Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2021 Dec;217:104907. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104907

Relationship between cardiac cycle and the timing of actions during action execution and observation

ER Palser a,b,c, J Glass a, A Fotopoulou b, JM Kilner a,
PMCID: PMC8748943  PMID: 34563865

Abstract

Previous research suggests that there may be a relationship between the timing of motor events and phases of the cardiac cycle. This relationship has thus far only been researched using simple isolated movements such as key-presses in reaction-time tasks and only in a single subject acting alone. Other research has shown both movement and cardiac coordination among interacting individuals. Here, we investigated how the cardiac cycle relates to ongoing self-paced movements in both action execution and observation using a novel dyadic paradigm. We recorded electrocardiography (ECG) in 26 subjects who formed 19 dyads containing an action executioner and observer as they performed a self-paced sequence of movements. We demonstrated that heartbeats are timed to movements during both action execution and observation. Specifically, movements were less likely to culminate synchronously with the heartbeat around the time of the R-peak of the ECG. The same pattern was observed for action observation, with the observer's heartbeats occurring off-phase with movement culmination. These findings demonstrate that there is coordination between an action executioner's cardiac cycle and the timing of their movements, and that the same relationship is mirrored in an observer. This suggests that previous findings of interpersonal coordination may be caused by the mirroring of a phasic relationship between movement and the heart.

Keywords: Action observation, Interoception, Baroreceptor, Motor, Heartbeat, Social interaction

Highlights

  • Voluntary movement and cardiac cycle are coordinated in humans.

  • Effects are seen for both movement execution and observation.

  • Movements are timed to occur off-phase with heartbeats.

  • Observer's heartbeats occur off-phase with actor's movements.

1. Introduction

A growing body of research, comprising physiological and psychological investigations, is consistent with the hypothesis that the central nervous system has access to cardiac information, and uses this information to guide behavior. One such instance is the bidirectional relationship between the cardiac cycle and the timing of movement. One of the earliest observations in this vein was that walking rate and heart rate typically show a one-to-one mapping (Anders, 1928; Coleman, 1921). It was proposed that the rhythm of the heart might represent a pacemaker, guiding the timing of movement.

Crucially, the effect of the heart on movement is bidirectional, with both effects of the cardiac cycle on movement and effects of movement on cardiac output observed. First, anticipatory cardiac slowing in the foreperiod of a reaction-time task while the subject prepares to move was reported (Lacey et al., 1963; Lacey and Lacey, 1970). Secondly, the motor response was also found to vary depending on when in the cardiac cycle it occurred (Birren et al., 1963; Jennings et al., 1992), with faster reaction times during atrial contraction. A similar effect has recently been observed for movement inhibition, with faster responses to stop cues during systole (Rae et al., 2018). Much of this previous physiological work on the relationship between the cardiac cycle and movement has employed simple prescribed movements within reaction-time and stop signal paradigms.

Cardiac-movement effects have been attributed to baroreceptors (Lacey and Lacey, 1978). Located within the aortic arch, the carotid sinuses and the coronary arteries, these stretch receptors represent one of the key communication channels between the heart and the brain. Baroreceptors are active during systole, conveying information on the timing and strength of the heartbeat to the central nervous system via the vagal and glossopharyngeal nerves, but are quiescent during diastole when there is no such information to convey (Brown and Eccles, 1934; Levy and Martin, 1984). The heart's rhythmicity is modified by humoral and neural factors and the heart period can be increased or decreased according to signals conveyed by vagal and sympathetic efferents (Brown and Eccles, 1934; Dong Jr and Reitz, 1970; Levy and Martin, 1984; Levy et al., 1970). There is therefore a closed loop communication reflex mediated by baroreceptors that allows the timing of the heart cycle to be controlled.

Interoceptive signals, particularly those originating from the heart, have been purported to play a role in social cognition (Palmer and Tsakiris, 2018; Shah et al., 2017). Simulation theories of action understanding posit that in order to understand another's behavior, we simulate their perceptual, motor, and bodily states in ourselves (Gallese and Goldman, 1998). As such, an action is understood based on the resonance of an observer's motor system (Rizzolatti et al., 2001). Such ideas have since been formalized by hierarchical predictive models, wherein it is argued that action observation results in an observer generating an internal model of how they would perform it (Press et al., 2011; Wicker et al., 2003). Such models are proposed to contain not only exteroceptive and proprioceptive predictions (Kilner et al., 2007), but also interoceptive predictions about how such an action might be performed (Ondobaka et al., 2015). That is, a good model contains information pertaining not only to the expected position of the body in space, and afferent tactile feedback from the effector, but also the somatovisceral conditions that would be generated by the execution of that action. These predicted conditions can then be used to infer the causes of another's behavior, based on the most likely causes of those conditions in the self. Including interoceptive information in predictive models may provide unique information about others' affective states, not otherwise accessible through exteroceptive and prioprioceptive inference alone, due to the ubiquitous role of somatovisceral states in emotion and feeling states (Barrett and Simmons, 2015; Joffily and Coricelli, 2013; Seth and Critchley, 2013; Seth and Friston, 2016).

Findings of resonance beyond the central nervous system suggest that this mirroring effect may be more widespread than previously thought, extending into the peripheral nervous system (Bernier and Rosenthal, 1991; Hove and Risen, 2009; Paccalin and Jeannerod, 2000; Semin and Cacioppo, 2008). Both external stimuli such as observing someone perform an action, and internal stimuli, such as mentally rehearsing an action, can elicit autonomic system activity (Condon and Ogston, 1971; Hadar et al., 1985). Watching an actor lift a weight or run on a treadmill produces changes in the respiration rate of an observer, such that it increased linearly with effort (LaFrance, 1982). These findings support the idea that bodily systems involved in interoception may also be candidates for resonance. The direct matching mechanism proposes a match between the conditions of action execution and observation (Rizzolatti et al., 2001). Therefore, if this mirroring effect does extend to the interoceptive system, we would predict that any relationship between the cardiac cycle and movement timing found during action execution should also be found during action observation.

