Abstract
Determining the meanings of words requires language learners to attend to what other people say. However, it behooves a young language learner to simultaneously attend to what other people attend to, for example, by following the direction of their eye gaze. Sensitivity to cues such as eye gaze might be particularly important for bilingual infants, as they encounter less consistency between words and objects than monolinguals, and do not always have access to the same word learning heuristics (e.g., mutual exclusivity). In a pre-registered study, we tested the hypothesis that bilingual experience would lead to a more pronounced ability to follow another’s gaze. We used the gaze-following paradigm developed by Senju and Csibra (2008) to test a total of 93 6–9 month-old and 229 12–15 month-old monolingual and bilingual infants, in 11 labs located in 8 countries. Monolingual and bilingual infants showed similar gaze-following abilities, and both groups showed age-related improvements in speed, accuracy, frequency and duration of fixations to congruent objects. Unexpectedly, bilinguals tended to make more frequent fixations to onscreen objects, whether or not they were cued by the actor. These results suggest that gaze sensitivity is a fundamental aspect of development that is robust to variation in language exposure.
Keywords: gaze following, bilingualism, infancy, replication, reproducibility, age-related changes
Bilingual infants face the remarkable task of acquiring two languages simultaneously. Bilinguals show developmental adaptations to their unique environments, which might support their observed success in learning their two languages (Werker & Byers-Heinlein, 2008). In comparison to monolinguals, bilingual infants show differences in early speech perception (Byers-Heinlein & Fennell, 2014), in word learning (Fennell, Byers-Heinlein, & Werker, 2007; Graf Estes & Hay, 2015; Singh, Fu, Tay, & Golinkoff, 2018), and in acquisition of grammatical structures (Antovich & Graf Estes, 2018; Kovács & Mehler, 2009b). They show different patterns of looking towards talking faces (Pons, Bosch, & Lewkowicz, 2015), and are more sensitive to facial cues that discriminate speakers of different languages (Sebastián-Gallés, Albareda-Castellot, Weikum, & Werker, 2012; Weikum et al., 2007). These differences have been attributed to specific features of bilingual environments that may influence developing cognitive processes. Specifically, the notion that bilingual infants attend to and learn two languages is thought to sharpen their capacity to flexibly switch between their languages (Antovich & Graf Estes, 2018; Kandhadai, Danielson, & Werker, 2014; Kovács & Mehler, 2009a) and to acquire the individual properties of two language systems (Kovács & Mehler, 2009b). Moreover, as bilingual infants typically encounter less single-language input than their monolingual peers, new information may be encoded with increased efficiency and detail (Brito & Barr, 2014; Liu & Kager, 2016; Singh et al., 2015). These findings suggest that, before infants begin to produce words in their native language(s), immersion in a bilingual environment modifies the development of some aspects of infants’ perception and learning.
More intriguingly, bilingualism also appears to impact abilities that do not directly involve language. For example, relative to monolinguals, bilingual infants are more likely to inhibit recently learned information (Kovács & Mehler, 2009a), generalize across visual features when categorizing objects (Brito & Barr, 2014), and encode and retrieve visual information (Singh et al., 2015). Here, we ask whether bilingual infants also show enhanced sensitivity to non-linguistic social information, a question that has thus far received very little attention. In an international, multi-site study, we investigated whether the ability to follow a social partner’s eye gaze follows the same developmental trajectory in monolingual and bilingual infants, and found overall no major differences in infants’ eye gaze following as a function of language background.
The development of gaze following
Infants show an early-emerging sensitivity to a social partner’s eye gaze. In a primitive form, very young infants are sensitive to the direction of a speaker’s gaze, attending to visual targets more rapidly when they are cued by an adult’s gaze (Farroni, Massaccesi, Pividori, & Johnson, 2004; Reid, Striano, Kaufman, & Johnson, 2004). Throughout the first year and a half of life, infants refine their interpretation of eye and head movements: they distinguish between head-turns with open versus closed eyes (Brooks & Meltzoff, 2005), become able to follow changes in gaze unaccompanied by a head turn (Corkum & Moore, 1995; Moore & Corkum, 1998), and attend to whether another’s gaze is obscured from view by a physical barrier (Meltzoff & Brooks, 2007). In sum, over the course of infancy, infants progress from attending to the direction of the eyes, to engaging in gaze following in a more selective fashion, to true gaze following where the actions of a social partner are interpreted as intentional and informative (Brooks & Meltzoff, 2014; Frischen, Bayliss, & Tipper, 2007; Moore, 2008).
A number of recent studies have highlighted the situations that most reliably elicit gaze following in infancy. As an example, Senju and Csibra (2008) investigated gaze-following abilities of 6-month-old infants. This age is of particular interest as it corresponds to the onset of word comprehension (Bergelson & Swingley, 2012; Fenson et al., 2007). An adult model sat in between two toys, one located to her left and one to her right. Infants were tested in one of two conditions. In the Eye Contact condition by Senju and Csibra (2008) the model looked into the camera, thus potentially making eye contact with the infant, and then directed her gaze at one of the toys. In the No Eye Contact condition, the model initially looked down instead of towards the infant, and a superimposed animation drew the infant’s attention to her head. Results revealed that infants followed the model’s gaze in the Eye Contact condition, but not in the No Eye Contact condition. In a replication and extension of Senju and Csibra (2008)’s paradigm, Szufnarowska, Rohlfing, Fawcett, and Gredebäck (2014) demonstrated that 6-month-old infants responsively followed an adult’s gaze similarly when it was preceded by attention-grabbing behaviors without eye contact, such as shivering or nodding. This suggests that the ability for eye gaze to elicit gaze following behavior may be partially related to its attentional draw.
Several studies have used the paradigm developed by Senju and Csibra (2008) to explore how infants’ individual experiences with gaze affect their gaze-following abilities. For example, one study investigated gaze following in sighted infants of blind parents (Senju et al., 2013). These infants showed a similar ability to follow the gaze of a sighted social partner as infants of sighted parents, despite having less experience with gaze behaviors. Another study looked at gaze following in infants at risk for communicative impairments (Bedford et al., 2012). Although both at-risk and low-risk infants were equally likely to follow an adult’s gaze, at-risk 13-month-olds spent less total time looking at objects to which an adult’s gaze was directed. This suggested that they might have been less able to make use of gaze as a socially relevant cue than typically developing infants. Together, these studies suggest that gaze following is an ability that develops across varied developmental circumstances, although the results from at-risk infants show that the use of gaze information can differ across populations. Importantly, these studies provide support for the use of Senju and Csibra (2008)’s task, which has elicited gaze following across studies and populations.
Gaze following in bilinguals
One group of infants that might differ in the development of gaze-following abilities is bilingual infants, although no study to date has specifically tested this group. There are several reasons to posit that bilinguals may demonstrate increased attention to gaze patterns of social partners. One reason is that gaze following is not only an important social skill, but it also contributes to early language learning. Language is a highly social system of communication. Speakers often look towards their intended referent. Thus the ability to follow a conversational partner’s gaze can guide children in correctly mapping words to objects, and help to resolve the problem of referential ambiguity (Baldwin, 1995; Brooks & Meltzoff, 2002; Tomasello, 2003; Woodward, 2003). Many theories of language acquisition emphasize the influence of social cues in the search for meaning, proposing that infants’ sensitivity to social cues scaffolds accurate and efficient vocabulary development (Baldwin, 1995; Bloom, 2000; Hollich et al., 2000; Mundy, Sullivan, & Mastergeorge, 2009; Tomasello, 2003). There is substantial empirical support for this theoretical stance: infant gaze following is both concurrently and predictively related to word learning (e.g., Brooks & Meltzoff, 2005, 2008; Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998; Morales et al., 2000; Mundy et al., 2007; Paulus & Fikkert, 2014; Tenenbaum, Sobel, Sheinkopf, Malle, & Morgan, 2015).
The ability to use gaze information in language learning might be particularly important for bilingual infants. Bilingual infants’ experiences are divided between their two languages, and they must learn two labels for each object (one in each language). When a monolingual English-learning infant encounters an object such as an apple, they will consistently hear the word “apple” to refer to that object. However, when a French-English bilingual encounters the same object, they will sometimes hear the English word “apple” and sometimes hear the French word “pomme”. For bilinguals, there may be less consistency in object-label correspondences. Unlike monolinguals, they eventually have to map at least two labels to each object (one in each language).
The need to map multiple labels onto the same object may make some of the word learning strategies used by monolingual learners less useful for bilingual learners. Both groups should share basic assumptions about the relationship between words and objects that can support word learning, like the assumption that words refer to whole objects rather than their parts, and that a new word should be extended to other objects of the same kind (Markman, 1990). However, one key assumption that may differ across monolinguals and bilinguals is mutual exclusivity, the assumption that each object has a unique label (Markman & Wachtel, 1988). Mutual exclusivity allows monolinguals to reject objects with a known label as a referent for a novel word. Strict use of such a heuristic would be less useful to bilingual learners, as they must learn two labels for each object. Indeed, evidence from bilingual infants at age 9 months (Byers-Heinlein, 2017) and 17–18 months (Byers-Heinlein & Werker, 2009, 2013; Houston-Price, Caloghiris, & Raviglione, 2010) indicates that bilinguals do not assume that each object has only one label, and are less likely to use word learning heuristics such as mutual exclusivity. If mutual exclusivity is less available to bilingual word learners, then they might need to more strongly rely on other cues to word meaning such as eye gaze.
Another important monolingual-bilingual difference is that bilingual learners receive less input in each language in comparison to monolingual learners. While this might be expected to delay word learning, bilingual infants comprehend and produce their first words on largely the same schedule as monolingual infants (De Houwer, Bornstein, & De Coster, 2006; Petitto et al., 2001). Moreover, when vocabulary in both languages is considered, monolinguals and bilinguals have similar vocabulary sizes (Core, Hoff, Rumiche, & Señor, 2013; Pearson, Fernández, & Oller, 1993). Thus, bilinguals appear to have a similar rate of vocabulary development despite reduced frequency of exposure to particular words (although see Bilson, Yoshida, Tran, Woods, & Hills, 2015, for a different perspective). This could imply that bilinguals are adept at leveraging other sources of information for word learning, such as eye gaze, which could offset the effects of reduced single-language input.
