females). Mice were bred in AAALAC certified specific pathogen-free conditions. They were housed in a 12:12 h light dark cycle at a temperature of 22 °C and a humidity level of 60–70%. Animals had ad libitum access to food and water. All procedures were carried out in accordance with the local regulations and approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Chang Gung University (Permit Number: CGU107-025).
Behavioral testing
For behavioral tests, the movement of animals was recorded and tracked using a video recording system. Mice for conducting the behavioral analysis were randomized to a separate cage for testing, with bedding, food and water before being transferred back to the home cage. The behavioral data of anxiety and water maze was recorded and acquired automatically by Ethovision software.
Rotarod
Mice were held by the tail and placed on the rotarod (Ugo Basile), facing away from the direction of rotation. The rotarod moved at an initial speed of 4 rpm. After 10 s, the rod speed accelerated at a rate of 9 rpm per minute. Once acceleration had been triggered, the time taken for mice to fall was noted as previously described23.
Von Frey
Mice were placed individually in an elevated acrylic chamber equipped with a wire mesh floor, and a von Frey filament (BiosebLab) was applied from underneath pricking the hind paws. The strength of von Frey filament would continuously increase until paw withdrawal and the withdrawal threshold was recorded. Each mouse was pricked for five times both on the right hind paw and the left hind paw. The average withdrawal threshold of each hind paw was recorded.
Open field
Animals were allowed to freely move for 5 min in a circular-shaped area (radius = 60 cm) with a lamp being the only light source illuminating the center zone. The time spent in the center zone (radius = 40 cm) and the frequency of transition to that center zone were recorded by Ethovision software.
Elevated-O-maze
The maze was a circle (radius = 55 cm) elevated 60 cm above the floor. The closed arms featured a 15 cm wall. Mice were first placed in the closed arm and allowed to freely move for 5 min under dim light. The time spent in the open arms and the exploration distance were measured as described previously24.
Light–dark box
At the start of the test, mice were put in the covered dark box and allowed to move freely between dark and the coverless light boxes via a door. Mice were allowed to freely move in the chamber for 5 min under white light. The time spent in the light box and number of light box entries were recorded and analyzed by Ethovision software.
Water maze
Mice were put into the 130 cm-radius-water tank. The tank was filled with water at 22 degrees Celsius that was coloured with white acrylic turpentine. A circular 10 cm-radius-platform was submerged 1.5 cm below the water surface. Each mouse was trained to stay in the platform for 30 s per trial, three trials per day for 4 days. Mice were put into the tank at three different positions and faced to the wall of the tank. Seven days after the last training, we then performed the memory test with the platform removed. We recorded the time each mouse spent in the correct quadrant (target zone) and the number of times they crossed the phantom platform location.
T-maze
T-maze was performed as described previously24. Mice were put into the T-maze for 10 min one day before testing. Before the test, all guillotine doors were raised. The mice were placed in the start area and allowed to choose a goal arm. A mouse was confined to the chosen arm and start area by quietly sliding the other door down. After 30 s, the mouse was removed, and immediately place back into the maze to select an arm. Each mouse repeated the experiment three times. We recorded the percentage of correct times each mouse explored the novel arm.
Contextual fear and fear memory
Context-conditioned learning was assessed in a footshock chamber placed on a weight transducer (Panlab/Harvard apparatus) and analyzed with PACKWIN software. Mice were allowed to explore the chamber for 3 min. At the end of the trial, an electric shock with current intensity of 0.7 mA was given through the underlying conducting rods. Mice were placed into the same footshock chamber the next day for 3 min and the immobile time was recorded as described previously24.
Active avoidance
Mice were placed in a two-compartment shuttle box equipped with a speaker and a light bulb in each compartment (Med Associates, Inc). Subjects were given conditioned stimuli (5 s of light and 8 kHz, 85 dB tone) followed by an unconditioned electric footshock (0.3 mA) from the underlying conducting rods. Once the mice moved to the other compartment or the cut-off time (10 s) was up, the conditioned stimuli ceased. After a random inter-session interval (range 3–10 s), the next session started. Mice repeated 50 sessions in this test, according to protocol described previously24.
mRNA quantification
Hippocampal tissue was collected from all mice 4 h after lights on/off (ZT 4 for inactive group, ZT 16 for active group), then homogenized in Trizol, followed by Phenol–Chloroform extraction of total RNA. cDNA was then made using SuperScript III reverse transcriptase (Invitrogen). Experiments were performed in duplicate. Gene expression levels were then calculated with the ΔΔCt method and normalized against a Gapdh control, according to protocol described previously25. Below table are the primers sequences for PCR:
Per1 Forward | TTCAA GCTCT CAGGA CTCTG |
Per1 Reverse | GGCAG TTTCC TATTG GTTGG |
Per2 Forward | CAGGA GAAGC TGAAG CTGC |
Per2 Reverse | GGACT GTCTT CCTCA TATGG |
Bmal1 Forward | TGGAA GAAGT TGACT GCCTG GAAGG |
Bmal1 Reverse | GGCCC AAATT CCCAC ATCTG AAGTT AC |
Statistical analysis
The mean ± SEM was determined for each group. Statistical analysis was performed using Graphpad Prism software. Data were analyzed via an analysis of variance (ANOVA), Mann–Whitney U test (For T-maze), and t-test as appropriate. For sex differences in behaviors, we measured the significance of sex as the improvement in fit conferred by sex in a model where the phase (“inactive” vs “active”) predicted the behavioral measure. The significance of the fixed effect sex was assessed using an approximation to the sequential F-test based on the Wald test 26. We fitted models by REML (Restricted maximum likelihood), using the lmer function from the R package lme4 27.