One consequence of resonance might be interpersonal coordination. Research in the field of social neuroscience has found both movement and cardiac coordination between interacting individuals. In the movement literature, interpersonal coordination may refer to either mimicry or synchronization (Hove and Risen, 2009). A distinction is generally made based on the temporal relations of the interacting individuals, with mimicry reflecting non-periodic matched movements and synchronicity used to describe movements temporally matched in time (Bernier and Rosenthal, 1991; Semin and Cacioppo, 2008). Early studies reported that listeners to a speaker tend to move in time with rhythms of a speaker's speech (Condon and Ogston, 1971) and mirror the postures of the speaker (LaFrance, 1982). In other interactions, the direction of the effect is less apparent, with partners mimicking each other's' postural sway (Shockley et al., 2003) and linguistic forms (Dale et al., 2013). In a recent study, groups of subjects (numbering between four and five individuals) constructed models out of Lego blocks (Fusaroli et al., 2016). They either worked collectively (where subjects took turns to work on one construction project) or individually (where subjects worked on their own construction projects side by side). More stable shared heart rate dynamics were observed in collective trials than individual trials, increasing across the course of collective trials (Fusaroli et al., 2016), suggesting that convergence was tied to the social nature of the task. Indeed, heart rate entrainment has been suggested as a mechanism underlying emotional commonality in social interaction, facilitating a sense of community between individuals (Konvalinka et al., 2011), empathy (Levenson and Gottman, 1983; Levenson and Ruef, 1992), and team performance (Elkins et al., 2009; Henning et al., 2001; Henning and Korbelak, 2005; Strang et al., 2014). In support of this, a study in which pairs of expert improvisers were asked to mirror each other's movements found that periods of both increased kinematic and subjective togetherness were characterized by higher heart rates and greater inter-player correlation of heart rate (Noy et al., 2015). Findings of interpersonal coordination and its influence on measures of social competence are compatible with the hypothesis that resonance supports action understanding.

The existing literature can thus be summarized as reflecting two broadly different methodological approaches to studying the relationship between the heart and movement. The first has studied the relationship between cardiac events and evoked movements such as those elicited by paradigms designed to gauge reaction time (Birren et al., 1963; Lacey and Lacey, 1977) and response inhibition (Jennings et al., 1992; Rae et al., 2018). These studies tested discrete, single movements. The second has taken the opposite approach and analyzed cardiac and movement parameters in a number of unconstrained self-paced environments, typically within a social context (Fusaroli et al., 2016; Xygalatas et al., 2011). In addition, most of the findings of movement synchronicity between individuals are based on subjective, observational techniques, although see (Hale et al., 2018) for an exception.

The current study was designed to answer the previously neglected question of whether cardiac-motor timing and inter-individual resonance are linked, bridging the gap between the two previous approaches, by investigating cardiac-motor synchronicity in both action execution and observation. Naturalistic actions are more likely to comprise a series of goal-directed self-paced movements than discrete, cued movements. Therefore, in the present study, we sought to investigate the bidirectional relationship between the cardiac cycle and movement during a sequence of meaningful self-paced movements. Subjects were divided into dyads and took turns to perform and observe a sequence of semi-controlled movements under experimental conditions, while electrocardiography (ECG) was recorded from both. In brief, the task required both subjects within a dyad to memorize a sequence of six movement locations and then take it in turn to replicate the sequence. In this way, subjects both executed and observed the movements. The timing of movement was recorded using touch-sensitive pads.

Firstly, we tested for coordination between execution and observation within a dyad, in both heart rate and movement speed. Secondly, we investigated whether there was any relationship between the timing of the movements made, operationalized as the endpoint of the action, and the location of this event within the cardiac cycle, both in terms of phase and time relative to R-peak. The endpoint of each action corresponded to each memorized location in the movement sequence, and thus likely represents the most salient movement event to both the subject performing the action, and the observer. Thirdly, we tested the prediction that any phasic or time-based relationship observed between the timing of movement events and the cardiac cycle during action observation would also be observed during action observation.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Subjects

A total of twenty-six healthy adult subjects with normal or corrected-to-normal vision were recruited. Subjects were aged between 19 and 36 years (M = 24.92, SD = 4.78). Fourteen were female and 12 were male. Twenty-four subjects reported being right-handed, and two reported being left-handed. Data were collected in two waves. In the first wave, due to practical reasons, subjects performed the dyadic behavioral turn taking task with an experimenter as their partner (n = 12). In the second wave, the remaining subjects were recruited in pairs and performed the task together (n = 14). For analysis, data were pooled (see Supplementary Materials for results reported separately). Ethical approval of the experimental protocol was granted by the Ethics Committee of University College London, and the methods were carried out in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki. All subjects gave written informed consent before testing.

2.2. Materials

Subjects were seated in pairs at a table in front of a Dell laptop computer. Stimuli were presented using MATLAB (version 7.8.0, Mathworks Inc., MA, USA) with the Cogent 2000 toolbox (www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent_2000.php). Three electrodes were affixed to the abdomens of subjects as per the standard Lead II configuration, to record their ECG. On the table were four touch-sensitive pads and a marble, which the subjects used to execute a series of memorized movements.

2.3. Procedure

Once seated comfortably, the experimenter affixed three disposable or washable electrodes to each subject. They were instructed that they would view movement sequences on the screen (see Fig. 1A) and would have to replicate them in turns afterwards using the marble and touch-sensitive pads in front of them. They were also instructed not to talk to or interact with their partner for the duration of the experiment. On the screen, subjects viewed an animation of a sequence of six movements (see Fig. 1B). The screen showed three white circles arranged in a triangle. One circle briefly turned red before returning to white to indicate the sequence. The order of the sequence was pseudo-randomized with the only constraint being that the same circle could not appear two or more times in a row. Subjects viewed the sequence twice and were instructed to memorize it. Subjects were informed that the ECG measures their heart, but were not made aware of the purpose of the experiment or that the timing of their movements was being recorded.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Task design. Panel A: Subjects were seated side by side in front of a laptop computer; Panel B: an example of the sequence of movements subjects were required to learn; Panel C: how they would accurately replicate the sequence in (B) using the touch-sensitive containers.

On the table were four touch-sensitive pads and a marble. Three of the pads were arranged in a triangle and corresponded to the circles that appeared on the screen. The fourth pad was placed in the middle and was the home pad where the marble was to be returned to at the end of each movement sequence and from where each movement sequence commenced. On each trial both subjects in the pair had to attempt to replicate the sequence, one after the other. To do this, subjects were instructed to move the marble between the touch sensitive pads in the order they believed to be the same as in the initial video (see Fig. 1C). Subjects were instructed to make the series of movements at a speed that was comfortable to them.