There is some evidence from older children to support the hypothesis that bilingual infants have an enhanced ability to follow a social partner’s gaze. For example, when object cues and eye gaze cues to meaning were pitted against one another, 2- to 3-year-old bilinguals weighed eye gaze cues more heavily than monolinguals to identify the referent of a newly learned word (Brojde, Ahmed, & Colunga, 2012). In a similar study, Yow and Markman (2011) demonstrated that 2- to 4-year-old bilingual children made greater use of eye gaze than monolingual peers to locate a hidden object. This effect was observed only under challenging circumstances in which the experimenter was seated at a distance from the referent and closer to a competing distractor object. In a study investigating children’s use of eye gaze to map novel words to referents and additionally, to infer the meanings of other words via mutual exclusivity, Yow et al. (2017) found that 4- to 5-year-old bilingual children made greater use of eye gaze to identify word-meaning links that were directly taught as well as those that were identifiable via mutual exclusivity.
Together, these studies provide evidence that preschool-aged bilingual children are more adept than monolinguals at using eye gaze cues in word learning contexts. This raises the possibility that bilinguals might also show enhanced sensitivity to a social partner’s eye gaze even earlier in development than monolinguals.
A multi-lab collaborative study
We conducted a multi-lab collaborative study to investigate whether infants’ language background can influence the development of gaze following. Multi-lab collaborative studies, which involve data collection across multiple sites to generate a large-scale data set, offer several promises for infant research. This approach allows us to increase the diversity and the size of the sample than can be collected in a single laboratory, protecting against incorrect conclusions due to sampling error. Moreover, comparisons across labs can speak to the generalizability of results. For example, such an approach could clarify whether any observed monolingual-bilingual differences generalize across different samples and could reveal whether any observed effects are likely due to bilingualism per se or could be attributed to other sample characteristics, for example, the specific language or cultural context. Within infant bilingualism research, very few studies have collected data from multiple groups of monolingual and bilingual infants on the same task, and cross-cultural comparisons on infant bilingual development are entirely absent.
There are many methodological challenges faced in conducting research with bilinguals, particularly in infancy, that motivate using a multi-lab collaborative approach. Many of these challenges are inherent to the nature of the population, and make it difficult to know whether and how findings from one population of bilinguals generalize to other populations. First, while the term “bilingual” can be used for any infant who is exposed to two or more languages, bilingual infants vary in the particular language pair they are learning. Some studies have included only groups of homogeneous bilinguals (i.e., infants exposed to the same pair of languages, such as Spanish-Catalan), while others have included heterogeneous bilinguals (i.e., infants exposed to different pairs of languages, having one language in common, for example, English-Japanese, English-Spanish, English-French). Different language combinations could present different language-learning challenges. While our study was not designed to tease apart the role of particular language pairings (although our data do allow us to explore this issue in a preliminary way), it will establish the generalizability of findings across different groups of bilinguals.
Second, given the continuous nature of language exposure, it is challenging to validly and consistently define what makes an infant “monolingual” versus “bilingual”. Specifically, few infants are exposed to their two languages in an exactly equal proportion. Instead, the amount of exposure to each of their languages can vary enormously, and there is not always consensus about how much exposure is necessary to acquire a language. As a result, different studies have defined bilingualism differently: while in some studies 10% exposure to the non-dominant language was enough for infants to be considered bilingual, other studies required at least 40% of exposure (Byers-Heinlein, 2015), although 25% is a commonly-used cutoff. An additional complication is that the onset of exposure to any additional languages is highly variable, and could be as early as birth or anytime thereafter. Published studies differ with respect to whether strict or relaxed inclusion requirements are set for the onset of exposure to different languages. A benefit of this collaborative approach is that there is a consistent definition of exposure across participating laboratories.
Finally, bilingualism cannot be randomly assigned. Thus, even when recruited from the same geographic region, monolingual and bilingual populations often differ systematically in culture or socio-economic status. Such confounds can make it difficult to determine whether bilingualism itself, rather than another correlated variable, drives observed monolingual–bilingual differences. While such factors can be statistically controlled, these confounds can raise issues about the validity of conclusions and the replicability of the results in bilingualism research. In particular, a number of reports have suggested that long-standing beliefs about the cognitive effects of bilingualism may not be as robust as previously assumed (de Bruin, Treccani, & Della Sala, 2015; Duñabeitia & Carreiras, 2015; Paap, Johnson, & Sawi, 2015; see also Klein, 2015). Indeed, such issues are of increasing concern in the wider field of psychology, where there are ongoing concerns about the replicability of psychological research in general (see Ioannidis, 2012), and specifically about the cross-cultural replicability of basic psychological phenomena thought to be universal (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). Concerns about the replicability and generalizability of research findings are particularly acute in the field of infant research, where single-lab studies tend to have small sample sizes, high variability, and use indirect experimental measures (see Frank et al., 2017, for a detailed discussion of these issues). Multi-lab studies can go further than single-lab studies to address many of these issues. Characteristics that are idiosyncratic to a particular sample will average out to some degree in a multi-lab study that includes samples from multiple cultures and language backgrounds. Our approach of comparing gaze following in monolinguals and bilinguals growing up in different contexts, tested across multiple labs, provides important information about the replicability and generalizability of the effects we observe.
Current study
The current study used a multi-lab approach to ask whether monolingual and bilingual infants differ in their basic gaze-following abilities. Data were collected from 11 labs in 8 countries. We tested the hypothesis that the challenging nature of bilingual language-learning environments enhances bilingual infants’ attention to the eye gaze of a social partner, even in non-linguistic situations. Our study compared monolingual and bilingual infants aged 6–9 and 12–15 months using the eye gaze stimuli from Senju and Csibra (2008)’s study. Note that our study did not include the No Eye Contact condition reported in Senju and Csibra’s paper, as our interest was in comparing gaze-following behavior in typical situations, across infants from different language backgrounds. On six test trials, infants saw a model look towards the camera, and then direct her head and eyes towards one of two objects located to her left and right. We measured the latency and accuracy of infants’ gaze following.
Previous studies have found that infants follow the actor’s gaze in this condition at above-chance level by 6 months, but their performance is not always reliable (Senju & Csibra, 2008; Szufnarowska et al., 2014). Moreover, there is evidence for improvement of infants’ gaze following in this paradigm from 7 to 13 months (Bedford et al., 2012). We thus expected to see improvement in all infants’ gaze following from the younger age to the older age. We also expected that both groups would demonstrate successful gaze following as demonstrated by Senju and Csibra (2008), but that bilingual infants would show faster and more accurate gaze following than monolingual infants. We also suggest that the effects of bilingualism might interact with age. On the one hand, we might observe a stronger effect of bilingualism at 6–9 months if gaze following emerges earlier for bilinguals; on the other hand we might observe a stronger effect of bilingualism at 12–15 months if this skill emerges at the same age, but is more relied upon by bilingual infants as the demands of language acquisition increase. Both of these findings would reflect interesting and meaningful differences between monolingual and bilingual infants.
Methods
We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions (if any), all manipulations, and all measures in the study.
Participation Details
Time frame.
An open call for labs to participate was issued on March 14, 2017. Participant testing began on July 1, 2017 and ended on August 31, 2018.
Age and language groups.
Labs contributed samples from one or both of two possible age bins: 6–9 months (184–274 days) and 12–15 months (366–456 days). Labs were asked to aim for a mean age at the centre of each bin, with distribution across the entire age window. Labs could contribute a monolingual and/or bilingual sample at one or both ages (see below for inclusion criteria for monolingual and bilingual groups).
Lab participation.
Considering the challenges associated with recruiting bilingual infants and the importance of counterbalancing in our experimental design, we asked labs to contribute a minimum of 16 healthy, full-term infants per age (6–9, 12–15) and language group (monolingual, bilingual). However, labs were encouraged to contribute data even if they were only able to provide a bin of data for a single age or for a single language group. Further, labs were invited to contribute additional data provided that decisions about when to stop data collection were made without looking at the data, to avoid biasing effect sizes. Labs were asked to screen ahead of time that infants met inclusion criteria. However, it was acknowledged that most labs would end up recruiting infants who did not necessarily meet our pre-defined criteria for bilingualism (detailed below) upon more detailed in-lab language background assessment. In such cases, the decision whether to test the infant was left up to individual laboratories’ policies, but we asked that data from any babies who entered the testing room be submitted for data processing (even though some such data might be excluded from the main analyses). Eleven labs contributed at least one data bin.
Nine of the 11 participating labs were also participating in two prior multi-lab collaborative studies (ManyBabies 1 study and/or ManyBabies 1 Bilingual study) investigating infants’ preference for infant-directed speech (Byers-Heinlein, Tsui, et al., 2020; ManyBabies Consortium, 2020). The current study emerged out of the unique opportunity afforded by a significant number of labs with a bilingual population coming together to run the Manybabies 1 Bilingual study, and the desire to make optimal use of these resources. As such, prior to completing the current study, 42.88% of the infants completed the ManyBabies 1 study on the same visit in the lab. Testing infants in two different studies on the same visit is a common practice in many, although not all, infant labs. We note that these two studies adopted different designs (listening preference vs. gaze following), and tracked sensitivity to different sorts of cues (auditory vs. visual). Moreover, the current study (gaze following) presented infants with engaging social stimuli and was short in duration. These features mitigated possible carryover effects.
Power analysis.
In their paper, Senju and Csibra (2008) report a comparison against chance of t(18) = 2.74 in our target condition (the Eye Contact condition of Experiment 2), yielding a calculated effect size of Cohen’s d = 1.29 for infants of this age for the first look measure. This would necessitate a sample size of only 6 infants to have an 80% chance of detecting a significant difference in a single-sample t-test. With our planned sample size of 16 infants/group per lab, power within each lab to detect this effect will be .94.
