Abstract
Objective
Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD) is common in Parkinson’s Disease (PD) and is associated with cognitive impairment; however, the majority of the evidence on the impact of RBD on multidomain cognitive batteries in PD is cross-sectional. This study evaluated the longitudinal impact of probable RBD (pRBD) on cognitive, psychiatric, and functional outcomes in people with PD.
Method
Case–control study. A total of 65 people with PD completed the study protocol at baseline and 16-to-47-month follow-up. Participants were classified as pRBD+ (n = 25) or pRBD− (n = 40) based on an established cutoff of 6 on the RBD Sleep Questionnaire (RBDSQ). Participants also completed a) comprehensive cognitive testing, b) self-report measures of depression, anxiety, and apathy, and c) performance-based and other-report forms of instrumental activities of daily living.
Results
Baseline mean age was 67.8 (SD = 8.1; range = 45–86) and baseline mean years of education was 16.4 (SD = 2.1; range = 12–20). The two groups did not differ on measured demographic characteristics. Baseline mean T-scores for cognitive tests were in the average range (46–55). Hierarchical linear models tested group differences in cognitive and functional decline from baseline to follow-up, controlling for appropriate demographic and psychiatric variables. Compared to the pRBD− group, pRBD+ participants showed greater decline in attention/working memory (r = −0.31; p = 0.01) and UPSA financial skills (r = −0.31; p = 0.01). No other group differences approached significance.
Conclusions
RBD may differentially affect attention/working memory and financial abilities in PD. Results underscore the importance of regular RBD screening in older adults with PD in order to triage symptomatic patients to appropriate cognitive and medical interventions.
Keywords: Neuropsychology, Neurodegeneration, Movement disorders, Activities of daily living, Sleep
Introduction
Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a movement disorder that has traditionally been characterized by motor symptoms such as bradykinesia, rest tremor, and rigidity (Postuma et al., 2015). In recent years, the scope of PD research has broadened to include nonmotor symptoms such as cognitive impairment, sleep disturbance, and psychological distress, which increase mortality and reduce quality of life in people with the disease (Chaudhuri, Healy, & Schapira, 2006; Marras & Chaudhuri, 2016; Poewe, 2008; Schapira, Chaudhuri, & Jenner, 2017; Sveinbjornsdottir, 2016). Cognitive impairment is common in PD (Aarsland, 2016; Domellof, Ekman, Forsfren, & Elgh, 2015; Goldman et al., 2018; Pedersen, Larsen, Tsynes, & Alves, 2017), with approximately one quarter of nondemented PD patients meeting criteria for mild cognitive impairment (MCI; Aarsland et al., 2017), and a disproportionately high number of these individuals ultimately transitioning to a dementia diagnosis (Aarsland et al., 2017; Kehagia, Barker, & Robbins, 2010; Pedersen et al., 2017). Moreover, in the context of significant relationships between cognitive abilities and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) in older adults (Hanks, Rapport, Millis, & Deshpande, 1999; Jefferson, Paul, Ozonoff, & Cohen, 2006; Razani et al., 2007), patients with PD-MCI show measurable difficulties with IADLs as measured by performance-based tests (Pirogovsky et al., 2014; Sumida et al. 2021).
In conjunction with motor symptoms and cognitive impairment, sleep disturbances such as Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD) are common in PD, with roughly one-third to one-half of PD patients meeting criteria for RBD (Gagnon et al., 2002; Rolinski et al., 2014; Sixel-Döring, Trautmann, Mollenhauer, & Trenkwalder, 2011). RBD is a parasomnia characterized by a lack of muscle atonia during REM sleep, resulting in physical dream enactments (Marques et al., 2010) and often occurring prodromal to the onset of motor symptoms in PD (Berg et al., 2014; Gagnon, Postuma, Mazza, Doyon, & Montplaisir, 2006). Idiopathic RBD is associated with cognitive impairment (Ferini-Strambi et al., 2004; Figorilli et al., 2020; Marques et al., 2010; Youn et al., 2016) and cognitive decline more broadly (Boeve, 2010). Additionally, some evidence suggests that RBD is associated with a faster rate of cognitive decline in PD (Folle, Paul, Bronstein, Keener, & Ritz, 2019), and the presence of comorbid RBD and MCI is related to a faster rate of progression of overall PD symptoms (Fereshtehnejad et al., 2015). Taken together, this suggests that early recognition of RBD is important for the identification of those PD patients who are at increased risk for later cognitive decline and disease progression (Lin & Chen, 2018).
A substantial cross-sectional literature suggests that PD patients with RBD (PD/RBD+) perform worse on cognitive batteries than PD patients without RBD (PD/RBD−; Mao et al., 2020), although limitations inherent in this methodology prevent a full understanding of the progression of PD/RBD and cognition over time. Specifically, past cross-sectional studies have shown that, compared to PD/RBD−, PD/RBD+ perform worse on measures of attention (Jozwiak et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2016), visuospatial abilities (Jozwiak et al., 2017; Pagano et al., 2018; Vendette et al., 2007), learning (Jozwiak et al., 2017; Mahmood et al., 2020; Vendette et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2016), memory (Jozwiak et al., 2017; Pagano et al., 2018; Vendette et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2016), and executive functions (Jozwiak et al., 2017; Trout et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2016). In contrast, the literature on RBD and psychiatric functioning in PD is mixed in that PD/RBD+ patients have not always differed from PD/RBD− in terms of depression, anxiety, and apathy (Jozwiak et al., 2017; Mahmood et al., 2020; Pagano et al., 2018; Xie, Shen, Zhou, & Xu, 2021). A large literature suggests that psychiatric symptoms are associated with both worse cognition (Robinson, Watkins, & Harmon-Jones, 2013) and lower quality sleep (Walker, 2009) in the general population, so it is important to account for psychiatric symptoms in investigations of cognition and RBD in PD.