Ethical approval
The study was carried out in compliance with the ARRIVE guidelines.
Author contributions
C.-H.T. and G.-J.H. conducted animal experiments and wrote the manuscript. J.F. performed the analysis of sex differences.
Funding
This study was supported by grants from the Chang Gung Memorial Hospital (CMRPD1L0291), Healthy Aging Research Center (EMRPD1I0501), and the Ministry of Science and Technology of Taiwan (MOST 105–2320-B-182–040) (to G-J.H.). This research was also supported by the “Molecular Medicine Research Center, Chang Gung University” (EMRPD1L0341) from The Featured Areas Research Center Program within the framework of the Higher Education Sprout Project by the Ministry of Education in Taiwan.
Data availability
The datasets generated and analysed during this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Solberg LC, et al. A protocol for high-throughput phenotyping, suitable for quantitative trait analysis in mice. Mamm. Genome. 2006;17:129–146. doi: 10.1007/s00335-005-0112-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Karatsoreos IN, Bhagat S, Bloss EB, Morrison JH, McEwen BS. Disruption of circadian clocks has ramifications for metabolism, brain, and behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2011;108:1657–1662. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018375108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Sakamoto K, Higo-Yamamoto S, Egi Y, Miyazaki K, Oishi K. Memory dysfunction and anxiety-like behavior in a mouse model of chronic sleep disorders. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 2020;529:175–179. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2020.05.218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Oster H, et al. The circadian rhythm of glucocorticoids is regulated by a gating mechanism residing in the adrenal cortical clock. Cell Metab. 2006;4:163–173. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2006.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hasegawa S, et al. Hippocampal clock regulates memory retrieval via Dopamine and PKA-induced GluA1 phosphorylation. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:5766. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13554-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Walker WH, Walton JC, DeVries AC, Nelson RJ. Circadian rhythm disruption and mental health. Transl. Psychiatry. 2020;10:28. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0694-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Mulder CK, Gerkema MP, Van der Zee EA. Circadian clocks and memory: Time-place learning. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2013;6:8. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2013.00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Richetto J, Polesel M, Weber-Stadlbauer U. Effects of light and dark phase testing on the investigation of behavioural paradigms in mice: Relevance for behavioural neuroscience. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2019;178:19–29. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2018.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Valentinuzzi VS, Menna-Barreto L, Xavier GF. Effect of circadian phase on performance of rats in the Morris water maze task. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2004;19:312–324. doi: 10.1177/0748730404265688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ago Y, et al. Social crowding in the night-time reduces an anxiety-like behavior and increases social interaction in adolescent mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2014;270:37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.04.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Matsui D, Takekida S, Okamura H. Molecular oscillation of Per1 and Per2 genes in the rodent brain: An in situ hybridization and molecular biological study. Kobe J. Med. Sci. 2005;51:85–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jilg A, et al. Temporal dynamics of mouse hippocampal clock gene expression support memory processing. Hippocampus. 2010;20:377–388. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Minett MS, Eijkelkamp N, Wood JN. Significant determinants of mouse pain behaviour. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e104458. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0104458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Goldstein AN, Walker MP. The role of sleep in emotional brain function. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2014;10:679–708. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Keers R, et al. Reduced anxiety and depression-like behaviours in the circadian period mutant mouse afterhours. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e38263. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Verma P, Hellemans KG, Choi FY, Yu W, Weinberg J. Circadian phase and sex effects on depressive/anxiety-like behaviors and HPA axis responses to acute stress. Physiol. Behav. 2010;99:276–285. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Nakano JJ, Shimizu K, Shimba S, Fukada Y. SCOP/PHLPP1beta in the basolateral amygdala regulates circadian expression of mouse anxiety-like behavior. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:33500. doi: 10.1038/srep33500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bilu C, Kronfeld-Schor N. Effects of circadian phase and melatonin injection on anxiety-like behavior in nocturnal and diurnal rodents. Chronobiol. Int. 2013;30:828–836. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2013.773439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sidor MM, et al. Daytime spikes in dopaminergic activity drive rapid mood-cycling in mice. Mol. Psychiatry. 2015;20:1479–1480. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Chaudhury D, Colwell CS. Circadian modulation of learning and memory in fear-conditioned mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2002;133:95–108. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00471-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Krohn J, Speed D, Palme R, Touma C, Mott R, Flint J. Genetic interactions with sex make a relatively small contribution to the heritability of complex traits in mice. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e96450. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Valdar W, et al. Genetic and environmental effects on complex traits in mice. Genetics. 2006;174:959–984. doi: 10.1534/genetics.106.060004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Huang GJ, et al. Ectopic cerebellar cell migration causes maldevelopment of Purkinje cells and abnormal motor behaviour in Cxcr4 null mice. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e86471. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Chen YJ, et al. Follistatin mediates learning and synaptic plasticity via regulation of Asic4 expression in the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2021;118:e2109040118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2109040118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Eu WZ, et al. The effect of nerve growth factor on supporting spatial memory depends upon hippocampal cholinergic innervation. Transl. Psychiatry. 2021;11:162. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01280-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Pinheiro JC, Bates DM. Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-PLUS. Springer; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Team RDC. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. In R: Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria (2004).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated and analysed during this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.