However, our primary hypothesis concerned the comparison of monolingual and bilingual infants. Because this is the first study to investigate this question, it is difficult to know what effect size might be expected in this comparison. We thus conducted a sensitivity analysis, setting target power at .8 and alpha at .05. For individual labs to detect a statistically significant difference between monolinguals and bilinguals (n = 16 infants per group) in an independent samples t-test, we would need to observe a large effect size of Cohen’s d = 1.0. However, collapsing across the labs (projected to be approximately 100 monolinguals and bilinguals per age group for a total sample of 400), we would be able to detect a small to medium effect size of Cohen’s d = .28 at either age. Conducting multiple regression models with 3–6 predictors (see analytic plan) with the data from all labs across both age groups, we would be able to detect statistically significant contribution(s) from between one (e.g., bilingualism) and three (e.g., bilingualism, age, and their interaction) predictors with a small effect size in the range of Cohen’s ƒ2 = .019–.028. Thus, we felt confident that our design would have sufficient statistical power to detect a difference between monolinguals and bilinguals that was small to medium in magnitude.
Ethics.
The present study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki guidelines, with written informed consent obtained from a parent or guardian for each child before any assessment or data collection. All procedures involving human subjects in this study were approved by the Institutional Review Board at the institutions where data was collected. Each lab followed the ethical guidelines and ethics review board protocols of their own institutions. Labs submitted anonymized data for central analysis that identified participants by code only. Data from individual participants were coded and stored locally at each lab, and, where possible, were uploaded to a central controlled-access databank accessible to other researchers.
Participants
Classification of participants into language groups.
As in previous studies, infants were categorized as bilingual or monolingual according to parent estimates of language input to their child. Infants were classified as monolingual if they heard the community language at least 90% of the time. There is some variation across studies in how much exposure to the non-dominant language is typically required for infants to be classified as bilingual, with a range of values from 10% to 40% (Byers-Heinlein, 2015). A widely accepted criterion is a range of a minimum exposure estimate of 25% and maximum exposure of 75% to each language, which served as a recruitment guideline for the present study. Thus, our bilingual sample included infants who heard their community language (e.g., the language learned by most monolinguals in their community) at least 25% of the time and an additional language at least 25% of the time. Infants with exposure to a third or fourth language were included as long as they met this criterion. We also asked labs to limit their sample to simultaneous bilingual infants, who heard both languages regularly from within the first month of life. Infants who did not meet inclusion criteria for either group (for example, an infant with 85% exposure to one language, and 15% exposure to another, or who began learning a second language at age 6 months) could be tested if they inadvertently arrived in the lab, according to each lab’s policy. However, their data were not included in the main sample, but were retained for further exploratory analysis. Each laboratory was asked to recruit a sample of bilingual infants who received exposure to the community language as one of their languages and to recruit monolingual infants exposed to the community language. As a result, some samples consisted of heterogeneous bilinguals and others of homogenous bilinguals.
Each laboratory was asked to administer their own adaptation of a day-in-the-life parental interview asking about proportionate exposure to each language, which were typically based on the approach developed by Bosch and Sebastián-Gallés (2001). As laboratories often customize questionnaires to suit their local environment, it was concluded that each laboratory would be best able to decide on the variation of the language exposure tool that was optimal for their participant population. As some participating laboratories had not collected bilingual data prior to the study, these laboratories were paired with laboratories more experienced in infant bilingualism research to receive support and guidance in selecting or adapting a suitable language exposure questionnaire.
Although adapted for their language environment by each lab, there is consistency in the information sought from different versions of the language exposure questionnaire. Specifically, each adaptation walks parents through a “day-in-the-life” of their infant, asking about routines, caregivers, and the languages that they speak. An interviewer notes how much each language is spoken to the child during weekdays, weekends, and at different points of the infants’ life from caregivers. Indirect exposure through media such as television and radio, as well as overhead speech, are typically excluded (Byers-Heinlein, 2015). Together, this information is used to calculate the total percentage that the infant is directly exposed to each language.
Demographics.
Each lab administered a questionnaire that gathered basic demographic data about infants, including age, health history, gestation, etc.
Final sample.
Our final sample of bilinguals included 131 infants tested in 9 labs. 45 were 6–9 months, and 86 were 12–15 months old. Each of these labs also collected data from monolingual infants (N = 149), of whom 30 were 6–9 months, and 119 were 12–15 months. Data from monolingual infants were available from two additional labs (N = 42), who did not contribute bilingual data. A list of monolingual and bilingual populations in each lab are reported in Table 1. In addition, 2 labs registered to participate but failed to collect data from at least 10 included infants, and so their data were not included. Information about all included labs is given in Table 1.
Table 1.
Lab | Age group | Lang group | Mean age (days) | N | Method |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||
babylab-brookes | 12–15 mo | bilingual | 394 | 15 | eye-tracking |
babylab-brookes | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 415 | 14 | eye-tracking |
babylab-brookes | 6–9 mo | bilingual | 242 | 8 | eye-tracking |
babylab-brookes | 6–9 mo | monolingual | 238 | 8 | eye-tracking |
babylab-princeton | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 421 | 14 | hand-coding |
babylab-princeton | 6–9 mo | bilingual | 239 | 9 | hand-coding |
cdc-ceu | 12–15 mo | bilingual | 420 | 11 | eye-tracking |
cdc-ceu | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 404 | 10 | eye-tracking |
elp-georgetown | 12–15 mo | bilingual | 416 | 4 | eye-tracking |
elp-georgetown | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 425 | 7 | eye-tracking |
elp-georgetown | 6–9 mo | bilingual | 260 | 4 | eye-tracking |
elp-georgetown | 6–9 mo | monolingual | 242 | 5 | eye-tracking |
infantlanglab-utk | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 408 | 15 | hand-coding |
infantlanglab-utk | 6–9 mo | monolingual | 239 | 13 | hand-coding |
irl-concordia | 12–15 mo | bilingual | 403 | 14 | eye-tracking |
irl-concordia | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 399 | 16 | eye-tracking |
irl-concordia | 6–9 mo | bilingual | 235 | 11 | eye-tracking |
irl-concordia | 6–9 mo | monolingual | 214 | 7 | eye-tracking |
koku-hamburg | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 419 | 9 | eye-tracking |
koku-hamburg | 6–9 mo | monolingual | 234 | 5 | eye-tracking |
lll-liv | 12–15 mo | bilingual | 390 | 7 | eye-tracking |
lll-liv | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 400 | 15 | eye-tracking |
lll-liv | 6–9 mo | bilingual | 235 | 7 | eye-tracking |
lll-liv | 6–9 mo | monolingual | 230 | 8 | eye-tracking |
nusinfantlanguagecentre | 12–15 mo | bilingual | 426 | 4 | hand-coding, eye-tracking |
nusinfantlanguagecentre | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 416 | 6 | hand-coding, eye-tracking |
nusinfantlanguagecentre | 6–9 mo | bilingual | 261 | 6 | eye-tracking |
nusinfantlanguagecentre | 6–9 mo | monolingual | 246 | 2 | eye-tracking, hand-coding |
upf_barcelona | 12–15 mo | bilingual | 414 | 7 | eye-tracking |
upf_barcelona | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 404 | 11 | eye-tracking |
weltentdecker-zurich | 12–15 mo | bilingual | 408 | 24 | eye-tracking |
weltentdecker-zurich | 12–15 mo | monolingual | 416 | 26 | eye-tracking |
Stimuli
Stimuli consisted of videos of a female actor sitting at a table, directing her gaze to one of two colorful toys. Each video had the following sequence: the video began with the actor looking straight ahead for 1 second. She looked down for two seconds, after which a beep sounded to attract infants’ attention prior to the actor directing her gaze to a toy. Upon presentation of the beep, the actor looked up at the camera and, maintaining a neutral expression, she raised her eyebrows. Four seconds into the video, she began to turn her head towards the left or right and gazed towards the toy in her line of sight until the end of the video. There were a total of 24 different videos in this style, using six different pairs of colourful objects. Video presentations were counterbalanced for the side of presentation of the objects and the object at which the actor gazed, and arranged such that there were six test trials per infant. Original movies were in .avi format, exported at a framerate of 25 frames/second. Each movie lasted a total of 10 seconds (250 frames).
Procedure
We replicated the Eye Contact condition of Experiment 1 from Senju and Csibra (2008), using the original stimuli provided by the authors. Infants were seated on their parents’ laps in a quiet, dimly lit testing booth. Caregivers and infants were seated facing a monitor. The caregiver wore an occluder (e.g., sleep mask or opaque sunglasses) to prevent him/her from viewing events on the monitor. An experimenter controlled the study from an area located out of view of the infant, either in the same or a different room. Infants’ eye gaze data were collected automatically via a corneal reflection eye-tracker, or on a digital videotape for later offline coding.
Each infant saw a series of 6 test videos. Infants were assigned to one of four possible trial orders that counterbalanced the direction of the actor’s gaze (either LRRLRL or RLLRLR, where L denotes gaze to the toy on the left and R denotes fixation to the toy on the right), as well as which particular toy was located on the actor’s left and right. Due to a programming error, one lab presented the same trials in a randomized order instead. Videos were separated by an unrelated attention-grabbing cartoon, which was played between trials until the infant had looked towards it for approximately 1–2 seconds. The experiment lasted approximately 1.5 minutes.
Analysis overview
Data exclusion
Laboratories were asked to submit all data collected as part of the study (i.e., every infant for whom a data file was generated, regardless of how many trials were completed) to the analysis team. Data were first screened to determine whether laboratories contributed useable data and whether infants met our inclusion criteria below. Note that some infants had more than one reason for exclusion, and exclusion criteria were applied sequentially and percentages reflect this sequential application.
Lab reliability. Data from two of the labs using the hand-coding method were excluded after extensive discussions with the participating laboratories. One lab could not achieve an acceptable level of inter-rater reliability, due to difficulty coding infant eye movements from the available videos. A second lab initially coded the data incorrectly (i.e., coded gaze shift from face to object differently than had been specified), but then had insufficient resources to re-code the data. There were 104 (14.50%) infants who were tested in these labs.
• Age. There were 55 (9%) infants who were tested but were out of our target age groups (6–9 months and 12–15 months).
Language background. There were 50 (9%) infants who were tested but did not meet our inclusion criteria for either the monolingual or bilingual group. For example, an infant who heard English 20% of the time and Italian 80% of the time would not meet the criteria as either monolingual (at least 90% exposure to one language) or bilingual (at least 25% exposure to each of two languages).