The majority of the current research investigating the impact of RBD on multiple domains of cognition is cross-sectional, thereby diminishing prognostic utility. In contrast, longitudinal investigations could provide evidence regarding the predictive validity of RBD in PD with respect to cognitive outcomes. Unfortunately, this literature is sparse and so firm conclusions cannot yet be drawn from it. Postuma, Lang, Gagnon, Pelletier, and Montplaisir (2012) showed that RBD is a risk factor for later conversion to PD dementia, and three studies reported that PD/RBD+ was associated with more rapid cognitive decline over time than PD/RBD−, using the Mini-Mental State Examination or Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; De La Riva, Smith, Xie, & Weintraub, 2014; Folle et al., 2019; Pagano et al., 2018). Only one longitudinal study to our knowledge examined the effects of RBD on cognitive decline in PD with a multi-domain neuropsychological battery. Chahine et al. (2016) administered the RBD Screening Questionnaire (RBDSQ; Stiasny-Kolster et al., 2007), the MoCA, and an abbreviated 6-test battery to 423 nondemented PD patients, with multiple yearly follow-up assessments. They reported that baseline probable RBD (pRBD) correlated with later cognitive decline on the MoCA, one test of attention/processing speed, and delayed verbal memory (free recall). However, Chahine and colleagues did not measure aspects of executive functioning (e.g., verbal abstraction, problem solving, inhibitory control) or visual memory, and they did not report on longitudinal relationships among baseline pRBD and changes in psychiatric symptomatology or IADLs. Both dysexecutive (Dirnberger & Jahanshahi, 2013; Kudlicka, Clare, & Hindle, 2011; Zgaljardic et al., 2006) and psychiatric symptoms (Aarsland, Marsh, & Schrag, 2009; den Brok et al., 2015; Dissanayaka et al., 2017) are common in PD (including PD/RBD+) and contribute to disease burden, so it is important to understand the relationship between RBD and these symptoms. Moreover, the ability to complete IADLs is key to healthy, autonomous aging, and yet IADLs have been understudied in PD/RBD; consequently, there is a need to further examine and report on these factors.
In the current study, we sought to expand on the results of Chahine et al. (2016) with a longitudinal investigation of the cognitive and functional effects of pRBD in PD, accounting for psychiatric symptomatology. We administered a comprehensive cognitive battery, measures of psychiatric symptoms, and tests of IADLs to PD patients at baseline and 16-to-47-month follow-up. Our primary hypothesis was that the baseline pRBD+ group would decline to a greater degree than the pRBD− group on tests of attention/working memory, visuospatial abilities, episodic memory, and executive functions. We also explored the impact of baseline pRBD+ on follow-up IADLs and symptoms of depression, anxiety, and apathy.
Methods
Participants and Procedure
We recruited participants from the community and the Movement Disorders Clinic at the University of San Diego and VA San Diego between 2009 and 2017 as part of a longitudinal parent study on cognitive decline in PD. Inclusion criteria for the current study were a) the completion of baseline RBDSQ and b) both baseline and a 16–47-month follow-up assessment of objective cognitive performance. Variability in follow-up duration occurred for several reasons, including missed appointments, transportation difficulties, and lack of patient availability for comprehensive research evaluations.
Board-certified neurologists specializing in movement disorders diagnosed PD based on the United Kingdom Parkinson’s Disease Society Bank Criteria (Hughes, Ben-Shlomo, Daniel, & Lees, 1992). We excluded PD patients for the following reasons: 1) the presence of plausible secondary causes of parkinsonism at baseline, 2) a comorbid neurodegenerative illness, 3) untreated major depression and/or psychotic symptoms, 4) prior treatment for substance abuse, 5) positive screen for dementia based on a baseline score of <124 on the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (MDRS; Llebaria et al., 2008; Schmidt et al., 1994), or 6) missing data from over half of the cognitive tests at both time points. Ultimately, 65 participants aged 48–86 (22 women; 12–20 years of education) met both inclusion and exclusion criteria (PD/pRBD+ = 25; PD/pRBD− = 40; see Table 1 for demographic characteristics), representing 38% of the sample reported in a cross-sectional analysis (Mahmood et al., 2020).
Table 1.
T1 PD/pRBD + Mean (SD) or n (%) (n = 25) |
T1 PD/pRBD− Mean (SD) or n (%) (n = 40) |
T2 PD/pRBD + Mean (SD) or n (%) (n = 25) |
T2 PD/pRBD− Mean (SD) or n (%) (n = 40) |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographic Characteristics | ||||
Age (years) | 68.20 (7.82) | 67.50 (8.32) | 70.56 (7.81) | 69.83 (8.32) |
Education (years) | 15.84 (2.21) | 16.80 (2.03) | -- | -- |
Gender (% men) | 64 | 68 | -- | -- |
Race/Ethnicity (% White) | 92 | 95 | -- | -- |
Disease Characteristics | ||||
Duration of PD (years) | 5.82 (4.08) | 5.93 (5.83) | 8.29 (3.71) | 8.09 (5.68) |
UPDRS Part III * | 12.88 (7.60) | 17.70 (13.31) | 17.09 (11.45) | 22.09 (12.58) |
Modified Hoehn and Yahr stage | 1.69 (0.59) | 1.77 (0.88) | 2.07 (0.68) | 2.03 (0.86) |
Levodopa dosage equivalence | 829.82 (336.56) | 649.32 (628.95) | 795.82 (410.09) | 678.90 (355.73) |
PD-MCI status (%) | 4 (16%) | 9 (23%) | 12 (50%) | 11 (28%) |
Mood/Sleep Characteristics | ||||
RBDSQ | 8.64 (2.34) | 3.18 (1.43) | 8.88 (3.09) | 4.82 (2.29) |
Geriatric Depression Scale | 7.20 (4.56) | 5.23 (4.14) | 6.48 (5.46) | 5.53 (4.57) |
Apathy Scale | 11.72 (4.70) | 10.23 (4.58) | 11.60 (6.38) | 10.27 (4.60) |
State Anxiety (STAI) | 35.00 (8.62) | 32.55 (9.52) | 36.52 (8.87) | 35.16 (11.61) |
Trait Anxiety (STAI) | 34.96 (8.51) | 33.30 (9.40) | 36.16 (9.41) | 34.03 (9.48) |
Cognitive Functioning | ||||
MDRS total score | 139.16 (3.40) | 139.03 (3.53) | 135.52 (8.21) | 138.25 (4.32) |
Global Cognition | 50.87 (6.91) | 53.40 (5.37) | 46.67 (9.14) | 50.94 (7.93) |
Attention/Working memory | 45.87 (7.58) | 46.77 (7.26) | 40.39 (9.45) | 45.13 (9.63) |
Language | 52.25 (9.96) | 51.46 (9.99) | 50.55 (10.32) | 49.41 (10.51) |
Visuospatial functioning | 53.28 (7.58) | 56.17 (9.23) | 46.69 (12.07) | 51.97 (10.33) |
Learning | 50.21 (9.90) | 56.03 (7.23) | 46.59 (11.52) | 53.40 (9.67) |
Memory | 52.52 (8.70) | 56.42 (7.46) | 50.08 (10.18) | 55.45 (9.25) |
Executive Functioning | 51.97 (8.32) | 54.28 (5.82) | 48.10 (9.88) | 50.31 (9.85) |
Functional Capacity | ||||
MMAA | 29.16 (5.15) | 30.68 (3.65) | 25.13 (8.20) | 29.44 (5.73) |
UPSA (Finance subscale) | 10.12 (0.83) | 10.03 (0.95) | 8.96 (2.51) | 10.14 (1.27) |
IADL Scale | 13.79 (2.59) | 14.32 (2.06) | 12.54 (4.20) | 13.00 (3.38) |
Global Cognition = global neuropsychological performance (calculated as an average of all individual cognitive measures); IADL = Instrumental Activities of Daily Living; MDRS = Mattis Dementia Rating Scale; MMAA = Medication Management Abilities Assessment; PD-MCI=Parkinson’s disease-Mild Cognitive Impairment status; RBDSQ = REM Behavior Disorder Screening Questionnaire; STAI=State/Trait Anxiety Inventory; T1 = time point 1 (baseline); T2 = time point 2 (follow-up); UPDRS=Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; UPSA = UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment.
*No on versus off medication differences between groups.
All cognitive domains report results using T-scores (M ± SD = 50 ± 10, range 20–80), except for the MDRS total score, which is expressed as a total raw score (range 0–144).
PD patients who were receiving dopaminergic therapy as part of their ongoing treatment completed the neurocognitive battery while on medication; levodopa equivalent dosage (LED) is presented in Table 1 (Mahmood et al., 2020). Motor symptoms were assessed using Part III of the Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS; Goetz et al., 2008) and a modified version of the Hoehn and Yahr rating scale (Goetz et al., 2004) to denote the disease stage. Of the 62 participants with available UPDRS-Part III data at baseline, 3% were “off” (i.e., overnight washout) medications and 97% were “on” (i.e., normal routine) medications during the UPDRS-Part III, with no differences between the PD/pRBD groups (p = 0.521). Of the 54 participants with available UPDRS-Part III data at follow-up, 100% were “on” at follow-up. PD-MCI status was based on Level II criteria of the Movement Disorder Society Task Force Guidelines (Litvan et al., 2012).
All 65 participants completed a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery (see Table 2) and additional psychiatric and clinical measures at baseline and 16–47-month follow-up. Participants provided informed consent and the local institutional review board approved the study.
Table 2.
Cognitive domain | Tests within domain | Normative data | Demographic adjustments |
---|---|---|---|
Attention/ Working Memory |
CVLT-II Trial 1 | Delis et al., 2000 | Age, gender |
DOT-A | Schiehser et al., in preparation | Gender | |
DKEFS Color Naming | Delis et al., 2001 | Age | |
Language | MDRS Similarities | Neurocognition Movement Lab norms | -- |
DKEFS Category Fluency | Delis et al., 2001 | Age | |
Visuospatial Functioning | JLOT | Schiehser et al., in preparation | Gender |
WMS-III Visual Reproduction Copy | Wechsler, 1997 | Age, education | |
Learning | CVLT II Trials 1–5 Total | Delis et al., 2000 | Age, gender |
WMS-III Logical Memory I Recall | Wechsler, 1997 | Age, education | |
WMS-III Visual Reproduction I Recall | Wechsler, 1997 | Age, education | |
Memory | CVLT II Long Delay Free Recall WMS-III Logical Memory II Recall WMS-III Visual Reproduction II Recall |
Delis et al., 2000
Wechsler, 1997 Wechsler, 1997 |
Age, gender Age, education Age, education |
Executive Functioning | WCST Total Errors | Heaton & Staff, 1993 | Age, education |
DKEFS Letter Fluency | Delis et al., 2001 | Age | |
DKEFS Color-Word Inhibition | Delis et al., 2001 | Age | |
DKEFS Inhibition/Switching | Delis et al., 2001 | Age |
CVLT-II = California Verbal Learning Test -Second Edition; DKEFS=Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; DOT-A = Ascending Digit Order Test; JLOT = Judgment of Line Orientation Test; MDRS = Mattis Dementia Rating Scale; WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WMS-III=Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition.
Measures
RBD
The REM Sleep Behavior Disorder Screening Questionnaire (RBDSQ; Stiasny-Kolster et al., 2007) is a self-report measure of RBD symptom severity. Prior research suggests that a cutoff score of 6 has good sensitivity (0.842) and specificity (0.962) in identifying RBD in PD (Nomura, Inoue, Kagimura, Uemura, & Nakashima, 2011; Wang et al., 2015). We administered the RBDSQ and grouped participants into pRBD+ and pRBD− groups, as described in the Statistical Analyses section.
Cognitive performance
Participants underwent a comprehensive neuropsychological battery assessing attention/working memory, language, visuospatial functioning, learning, memory, and executive functions (Table 2), and comprised of tests that are frequently administered in PD patients (Pirogovsky-Turk et al., 2017). We calculated cognitive composites for the six cognitive domains by averaging T scores for tests within each domain, with the constraint that at least two individual measures were required to calculate a composite (Miller & Rohling, 2001; see Table 2).
Functional capacity
Participants completed the UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment (UPSA; Patterson, Goldman, McKibbin, Hughs, & Jeste, 2001) finance subtest to assess financial management skills and the Medication Management Abilities Assessment (MMAA; Patterson et al., 2002) to measure the ability to take medications as instructed. PD patients’ caregivers completed the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale (IADL; Lawton & Brody, 1969). We analyzed total scores from these measures, with higher scores reflecting better performance and functioning.
Neuropsychiatric symptoms
We measured mood with the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; Yesavage et al., 1982), anxiety with the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983) Trait and State total scores (Barnes, Harp, & Jung, 2002), and apathy with the Apathy Scale (Starkstein et al., 1992). Empirical support exists for the psychometric properties of each of these measures in PD (Dissanayaka et al., 2017; Leentjens et al., 2008a, 2008b). Higher scores indicate more severe neuropsychiatric symptoms.
Statistical Analyses
We classified PD patients into PD/pRBD+ (n = 25) and PD/pRBD− (n = 40) using an established cutoff score of 6 on the RBDSQ (Nomura et al., 2011). Baseline sample and PD disease characteristics are summarized in Table 1. Four participants (three PD/pRBD−, one PD/pRBD+) were missing the IADL Scale at baseline, two (one PD/pRBD−, one PD/pRBD+) were missing the MMAA at follow-up, six (five PD/pRBD−, one PD/pRBD+) were missing the IADL Scale at follow-up, 12 (eight PD/pRBD−, four PD/pRBD+) were missing the GDS at follow-up, three (all PD/pRBD−) were missing the AS at follow-up, and two (both PD/pRBD−) were missing the STAI at follow-up.
