Skip to main content
Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2022 Jan 20:1–5. Online ahead of print. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02574-x

Profiles of family pet ownership during the COVID-19 pandemic

Eli D Halbreich 1,, Megan K Mueller 2,3
PMCID: PMC8769778  PMID: 35075340

The pandemic caused by SARS-CoV-2 (COVID-19) has affected both people and animals globally (Bowen et al., 2020). Research on COVID-19 with both children and adults has indicated negative psychological impacts on mental health outcomes and stress (Odriozola-González et al., 2020; Rothe et al., 2021; Saddik et al., 2021), and significant variation in mental health outcomes by demographic variables (Pierce et al., 2020).

Pets are an underexplored source of emotional support that may foster resilience within families in times of stress (Bowen et al., 2020; Bussolari et al., 2021; Chadwin, 2017). However, research investigating the mental health benefits of pet ownership prior to COVID-19 has produced mixed outcomes (e.g., Powell et al., 2019) and further research is needed to elucidate the potential benefits of companion animals (Gee & Mueller, 2019; Wells, 2019). Human-pet relationships have the potential to be amplified—both positively and negatively—during times of crisis. For example, the effects of unemployment and resource scarcity during the pandemic may intersect significantly with pet ownership. The perception that companion animals may reduce loneliness may be driving increased pet acquisition, and some initial data have suggested that adoption rates increased during the pandemic (Morgan et al., 2020). However, relinquishment of pets may be related to economic stress associated with the pandemic, suggesting a need to explore transitions in pet ownership in the context of psychological well-being.

Existing research on pets and COVID-19 suggests that it is imperative to understand sociodemographic profiles of pet ownership during times of increased societal stress, to better understand how pets contribute to psychological well-being for diverse families. Therefore, this study aims to extend existing work with convenience samples to explore the role of pets during COVID-19 using a nationally-representative sample of United States families to assess if there are systematic sociodemographic differences between families with and without pets during COVID-19, and explore if these differences are related to acquisition of a pet.

Methods

Participants and Procedure

This study used data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study®. We analyzed youth data from the ABCD COVID Rapid Response Research Survey, which was administered electronically three times: May 16–22, June 24–27, and August 4–5, 2020. Parent-reported demographic data are from the ABCD Annual Curated Release 3.0, collected between September 1, 2016 and February 15, 2020.

For this analysis, we used a subsample of 8,158 participants who had data for pet ownership in at least one of the three surveys. Youth reported their age (ranging from 10.6 to 14.8 years) and gender. Parents/guardians reported their child’s racial/ethnic identities, which were combined into non-mutually exclusive analytic categories of White, Black/African American, Asian, Indigenous, and Hispanic. See Table 1 for demographic data.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics/frequencies for demographic/contextual characteristics, stratified by pet ownership status

Non-Pet Owners Pet Owners Dog Owners Cat Owners Other Pet Owners Pet to No Pet No Pet to Pet
n = 2,099 n = 5,849 n = 3,032 n = 1,705 n = 301 n = 106 n = 191
M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD)
Household Income 6.93 (2.51) 7.87 (1.95) 7.88 (1.92) 7.64 (2.04) 7.62 (2.04) 7.21 (2.31) 7.14 (2.38)
Sex n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%)
 Female 1,044 (45.2%) 2,921 (49.9%) 1,533 (50.6%) 896 (52.6%) 163 (51.3%) 48 (45.3%) 105 (55.0%)
 Male 1,265 (54.8%) 2,928 (50.1%) 1,499 (49.4%) 809 (47.4%) 155 (48.7%) 58 (54.7%) 86 (45.0%)
Race/Ethnicity
 White 1,426 (61.8%) 5,077 (86.8%) 2,640 (87.1%) 1,543 (90.5%) 279 (87.7%) 77 (72.6%) 135 (70.7%)
 Black 713 (30.9%) 618 (10.6%) 317 (10.5%) 137 (8.0%) 40 (12.6%) 21 (19.8%) 46 (24.1%)
 Asian 218 (9.4%) 386 (6.6%) 200 (6.6%) 103 (6.0%) 15 (4.7%) 9 (8.5%) 15 (7.9%)
 Indigenous 52 (2.3%) 210 (3.6%) 121 (4.0%) 64 (3.8%) 12 (3.8%) 3 (2.8%) 7 (3.7%)
 Other Race 187 (8.1%) 325 (5.6%) 158 (5.2%) 70 (4.1%) 15 (4.7%) 6 (5.7%) 17 (8.9%)
 Hispanic 501 (21.7%) 958 (16.4%) 517 (17.1%) 238 (14.0%) 50 (15.7%) 25 (23.6%) 51 (26.7%)
Education
 HS or less 379 (16.4%) 580 (9.9%) 309 (10.2%) 179 (10.5%) 41 (12.9%) 12 (11.3%) 23 (12.0%)
 Some College 1,326 (57.4%) 3,463 (59.2%) 1,801 (59.4%) 1,088 (63.8%) 199 (62.6%) 64 (60.4%) 114 (59.7%)
 Graduate degree +  601 (26.0%) 1,799 (30.8%) 919 (30.3%) 436 (25.6%) 78 (24.5%) 30 (28.3%) 54 (28.3%)
Experienced scarcity
 No scarcity 1,793 (77.7%) 4,936 (84.4%) 2,559 (84.4%) 1,397 (81.9%) 259 (81.4%) 88 (83.0%) 151 (79.1%)
 Scarcity 516 (22.3%) 913 (15.6%) 473 (15.6%) 308 (18.1%) 59 (18.6%) 18 (17.0%) 40 (20.9%)
 Employment
 Not employed 728 (31.5%) 1,424 (24.3%) 719 (23.7%) 436 (25.6%) 84 (26.4%) 31 (29.2%) 51 (26.7%)
 Employed 1,547 (67.0%) 4,299 (73.5%) 2,242 (73.9%) 1,219 (71.5%) 222 (69.8%) 74 (69.8%) 135 (70.7%)