The order of which subject executed first and which second was counter-balanced across blocks for each pair. That is, turn-taking occurred by block, where the same subject took the first turn for a whole block, and then they swapped. Which subject was instructed to go first was alternated, so that no subject took the first turn for more than one block in a row. For 12 subjects there were two blocks of ten trials in total. In one block one subject would go first for all ten trials, and in the other the other subject would go first for all ten trials. For the remaining 14 subjects there were four blocks of ten trials in total. In two blocks one subject would go first for all ten trials, and in the other two the other subject would go first for all ten trials. ECGs were recorded throughout the duration of each block.

For 18 subjects the ECG was recorded using an Active 2 Biosemi amplifier. The ECG was recorded at 2048 Hz. For the remaining seven subjects, the ECG was recorded using a CED 1401 in Spike2 at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. The time at which the marble was placed on the touch-sensitive pad was also recorded. The experiment lasted approximately 40 min. Afterwards, subjects were debriefed about the purpose of the experiment and their experience of taking part, and were given the opportunity to ask the experimenter any questions.

2.4. Analysis

For all subjects the three electrode ECG recordings were transformed offline by linear subtraction into two sets of bipolar recordings, which in turn were averaged to produce a single ECG recording for each subject. For all subjects, the time of the peak of the R-wave of each heartbeat was calculated from the ECG. First, the ECG data were high pass filtered at 0.01 Hz to remove any linear drift. Second, a threshold was determined from the data to isolate the R-wave and the time point of local maximum for each suprathreshold peak was calculated. All components were visually inspected for error and noise. For each subject, this time point was determined at 1000 Hz (i.e., to millisecond accuracy) and in this way all data were then encoded at the same sampling rate. For each block the time the touch-sensitive pads were contacted was extracted from the event channel and also resampled at 1000 Hz where necessary. For one subject there was a failure of the touch-sensitive pads and this subject was excluded from further analysis, leaving 25 subjects for data analysis.

2.4.1. Analysis of movements and phase of the cardiac cycle

To address whether or not there was a statistical relationship between the time the marble was placed on the touch-sensitive pad and the phase of cardiac cycle, two different analyses were performed, one in the time domain and one in the phase domain.

2.4.1.1. Time domain analysis

In the time domain, we calculated the proportion of motor events that occurred in four time windows relative to the R-peak: −100–100 ms, 100–300 ms, 300–500 ms and 500–700 ms, centered around 0, 200, 400 and 600 ms respectively (Fig. 3A). The proportion of motor events that occurred in these windows was calculated for both executed and observed events. To determine if there was any effect of time on the proportion of events that occurred, a repeated measures ANOVA was run with two factors, Condition (execution and observation) and Time (0, 200, 400, and 600). Significant effects were further investigated with post-hoc t-tests, Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

. Relationship between time of movement culmination and cardiac cycle in the time domain. Panel A shows one cycle of the cardiac cycle (R-R) and the four time windows used for the analysis. Panel B shows the mean proportion of events that occurred in each of the four windows shown in A. The dots show the mean data for each subject, the bar is the mean across subjects.

2.4.1.2. Phase domain analysis

In the phase domain, we calculated the phase of the movement event as a function of the R-R interval (Kunzendorf et al., 2019). Circular statistics were employed in order to exploit the repeating nature of the cardiac cycle. To this end, for each action endpoint the time of both the preceding and proceeding R-wave was calculated and the phase of the action endpoint was calculated as a function of the R-R interval in which it occurred. For example, for an R-R interval of time tR, where the movement event occurred at time te the phase, in degrees, of that event was calculated as te/TR x 360. For each subject, the mean phase was then calculated for the execution and observation blocks separately using circular averaging. This resulted in two mean phases per subject, one for each condition (execution and observation). We then tested, separately for each condition, whether these phases differed from uniformity using Rayleigh tests. In addition, we also calculated the distribution of phases in a circular histogram, where each bin was a 20th of a circle. These data were normalized so each bin represented the proportion of total events for each subject for each condition. These were smoothed by averaging over three neighboring bins, the central bin and the two neighboring bins. The first analysis shows the uniformity of the mean phase across subjects but gives no indication of the degree of uniformity of the phases for each subject. This is shown by the second analysis. Bonferroni correction was applied to multiple comparisons.

2.5. Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study and code to produce Fig. 2, Fig. 3, Fig. 4 are available in the Open Science Framework repository [osf.io/8r39c].

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

. Relationship between cardiac and movement parameters. Panels A and B show the relationship between the mean movement time and the mean R-R interval for each subject, during action execution (A) and action observation (B). In each panel the circles show the data from each subject and the solid line shows the line of best fit. Panel C shows the relationship between the mean movement time for the two people that comprised the dyad. The significant positive relationship is consistent with the hypothesis that subjects imitated the movement speed of the other. In each panel each circle shows the data from one dyad.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Relationship between time of movement culmination and cardiac cycle in the phase domain. Panels A-F show the relationship between the time of the movement event (action end point) with respect to the phase of the R-R interval. In each panel the R peaks occurred at 0o and the phase through the R-R interval cycles clockwise round the circle. On the left-hand side of the figure, panels A, C and E show the data for executed actions and on the right-hand side, panels B, D and F show the data for observed actions. In Panel A and B the circles show the mean phase across all movements for each subject; white circles indicate wave 1 and grey circles indicate wave 2 of data collection. The circular histogram shows the frequency of the data as a function of phase. The solid black arrow shows the circular mean phase across subjects. Panels C and D depict the distribution of the mean normalized circular histograms of all movements across subjects. The solid black line depicts the mean phase distribution and the dotted lines show the standard error around this mean (SEM). Panels E and F show the same data in C and D but the whole distribution. The solid grey line shows the null distribution (a uniform distribution). Error bars represent the SEM. * denotes phases were the proportion of events are significantly different from a uniform distribution at p < .05 (uncorrected for multiple comparisons).

2.6. Code availability statement

Code to produce all data figures is available in the Open Science Framework repository [osf.io/8r39c].

3. Results

Prior to testing our main hypothesis that there would be a relationship between movement timing and the period of the cardiac cycle, we tested for a significant difference in the length of the cardiac cycle (operationalized as the R-R interval) between observation and execution conditions. We also tested whether there was a relationship between movement time of both subjects in a dyad and if there was a relationship between the length of the cardiac cycle and movement time.

There was no significant difference in the duration of the mean R-R interval between action execution and action observation (825.3 and 822.9 ms respectively; t(24) = 0.64, p = .53). There was no significant linear correlation between mean R-R interval and mean movement time. This was non-significant for both action execution (r = −0.37, p = .07) (Fig. 2A) and action observation (r = −0.12, p = .58) (Fig. 2B). There was, however, a significant linear correlation between the movement time of subjects within a dyad (r = 0.61, p < .007) (Fig. 2C). This significant relationship is consistent with the hypothesis that a common movement speed was adopted by the dyad but this was not intrinsically tied to the length of the cardiac cycle.

The main aim of this study was to test whether there was evidence of a consistent relationship between the timing of movement events and the cardiac cycle phase both when performing actions and when observing someone else perform them. This was tested in both time and phase domains.

3.1. Time domain analysis

The data were divided into four time windows, centered around 0 (−100–100 ms), 200 (100–300 ms), 400 (300–500 ms), and 600 (500–700 ms) milliseconds post R-peak (Fig. 3A). The repeated measures ANOVA with factors Condition (execution and observation) and Time (0, 200, 400, and 600) revealed a significant main effect of Time (F(2.21, 53.1) = 21.4, p < .001) and no significant effects of Condition or interaction between the two factors (F(1,24) = 0.01, p = .92; F(2.39, 57.24) = 0.10, p = .93) respectively. Post-hoc t-tests revealed that the significant effect of Time was driven by there being significantly less movement events in the 200 ms time period around the R-peak than the other three time periods (Fig. 3B). For the execution condition, the number of events in the −100–100 ms time window was significantly less than those in the 100–300 ms, and 300–500 ms time windows (Execution: t(24) = −4.59; d = −0.918, p < .05 & t(24) = −4.04, d = −0.81, p < .001). For the observation condition the number of events in the −100–100 ms time window was significantly less than those in the 100–300 ms, 300–500 ms, and 500–700 ms time windows t(24) = −7.404, d = −1.48, p < .05, t(24) = −4.91, d = −0.98, p < .001 & t(24) = −3.65, d = −0.73, p = .001). All other within Condition post-hoc comparisons were non-significant when corrected for multiple comparisons.

3.2. Phase domain analysis

In contrast to the time-domain analysis, there was no evidence that the mean phase of each subject's movements differed significantly from the uniform distribution in either the Execution or Observation conditions (Rayleigh test z = 0.07, p = .93 & z = 0.84, p = .43 respectively [Fig. 4A & B]). To investigate the phase data further we analyzed the mean distribution of the phase of all events across subjects during action execution and observation conditions. If there was no consistent phase relationship these values should be close to the expected value for a uniform distribution (Fig. 4C & D). Here, with 20 phase bins, the proportion in each bin with a uniform distribution would be 0.05 (1/20). Results indicated that in both conditions there were more motor events than expected at particular phases of the cardiac cycle (Fig. 4C–F). For the execution condition there were significantly more events than expected in the phase bin centred on 81o (t(24) = 2.07, p = .049). For the observation condition this occurred later in the cycle, in the phase bin centred on 153o (t(24) = 2.32, p = .029). It should be noted, however, that these were small effects that did not survive correction for multiple comparisons.

4. Discussion

Here, we showed that the timing of self-paced movements and periods of the cardiac cycle are linked in a similar way for both action execution and observation. We showed that the proportion of movement events was significantly less during heartbeats, synchronous with the R-peak, and significantly more likely to occur between heartbeats. This same relationship was also observed between the motor events of an actor and the cardiac cycle of an observer. That is, the observer was significantly less likely to experience a heartbeat when observing movement endpoints. Additionally, there was a significant positive correlation between movement time in execution and observation conditions, suggesting dyads adopted a common movement speed. These results build on previously separate literatures that find both a relationship between discrete motor events and cardiac timing, and cardiac coordination between interacting individuals.

In this study we investigated the relationship between movements and the cardiac cycle in both the time and phase domains. The effect of a relationship between the two was largest in the time domain analysis. Indeed, despite there being a clear and significant reduction in the proportion of events around the time of the R-peak in the time domain analysis, the group level phase analysis showed no evidence that the distribution was non-uniform. The time domain analysis assumes that any cardiac effects are time locked to the R-peak and are independent of length of the R-R interval. Whereas the phase analysis assumes that any cardiac effects are locked to a particular phase of the R-R interval and are not time locked. This difference in the effect sizes reported here most likely reflects the different sensitivities of the two analyses. For example, there is greater inter-individual variability in the duration of diastole than the duration of systole. Phase domain analyses may be more sensitive to individual differences in the length of diastole, whereas time domain analyses may be more sensitive to effects associated with systole. Future work should focus on understanding the different specificities and sensitivities of different analysis strategies to understand when they should be used and how to interpret differences between them.

Many have suggested that an internal central pacemaker guides the timing of motor actions (Anders, 1928; Coleman, 1921; McCraty et al., 2009; Treisman et al., 1990). The heart, with its regular rhythmicity that modulates with arousal level, intuitively represents a good candidate for this role. Information on the strength and timing of heartbeats is conveyed to the central nervous system via the vagal and glossopharyngeal nerves. Such information could thus be capitalized on by the motor system when timing the occurrence of actions. Indeed, there is evidence that stimulation of afferent vagal and glossopharyngeal pathways is associated with modulations of efferent motor pathways. For example, pressure applied to the carotid sinus, which promotes baroreceptor firing (Mancia and Mark, 1983), inhibits spontaneous movements and reduces muscle tone in anaesthetized animals (Koch, 1931). Efferent projections, which can delay the onset of the next heartbeat, could further optimize control over timing relationships.

The benefits of timing behavior to the cardiac cycle, however, remain unknown. Global perceptual attenuation during heartbeats has been proposed, as sensations are inferred as consequences of internal cardiac activity and therefore down-weighted (Salomon et al., 2016). Pain and somatosensory perception are increased during diastole and reduced during heartbeats (Al et al., 2020; Gray et al., 2010; Levy et al., 1970; Reid, 1969; Wilkinson et al., 2013). A recent study purports that exteroceptive sensory confidence is greatest during diastole and at its lowest concomitant with the heartbeat (Allen et al., 2019), providing a neat explanation for the preference to time movements in between heartbeats observed here.

It is possible that actions are timed to the point in the cardiac cycle where the ability to balance autonomic and behavioural demands is greatest. Previous research has shown that as an individual attends to and prepares to interact with a stimulus, vagal activation lengthens the cardiac cycle (van der Molen et al., 1985). In turn, vagal withdrawal, when the individual initiates a response, shortens the cardiac cycle (van der Molen et al., 1991; Somsen et al., 1985). However, the timing of the behavioral response also impacts the cardiac consequences. Responses made at or just after the R-wave impact the length of the current cardiac cycle, whereas responses made later in the R-R interval impact the length of subsequent cycles. Furthermore, the sensitivity of the sinoatrial node to input from the vagus is not uniform across the cardiac cycle. Instead, a period of heightened sensitivity is estimated to occur between 250 and 400 milliseconds post R-wave, followed by a period of relative insensitivity (Jennings et al., 1987; Jennings and Wood, 1977). This sensitive period broadly corresponds with the period of increased movement events in the current study, and that observed by Kunzendorf et al. (2019), also in the context of self-paced movement. Considering that sympathetic efferents typically impact heart rate relatively slowly, on a timescale of between 1300 and 2000 milliseconds (Berger et al., 1989; Salata and Zipes, 1991; Warner and Russell Jr, 1969), timing movement events may capitalize on these effects to more rapidly adjust autonomic outflow to match demand.

Additionally, it is possible that as agents, we act upon the environment such that relevant signals appear during optimal phases of the cardiac cycle (Galvez-Pol et al., 2020; Kunzendorf et al., 2019). It may be that stimuli whose processing would benefit from information on the state of cardiovascular arousal are more acutely perceived at systole, when baroreceptor feedback occurs, while stimuli that would not benefit from such signalling are better processed during diastole. In support of this, during systole, fearful faces are more readily detected, result in greater amygdala activation (Garfinkel et al., 2014), and race-related threat appraisal is heightened (Azevedo et al., 2017). Experimentally increasing heartrate (and therefore the amount of time spent in systole) heightens fear responses but not disgust responses (Pezzulo et al., 2018). Meanwhile, during diastole, target shooting (Bellamy et al., 1999; Bothwell et al., 1997) and memory for words (Garfinkel et al., 2013) are more accurate. The current results suggest we may act upon our environment in such a way as to maximize this interoceptive distinction.

Respiration was not measured in the current study but could play a role in the current findings. A recent study identified a correspondence between the timing of voluntary movement and the respiratory cycle, with movement initiated more frequently during expiration (Park et al., 2020). Expiration coincides with a decrease in heart rate, compared to the increase observed during inspiration. Therefore, movements made during inspiration are more likely to coincide with heartbeats than those made during expiration. We have greater voluntary control over respiratory than cardiac events, control that could be leveraged to better govern cardiac timing. Holding one's breath or breathing out slowly when performing a precise movement may be one way of decreasing the chances of a heartbeat occurring at the crucial moment.

In the present study, we not only observed a relationship between the cardiac cycle and action execution, but also observed a similar relationship for action observation. Observation of another may produce resonance of the mechanisms involved in executing that action. Activation may remain sub-threshold for actually producing the motor output, however associated somatovisceral changes may be produced (Paccalin and Jeannerod, 2000). Afferent interoceptive feedback resulting from these changes may in turn support inferences about the causes of the other's behavior. As such, it is possible that inference about the mental states of others is concurrently supported by a form of interoceptive mirroring. Such a mechanism might explain how, in the present data, action observation resulted in a similar movement-cardiac rhythm to that seen in the individual executing the action. As the observer's motor system predicts the actor's next movements, comparable interoceptive conditions are simulated, much like action observation results in an increase in excitability in the muscles required to perform the observed action (Gallese and Goldman, 1998).

The mutual prediction of each other's' actions is thought to facilitate joint coordinated action and the achievement of shared goals (Flanagan and Johansson, 2003; Kilner et al., 2004). There is evidence to suggest that shared representations of action form automatically even when no joint action is required and it would be more effective to ignore the other person (Sebanz et al., 2003). The results found here suggest the possibility that cardiac-movement synchronicity may support the interpersonal coordination that underlies joint action, through physiological attunement. Such coordination may serve as “social glue”, fostering feelings of group belonging and closeness (Lakin et al., 2003). A number of findings appear to support this. Professional interviewers' judgements of trainees' competence have been found to correlate positively with the trainees' convergence of speech rate and response latency (Street Jr, 1984). The degree of convergence appears to vary based on personality traits and social standing. For example, subjects who scored higher on a measure of the need for social approval converged more to their partner's speech intensity and pause length than subjects who scored lower (Natale, 1975). Moreover, greater language convergence is observed towards occupational superiors than subordinates, in that foremen converge more to the language of managers. Increased physiological covariation, in the form of galvanic skin response (GSR) has been observed in dyads who either like or dislike each other, compared to those who hold neutral opinions of each other (Kaplan et al., 1964). Future work should seek to investigate whether inter-dyad factors such as inter-individual liking and physical similarity to one's partner, or individual differences such as personality traits, are predictive of the cardiac-movement coordination effect observed here.

Previous investigations have suggested that the influence of interoceptive signals on behavior may be mediated by the individual's degree of conscious awareness of those signals (Garfinkel et al., 2013; Park et al., 2020). However, it is unclear whether the present effects rely on conscious control of behavior, or reflexive processes below the level of conscious awareness. In a previous study investigating the effect of the cardiac cycle on timing preferences for actively sampling the environment, operationalized as the time subjects chose to press a key prompting a visual stimulus relative to their cardiac phase, no effect of inter-individual differences in awareness of interoceptive signals was observed (Kunzendorf et al., 2019) If the action observation effects observed here are consciously mediated, it raises the possibility that a greater awareness of cardiac signals might facilitate greater social mimicry. Future research could test the possibility that inter-individual variability in the action observation effect reported here is related to social competencies and is mediated by interoceptive awareness.

It has been argued that a dysfunction in movement dynamics would have implications for the experience of social connectedness (Marsh et al., 2009). Individuals who do not attend to the dynamics of others' movements, either due to a lack of ability or motivation, are likely to feel less social connectedness when engaging in joint activities. Engaging in synchronous joint action may foster cognitive benefits. Synchronicity may engage the same neural processes that allow access to others' mental states (Hutchins, 1995; Semin, 2007; Semin and Cacioppo, 2008). This is in line with data suggesting that high-empathy individuals show greater non-conscious mimicry of others, termed the ‘chameleon effect’ (Chartrand and Bargh, 1999). Perhaps tellingly, the severity of positive symptoms (hallucinations and delusions) in schizophrenia is inversely correlated to the degree of movement synchronicity the individual displays in conversation with a partner (Kupper et al., 2015). Autistic children show significantly less in-sync rocking with a caregiver than neurotypical children in a rocking chair paradigm (Marsh et al., 2009) and engage in briefer turn-taking sequences than other children (Mundy et al., 1986).

Important questions remain regarding precisely under what conditions cardiac-movement coordination occurs. Understanding this may aid in interpreting previous findings that report no relationship between cardiac parameters and movement (Wallot et al., 2016). At the level of a single subject, it is likely that not all centrally mediated sensorimotor events show a phased relationship with the heart. Some actions may occur at a frequency higher than the cardiac cycle, impeding coordination between the two. Alternatively, a sequence of rhythmic movements may be required to invoke cardiac-movement coordination. It is perhaps noteworthy that in the present study, many of the subjects reported during debriefing that they found replicating the sequence of movements challenging. The cognitive demands of the task may be an essential difference between the present design and other studies that report no relationship between heart and movement. In dyads, possible mediators may include the history of interpersonal interaction, speech coordination, the structure of the task, and the dynamics of turn-taking, and the degree of behavioral coordination between the agents (Fusaroli et al., 2016). Different patterns of kinematic and autonomic coordination have been reported. Under some circumstances, a leader-follower pattern is observed (Condon and Ogston, 1971; LaFrance, 1982), while in other scenarios, convergence appears to be more bi-directional (Dale et al., 2013; Fusaroli et al., 2016; Shockley et al., 2003). It would be interesting for future research to explore how the heart rates and kinematics of individuals in a dyad change over time. A plausible prediction is that over the course of a trial, the observer's heartbeats become increasingly linked to the timing of the actor's movements. Finally, research suggests an association between elevated blood pressure and reduced social competence (Diamond, 1982; Sommers-Flanagan and Greenberg, 1989). As baroreceptor activity is tied to blood pressure, the current findings raise the possibility that reduced cardiac mirroring may underlie these findings.

In summary, these findings demonstrate a relationship between the timing of motor events and the cardiac cycle in both action execution and observation. These results suggest that interoceptive signals from the heart and self-paced movement are intrinsically linked. The observation of this phasic relationship both when executing and observing action raises the possibility that this relationship may be driving previous interpersonal coordination effects, which has been found to foster successful social interaction.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a PhD studentship awarded to E. R. Palser from the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) and the Medical Research Council (MRC). A. Fotopoulou was supported by a ‘European Research Council Starting Investigator Award’ [ERC-2012-STG GA313755].

Footnotes

Appendix A

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104907.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (285.7KB, docx)

References

  1. Al E., et al. Heart–brain interactions shape somatosensory perception and evoked potentials. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2020;117:10575–10584. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1915629117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen M., Levy A., Parr T., Friston K.J. In the body’s eye: the computational anatomy of interoceptive inference. BioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/603928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anders P. Über den individuellen Eigenrhythmus beim menschlichen Gange und seine Beziehungen zum Rhythmus der Herz- und Atemtätigkeit. Pflügers Archiv. 1928;220:287–299. doi: 10.1007/BF01780282. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Azevedo R.T., Garfinkel S.N., Critchley H.D., Tsakiris M. Cardiac afferent activity modulates the expression of racial stereotypes. Nature Communications. 2017;8:13854. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barrett L.F., Simmons W.K. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2015;16:419. doi: 10.1038/nrn3950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bellamy M., Collins D., Holmes P., Loze G. Shot patterns in ECG recordings for elite air-pistol shooters. Journal of Sports Sciences Conference Communications. 1999;17:48–49. [Google Scholar]
  7. Berger R.D., Saul J.P., Cohen R.J. Transfer function analysis of autonomic regulation, I. Canine atrial rate response. American Journal of Physiology-Heart and Circulatory. 1989;256:142–152. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.1989.256.1.H142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bernier F.J., Rosenthal R. In: Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 401–432) Feldman R.S., Rime B., editors. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, England: 1991. Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interpersonal synchrony. [Google Scholar]
  9. Birren J.E., Cardon P.V., Phillips S.L. Reaction time as a function of the cardiac cycle in young adults. Science. 1963;140 doi: 10.1126/science.140.3563.195-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bothwell S., Donne B., Andrews J.F. Does the heart rate or phase of cardiac cycle at triggering affect the performance of elite marksmen? The Journal of Physiology. 1997;501:6. [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown G.L., Eccles J.C. The action of a single vagal volley on the rhythm of the heart beat. The Journal of Physiology. 1934;82:211–241. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1934.sp003176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chartrand T.L., Bargh J.A. The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999;76:893. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Coleman W.M. The psychological significance of bodily rhythms. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1921;1:213. doi: 10.1037/h0067228. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Condon W.S., Ogston W.D. In: The Perception of Language (pp. 150–184) Horton D.L., Jenkins J.J., editors. Charles E. Merrill Press; Columbus, USA: 1971. Speech and body motion synchrony of the speaker-hearer. [Google Scholar]
  15. Dale R., Fusaroli R., Duran N.D., Richardson D.C. Chapter 2 - The self-organization of human interaction. Psychology of Learning and Motivation. 2013;59:43–95. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-407187-2.00002-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Diamond E.L. The role of anger and hostility in essential hypertension and coronary heart disease. Psychological Bulletin. 1982;92:410. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dong E., Jr., Reitz B.A. Effect of timing of vagal stimulation on heart rate in the dog. Circulation Research. 1970;27:635–646. doi: 10.1161/01.RES.27.5.635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Elkins A.N., Muth E.R., Hoover A.W., Walker A.D., Carpenter T.L., Switzer F.S. Physiological compliance and team performance. Applied Ergonomics. 2009;40:997–1003. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2009.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Flanagan J.R., Johansson R.S. Action plans used in action observation. Nat. 2003;424:769. doi: 10.1038/nature01861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fusaroli R., Bjørndahl J.S., Roepstorff A., Tylén K.A. Heart for interaction: Shared physiological dynamics and behavioral coordination in a collective, creative construction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 2016;42:1297. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gallese V., Goldman A. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1998;2:493–501. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01262-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Galvez-Pol A., McConnell R., Kilner J.M. Active sampling in visual search is coupled to the cardiac cycle. Cognition. 2020;196:104149. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Garfinkel S.N., et al. What the heart forgets: Cardiac timing influences memory for words and is modulated by metacognition and interoceptive sensitivity. Psychophysiology. 2013;50:505–512. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Garfinkel S.N., et al. Fear from the heart: Sensitivity to fear stimuli depends on individual heartbeats. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:6573–6582. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3507-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gray M.A., Minati L., Paoletti G., Critchley H.D. Baroreceptor activation attenuates attentional effects on pain-evoked potentials. PAIN®. 2010;151:853–861. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hadar U., Steiner T.J., Rose F.C. Head movement during listening turns in conversation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 1985;9:214–228. doi: 10.1007/BF00986881. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hale J., Ward J.A., Buccheri F., Oliver D., Hamilton A. Are you on my wavelength? Interpersonal coordination in naturalistic conversations. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour. 2018 doi: 10.1007/s10919-019-00320-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Henning R.A., Korbelak K.T. Social-psychophysiological compliance as a predictor of future team performance. Psychologia. 2005;48:84–92. doi: 10.2117/psysoc.2005.84. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Henning R.A., Boucsein W., Gil M.O. Social-physiological compliance as a determinant of team performance. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2001;40:221–232. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00190-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hove M.J., Risen J.L. It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition. 2009;27:949–960. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hutchins E. MIT press; London: 1995. Cognition in the Wild. [Google Scholar]
  32. Jennings J.R., Wood C.C. Cardiac cycle time effects on performance, phasic cardiac responses, and their intercorrelation in choice reaction time. Psychophysiol. 1977;14:297–307. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1977.tb01179.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jennings J.R., van der Molen M.W., Terezis C. Primary bradycardia and vagal inhibition as two manifestations of the timing of vagal influence on the heart beat. Journal of Psychophysiology. 1987;1:361–374. [Google Scholar]
  34. Jennings J.R., van der Molen M.A., Brock K., Somsen R.J.M. On the synchrony of stopping motor responses and delaying heartbeats. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 1992;18:422–436. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.2.422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Joffily M., Coricelli G. Emotional valence and the free-energy principle. PLoS Computational Biology. 2013;9 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kaplan H.B., Burch N.R., Bloom S.W. In: Psychobiological Approaches to Social Behavior. Leiderman P.H., Shapiro D., editors. Stanford University Press; Stanford, CA: 1964. Physiological covariation and sociometric relationships in small peer groups. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kilner J.M., Vargas C., Duval S., Blakemore S.J., Sirigu A. Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:1299. doi: 10.1038/nn1355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kilner J.M., Friston K.J., Frith C.D. Predictive coding: An account of the mirror neuron system. Cognitive Processing. 2007;8:159–166. doi: 10.1007/s10339-007-0170-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Koch E. In: Ergebnisse der Kreislaufforschung. Kisch E., editor. Dresden; Steinkopff: 1931. Die reflektorische selbststeuerung des kreislaufes. [Google Scholar]
  40. Konvalinka I., et al. Synchronized arousal between performers and related spectators in a fire-walking ritual. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:8514–8519. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016955108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kunzendorf S., et al. Active information sampling varies across the cardiac cycle. Psychophysiol. 2019 doi: 10.1111/psyp.13322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kupper Z., Ramseyer F., Hoffmann H., Tschacher W. Nonverbal synchrony in social interactions of patients with schizophrenia indicates socio-communicative deficits. PLoS One. 2015;10 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lacey B.C., Lacey J.I. Change in heart period: A function of sensorimotor event timing within the cardiac cycle. Physiological Psychology. 1977;5 doi: 10.3758/BF03335349. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lacey B.C., Lacey J.I. Two-way communication between the heart and the brain: Significance of time within the cardiac cycle. The American Psychologist. 1978;33:99. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.33.2.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lacey J.I., Lacey B.C. In: Physiological Correlates of Emotion (pp. 205–227) Black P., editor. Academic Press; New York, USA & London, UK: 1970. Some autonomic-central nervous system interrelationships. [Google Scholar]
  46. Lacey J.I., Kagan J., Lacey B.C., Moss H.A. In: Expression of the Emotions in Man (pp. 161–196) Knapp P.H., editor. International Universities Press; New York, USA: 1963. The visceral level: Situational determinants and behavioral correlates of autonomic response patterns. [Google Scholar]
  47. LaFrance M. In: Interaction Rhythms: Periodicity in Communicative Behavior, (pp. 279–298) Davis M., editor. Human Scientific Press; New York, NY: 1982. Posture mirroring and rapport. [Google Scholar]
  48. Lakin J.L., Jefferis V.E., Cheng C.M., Chartrand T.L. The Chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 2003;27:145–162. doi: 10.1023/A:1025389814290. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Levenson R.W., Gottman J.M. Marital interaction: Physiological linkage and affective exchange. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1983;45:587. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Levenson R.W., Ruef A.M. Empathy: A physiological substrate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;63:234. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Levy M.N., Martin P.J. Physiology and Pathophysiology of the Heart (pp. 337–354) Springer; Boston, MA: 1984. Neural control of the heart. [Google Scholar]
  52. Levy M.N., Martin P.J., Iano T., Zieske H. Effects of single vagal stimuli on heart rate and atrioventricular conduction. The American Journal of Physiology. 1970;218:1256–1262. doi: 10.1152/ajplegacy.1970.218.5.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mancia G., Mark A.L. In: Handbook of Physiology, Section 2: The Cardiovascular System (pp. 755–793) Shepherd J.T., Abboud F.M., editors. American Physiological Society; Bethesda, MD: 1983. Arterial baroreflexes in humans. [Google Scholar]
  54. Marsh K.L., Richardson M.J., Schmidt R.C. Social connection through joint action and interpersonal coordination. Topics in Cognitive Science. 2009;1:320–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01022.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. McCraty R., Atkinson M., Tomasino D., Bradley R.T. The coherent heart: Heart-brain interactions, psychophysiological coherence, and the emergence of system-wide order. Integral Review: A Transdisciplinary & Transcultural Journal for New Thought, Research, & Praxis. 2009;5 [Google Scholar]
  56. van der Molen M.W., Somsen R.M.J., Orlebeke J.F. In: Advances in Psychophysiology. Ackles P.K., Jennings J.R., Coles M.G.H., editors. Jai Press; NY, USA: 1985. The rhythm of the heart beat in information processing. [Google Scholar]
  57. van der Molen M.W., Bashore T.R., Halliday R., Callaway E. In: Handbook of cognitive psychophysiology: Central and autonomic nervous system approaches. Jennings J.R., Coles M.G.H., editors. John Wiley & Sons; NJ, USA: 1991. Chronopsychophysiology: Mental chronometry augmented by psychophysiological time markers. [Google Scholar]
  58. Mundy P., Sigman M., Ungerer J., Sherman T. Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1986;27:657–669. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00190.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Natale M. Convergence of mean vocal intensity in dyadic communication as a function of social desirability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1975;32:790. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.32.5.790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Noy L., Levit-Binun N., Golland Y. Being in the zone: physiological markers of togetherness in joint improvisation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2015;9:187. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ondobaka S., Kilner J., Friston K. The role of interoceptive inference in theory of mind. Brain and Cognition. 2015;112:64–68. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Paccalin C., Jeannerod M. Changes in breathing during observation of effortful actions. Brain Research. 2000;862:194–200. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02145-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Palmer C.E., Tsakiris M. Going at the heart of social cognition: is there a role for interoception in self-other distinction? Current Opinion in Psychology. 2018;24:21–26. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Park H.D., Barnoud C., Trang H., Kannape O.A., Schaller K., Blanke O. Breathing is coupled with voluntary action and the cortical readiness potential. Nature Communications. 2020;11:1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13967-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pezzulo G., Iodice P., Barca L., Chausse P., Monceau S., Mermillod M. Increased heart rate after exercise facilitates the processing of fearful but not disgusted faces. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:398. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18761-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Press C., Heyes C., Kilner J.M. Learning to understand others’ actions. Biology Letters. 2011;7:457–460. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Rae C.L., et al. Response inhibition on the stop signal task improves during cardiac contraction. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:9136. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27513-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Reid J.V.O. The cardiac pacemaker: Effects of regularly spaced nervous input. American Heart Journal. 1969;78:58–64. doi: 10.1016/0002-8703(69)90259-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Rizzolatti G., Fogassi L., Gallese V. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2001;2:661. doi: 10.1038/35090060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Salata J.J., Zipes D.P. Reflex control of the circulation. CRC Press; FL, USA: 1991. Autonomic nervous system control of heart rate and atrioventricular nodal conduction. [Google Scholar]
  71. Salomon R., et al. The insula mediates access to awareness of visual stimuli presented synchronously to the heartbeat. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:5115–5127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4262-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sebanz N., Knoblich G., Prinz W. Representing others' actions: Just like one's own? Cogn. 2003;88:B11–B21. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00043-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Semin G.R. In: Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 630–649) Kruglanski A.W., Higgins E.T., editors. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2007. Grounding communication: Synchrony. [Google Scholar]
  74. Semin G.R., Cacioppo J.T. In: Embodied Grounding: Social, Cognitive, Affective, and Neuroscientific approaches (pp. 119–147) Semin G.R., Smith E.R., editors. Cambridge University Press; New York, NY: 2008. Grounding social cognition: Synchronization, entrainment, and coordination. [Google Scholar]
  75. Seth A.K., Critchley H.D. Extending predictive processing to the body: emotion as interoceptive inference. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2013;36:227–228. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12002270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Seth A.K., Friston K.J. Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences. 2016;371:20160007. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Shah P., Catmur C., Bird G. From heart to mind: linking interoception, emotion, and theory of mind. Cortex. 2017;93:220. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Shockley K., Santana M.V., Fowler C.A. Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 2003;29:326. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sommers-Flanagan J., Greenberg R.P. Psychosocial variables and hypertension: A new look at an old controversy. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1989;177:15–24. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198901000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Somsen R.J.M., Van der Molen M.W., Jennings J.R., Orlebeke J.F. Response initiation, not completion, seems to alter cardiac cycle length. Psychophysiol. 1985;22:319–325. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1985.tb01607.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Strang A.J., Funke G.J., Russell S.M., Dukes A.W., Middendorf M.S. Physio-behavioral coupling in a cooperative team task: Contributions and relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 2014;40:145–158. doi: 10.1037/a0033125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Street R.L., Jr. Speech convergence and speech evaluation in fact-finding interviews. Human Communication Research. 1984;11:139–169. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1984.tb00043.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. Treisman M., Faulkner A., Naish P.L., Brogan D. The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Percept. 1990;19:705–742. doi: 10.1068/p190705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Wallot S., Mitkidis P., McGraw J.J., Roepstorff A. Beyond synchrony: joint action in a complex production task reveals beneficial effects of decreased interpersonal synchrony. PLoS One. 2016;11 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0168306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Warner H.R., Russell R.O., Jr. Effect of combined sympathetic and vagal stimulation on heart rate in the dog. Circulation Research. 1969;24:567–573. doi: 10.1161/01.RES.24.4.567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Wicker B., et al. Both of us disgusted in my insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron. 2003;40:655–664. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wilkinson M., McIntyre D., Edwards L. Electrocutaneous pain thresholds are higher during systole than diastole. Biological Psychology. 2013;94:71–73. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Xygalatas D., Konvalinka I., Bulbulia J., Roepstorff A. Quantifying collective effervescence: Heart-rate dynamics at a fire-walking ritual. Communicative & Integrative Biology. 2011;4:735–738. doi: 10.4161/cib.17609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (285.7KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study and code to produce Fig. 2, Fig. 3, Fig. 4 are available in the Open Science Framework repository [osf.io/8r39c].

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

. Relationship between cardiac and movement parameters. Panels A and B show the relationship between the mean movement time and the mean R-R interval for each subject, during action execution (A) and action observation (B). In each panel the circles show the data from each subject and the solid line shows the line of best fit. Panel C shows the relationship between the mean movement time for the two people that comprised the dyad. The significant positive relationship is consistent with the hypothesis that subjects imitated the movement speed of the other. In each panel each circle shows the data from one dyad.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Relationship between time of movement culmination and cardiac cycle in the phase domain. Panels A-F show the relationship between the time of the movement event (action end point) with respect to the phase of the R-R interval. In each panel the R peaks occurred at 0o and the phase through the R-R interval cycles clockwise round the circle. On the left-hand side of the figure, panels A, C and E show the data for executed actions and on the right-hand side, panels B, D and F show the data for observed actions. In Panel A and B the circles show the mean phase across all movements for each subject; white circles indicate wave 1 and grey circles indicate wave 2 of data collection. The circular histogram shows the frequency of the data as a function of phase. The solid black arrow shows the circular mean phase across subjects. Panels C and D depict the distribution of the mean normalized circular histograms of all movements across subjects. The solid black line depicts the mean phase distribution and the dotted lines show the standard error around this mean (SEM). Panels E and F show the same data in C and D but the whole distribution. The solid grey line shows the null distribution (a uniform distribution). Error bars represent the SEM. * denotes phases were the proportion of events are significantly different from a uniform distribution at p < .05 (uncorrected for multiple comparisons).

RESOURCES