Given variability in duration between baseline and follow-up assessment (16–47 months), study hypotheses were tested using hierarchical linear models (HLM) in R (R Core Team, 2020) with lme4 (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015). All available data were used for parameter estimates, via maximum likelihood estimation, in order to account for missing data (Schafer & Graham, 2002; Singer, Willett, & Willett, 2003). The random effect of intercept for individuals was included in all models; model convergence issues precluded the inclusion of the random effect of visit. Visit was modeled as a continuous parameter representing time in months. In addition to visit as the lower level predictor, we entered a dummy-coded variable representing the comparison between PD/pRBD− and PD/pRBD+ patients (coded as 0 and 1, respectively) as the higher order predictor variable (i.e., between person variables). The resulting group × visit cross-level interaction term represents the parameter of interest in the current study.
Based on established methods for covariate selection (Raab, Day, & Sales, 2000), bivariate Pearson/Spearman correlations determined that demographic characteristics (i.e., age, gender, education) and baseline severity of depression and apathy were significantly associated with cognitive outcomes; thus, these variables were included as covariates within the HLMs. Age emerged as the only statistically significant bivariate correlate of functional and psychiatric outcomes, with depression and apathy also emerging as a bivariate correlate of IADL scores. As such, these variables were included as covariates within their respective models. Time since PD diagnosis did not correlate with any outcomes of interest, so it was not included as a covariate in the models. Moreover, results from models covarying for LED were comparable to models that did not; as such, results from the latter are reported.
We performed backward selection elimination of covariates with p > 0.05 in the HLMs. Examination of model fit statistics (−2 log likelihood [−2LL], Akaike Information Criterion [AIC], and Schwarz's Bayesian Criterion [BIC]) determined that the inclusion of these additional covariates did not improve model fit. As such, data from the more parsimonious models are reported, with remaining covariates specified for each model. Estimate effect sizes for the HLM analyses are reported as r values (small = 0.10; medium = 0.30; large = 0.50; Cohen, 1992). Alpha was set a priori at p < .05.
Results
Baseline RBDSQ scores were as follows: PD/pRBD+: M = 8.64 (SD = 2.34), and PD/pRBD−: M = 3.18 (SD = 1.43). At baseline, there were no group differences on MDS-UPDRS Part III, U = 381, p = 0.17, Hoehn & Yahr stage t(61) = 0.40, p = 0.69, or time since PD diagnosis, t(62) = 0.08, p = 0.94; however, the groups differed on LED, U = 703.5, p = 0.006, with PD/pRBD+ evidencing higher LED. Controlling for gender and education, the PD/pRBD+ group had lower learning at baseline, but the difference was not statistically significant, B = −4.08, t(1, 84.27) = −1.89, p = 0.06, r = −0.20. Similarly, after controlling for gender, depressive symptom severity was higher in the PD/pRBD+ group at baseline, but this difference was also not statistically significant, B = 2.01, t(1, 96.61) = 1.80, p = 0.08, r = 0.18. There were no other group differences at baseline.
Controlling for education and gender, PD/pRBD+ patients’ global cognitive scores were not significantly different than those of PD/pRBD− patients’ at follow-up, B = −0.07, t(1, 65.62) = −1.62, p = 0.11, r = −0.20, although the effect was in the prediction direction. Controlling for age and education, HLMs examining group-related change in individual cognitive domains revealed that PD/pRBD+ patients declined to a greater degree in attention/working memory performance than PD/pRBD− patients, B = −0.16, t(1, 65.85) = −2.61, p = 0.01, r = −0.31. No other group-related changes in individual cognitive domains were significant; however, all effects were in the predicted direction except for the language domain (r = 0.03; p = 0.84).
Controlling for the effects of age at baseline, PD/pRBD+ patients showed a greater decline in UPSA financial subscale scores than PD/pRBD− patients, B = −0.05, t(1, 67.50) = −3.39, p = 0.001, r = −0.38, and MMAA, B = −0.12, t(1, 63.34) = −2.03, p = 0.046, r = −0.25. Given non-normal residual distributions for the HLMs examining group-related differences in change in UPSA and MMAA scores, the models were rerun excluding outliers (six participants with an MMAA score of ≤9 at visit 2; three participants with an UPSA score ≤5 at visit 2). The UPSA results remained statistically significant, p = 0.01, r = −0.31, but the MMAA findings were no longer significant, p = 0.38, r = −0.12. No other group-related differences in change in functional or psychiatric outcomes were significant. Table 3 presents the parameter estimates, p-values, and effect sizes for the group × visit interaction for all HLM outcome measures.
Table 3.
Measures | PD/pRBD− vs. PD/pRBD+ | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE | t | p | r | |
Cognitive Functioning a | |||||
Global Cognition | −0.07 | 0.05 | −1.62 | 0.11 | −0.20 |
Attention/Working memory | −0.16 | 0.06 | −2.61 | 0.01 | −0.31 |
Executive Functioning | −0.02 | 0.07 | −0.35 | 0.73 | −0.04 |
Language* | 0.02 | 0.08 | 0.21 | 0.84 | 0.03 |
Learning | −0.05 | 0.06 | −0.85 | 0.40 | −0.10 |
Memory | −0.07 | 0.06 | −1.19 | 0.24 | −0.15 |
Visuospatial Functioning | −0.10 | 0.08 | −1.28 | 0.21 | −0.16 |
Functional Capacity b | |||||
MMAA | −0.05 | 0.05 | −0.89 | 0.38 | −0.12 |
UPSA (Finance subscale) | −0.03 | 0.01 | −2.69 | 0.01 | −0.31 |
IADL Scale** | −0.002 | 0.03 | −0.08 | 0.94 | −0.01 |
Symptom severity | |||||
Geriatric Depression Scalec | −0.03 | 0.04 | −0.78 | 0.44 | −0.10 |
Trait Anxiety (STAI-T)c | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.42 | 0.68 | 0.05 |
Apathy Scale | −0.004 | 0.04 | −0.01 | 0.92 | −0.01 |
Note. IADL= Instrumental Activities of Daily Living; MMAA = Medication Management Abilities Assessment; PD/pRBD− = Parkinson’s Disease, no REM Behavior Disorder; PD/pRBD+ = Parkinson’s Disease, with REM Behavior Disorder; STAI = State Trait Anxiety Inventory; UPSA = UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment.
Controlling for education and gender
Controlling for age at baseline
Controlling for gender
*also controlling for age at baseline
**also controlling for baseline depression severity
Discussion
This study evaluated the longitudinal impact of PD/pRBD status on cognitive and functional outcomes, accounting for psychiatric symptomatology. Of the 65 participants, 25 (38.5%) met criteria for pRBD at baseline, which is consistent with prior prevalence estimates (Gagnon et al., 2002; Rolinski et al., 2014; Sixel-Döring et al., 2011). Partially consistent with a priori hypotheses, the PD/pRBD+ group declined to a greater degree in attention/working memory than did the PD/pRBD− group, with a medium effect size (r = −0.31). That is, while the PD/RBD− group mean T score remained in the average range from time 1 (M = 46.77 [7.26]) to time 2 (M = 45.13 [9.63]), the PD/RBD+ group mean T score declined from the average range at time 1 (M = 45.87 [7.58]) to the low average range at time 2 (M = 40.39 [9.45]). Additionally, the PD/pRBD+ group also declined to a greater degree on a test of financial skills, with a medium effect size (r = −0.31). There were no other significant group differences in change in cognitive or functional outcomes from baseline to 16–47-month follow-up.
Our primary finding that the baseline PD/pRBD+ group declined more across time in attention/working memory than the PD/pRBD− group extends cross-sectional literature suggesting that PD/pRBD+ patients perform worse on measures of attention compared to PD/pRBD− patients (Jozwiak et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2016). Our finding also partially aligns with evidence of more pronounced deterioration in attention/processing speed in a longitudinal investigation showing RBD-associated neuropsychological decline in PD (Chahine et al., 2016). Importantly, although we measured attention using different tests than Chahine et al. (2016), the sensitivity of this domain to PD/RBD+ in both studies suggests that it may be uniquely vulnerable to the additive impact of the RBD syndrome in PD. Consistent with this notion, broader literature suggests that attentional weaknesses are an early and prominent cognitive feature in patients with parkinsonian disorders (e.g., dementia with Lewy bodies) who have RBD and later develop MCI or dementia (Genier Marchand et al., 2018), and some evidence suggests that idiopathic RBD is associated with attentional deficits as well (Youn et al., 2016).
In addition to a more pronounced cognitive decline, the PD/pRBD+ group also declined to a greater degree than the PD/pRBD− group in an IADL specific to financial management skills (measured by the UPSA). Cognitive abilities are associated with IADLs (including UPSA performance) in a variety of clinical populations (e.g., Moore et al., 2017; Spitz, Ponsford, Rudzki, & Maller, 2012; Twamley et al., 2002), including PD (Holden et al., 2018; Pirogovsky et al., 2013, 2014). Moreover, attention and working memory have specifically been shown to share variance with performance-based functional capacity in older adults with varying degrees of cognitive impairment (Garcia-Alvarez, Gomar, Sousa, Garcia-Portilla, & Goldberg, 2019), suggesting that the RBD-associated decline in attention/working memory in the current study may have at least partially explained the decline in UPSA financial skills.
Aside from the attention/working memory domain, our hypotheses involving RBD-associated decline in neurocognition were largely unsupported, although results were generally in the predicted direction (i.e., greater decline in PD/pRBD+ compared to PD/pRBD−). In contrast, Chahine et al. (2016) demonstrated longitudinal decline in PD/pRBD+ compared to PD/pRBD− in general cognition and delayed recall. The authors did not measure visual memory or executive functioning, making the current study the only longitudinal investigation to our knowledge to assess the impact of these domains in PD/pRBD+ and PD/pRBD− groups. Overall, discrepancies between our focal cognitive results and other work suggesting widespread RBD-associated changes (e.g., Folle et al., 2019; Jozwiak et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2016), may result from a confluence of factors, including our smaller sample size (compared to Chahine et al., 2016), and differences in sample characteristics, follow-up windows, RBD assessment, and disease duration.
Several important scientific and clinical implications follow from the current study findings. From a research perspective, when measuring cognition in longitudinal studies of PD, it is important to assess and account for RBD. Clinically, if RBD is present in evaluations of PD patients, neuropsychologists may expect a greater decline across time in attention/working memory and financial skills than they would if the patient did not have RBD. In the presence of PD and RBD, measurement of attention and working memory could be made a higher priority during test selection and the clinical interview could include detailed questions regarding finances, with the goal of identifying early signs of errors and loss of independence. Moreover, during feedback sessions, knowledge of possible future decline in attention/working memory and financial skills could inform psychoeducation and recommendations. For example, if clinically indicated, neuropsychologists might suggest that patients receive extra assistance with finances, and compensatory cognitive strategies could specifically target the domain of attention and working memory.
The current study has several limitations, and results should be interpreted in light of these factors. First, although multiple follow-up evaluations better elucidate temporal patterns of change, the current study was limited to baseline and a single follow-up visit. Second, our sample was relatively small and was comprised of predominantly white, highly educated participants, thereby reducing statistical power and generalizability. Although the gender distribution of our PD groups was imbalanced (approximately two-thirds men), this is consistent with previous demographic prevalence estimates in PD (Kim & Jeon, 2014; Van Den Eeden et al., 2003). Regarding ethnic/racial and socioeconomic diversity, recruitment took place primarily in the region surrounding the University of San Diego and VA San Diego, although efforts were made to recruit PD patients from the larger San Diego county area as well. To encourage PD patients of lower socioeconomic status to participate, all patients were offered free transportation to and from appointments. Still, the sample was relatively homogeneous in terms of race/ethnicity and education, and we have since widened our catchment area so as to recruit more diverse samples for our research program. Third, we excluded patients with comorbid neurodegenerative illnesses, dementia, and untreated major depressive disorder or psychotic symptoms, which further limits external validity. Finally, we assessed RBD with a self-report measure (the RBDSQ), as opposed to objective polysomnography, and there are limitations to assessment of RBD via self-report (Stiasny-Kolster et al., 2015). Consequently, our interpretations are confined to probable RBD rather than definitive RBD.
The current study also has several strengths. First, in contrast to past cross-sectional work in PD/RBD and cognition, the current study’s longitudinal design allowed us to assess the predictive validity of baseline pRBD symptoms on future cognitive decline. Specifically, as mentioned earlier, we only measured two time points, which limits our ability to draw conclusions about the slope of decline across many years; however, we can suggest that baseline pRBD confers a greater risk for decline in attention/working memory and financial skills in PD across a 1–4 year time interval. Second, we administered a comprehensive battery of cognitive, psychiatric, and functional tests, thereby allowing for a thorough evaluation of relevant covariates, potential confounds, and outcomes.
In summary, we found evidence that PD patients with pRBD declined to a greater degree over time in attention/working memory and UPSA financial skills than those with PD without pRBD. There were no other significant RBD-associated differences in longitudinal change rates. Our findings underscore the importance of regular RBD screening in older adults at risk for PD, in order to triage symptomatic patients to appropriate support and care. Specifically, identifying patients who are at greater risk for domain-specific cognitive and IADL change may allow for targeted behavioral and/or medical interventions. This may, in turn, improve cognitive functioning, stave off decline, and/or reduce the functional consequences of such decline in this patient population.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank all the study participants for their contributions to this work.
Contributor Information
Ryan Van Patten, Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA; Spaulding Rehabilitation Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA.
Zanjbeel Mahmood, Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA; SDSU/UC San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, CA, USA.
Delaney Pickell, Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA.
Jacqueline E Maye, Research Service, VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA; Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health, VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA.
Scott Roesch, SDSU/UC San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, CA, USA.
Elizabeth W Twamley, Research Service, VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA; Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA.
J Vincent Filoteo, Research Service, VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA; Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA; Department of Neuroscience, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA.
Dawn M Schiehser, Research Service, VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA; Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA.
Funding
This work was supported by VA CSR&D [Grant #: I01 CX000813 to J.V.F.] and RR&D [Grant #: I01 RX001691 to D.M.S.] Merit Awards. Z.M. and R.V.P. were supported by the National Institute of Mental Health Institutional Training Grant in Geriatric Mental Health [Grant #: T32 MH 19934 to D.V.J. and E.W.T.].
Conflict of Interest
None declared.
References
- Aarsland, D., Marsh, L., & Schrag, A. (2009). Neuropsychiatric symptoms in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders, 24(15), 2175–2186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aarsland, D. (2016). Cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 22, S144–S148. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.09.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aarsland, D., Creese, B., Politis, M., Chaudhuri, K. R., Ffytche, D. H., Weintraub, D., et al. (2017). Cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease. Nature Reviews Neurology, 13(4), 217–231. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2017.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes, L. L. B., Harp, D., & Jung, W. S. (2002). Reliability generalization of scores on the Spielberger state-trait anxiety inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62(4), 603–618. doi: 10.1177/0013164402062004005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistics Software, 67(1), 1–48. [Google Scholar]
- Berg, D., Postuma, R. B., Bloem, B., Chan, P., Dubois, B., Gasser, T., et al. (2014). Time to redefine PD? Introductory statement of the MDS task force on the definition of Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 29(4), 454–462. doi: 10.1002/mds.25844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boeve, B. F. (2010). REM sleep behavior disorder updated review of the core features, the REM sleep behavior disorder-neurodegenerative disease association, evolving concepts, controversies, and future directions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1184, 17–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chahine, L. M., Xie, S. X., Simuni, T., Tran, B., Postuma, R., Amara, A., et al. (2016). Longitudinal changes in cognition in early Parkinson’s disease patients with REM sleep behavior disorder. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 27, 102–106. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaudhuri, K. R., Healy, D. G., & Schapira, A. H. (2006). Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson's disease: Diagnosis and management. The Lancet Neurology, 5(3), 235–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J. (1992). Quantitative methods in psychology: A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 1155–1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De La Riva, P., Smith, K., Xie, S. X., & Weintraub, D. (2014). Course of psychiatric symptoms and global cognition in early Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 83, 1096–1103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delis D. C., Kramer J. H., Kaplan E., & Ober B. A. (2000). CVLT-II: California Verbal Learning Test: Adult Version. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., Kramer, J.H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (DKEFS). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- den Brok, M. G., van Dalen, J. W., van Gool, W. A., Moll van Charante, E. P., de Bie, R. M., & Richard, E. (2015). Apathy in Parkinson's disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Movement Disorders, 30(6), 759–769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dirnberger, G., & Jahanshahi, M. (2013). Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: A review. Journal of Neuropsychology, 7, 193–224. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dissanayaka, N. N. W., Lawson, R. A., Yarnall, A. J., Duncan, G. W., Breen, D. P., Khoo, T. K., et al. (2017). Anxiety is associated with cognitive impairment in newly- diagnosed Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 36, 63–68. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domellof, M. E., Ekman, U., Forsfren, L., & Elgh, E. (2015). Cognitive function in the early phase of Parkinson’s disease, a five year follow up. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 132, 79–88. doi: 10.1111/ane.12375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fereshtehnejad, S. M., Romenents, S. R., Anang, J. B. M., Latreille, V., Gagnon, J. F., & Postuma, R. B. (2015). New clinical subtypes of Parkinson’s disease and their longitudinal progression a prospective cohort comparison with other phenotypes. JAMA Neurology, 72(8), 863–873. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.0703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferini-Strambi, L., Di Gioia, M. R., Castronovo, V., Oldani, A., Zucconi, M., & Cappa, S. F. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD): Does the idiopathic form of RBD really exist? Neurology, 62, 41–45. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000101726.69701.FA. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Figorilli, M., Marques, A. R., Vidal, T., Delaby, L., Meloni, M., Pereira, B., et al. (2020). Does REM sleep behavior disorder change in the progression of Parkinson’s disease? Sleep Medicine, 68, 190–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folle, A. D., Paul, K. C., Bronstein, J. M., Keener, A. M., & Ritz, B. (2019). Clinical progression in Parkinson’s disease with features of REM sleep behavior disorder: A population based longitudinal study. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 62, 105–111. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2019.01.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gagnon, J. F., Bédard, M. A., Fantini, M. L., Petit, D., Panisset, M., Rompre, S., et al. (2002). REM sleep behavior disorder and REM sleep without atonia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 59(4), 585–589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gagnon, J. F., Postuma, R. B., Mazza, S., Doyon, J., & Montplaisir, J. (2006). Rapid-eye-movement sleep behaviour disorder and neurodegenerative diseases. The Lancet Neurology, 5(5), 424–432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Alvarez, L., Gomar, J. J., Sousa, A., Garcia-Portilla, M. P., & Goldberg, T. E. (2019). Breadth and depth of working memory and executive function compromises in mild cognitive impairment and their relationships to frontal lobe morphometry and functional competence. Alzheimer's & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, 11, 170–179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Genier Marchand, D., Postuma, R. B., Escudier, F., De Roy, J., Pelletier, A., Montplaisir, J., et al. (2018). How does dementia with Lewy bodies start? Prodromal cognitive changes in REM sleep behavior disorder. Annals of Neurology, 83(5), 1016–1026. doi: 10.1002/ana.25239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz, C. G., Poewe, W., Rascol, O., Sampaio, C., Stebbins, G. T., Counsell, C., et al. (2004). Movement disorders society task force report on the Hoehn and Yahr staging scale: Status and recommendations. Movement Disorders, 19(9), 1020–1028. doi: 10.1002/mds.20213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz, C. G., Tilley, B. C., Shaftman, S. R., Stebbins, G. T., Fahn, S., Martinez-Martin, P., et al. (2008). Movement disorders society- sponsored revision of the unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale (MDS-UPDRS): Scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Movement Disorders, 23(15), 2129–2170. doi: 10.1002/mds.22340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman, J. G., Vernaleo, B. A., Camicioli, R., Dahodwala, N., Dobkin, R. D., Ellis, T., et al. (2018). Cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: A report from a multidisciplinary symposium on unmet needs and future directions to maintain cognitive health. Nature Partner Journals, 4(19), 1–19. doi: 10.1038/s41531-018-0055-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanks, R. A., Rapport, L. J., Millis, S. R., & Deshpande, S. A. (1999). Measures of executive functioning as predictors of functional ability and social integration in a rehabilitation sample. Archives of Physical Medicine Rehabilitation, 80(9), 1030–1037. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(99)90056-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holden, S. K., Medina, L. D., Hoyt, B., Sillau, S. H., Berman, B. D., Goldman, J. G., et al. (2018). Validation of a performance-based assessment of cognitive functional ability in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders, 33(11), 1760–1768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes, A. J., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Daniel, S. E., & Lees, A. J. (1992). What features improve the accuracy of clinical diagnosis in Parkinson’s disease: A clinicopathologic study. Neurology, 57, s34–s38. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)00049-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jefferson, A. L., Paul, R. H., Ozonoff, A., & Cohen, R. A. (2006). Evaluating elements of executive functioning as predictors of instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 311–320. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jozwiak, N., Postuma, R. B., Montplaisir, J., Latreille, V., Panisset, M., Chouinard, S., et al. (2017). REM sleep behavior disorder and cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Sleep, 40, 1–10. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kehagia, A. A., Barker, R. A., & Robbins, T. W. (2010). Neuropsychological and clinical heterogeneity of cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with Parkinson’s disease. The Lancet Neurology, 9(12), 1200–1213. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70212-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim, Y. E., & Jeon, B. S. (2014). Clinical implication of REM sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Parkinson's disease, 4(2), 237–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kudlicka, A., Clare, L., & Hindle, J. V. (2011). Executive functions in Parkinson’s disease: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Movement Disorders, 26(13), 2305–2315. doi: 10.1002/mds.23868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist, 9(3), 179–186. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leentjens, A. F. G., Dujardin, K., Marsh, L., Martinez-Martin, P., Richard, I. H., Starkstein, S. E., et al. (2008a). Anxiety rating scales in Parkinson’s disease: Critique and recommendations. Movement Disorders, 23(14), 2015–2025. doi: 10.1002/mds.22233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leentjens, A. F. G., Dujardin, K., Marsh, L., Martinez-Martin, P., Richard, I. H., Starkstein, S. E., et al. (2008b). Apathy and anhedonia rating scales in Parkinson’s disease: Critique and recommendations. Movement Disorders, 23(14), 2004–2014. doi: 10.1002/mds.22229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin, Y., & Chen, S. (2018). RBD: A red flag for cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease? Sleep Medicine, 44, 38–44. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litvan, I., Goldman, J. G., Tröster, A. I., Schmand, B. A., Weintraub, D., Petersen, R. C., et al. (2012). Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: Movement Disorder Society task force guidelines. Movement Disorders, 27(3), 349–356. doi: 10.1002/mds.24893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llebaria, G., Pagonabarraga, J., Kulisevsky, J., García-Sánchez, C., Pascual-Sedano, B., Gironell, A., et al. (2008). Cut-off score of the Mattis dementia rating scale for screening dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 23(11), 1546–1550. doi: 10.1002/mds.22173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahmood, Z., Van Patten, R., Nakhla, M. Z., Twamley, E. W., Filoteo, J. V., & Schiehser, D. F. (2020). REM sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson’s disease: Effects on cognitive, psychiatric, and functional outcomes. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 26(9), 894–905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mao, J., Huang, X., Yu, J., Chen, L., Huang, Y., Tang, B., et al. (2020). Association between REM sleep behavior disorder and cognitive dysfunctions in Parkinson's disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Frontiers in Neurology, 11, 1366–1389. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.577874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marques, A., Dujardin, K., Boucart, M., Pins, D., Delliaux, M., Defebvre, L., et al. (2010). REM sleep behavior disorder and visuoperceptive dysfunction: A disorder of the ventral visual visual stream? Journal of Neurology, 257, 383–391. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-5328-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marras, C., & Chaudhuri, K. R. (2016). Nonmotor features of Parkinson’s disease subtypes. Movement Disorders, 31(8), 1095–1102. doi: 10.1002/mds.26510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, L. S., & Rohling, M. L. (2001). A statistical interpretive method for neuropsychological test data. Neuropsychology Review, 11, 143–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore, R. C., Paolillo, E. W., Heaton, A., Fazeli, P. L., Jeste, D. V., & Moore, D. J. (2017). Clinical utility of the UCSD performance-based skills assessment—Brief (UPSA-B) in adults living with HIV: Associations with neuropsychological impairment and patient-reported everyday functioning difficulties. PloS one, 12(8), e0183614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nomura, T., Inoue, Y., Kagimura, T., Uemura, Y., & Nakashima, K. (2011). Utility of the REM sleep behavior disorder screening questionnaire in Parkinson’s disease patients. Sleep Medicine, 12, 711–713. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2011.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pagano, G., Micco, R. D., Tayyabah, Y., Wilson, H., Chandra, A., & Politis, M. (2018). REM behavior disorder predicts motor progression and cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 91(10), e894–e905. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000006134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson, T. L., Goldman, S., McKibbin, C. L., Hughs, T., & Jeste, D. V. (2001). UCSD performance-based skills assessment: Development of a new measure of everyday functioning for severely mentally ill adults. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27(2), 235–245. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson, T. L., Lacro, J., McKibbin, C. L., Moscona, S., Hughs, T., & Jeste, D. V. (2002). Medication management ability assessment: Results from a performance-based measure in older outpatients with schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 22(1), 11–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen, K. F., Larsen, J. P., Tsynes, O., & Alves, G. (2017). Natural course of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 88, 1–8. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003634 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pirogovsky, E., Martinez-Hannon, M., Schiehser, D. M., Lessig, S. L., Song, D. D., Litvan, I., et al. (2013). Predictors of performance-based measures of instrumental activities of daily living in nondemented patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35(9), 926–933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pirogovsky, E., Schiehser, D. M., Obtera, K. M., Burke, M. M., Lessig, S. L., Song, D. D., et al. (2014). Instrumental activities of daily living are impaired in Parkinson’s disease patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 28(2), 229–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pirogovsky-Turk, E., Moore, R. C., Filoteo, J. V., Litvan, I., Song, D. D., Lessig, S. L., et al. (2017). Neuropsychiatric predictors of cognitive decline in parkinson disease: A longitudinal study. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25(3), 279–289. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poewe, W. (2008). Non-motor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease. European Journal of Neurology, 15(s1), 14–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2008.02056.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Postuma, R. B., Lang, A. E., Gagnon, J. F., Pelletier, A., & Montplaisir, J. Y. (2012). How does parkinsonism start? Prodromal parkinsonism motor changes in idiopathic REM sleep behaviour disorder. Brain, 135(6), 1860–1870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Postuma, R. B., Berg, D., Stern, M., Poewe, W., Olanow, C. W., Oertel, W., et al. (2015). MDS clinical diagnosis criteria for Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 30(12), 1591–1599. doi: 10.1002/mds.26424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
- Raab, G. M., Day, S., & Sales, J. (2000). How to select covariates to include in the analysis of a clinical trial. Controlled Clinical Trials, 21(4), 330–342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Razani, J., Casas, R., Wong, J. T., Lu, P., Alessi, C., & Josephson, K. (2007). Relationship between executive function and activities of daily living in patients with relatively mild dementia. Applied Neuropsychology, 14(3), 208–214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson, M. D., Watkins, E. R., & Harmon-Jones, E. (Eds.) (2013). Handbook of cognition and emotion. New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Rolinski, M., Szewczyk-Krolikowski, K., Tomlinson, P. R., Nithi, K., Talbot, K., Ben-Shlomo, Y., et al. (2014). REM sleep behavior disorder is associated with worse quality of life and other non-motor features in early Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 85, 560–566. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-306104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7(2), 147–177. doi: 10.1037//1082-989X.7.2.147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schapira, A. H. V., Chaudhuri, K. R., & Jenner, P. (2017). Non-motor features of Parkinson’s disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18, 435–450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt, R., Freidl, W., Fazekas, F., Reinhart, B., Grieshofer, P., Koch, M., et al. (1994). The Mattis dementia rating scale: Normative data from 1,001 healthy volunteers. Neurology, 44(5), 964–964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer, J. D., Willett, J. B., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sixel-Döring, F., Trautmann, E., Mollenhauer, B., & Trenkwalder, C. (2011). Associated factors for REM sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson disease. Neurology, 77(11), 1048–1054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. [Google Scholar]
- Spitz, G., Ponsford, J. L., Rudzki, D., & Maller, J. J. (2012). Association between cognitive performance and functional outcome following traumatic brain injury: A longitudinal multilevel examination. Neuropsychology, 26(5), 604–612. doi: 10.1037/a0029239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starkstein, S. E., Mayberg, H. S., Preziosi, T., Andrezejewski, P., Leiguarda, R., & Robinson, R. G. (1992). Reliability, validity, and clinical correlates of apathy in Parkinson’s disease. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 4(2), 134–139. doi: 10.1176/jnp.4.2.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stiasny-Kolster, K., Mayer, G., Schafer, S., Moller, J. C., Heinzel-Gutenbrunner, M., & Oertel, W. (2007). The REM sleep behavior disorder screening questionnaire: A new diagnostic instrument. Movement Disorders, 22(16), 2386–2393. doi: 10.1002/mds.21740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stiasny-Kolster, K., Sixel-Döring, F., Trenkwalder, C., Heinzel-Gutenbrunner, M., Seppi, K., Poewe, W., et al. (2015). Diagnostic value of the REM sleep behavior disorder screening questionnaire in Parkinson's disease. Sleep Medicine, 16(1), 186–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sumida, C. A., Lopez, F. V., Van Ettenc, E. J., Whiteley, N., Moore, R. C., Litvan, I., et al. (2021). Medication management performance in Parkinson’s disease: Examination of process errors. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acab004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sveinbjornsdottir, S. (2016). The clinical symptoms of Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurochemistry, 139(s1), 318–324. doi: 10.1111/jnc.13691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trout, J., Christiansen, T., Bulkley, M. B., Tanner, J. J., Sozda, C. N., Bowers, D., et al. (2020). Cognitive impairments and self-reported sleep in early-stage Parkinson’s disease without probable REM sleep behavior disorder. Brain Sciences, 10(9), 1–11. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10010009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner J. (n.d.) Parkinson’s Measurement Graph Levodopa Equivalent Plasma Levels. https://www.parkinsonsmeasurement.org/toolBox/pharmacokinetics.htm
- Twamley, E. W., Doshi, R. R., Nayak, G. V., Palmer, B. W., Golshan, S., Heaton, R. K., et al. (2002). Generalized cognitive impairments, ability to perform everyday tasks, and level of independence in community living situations of older patients with psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(12), 2013–2020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Den Eeden, S. K., Tanner, C. M., Bernstein, A. L., Fross, R. D., Leimpeter, A., Bloch, D. A., et al. (2003). Incidence of Parkinson’s disease: Variation by age, gender, and race/ethnicity. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157(11), 1015–1022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vendette, M., Gagnon, J. F., Decary, A., Massicotte-Marquez, K., Postuma, R. B., Doyon, J., et al. (2007). REM sleep behavior disorder predicts cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease without dementia. Neurology, 69(19), 1843–1849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker, M. P. (2009). The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156(1), 168–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, Y., Wang, Z. W., Yang, Y. C., Wu, H., Zhao, H., & Zhao, Z. (2015). Validation of the rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder screening questionnaire in China. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 22, 1420–1424. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2015.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. (1997). WMS-III: Wechsler Memory Scale Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Xie, D., Shen, Q., Zhou, J., & Xu, Y. (2021). Non-motor symptoms are associated with REM sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson’s disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurological Sciences, 42, 47–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al. (1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17(1), 37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Youn, S., Kim, T., Yoon, I., Jeong, J., Kim, H. Y., Han, J. W., et al. (2016). Progression of cognitive impairments in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 87(8), 890–896. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2015-311437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zgaljardic, D. J., Borod, J. C., Foldi, N. S., Mattis, P. J., Gordon, M. F., Feigin, A., et al. (2006). An examination of executive dysfunction associated with frontostriatal circuitry in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(7), 1127–1144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, J. R., Chen, J., Yang, Z. J., Zhang, H. J., Fu, Y. F., Shen, Y., et al. (2016). Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder symptoms correlate with domains of cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Chinese Medical Journal, 129(4), 379–385. doi: 10.4103/0366-6999.176077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]