Full-term. We defined full-term as gestation times greater than or equal to 37 weeks. There were 10 (2%) infants who were tested but did not meet this criterion.
No diagnosed developmental disorders. We excluded data from 1 (0.20%) infant with a parent-reported developmental disorder.
Session errors. There were 25 (5.07%) infants excluded from the analysis due to issues including: 12 for equipment failure, 10 for fussiness, and 3 for parental/external interference.
Insufficient face-to-object saccades. Following Senju and Csibra (2008), and per our pre-registration, we also excluded any infant who did not make at least one gaze shift from face to object during the window of analysis in at least three of the six trials. A further 145 (31.05%) infants were excluded from analyses for this reason.
Failure to attend. We also excluded any trials in which infants did not look at the congruent or incongruent object during the window of analysis. This meant that each infant contributed a different number of trials. An additional 360 trials (23.03%) were excluded from the analyses. This left us with a total number of 1563 valid trials (81.28% of the data after the previous screenings) for later analyses: 211 trials for 6-to-9-month-old monolinguals (73.52% of the data), 714 trials for 12-to-15-month-old monolinguals (83.80% of the data), 201 trials for 6-to-9-month-old bilinguals (74.44% of the data), and 437 trials for 12-to-15-month-old bilinguals (85.02% of the data).
One lab mistakenly used a preliminary rather than the final version of the experiment. The version used contained the same experimental stimuli and events as the final version with two exceptions: the attention getter to recruit the infant’s attention to the screen differed and the aspect ratio of the on-screen stimuli differed slightly. As this version of the experiment was only very slightly different from the final version, these data were retained for analysis.
Areas of interest and data pre-processing.
On eye-tracking setups, following Senju and Csibra (2008), we established three areas of interest (AOIs) on each trial (see Figure 1): the actor’s entire face (taking into account the model’s head movements) and two areas surrounding each of the two objects (corresponding to the size of the largest object). These rather generous AOIs maximized consistency between eye-tracking coding and human coding. The two object AOIs were labeled as congruent (i.e., the object target of the actor’s gaze) and incongruent (i.e., the object that was not the target of the actor’s gaze). Pixel coordinates for the AOIs were amended proportionally to each individual lab’s screen resolution.
Eye-trackers measured the coordinates of eye gaze, from which the direction and duration of fixations and gaze shifts were calculated. See supplemental materials for details of hardware used in each lab. Most eye-tracking software comes with built-in algorithms to parse fixations and gaze shifts, but these are optimized for adult data and perform suboptimally in noisy infant data (Hessels, Andersson, Hooge, Nyström, & Kemner, 2015; van Renswoude et al., 2018; Wass, Smith, & Johnson, 2013). To overcome this, and to standardize results between labs using different eye-tracking systems, we implemented a common approach using the GazePath tool for fixation and saccade detection, as outlined in van Renswoude et al. (2018). This approach is optimized for dealing with noisy infant data and individual differences that are expected between infants of different ages.
For labs that did not have an eye-tracker, trained human coders examined videos of infants’ faces frame-by-frame to identify fixations and gaze shifts. Fixations were coded for duration and location with respect to the areas of interest (i.e., congruent object, incongruent object, actor, or off-target). Shifts were coded for direction, defined with respect to the horizontal and vertical midlines; i.e., movement could be left, right, down, and/or up. For these labs, a target minimum of 25% of participants were double-coded by a second human coder and reliability estimates computed. Ultimately 27% of participants were double-coded.
Data reliability
Because of the variability across labs in terms of methods and setups, different intrinsic reliability issues emerged regarding data consistency across different eye-tracker setups, between different human coders, and between eye-tracker and manual coding setups. These issues have been addressed in three different ways. First, as described above, all eye-tracking data were processed using the same GazePath tool, which is optimized to account for variability across different ages, populations, and setups (van Renswoude et al., 2018). Second, all labs using human coding rather than an eye-tracker double coded a minimum of 25% of their data. For 6- to 9-month-olds, frame and shift agreement ranged from 98.27–99.27% and 95.40–99.55%, respectively. For 12- to 15-month-olds, frame and shift agreement ranged from 96.01–99.30% and 90.54–99.63%, respectively. These numbers do not include the one laboratory described above whose data were excluded due to low inter-rater reliability, which obtained well below 70% agreement due to poor video quality. One lab had additionally planned to hand-code eye-tracking data to assess the comparability of eye-tracking and human-coded data, but was unable to successfully do so due to unforeseen technical and staffing issues. Overall, offline and eye-tracking-coded data each appeared to have good reliability, although we were not able to assess the comparability of these approaches.
Results
Dependent variables
Following previous studies using this paradigm (Senju & Csibra, 2008; Szufnarowska et al., 2014), we investigated infants’ gaze-following abilities via several different approaches. Each approach focused on infants’ looking behaviors to the areas of interest starting from the point in time when the model started to turn her head (4 seconds – 100 frames – from the beginning of the trial) to the end of the trial (10 seconds – 250 frames – from the beginning of the trial). We measured four different dependent variables for each infant on each trial. Three measures have been used in previous studies: first look, frequency of looks, and duration of looks (Senju & Csibra, 2008; Szufnarowska et al., 2014). We included an additional measure, latency, as we reasoned that infants’ reaction time to follow an actor’s gaze might show interesting development over the first two years of life, and might be a potentially sensitive measure. Exploring these four variables in the context of our large sample size can provide insight for future studies about the expected effect sizes for different analytic approaches.
First look.
This measured whether the infant shifted their gaze from the face AOI to one of the object AOIs. This yielded a binary variable indicating whether the infant showed a congruent gaze shift towards the actor’s target (coded as 1), an incongruent gaze shift towards the other object (coded as 0), or no shift (coded as missing).
Frequency of looks.
This yielded two values for each infant: the number of times the infant shifted their gaze from the face AOI to the congruent AOI, and the number of times the infant shifted their gaze from the face AOI to the incongruent AOI.
Duration of looks.
This measured the total duration of fixation to the congruent AOI and to the incongruent AOI. Thus, each infant had two values. These values were log-transformed prior to analysis in order to correct for the skew typical of looking time data (Csibra, Hernik, Mascaro, Tatone, & Lengyel, 2016).
Latency.
This established infants’ reaction times to follow the actor’s gaze in milliseconds. On each trial, latency was coded as the latency of the first face-to-object gaze shift, irrespective of whether the first look was to the congruent or incongruent AOIs. Our pre-registered analysis plan for latency was to transform the data if needed to correct issues of non-normality. Raw latencies were somewhat left-skewed, but this was not improved by log-transformation, so we did not transform the data.
Analysis approach
All planned analyses were pre-registered at osf.io/2ey3k/. Following previous large-scale multi-lab studies with infants (e.g., ManyBabies Consortium, 2020; Byers-Heinlein, Tsui, et al., 2020), we used two complementary data analysis frameworks: meta-analysis and mixed-effects regression. Under the meta-analytic framework, we conducted standard analyses within each lab and then combined these results across labs. An advantage of this approach is that it is easy to understand, and is comparable to results from meta-analyses that gather data from published studies. Under the mixed-effects regression framework, we modeled raw trial-by-trial data from each infant. Because this approach models raw data directly, it can have greater statistical power to detect effects.
Confirmatory Analyses
Meta-analytic framework.
Under this framework, we first calculated mean scores for each individual infant on the four dependent variables. For first look, frequency of looks, and total duration of looks, we calculated proportion difference scores for each infant, which subtracted the mean value for incongruent trials (i) from the mean for congruent trials (c), and divided by the total number of trials that contributed to that measure [(c − i)/(c + i)]. Trials without values for a particular measure were excluded from the calculation. For latency, we limited the analysis to only those trials with a congruent first look, and for the meta-analytic model, we focused on the mean latency for each infant to look towards the congruent AOI. We then collapsed these for each dataset (i.e., a combination of lab, bilingualism status, and age group) to calculate a grand mean (M) and standard deviation (sd) across participants in each dataset. Finally, using the formula dz = M/sd, the derived M and sd were used to compute a within-subject Cohen’s d for first look, frequency of looks, and total duration of looks. For latency, we deviated from the pre-registered analysis plan. As the analysis was limited to latency towards the congruent AOI, it was not ideal to generate a Cohen’s d effect size without a comparison between two means. Instead of computing a within-subject Cohen’s d, the raw grand mean (M) and standard deviation (sd) in milliseconds across participants were entered into the meta-analytic model for latency. Sampling variance for each mean was calculated based on the formula sd ˆ 2/n.
Random-effects meta-analysis models with a restricted maximum-likelihood estimator (REML) were fit with the metafor package (Viechtbauer, 2010). A logistic model was fit for first look, frequency of looks, and total duration of looks as each infant’s score was bounded between 0 and 1. A linear model was fit for latency. To account for the dependence between mono- and bilingual datasets stemming from the same lab, we included laboratory as a random factor. Bilingualism (0 = monolingual, 1 = bilingual), and age group (0 = 6–9 months, 1 = 12–15 months) were dummy coded.
Our main meta-analytic model for each dependent variable was:
First look.
We began by examining the relation of the proportion of congruent first looks to bilingualism and age, fitting the main effect model to the 32 separate group means and variances (after aggregating by lab, age, and language group). Note that, because incongruent trials are subtracted from congruent in the numerator of this calculation, the first look proportion scores are centered around 0 with negative values indicating behaviors in the direction of incongruent trials, and positive values indicating greater proportion of behaviors in the direction of congruent trials. The meta-analysis on first look yielded a mean effect size estimate of 0.79 (CI = [0.28 – 1.29], z = 3.07, p = .002) for 6–9 month-old monolingual infants (the reference level). Age yielded an additional effect of 0.43 (CI = [−0.17 – 1.03], z = 1.39, p = .165), suggesting a mean increase in the proportion of first looks to the target for 12–15 month-old monolingual infants, although this effect was not statistically significant. The bilingual coefficient of 0 (CI = [−0.72 – 0.72], z = 0, p = .997) suggests no difference between bilingual and monolingual infants at 6–9 months (the reference age). Moreover, the interaction between bilingualism and age was small and not statistically different from zero (β = −0.02, CI = [−0.91 – 0.88], z = −0.04, p = .970). Taken together, this suggests no reliable difference in proportion of first looks to the target between bilingual and monolingual infants at either age. A forest plot for this meta-analysis is shown in Figure 2.
Frequency of looks.
We then investigated the relation of frequency of looks to bilingualism and age group. The overall mean effect size estimate for 6–9 month-old monolingual infants was 0.73 (CI = [0.22 – 1.23], z = 2.83, p = .005). Age yielded an additional effect of 0.48 (CI = [−0.13 – 1.08], z = 1.55, p = .121), but was not statistically significant. There was no evidence that bilingual infants differed from monolingual infants at 6–9 months, as the additional effect of bilingualism was 0 (CI = [−0.72 – 0.72], z = 0, p = .998). Moreover, the interaction between bilingualism and age yielded a very small effect of 0.10 (CI = [−0.80 – 0.99], z = 0.22, p = .829), implying no differences between monolingual and bilingual infants in frequency of target looks at both ages. A forest plot for this meta-analysis is shown in Figure 3.
Duration of looks.
The cross-lab meta-analysis on duration of looks yielded a significant mean effect size estimate for 6–9 month-old monolingual infants of 0.61 (CI = [0.19 – 1.04], z = 2.84, p = .004). Age yielded a non-significant additional effect of 0.17 (CI = [−0.32 – 0.66], z = 0.67, p = .501). The additional bilingualism effect of −0.02 (CI = [−0.62 – 0.58], z = −0.05, p = .958) was also not statistically significant, suggesting that 6–9 month-old bilingual infants did not look significantly longer to the target relative to the distractor compared to monolingual infants. Moreover, the interaction between bilingualism and age yielded a very small effect of 0.05 (CI = [−0.68 – 0.77], z = 0.12, p = .903), suggesting no evidence of differences between monolingual and bilingual infants across both ages. A forest plot for this meta-analysis is shown in Figure 4.
Latency.
The cross-lab meta-analysis on latency towards the congruent object yielded a reference-level mean latency estimate of 2,345.76 milliseconds (CI = [2,056.47 – 2,635.06], z = 15.89, p =< .001) for 6–9 month-old monolingual infants. With the effect of age, the mean latency estimate decreased significantly, with an estimated difference for the older group of −493.06 milliseconds (CI = [−835.03 - −151.09], z = −2.83, p = .005); in other words, 12–15 month-old monolingual infants were faster than 6–9 month-old monolingual infants to fixate the congruent object. Bilingualism increased the mean latency estimate by 378.29 milliseconds (CI = [−26.76 – 783.34], z = 1.83, p = .067); in other words, the estimate for bilinguals suggested they might be slower than monolingual infants to fixate the congruent object, but this was non-significant. The interaction between bilingualism and age suggested a possible attenuation of this pattern for older 12–15 month-old bilingual versus monolingual infants, although again this did not reach statistical significance (estimate = −437.30, CI = [−930.57 – 55.97], z = −1.74, p = .082). Pairwise comparisons revealed that, at the age of 12–15 months, there was no longer any evidence of a difference in target fixation latency between monolingual and bilingual infants (estimate = 59.01, se = 143.63, z = 0.41, p = .681). A forest plot for this meta-analysis is shown in Figure 5.
Summary of meta-analysis.
Overall, our meta-analytic models revealed that infants followed the actor’s gaze to the congruent object, as measured by their first looks, frequency of looks, and duration of looks. Bilingualism was not a significant moderator for any of these three variables. The first look and frequency of looks models revealed medium effects for age while the duration of looks model revealed a small effect, although age was not statistically significant in any of those models. The direction of these effects would suggest that 12–15 month old infants are better at gaze-following than 6–9 month old infants. This pattern was repeated in our meta-analytic model of latency, which revealed that older infants were significantly faster than younger infants to fixate the congruent object after the actor’s gaze shift. Latency of fixation, moreover, was the only measure where we found any suggestion of a difference between bilingual and monolingual infants. Though it did not reach the significance threshold of p < .05, the coefficient direction and magnitude of the latency model showed that younger bilinguals were slower to fixate on the target object than their monolingual peers. This possible effect was not observed for older infants, where by 12–15 months there was no evidence for different latencies between bilinguals and monolinguals. Together, all these results imply that older infants show more reliable gaze following than younger infants.
Mixed-effects regression framework
As opposed to the meta-analytic framework, the mixed-effects regression framework allowed us to model trial-level data from individual infants rather than analyzing averages. Mixed-effects models are described as such because they include both fixed effects and random effects. Our fixed effects modeled the main variables of interest: age, bilingualism, and aoi. Our random effects accounted for correlations in the data that could arise due to dependency between data from the same infants, lab, and test items. For each model, we planned to initially fit a maximal random effects structure (Barr, Levy, Scheepers, & Tily, 2013), while anticipating the need for pruning. We aimed to identify a pruned random-effects structure that would be well-supported by our data while conserving the most theoretically important effects (Matuschek, Kliegl, Vasishth, Baayen, & Bates, 2017). The approach to pruning random effects was somewhat exploratory, as we did not have a specific hypothesis about the random effects. Note that while the particular random effects structure of the model can affect the estimates of standard errors, in a balanced design it does not affect the estimates of the fixed effects, which were our main interest.
We modeled trial-level data for each infant, for the following dependent variables (DV):
first_shift: A binary variable denoting the AOI of the first shift, where 0 is the incongruent object and 1 is the congruent object.
latency: The time interval in milliseconds between the onset of the actor’s head-turn, and the moment of first fixation on an object AOI.
freq_shift: The number of times in the trial an infant shifted gaze towards the AOI.
total_look: The total duration of fixations towards the AOI during the trial.
Our predictor variables were:
bilingual: A dummy-coded variable where 0 is monolingual, 1 is bilingual.
age_days: The infant’s age in days, scaled and centred for ease of interpretation.
aoi: A dummy-coded variable for analysis of freq_shift, total_look, and latency, for which data from both AOIs are reported. Here, 0 denotes the congruent AOI, and 1 denotes the incongruent AOI.
We ran separate models for each DV. We fit all models using the lme4 package (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015). For first_shift, we fit a logistic model as this variable is binary at the trial level. The initial model specification was:
For latency, freq_shift, and total_look, we used a similar model with two modifications. First, we fit a linear model rather than a logistic model as these variables are continuous and unbounded. Second, we included an interaction with aoi in the fixed effects, and estimated corresponding random slopes where appropriate. This was necessary in order to estimate separate parameters for the congruent and incongruent AOIs (i.e., to model whether latency to first fixation varies as a function of whether it is to the congruent or incongruent AOI; whether infants shift more frequently to the congruent than the incongruent AOI; and whether infants fixate more on the congruent than incongruent AOI). For these three DVs, the initial model specification was:
First shift towards the AOI.
Our final logistic model specification for first shift was:
Table 2 shows coefficient estimates from this model and Figure 6 visualizes this model. Positive coefficients indicate a higher probability of making a first look to the congruent object. The significant intercept indicated that infants were more likely to first look to the congruent versus the incongruent object; moreover, a significant positive coefficient for age indicated that older infants did so at an even higher rate. There was no obvious evidence for a difference between monolingual and bilingual infants, and the interaction of bilingualism and age was also not significant. Monolingual and bilingual infants, therefore, did not differ in their probabilities of first looking to the congruent object across ages.
Table 2.
Estimate | SE | z | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Intercept | 0.971 | 0.105 | 9.270 | <.001 |
bilingual | −0.010 | 0.126 | −0.078 | 0.938 |
age_days | 0.197 | 0.079 | 2.500 | <.05 |
bilingual * age_days | −0.096 | 0.123 | −0.779 | 0.436 |
Frequency of shifting gaze towards the AOI.
The final model specification for frequency of shift was:
Table 3 shows coefficient estimates from this model and Figure 7 visualizes this model. The significant main effect of age indicated that older monolingual infants looked more frequently at the objects as compared to younger monolingual infants. More centrally, there were both a significant main effect of aoi and an interaction between aoi and age, suggesting that infants shifted more often to the congruent object as opposed to the incongruent object and that this pattern of looking increased as infants aged. The effect of bilingualism, however, was not significant, and neither were its 2-way interaction with aoi nor its 3-way interaction with age and aoi; this suggests that there was not a reliable difference between bilingual and monolingual infants in the number of times they shifted gaze towards the congruent object. However, the direction of the interaction effect between age and bilingualism, although not significant, would indicate that bilingual infants might show a greater increase in their frequency of looks towards the objects with age compared to monolinguals.
Table 3.
Estimate | SE | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Intercept | 1.160 | 0.041 | 28.500 | <.001 |
bilingual | 0.066 | 0.039 | 1.700 | 0.09 |
age_days | 0.087 | 0.025 | 3.460 | <.01 |
aoi | −0.626 | 0.035 | −17.800 | <.001 |
bilingual * age_days | 0.069 | 0.039 | 1.790 | 0.074 |
bilingual * aoi | −0.054 | 0.055 | −0.972 | 0.331 |
age_days * aoi | −0.104 | 0.036 | −2.920 | <.01 |
bilingual * age_days * aoi | −0.029 | 0.055 | −0.525 | 0.599 |
Duration of fixations towards the AOI during the trial.
The final model specification for duration of fixations was:
Table 4 shows coefficient estimates from this model and Figure 8 visualizes this model. There were two main effects (age and aoi), but no significant interactions. This suggests that monolingual infants looked longer to congruent versus incongruent objects, and that in general older infants looked longer at the objects than did younger infants. The effect of bilingualism was, however, not significant as a main effect or in interaction with any other factors, suggesting no reliable differences between bilingual and monolingual infants in terms of their duration of looking at the congruent versus incongruent objects.
Table 4.
Estimate | SE | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Intercept | 5.640 | 0.147 | 38.400 | <.001 |
bilingual | 0.142 | 0.155 | 0.919 | 0.358 |
age_days | 0.345 | 0.098 | 3.500 | <.001 |
aoi | −1.690 | 0.135 | −12.500 | <.001 |
bilingual * age_days | 0.137 | 0.152 | 0.898 | 0.369 |
bilingual * aoi | 0.225 | 0.213 | 1.060 | 0.289 |
age_days * aoi | −0.016 | 0.136 | −0.114 | 0.909 |
bilingual * age_days * aoi | 0.106 | 0.211 | 0.501 | 0.616 |
Latency.
The final model specification for latency was:
Table 5 shows coefficient estimates from this model, and Figure 9 visualizes this model. The significant main effect of age suggested that older monolingual infants were more rapid than younger monolingual infants in fixating their first look at the congruent objects. The main effect of aoi was also significant, suggesting that younger monolingual infants made faster first fixations to the congruent objects than to the incongruent objects. The effect of bilingualism was, however, not significant as a main effect or in interaction with any other factors, implying no reliable differences between bilingual and monolingual infants in latency of their first fixation. Taken together, then, the model reveals that older infants are quicker to make fixations than younger infants, and that language background do not reliably impact fixation latency.
Table 5.
Estimate | SE | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Intercept | 2,010.000 | 80.300 | 25.000 | <.001 |
bilingual | 10.900 | 95.000 | 0.115 | 0.909 |
age_days | −260.000 | 61.500 | −4.230 | <.001 |
aoi | 310.000 | 92.100 | 3.370 | <.01 |
bilingual * age_days | −117.000 | 93.300 | −1.250 | 0.211 |
bilingual * aoi | 149.000 | 144.000 | 1.040 | 0.3 |
age_days * aoi | −84.000 | 90.200 | −0.931 | 0.352 |
bilingual * age_days * aoi | 62.400 | 142.000 | 0.439 | 0.661 |
Summary of mixed-effects regression.
Overall, our mixed-effects regression revealed that early gaze-following development is significantly modulated by age-related changes, where older infants showed a more reliable gaze-following ability in every available measure as compared to younger infants. That is, older infants were more accurate and more rapid than younger infants in directing their first gaze towards the congruent objects, and they looked longer and more frequently at the congruent objects than at the incongruent objects. In contrast, bilingualism did not significantly predict infants’ gaze-following accuracy, duration, or speed of fixations. However, there was a trend where, as they aged, bilingual infants showed a steeper increase in frequency of fixations compared to monolinguals. Regardless of bilinguals’ more frequent fixations, however, these results most robustly support the interpretation that monolingual and bilingual infants follow a similar trajectory of gaze-following development despite their differences in language experience.
General Discussion
The objective of this study was to launch a large-scale, multi-site study on the effects of bilingualism on gaze following at two age groups (6–9 and 12–15 months). Using the gaze-following task developed by Senju and Csibra (2008), we investigated the effects of bilingual exposure and age on several measures of gaze following (i.e., first look, frequency of looks, total duration of looks, and latency). Data were analyzed in accordance with a pre-registered analysis plan, comprising a meta-analytic approach and mixed-effects regression models. At the outset, we introduced three hypotheses. First, we hypothesized that all infants would demonstrate an improvement in gaze following towards congruent objects (i.e., those cued by an adult model) between the two age groups tested. Second, we hypothesized that bilingual infants would demonstrate more successful gaze following to congruent objects than monolingual infants, both in terms of accuracy and latency. Finally, we hypothesized an interaction of age and bilingual exposure on gaze following. We discuss the first hypothesis concerning all infants, and then turn to the second and third hypotheses that pertain to effects of bilingualism and its interaction with age.
First, we predicted an effect of age on gaze-following behavior. Overall, infants followed the gaze of an adult model to the congruent object across a variety of measures. Our meta-analytic models yielded a medium, but non-significant effect of improved performance on first-looks and frequency of looks to the congruent object as infants aged. The meta-analytic models further revealed a significant effect of age on the latency to first look: older infants were faster to fixate congruent objects than were younger infants. Mixed-effects models, which allow us to model trial-level behavior and thus gain statistical power, revealed stronger evidence of age effects: older infants gazed at the congruent object with significantly greater efficiency and accuracy than younger infants. These findings are consistent with prior research demonstrating that infants improve their gaze-following as they get older (e.g., Butterworth & Jarrett, 1991; Gredebäck, Fikke, & Melinder, 2010; Moll & Tomasello, 2004), and thus extend this pattern to Senju and Csibra’s paradigm. In contrast to our study, Senju and Csibra tested infants at a single age-group (6 months). Our study demonstrated that the same infant gaze-following behaviors reported by Senju and Csibra remained evident between 6 and 9 months and significantly improved by 12 to 15 months.
In addition to demonstrating age-related change, our findings offer a methodological contribution. With respect to how gaze following is operationalized, our study diversifies the range of dependent variables through which gaze following can be expressed. Specifically, our study revealed preferential fixation to the congruent object using first looks and frequency of looks, as did Senju and Csibra. However, unlike Senju and Csibra, we also found evidence of preferential fixation when fixation duration was used, albeit the duration effects were weaker compared to first looks and frequency of looks. Furthermore, as a complement to accuracy measures, older infants had a tendency towards shorter latencies, which provides a measure of gaze-following efficiency. Overall, this suggests firstly, that the paradigm used by Senju and Csibra in a relatively small sample of 20 infants was replicable in a much larger and more diverse sample of over 300 infants. Secondly, our study provides evidence not only for continuity in gaze-following behavior after 6 months, but additionally evidence for more efficient gaze-following behaviors at age 12–15 months.
The primary objective of our study was to investigate the effects of bilingualism on gaze-following behavior. Our second hypothesis was therefore that bilingual infants would demonstrate greater gaze-following behavior relative to monolingual infants, and our third hypothesis was that this would interact with age. Based on our meta-analyses, there was limited support for these hypotheses. We tested effects of bilingualism across four different dependent variables, using two different analytic techniques. The only evidence we found was in our meta-analysis for latency, which revealed a non-significant trend for slower fixation to congruent objects in bilinguals versus monolinguals in the younger age group, but not in the older age group. In general, however, gaze-following behavior was strikingly similar in monolingual and bilingual infants, suggesting that gaze following is robust to variations in language experience.
At first glance, these findings are seemingly inconsistent with findings from prior studies demonstrating that bilingual children may be more sensitive to eye gaze when learning words than monolingual children (e.g., Brojde et al., 2012; Yow & Markman, 2011). However, the present results are compatible with a recent comparison of bilingual and monolingual infants’ gaze-following behavior. Singh, Quinn, Xiao, and Lee (2019) demonstrated similarity in basic gaze-following behavior in monolingual and bilingual groups, using a similar paradigm at 18 months. Similarly, Schonberg, Sandhofer, Tsang, and Johnson (2014) reported that there were no differences between monolingual and bilingual 3- and 6-month-olds looking patterns when viewing faces, objects and complex scenes.
We offer two possible accounts for the null effects of bilingualism reported here: a conceptual account and a methodological account. Conceptually, in contrast to the present study, prior studies found that when faced with referential ambiguity, bilingual children were better able to use gaze to resolve the conflict and disambiguate the meanings of words (e.g., Yow et al., 2017; Yow & Markman, 2011). It is possible that bilingual children attend more closely to gaze when gaze truly helps to resolve referential ambiguity. Given that bilinguals likely encounter greater referential ambiguity on account of learning two languages, it is possible that drawing on gaze cues provides a useful strategy for bilingual infants. This is aligned with prior research demonstrating that while monolingual children can resolve referential ambiguity using stored linguistic knowledge (e.g., via mutual exclusivity), multilingual children may need to appeal to other strategies (see Byers-Heinlein & Werker, 2009). In the present task, there were no word learning or language comprehension demands, nor was there any ambiguity as to which object served as the target of the adult’s gaze. Moreover, gaze cues did not have to be integrated with other sources of information in order to identify the cued object. Instead, this task measured a much more fundamental ability to look at the object looked at by another person. One possibility is therefore that monolingual and bilingual infants begin with similar basic gaze sensitivity and differ in their use of gaze to learn the meanings of words. Effects of bilingualism on word learning may set in closer to 18 months, when strategies for referential disambiguation first emerge (Halberda, 2003; Markman, Wasow, & Hansen, 2003). For example, 14- to 17-month-old bilinguals are more sensitive than monolinguals to the objects that a speaker has in her line of sight (Liberman, Woodward, Keysar, & Kinzler, 2017).
It is also possible that methodological differences contribute to discrepancies between our findings and prior studies. Prior studies demonstrating bilingual advantages have used much smaller sample sizes, ranging from 16–24 children per group. Two core advantages of large-scale, pre-registered reports is i) that they have the potential to investigate whether effects are replicated in larger, diverse samples with a standardized protocol (Frank et al., 2017) and ii) that they are somewhat spared from possible confirmation biases in the publication process, which often favor evidence for a bilingual advantage (see de Bruin et al., 2015). It is possible that prior evidence of bilingual advantages in gaze sensitivity are not as replicable or stable than smaller-scale studies would suggest. This is not intended as a criticism or indictment of any prior study, but rather as a reference to the promises of methodological standardization, predetermined protocols, and increased statistical power.
Although we did not observe striking differences between monolinguals and bilinguals in gaze following ability, we did observe some suggestive differences between monolinguals and bilinguals in their overall attention to the objects (both congruent and incongruent). Compared to monolinguals, bilinguals showed some evidence of steeper changes in the frequency of fixations to congruent objects in general as they aged, although these findings were not particularly statistically robust. These tendencies would seem consistent with other studies suggesting that allocation of attention is sensitive to environmental experience from early in life. For example, sighted infants of blind parents showed a decrease in gaze-following attention compared to the control infants; furthermore, this difference increased between 6–10 and 12–16 months of age (Senju et al., 2015). Conversely, deaf 7- to 20-month-old infants of deaf parents showed enhanced gaze-following attention to visual communicative signals, with the younger infants showing more robust gaze-following behavior relative to hearing infants (Brooks, Singleton, & Meltzoff, 2020). Overall, subtle changes in selective attention to objects early in development, as might be the case here with bilingual infants’ tendency to look more frequently at objects, may be relevant for everyday processing of socially relevant information and subsequent language outcomes. However, given that our findings were not predicted and failed to reach statistical significance, this pattern will need to be replicated.
Challenges and Limitations
Here, we address some of the challenges and limitations of the present study. We begin broadly with challenges common to other studies launched under the ManyBabies initiative (Byers-Heinlein, Bergmann, et al., 2020). To some extent, these challenges may reflect “teething problems” associated with adapting more traditional individual laboratory studies to cross-laboratory collaborative studies. At the outset, it became clear that participating labs had different protocols for collecting data, surveying language background, and administering studies. We encountered several procedural challenges in determining how to work with differences in equipment, personnel, and other resources available to different investigators. A very basic difference in the present study was how different laboratories tracked gaze following: some used manual video recording while others used eye-trackers. Even within the labs with eye-trackers, there was likely considerable variation in how robustly different eye-trackers captured gaze data. Similarly, there was variation in the quality of video-records obtained by labs that did not use eye-trackers. This provides one of several examples where efforts towards methodological standardization (or “streamlining”) cannot wholly eliminate effects of methodological variation across labs. While some of this variation can be captured in data processing (in our case, analysis scripts had to be adapted to each eye-tracking setup), other sources of variation cannot easily be identified or controlled. In this way, sources of unexplained error variance in multi-site large-scale studies are likely different from those obtained in single-laboratory studies, which can affect the interpretation of findings.
A second consideration relates to analyses. We pre-registered two analytic approaches: meta-analysis and mixed-effects regression models. However, these two approaches pointed to different conclusions in some cases, and thus made interpretation challenging. In general, we interpret these differences in light of the additional statistical power provided by the regression models, which were ultimately more sensitive and revealed more nuance in our data set. While this is likely due to averaging across groups of infants in the meta-analytic models which decreases statistical power relative to linear mixed-effects models, it raises questions for interpretation. For example, we hypothesized effects of age, which were more evident in the mixed-effects models than in the meta-analyses. We hope that by transparently pre-registering and reporting all analyses, readers will feel more convinced by our interpretations, or at least be more able to draw their own conclusions.
Finally, we acknowledge that in spite of having recruited a geographically diverse sample, our samples were likely similar in several ways. First, our samples were all drawn from developed, Westernized countries. Within each country, participation was limited to families who were available and interested to come to a university laboratory, likely limiting socio-economic diversity. Our sample probably included mainly infants of higher socio-economic status, as is typical in laboratory-based developmental research. We had no participating labs from Latin America, Africa, South Asia, East Asia or the Middle East. Therefore, the typical limitations of convenience sampling no doubt applied to our study. This is relevant to studies of gaze following preceded by eye contact, as ethnographic reports of parent-infant interactions reveal considerable cross-cultural variation in the extent to which adults engage in eye contact with their infants (LeVine & Norman, 2001). In some societies such as the Gusii of Kenya, eye contact with infants is far less common. For example, in 6-month-old infants, eye contact occurs in less than 10% of interactions between infants and caregivers (Tronick, 2007). Similarly, in some cultures, such as the Nso in Northern Cameroon, parents blow into the eyes of infants to actively avoid eye contact (LeVine & LeVine, 2016). As a result, there is reportedly much less intentional eye contact between adults and infants in the first year of life than is often reported in Westernized societies (see LeVine et al., 1994). Examples of reduced eye contact are primarily drawn from non-Western rural societies, which were not represented in our study. Consequently, infants’ responsiveness to gaze-cuing may depend on its frequency and functionality in their natural environment. For example, one study in a rural small-scale society in Tanna island in Vanuatu found evidence of gaze following in infants as young as 5 to 7 months of age (Hernik & Broesch, 2019), despite reports of relatively lower frequency of face-to-face mother-infant interactions in the same community (Little, Carver, & Legare, 2016). Having greater geographical and socioeconomic variation within participating labs in the current study would have helped to qualify evidence of uniformity in gaze following across infants being brought up in diverse cultural contexts.
Summary
This study forms part of a groundswell of large-scale, multi-lab initiatives all working towards the common goal of investigating generalizability and replicability of core findings in infant cognition (c.f., ManyBabies Consortium, 2020; Byers-Heinlein, Bergmann, et al., 2020; Byers-Heinlein, Tsui, et al., 2020). Sampling 322 infants distributed across 8 countries and 3 continents, this study provides confirmatory evidence for the replicability and generalizability of past evidence for infants’ sensitivity to gaze cues. Given the developmental significance often ascribed to infant gaze following (see Moore, 2008), there are clear scientific gains in knowing that infant gaze-following behaviors withstand the kind of geographical and cultural variation captured in our sample. That gaze-following does not appear to be influenced by bilingualism suggests that fundamental gaze sensitivity also withstands variation in language exposure. The results of the current study point to striking uniformity in how different samples respond to gaze cues in infancy, at least within a westernized cultural context. The findings of this study speak to the stability of infant gaze-following behaviors, but also inform the vast body of literature that invokes gaze following as a critical social response upon which much of later language learning depends (see Baldwin, 1995; Brooks & Meltzoff, 2014).
Funding
Individual participating labs acknowledge funding support from: the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (402470-2011; 2018-04390); the National Science Foundation (BCS-1551719); the UK Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC ES/L008955/1); the European Research Council Advanced Grant, UNDER CONTROL (323961); the European Research Council Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant (798658); the Leverhulme Trust (ECF-2015-009); the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01HD083312); the European Research Council Synergy Grant (SOMICS 609819); the Early Career Research Grant & Start-up Grant, Western Sydney University; Research Manitoba, University of Manitoba, Children’s Hospital Research Institute of Manitoba; and the ODPRT funds, National University of Singapore.
Footnotes
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare that there were no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship or the publication of this article.
Disclosures
Preregistration
Our manuscript was reviewed prior to data collection; in addition, we registered our instructions and materials prior to data collection osf.io/2ey3k/.
Data, materials, and online resources
All materials, data, and analytic code are available at osf.io/2ey3k/.
Reporting
We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures in the study.
References
- Antovich DM, & Graf Estes K (2018). Learning across languages: Bilingual experience supports dual language statistical word segmentation. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12548. 10.1111/desc.12548 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin DA (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In Moore C & Dunham PJ (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131–158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, & Tily HJ (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, & Walker S (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bedford R, Elsabbagh M, Gliga T, Pickles A, Senju A, Charman T, . . . BASIS team. (2012). Precursors to social and communication difficulties in infants at-risk for autism: Gaze following and attentional engagement. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(10), 2208–2218. 10.1007/s10803-012-1450-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergelson E, & Swingley D (2012). At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(9), 3253–3258. 10.1073/pnas.1113380109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bilson S, Yoshida H, Tran CD, Woods EA, & Hills TT (2015). Semantic facilitation in bilingual first language acquisition. Cognition, 140, 122–134. 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloom P (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bosch L, & Sebastián-Gallés N (2001). Evidence of early language discrimination abilities in infants from bilingual environments. Infancy, 2, 29–49. 10.1207/S15327078IN0201_3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brito N, & Barr R (2014). Flexible memory retrieval in bilingual 6-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(5), 1156–1163. 10.1002/dev.21188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brojde CL, Ahmed S, & Colunga E (2012). Bilingual and monolingual children attend to different cues when learning new words. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 155. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00155 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks R, & Meltzoff AN (2002). The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 958–966. 10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.958 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks R, & Meltzoff AN (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science, 8(6), 535–543. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00445.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks R, & Meltzoff AN (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 207–220. 10.1017/S030500090700829X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks R, & Meltzoff AN (2014). Gaze following: A mechanism for building social connections between infants and adults. In Mikulincer M & Shaver PR (Eds.), Mechanisms of social connection: From brain to group (pp. 167–183). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 10.1037/14250-010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks R, Singleton JL, & Meltzoff AN (2020). Enhanced gaze-following behavior in deaf infants of deaf parents. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12900. 10.1111/desc.12900 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butterworth G, & Jarrett N (1991). What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(1), 55–72. 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1991.tb00862.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Byers-Heinlein K (2015). Methods for studying infant bilingualism. In Schwieter JW (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingual Processing (pp. 133–154). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781107447257 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Byers-Heinlein K (2017). Bilingualism affects 9-month-old infants’ expectations about how words refer to kinds. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12486. 10.1111/desc.12486 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byers-Heinlein K, Bergmann C, Davies C, Frank MC, Hamlin JK, Kline M, . . . Soderstrom M (2020). Building a collaborative psychological science: Lessons learned from manybabies 1. Canadian Psychology. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byers-Heinlein K, & Fennell CT (2014). Perceptual narrowing in the context of increased variation: Insights from bilingual infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(2), 274–291. 10.1002/dev.21167 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byers-Heinlein K, Tsui ASM, Bergmann C, Black A, Brown A, Carbajal MJ, . . . Wermelinger S (2020). Quantifying sources of variability in infancy research using the infant-directed speech preference. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science. Pre-Registered Report Accepted for Publication. In press. [Google Scholar]
- Byers-Heinlein K, & Werker JF (2009). Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: Infants’ language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic. Developmental Science, 12(5), 815–823. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00902.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byers-Heinlein K, & Werker JF (2013). Lexicon structure and the disambiguation of novel words: Evidence from bilingual infants. Cognition, 128(3), 407–416. 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter M, Nagell K, & Tomasello M (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), i–174. 10.2307/1166214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Core C, Hoff E, Rumiche R, & Señor M (2013). Total and conceptual vocabulary in spanish-english bilinguals from 22 to 30 months: Implications for assessment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(5), 1637–1649. 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/11-0044) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corkum V, & Moore C (1995). Development of joint visual attention in infants. In Moore C & Dunham PJ (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 61–8361–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Csibra G, Hernik M, Mascaro O, Tatone D, & Lengyel M (2016). Statistical treatment of looking-time data. Developmental Psychology, 52(4), 521–536. 10.1037/dev0000083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bruin A, Treccani B, & Della Sala S (2015). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26(1), 99–107. 10.1177/0956797614557866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Houwer A, Bornstein MH, & De Coster S (2006). Early understanding of two words for the same thing: A cdi study of lexical comprehension in infant bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10(3), 331–347. 10.1177/13670069060100030401 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Duñabeitia JA, & Carreiras M (2015). The bilingual advantage: Acta est fabula. Cortex, 73, 371–372. 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farroni T, Massaccesi S, Pividori D, & Johnson MH (2004). Gaze following in newborns. Infancy, 5(1), 39–60. 10.1207/s15327078in0501_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fennell CT, Byers-Heinlein K, & Werker JF (2007). Using speech sounds to guide word learning: The case of bilingual infants. Child Development, 78(5), 1510–1525. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01080.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenson L, Marchman VA, Thal D, Dale P, Reznick J, & Bates E (2007). MacArthur-bates communicative development inventories (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. [Google Scholar]