Dog, cat, and pet ownership categories are not mutually exclusive

Measures

Household Income

Parents/guardians reported their combined family income for the past 12 months on a 10-point scale (1 = Less than $5,000; 10 = $200,000 +).

Parent Education

Parents/guardians reported their highest level of education in 20 categories from never attended/kindergarten through professional/doctoral degree, which was recoded into three categories (see Table 1).

Scarcity

Parents/guardians were asked to report whether they had experienced specific instances of scarcity in the past year: could not afford: food, telephone service, rent/mortgage, utilities, medical care, dental care; were evicted for not paying rent/mortgage. A dichotomous composite variable was created to indicate if families had experienced at least one of these circumstances.

Employment

Parents/guardians reported their current employment status in 12 categories, which was recoded into a dichotomous variable (employed, unemployed).

Pet Ownership

Youth were asked whether they currently have any pets (1 = Yes; 0 = No), and to specify the type/species of pets. Participants were considered an overall pet owner if they consistently had a pet during the three time points (with similar coding by species for dog, cat, and other pet categories). To assess transitions in pet ownership, participants were categorized as 1) consistent pet owners (had a pet at all time points with data), 2) consistent non-pet owners (no pet at any time point), 3) pet to no pet (had a pet at the first and/or second time point but not the second and/or third time point), and 4) no pet to pet (did not have a pet at the first and/or second time point but had a pet at the second and/or third time point).

Data Analysis

Analyses were performed in SPSS 27.0. Logistic regression was used to assess how sociodemographic variables predicted overall pet ownership, pet ownership by species, and pet ownership consistency status. Prior to analysis, variables were tested for the necessary assumptions for regression modeling. To interpret significance, we report p-values, odds ratios, and 95% confidence intervals.

Results

Regression analyses indicated that odds of pet ownership differed significantly on several sociodemographic variables. Higher odds of pet ownership were associated with identifying as White or Indigenous, and income was positively associated with pet ownership. Being male, Black, Asian, Hispanic, and having a parent with a graduate degree were all associated with lower odds of pet ownership. There were some differential patterns by species for dog and cat owners regarding race/ethnicity, experiences of scarcity, and employment (see Table 2). Participants whose family transitioned out of pet ownership during the pandemic were less likely to be White and more likely to have reported lower household income compared to consistent pet owners. Male participants were more likely to have acquired a pet compared to female participants. Table 2 includes full regression results.

Table 2.

Logistic regression results for demographic/contextual variables predicting pet ownership by species and transitions in and out of pet ownership during COVID-19

Pet Ownership Dog Ownership Cat Ownership Pet to no pet (lost pet) vs. pet consistent No pet to pet (got pet) vs. no pet consistent
OR p 95% C.I. for OR OR p 95% C.I. for OR OR p 95% C.I. for OR OR p 95% C.I. for OR OR p 95% C.I. for OR
Sex
 Female*
 Male 0.78  < 0.001 0.70, 0.87 0.88 0.007 0.80, 0.97 0.80  < 0.001 0.71, 0.90 1.11 0.61 0.74, 1.67 0.63 0.005 0.46, 0.87
Race/Ethnicity**
 White 2.43  < 0.001 2.03, 2.92 1.75  < 0.001 1.44, 2.11 2.19  < 0.001 1.70, 2.82 0.33 0.003 0.16, 0.69 1.29 0.36 0.75, 2.20
 Black 0.46  < 0.001 0.38, 0.55 0.70  < 0.001 0.58, 0.85 0.48  < 0.001 0.37, 0.62 0.90 0.80 0.41, 1.98 0.81 0.46 0.47, 1.41
 Asian 0.73 0.003 0.59, 0.89 0.89 0.24 0.73, 1.08 0.93 0.57 0.73, 1.19 0.96 0.93 0.42, 2.22 0.79 0.48 0.41, 1.52
 Indigenous 2.02  < 0.001 1.43, 2.85 1.68  < 0.001 1.28, 2.20 1.19 0.28 0.87, 1.64 0.48 0.31 0.11, 1.97 1.64 0.27 0.68, 4.00
 Other Race 1.38 0.01 1.07, 1.79 0.99 0.92 0.77, 1.27 0.96 0.80 0.70, 1.32 0.22 0.02 0.06, 0.81 0.81 0.57 0.40, 1.66
 Hispanic 0.69  < 0.001 0.59, 0.80 0.96 0.53 0.83, 1.10 0.66  < 0.001 0.55, 0.79 1.49 0.16 0.86, 2.59 1.51 0.05 1.00, 2.28
Parent Education
HS or less*
 Some College 0.86 0.13 0.71, 1.04 0.89 0.21 0.74, 1.07 1.01 0.96 0.81, 1.25 1.74 0.17 0.80, 3.79 1.29 0.38 0.73, 2.28
 Graduate degree 0.77 0.02 0.62, 0.97 0.81 0.05 0.66, 1.00 0.74 0.02 0.58, 0.95 2.07 0.11 0.85, 5.04 1.27 0.48 0.65, 2.48
 Household Income 1.11  < 0.001 1.08, 1.15 1.07  < 0.001 1.04, 1.10 0.95 0.005 0.91, 0.98 0.84 0.002 0.75, 0.94 1.01 0.79 0.92, 1.11
Experienced scarcity
No scarcity*
 Scarcity 1.13 0.13 0.97, 1.32 1.04 0.61 0.90, 1.20 1.20 0.03 1.01, 1.42 0.69 0.23 0.37, 1.27 0.98 0.95 0.63, 1.54
Employment
 Not employed*
 Employed 1.34  < 0.001 1.18, 1.52 1.22 0.001 1.09, 1.37 1.10 0.17 0.96, 1.26 1.00 0.99 0.61, 1.63 1.29 0.19 0.88, 1.88

*indicates reference group category; OR = odds ratio; **no reference group for race/ethnicity; non-mutually exclusive categories

Discussion

Results indicated that there are significant systematic differences in the experience of pet ownership based on sociodemographic variables. Specifically, the findings related to families transitioning from having a pet to not having a pet during COVID-19 point to potential systemic inequities and economic factors (e.g., access to veterinary care and job loss; Applebaum et al., 2020) driving some relinquishments. These findings are similar to other studies looking at sociodemographic factors in pet ownership from prior to COVID-19 (e.g., Mueller et al., 2021). Additionally, a relatively small number of families changed their pet ownership status, which could indicate the presence of protective factors—such as enhanced unemployment insurance relief—or the importance of pets within the family context.

A limitation of this analysis is that we do not know the reasons why families either lost (through relinquishment, death, or other reasons) or acquired (through a relative or friend, or adoption) pets during COVID-19. This study provides initial evidence that pet ownership is a factor in the lives of many families but there is significant inter-individual variation. Although these findings are within the context of COVID-19, external and internal stressors will continue to affect families going forward, and so the precise role of pets within the family needs further exploration in the context of psychological well-being.

Authors' Contributions

Conceptualization: Eli D. Halbreich, Megan K. Mueller; Methodology: Megan K. Mueller; Formal analysis and investigation: Megan K. Mueller; Writing—original draft preparation: Eli D. Halbreich; Writing—review and editing: Megan K. Mueller; Funding acquisition: Megan K. Mueller; Resources: Megan K. Mueller; Supervision: Megan K. Mueller.

Funding

The analyses for this study were funded by the Human-Animal Bond Research Institute (HABRI), grant #HAB19-001. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of HABRI.

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. Additional support for this work was made possible from supplements to U24DA041123 and U24DA041147, the National Science Foundation (NSF 2028680), and Children and Screens: Institute of Digital Media and Child Development Inc.