- Frank MC, Bergelson E, Bergmann C, Cristia A, Floccia C, Gervain J, . . . Yurovsky D (2017). A collaborative approach to infant research: Promoting reproducibility, best practices, and theory-building. Infancy, 22(4), 421–435. 10.1111/infa.12182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frischen A, Bayliss AP, & Tipper SP (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 694–724. 10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graf Estes K, & Hay JF (2015). Flexibility in bilingual infants’ word learning. Child Development, 86(5), 1371–1385. 10.1111/cdev.12392 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gredebäck G, Fikke L, & Melinder A (2010). The development of joint visual attention: A longitudinal study of gaze following during interactions with mothers and strangers. Developmental Science, 13(6), 839–848. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00945.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halberda J (2003). The development of a word-learning strategy. Cognition, 87(1), B23–B34. 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00186-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henrich J, Heine SJ, & Norenzayan A (2010). Most people are not weird. Nature, 466(7302), 29–29. 10.1038/466029a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernik M, & Broesch T (2019). Infant gaze following depends on communicative signals: An eye-tracking study of 5- to 7-month-olds in vanuatu. Developmental Science, 22(4), e12779. 10.1111/desc.12779 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hessels RS, Andersson R, Hooge ITC, Nyström M, & Kemner C (2015). Consequences of eye color, positioning, and head movement for eye-tracking data quality in infant research. Infancy, 20(6), 601–633. 10.1111/infa.12093 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hollich GJ, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM, Brand RJ, Brown E, Chung HL, . . . Bloom L (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65(3), i–35. 10.1111/1540-5834.00090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houston-Price C, Caloghiris Z, & Raviglione E (2010). Language experience shapes the development of the mutual exclusivity bias. Infancy, 15(2), 125–150. 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00009.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ioannidis JPA (2012). Why science is not necessarily self-correcting. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 645–654. 10.1177/1745691612464056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandhadai P, Danielson DK, & Werker JF (2014). Culture as a binder for bilingual acquisition. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 3(1), 24–27. 10.1016/j.tine.2014.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Klein RM (2015). Is there a benefit of bilingualism for executive functioning? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 29–31. 10.1017/S1366728914000613 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kovács ÁM, & Mehler J (2009a). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(16), 6556–6560. 10.1073/pnas.0811323106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovács ÁM, & Mehler J (2009b). Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science, 325, 611–612. 10.1126/science.1173947 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeVine RA, Dixon S, LeVine S, Richman A, Leiderman PH, Keefer CH, & Brazelton TB (1994). Child care and culture: Lessons from africa. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- LeVine RA, & LeVine S (2016). Do parents matter? New York, NY: Public Affairs. [Google Scholar]
- LeVine RA, & Norman K (2001). The infant’s acquisition of culture: Early attachment re-examined in anthropological perspective. In Moore CC & Mathews HF (Eds.), The psychology of cultural experience (pp. 83–104). Cambridge, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Liberman Z, Woodward AL, Keysar B, & Kinzler KD (2017). Exposure to multiple languages enhances communication skills in infancy. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12420. 10.1111/desc.12420 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Little EE, Carver LJ, & Legare CH (2016). Cultural variation in triadic infant–caregiver object exploration. Child Development, 87(4), 1130–1145. 10.1111/cdev.12513 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu L, & Kager R (2016). Perception of a native vowel contrast by dutch monolingual and bilingual infants: A bilingual perceptual lead. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20(3), 335–345. 10.1177/1367006914566082 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- ManyBabies Consortium. (2020). A multi-lab study of bilingual infants: Exploring the preference for infant-directed speech. Preprint. 10.31234/osf.io/sqh9d [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Markman EM (1990). Constraints children place on word meanings. Cognitive Science, 14(1), 57–77. 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Markman EM, & Wachtel GF (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121–157. 10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markman EM, Wasow JL, & Hansen MB (2003). Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners. Cognitive Psychology, 47(3), 241–275. 10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00034-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matuschek H, Kliegl R, Vasishth S, Baayen H, & Bates D (2017). Balancing type i error and power in linear mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 305–315. 10.1016/j.jml.2017.01.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzoff AN, & Brooks R (2007). Eyes wide shut: The importance of eyes in infant gaze-following and understanding other minds. In Flom R, Lee K, & Muir D (Eds.), Gaze-following: Its development and significance (pp. 217–241). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Moll H, & Tomasello M (2004). 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. Developmental Science, 7(1), F1–F9. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00315.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore C (2008). The development of gaze following. Child Development Perspectives, 2(2), 66–70. 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00052.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moore C, & Corkum V (1998). Infant gaze following based on eye direction. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(4), 495–503. 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00767.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Morales M, Mundy P, Delgado CEF, Yale M, Neal R, & Schwartz HK (2000). Gaze following, temperament, and language development in 6-month-olds: A replication and extension. Infant Behavior and Development, 23(2), 231–236. 10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00038-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mundy P, Block J, Delgado C, Pomares Y, Van Hecke AV, & Parlade MV (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–954. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mundy P, Sullivan L, & Mastergeorge AM (2009). A parallel and distributed-processing model of joint attention, social cognition and autism. Autism Research, 2(1), 2–21. 10.1002/aur.61 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paap KR, Johnson HA, & Sawi O (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus M, & Fikkert P (2014). Conflicting social cues: Fourteen- and 24-month-old infants’ reliance on gaze and pointing cues in word learning. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(1), 43–59. 10.1080/15248372.2012.698435 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson BZ, Fernández SC, & Oller DK (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43(1), 93–120. 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Petitto LA, Katerelos M, Levy BG, Gauna K, Tétreault K, & Ferraro V (2001). Bilingual signed and spoken language acquisition from birth: Implications for the mechanisms underlying early bilingual language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 28(2), 453–496. 10.1017/S0305000901004718 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pons F, Bosch L, & Lewkowicz DJ (2015). Bilingualism modulates infants’ selective attention to the mouth of a talking face. Psychological Science, 26(4), 490–498. 10.1177/0956797614568320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid VM, Striano T, Kaufman J, & Johnson MH (2004). Eye gaze cueing facilitates neural processing of objects in 4-month-old infants. NeuroReport, 15(16), 2553–2555. 10.1097/00001756-200411150-00025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schonberg C, Sandhofer CM, Tsang T, & Johnson SP (2014). Does bilingual experience affect early visual perceptual development? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1429. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01429 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sebastián-Gallés N, Albareda-Castellot B, Weikum WM, & Werker JF (2012). A bilingual advantage in visual language discrimination in infancy. Psychological Science, 23(9), 994–999. 10.1177/0956797612436817 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senju A, & Csibra G (2008). Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology, 18(9), 668–671. 10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senju A, Tucker L, Pasco G, Hudry K, Elsabbagh M, Charman T, & Johnson MH (2013). The importance of the eyes: Communication skills in infants of blind parents. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1760), 1–7. 10.1098/rspb.2013.0436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senju A, Vernetti A, Ganea N, Hudry K, Tucker L, Charman T, & Johnson MH (2015). Early social experience affects the development of eye gaze processing. Current Biology, 25(23), 3086–3091. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh L, Fu CSL, Rahman AA, Hameed WB, Sanmugam S, Agarwal P, . . . GUSTO Research Team. (2015). Back to basics: A bilingual advantage in infant visual habituation. Child Development, 86(1), 294–302. 10.1111/cdev.12271 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh L, Fu CSL, Tay ZW, & Golinkoff RM (2018). Novel word learning in bilingual and monolingual infants: Evidence for a bilingual advantage. Child Development, 89(3), e183–e198. 10.1111/cdev.12747 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh L, Quinn PC, Xiao NG, & Lee K (2019). Monolingual but not bilingual infants demonstrate racial bias in social cue use. Developmental Science, 22(6), e12809. 10.1111/desc.12809 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szufnarowska J, Rohlfing KJ, Fawcett C, & Gredebäck G (2014). Is ostension any more than attention? Scientific Reports, 4(5304), 1–4. 10.1038/srep05304 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tenenbaum EJ, Sobel DM, Sheinkopf SJ, Malle BF, & Morgan JL (2015). Attention to the mouth and gaze following in infancy predict language development. Journal of Child Language, 42(6), 1173–1190. 10.1017/S0305000914000725 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tronick E (2007). The neurobehavioral and social-emotional development of infants and children. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Co. [Google Scholar]
- van Renswoude DR, Raijmakers MEJ, Koornneef A, Johnson SP, Hunnius S, & Visser I (2018). Gazepath: An eye-tracking analysis tool that accounts for individual differences and data quality. Behavior Research Methods, 50(2), 834–852. 10.3758/s13428-017-0909-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viechtbauer W (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in r with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 1–48. 10.18637/jss.v036.i03 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wass SV, Smith TJ, & Johnson MH (2013). Parsing eye-tracking data of variable quality to provide accurate fixation duration estimates in infants and adults. Behavior Research Methods, 45(1), 229–250. 10.3758/s13428-012-0245-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weikum WM, Vouloumanos A, Navarra J, Soto-Faraco S, Sebastián-Gallés N, & Werker JF (2007). Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science, 316(5828), 1159. 10.1126/science.1137686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werker JF, & Byers-Heinlein K (2008). Bilingualism in infancy: First steps in perception and comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 144–151. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodward AL (2003). Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object. Developmental Science, 6(3), 297–311. 10.1111/1467-7687.00286 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yow WQ, Li X, Lam S, Gliga T, Chong YS, Kwek K, & Broekman BFP (2017). A bilingual advantage in 54-month-olds’use of referential cues in fast mapping. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12482. 10.1111/desc.12482 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yow WQ, & Markman EM (2011). Young bilingual children’s heightened sensitivity to referential cues. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(1), 12–31. 10.1080/15248372.2011.539524 [DOI] [Google Scholar]