The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ABCD data used in this report came from the ABCD Annual Curated Release 3.0 (https://doi.org/10.15154/1519007) and the COVID Rapid Response Research (RRR) Survey First data release (https://doi.org/10.15154/1520584).

Data Availability

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the ABCD Annual Curated Release 3.0 (https://doi.org/10.15154/1519007) and the COVID Rapid Response Research (RRR) Survey First data release (https://doi.org/10.15154/1520584).

Code Availability

Analyses were performed in SPSS (IBM Corp. Released 2020. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp). Analysis code is publicly available via Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/hct46/?view_only=480dd9ff3a9446d5927545a6a583d785.

Declarations

Conflicts of Interest/Competing Interests

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Ethics Approval

This study was approved by each local ethics committee of the relevant institutions, including but not limited to the University of California, San Diego (ABCD Study Coordinating Center). The procedures used in this study adhere to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent to Participate

All adult participants (parents/guardians) provided written informed consent, and all youth provided written assent along with permission from a parent/guardian.

Consent for Publication

Participants signed informed consent regarding publishing their data as part of their participation in the ABCD Study.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Applebaum JW, Tomlinson CA, Matijczak A, McDonald SE, Zsembik BA. The concerns, difficulties, and stressors of caring for pets during COVID-19: Results from a large survey of U.S. pet owners. Animals. 2020;10:1882. doi: 10.3390/ani10101882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bowen J, García E, Darder P, Argüelles J, Fatjó J. The effects of the Spanish COVID-19 lockdown on people, their pets, and the human-animal bond. Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 2020;40:75–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2020.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bussolari C, Currin-McCulloch J, Packman W, Kogan L, Erdman P. "I couldn't have asked for a better quarantine partner!": Experiences with companion dogs during Covid-19. Animals. 2021;11(2):330. doi: 10.3390/ani11020330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Chadwin R. Evacuation of pets during disasters: A public health intervention to increase resilience. American Journal of Public Health. 2017;107(9):1413–1417. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2017.303877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Gee NR, Mueller MK. A systematic review of research on pet ownership and animal interactions among older adults. Anthrozoös. 2019;32(2):183–207. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2019.1569903. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Morgan L, Protopopova A, Birkler RID, Itin-Shwartz B, Sutton GA, Gamliel A, Yakobson B, Raz T. Human–dog relationships during the COVID-19 pandemic: Booming dog adoption during social isolation. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications. 2020;7:155. doi: 10.1057/s41599-020-00649-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Mueller MK, King EK, Callina K, Dowling-Guyer S, McCobb E. Demographic and contextual factors as moderators of the relationship between pet ownership and health. Health Psychology and Behavioral Medicine. 2021;9(1):701–723. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2021.1963254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Odriozola-González P, Planchuelo-Gómez Á, Irurtia MJ, de Luis-García R. Psychological effects of the COVID-19 outbreak and lockdown among students and workers of a Spanish university. Psychiatry Research. 2020;290:113108. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Pierce M, Hope H, Ford T, Hatch S, Hotopf M, John A, Kontopantelis E, Webb R, Wessely S, McManus S, Abel KM. Mental health before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: A longitudinal probability sample survey of the UK population. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7(10):883–892. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(20)30308-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Powell L, Edwards KM, McGreevy P, Bauman A, Podberscek A, Neilly B, Sherrington C, Stamatakis E. Companion dog acquisition and mental well-being: A community-based three-arm controlled study. BMC Public Health. 2019;19:1428. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-7770-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Rothe J, Buse J, Uhlmann A, Bluschke A, Roessner V. Changes in emotions and worries during the Covid-19 pandemic: An online-survey with children and adults with and without mental health conditions. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health. 2021;15:11. doi: 10.1186/s13034-021-00363-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Saddik B, Hussein A, Albanna A, Elbarazi I, Al-Shujairi A, Temsah M-H, Saheb Sharif-Askari F, Stip E, Hamid Q, Halwani R. The psychological impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on adults and children in the United Arab Emirates: A nationwide cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry. 2021;21:224. doi: 10.1186/s12888-021-03213-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Wells DL. The state of research on human–animal relations: Implications for human health. Anthrozoös. 2019;32(2):169–181. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2019.1569902. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the ABCD Annual Curated Release 3.0 (https://doi.org/10.15154/1519007) and the COVID Rapid Response Research (RRR) Survey First data release (https://doi.org/10.15154/1520584).

Analyses were performed in SPSS (IBM Corp. Released 2020. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp). Analysis code is publicly available via Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/hct46/?view_only=480dd9ff3a9446d5927545a6a583d785.


Articles from Current Psychology (New Brunswick, N.j.) are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES