Skip to main content
Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI logoLink to Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI
. 2022 Jan 7;12(2):141. doi: 10.3390/ani12020141

Effects of Environmental Enrichment on Dog Behaviour: Pilot Study

Rebecca L Hunt 1,*, Helen Whiteside 1, Susanne Prankel 2
Editors: Wendy Y Brown, Peta Taylor, Megan Verdon, Mandy Paterson
PMCID: PMC8772568  PMID: 35049764

Abstract

Simple Summary

The quality of life for domestic and captive animals can be enhanced and improved by providing additional stimuli and activities, known as environmental enrichment (EE). However, it is important to ensure the enrichment provides benefit to the animal, and as such the behaviour of the animals can be monitored to understand the activities’ impacts. A pilot study was undertaken to evaluate the impact of seven different EE activities on the behaviour of 10 training dogs housed in an office environment during training within an assistance dog charity. Results suggest there is a need to diversify thinking around EE, shifting common thinking of EE as one entity to instead consider EE in different categories and to ensure animals receive a mixture of EE types.

Abstract

Environmental enrichment (EE) can be used to enhance the environment of various animals. The aim of this pilot study was to determine the effects of seven EE activities (Bonding, Bubble machine, Conspecific play, Interactive toy, Playhouse, Stuffed food toy and Tug play) on dog behaviour, pre- and post-EE for dogs housed in an office environment during training as part of an assistance dog training programme. EE activities resulted in a significant increase in the frequency of relaxation behaviours (p < 0.01) and a significant reduction in alert (p < 0.01) and stress behaviours (p = 0.02). Results suggest various benefits of the different activities with Conspecific Play and Playhouse activities having the greatest overall positive behaviour change when compared to the other activities. The food-based EE activities (Interactive toy and Stuffed food toy) had the least behaviour change of all the activities provided. Findings will be of interest to pet owners, animal rescue centres, dog trainers and working dog organisations.

Keywords: environmental enrichment, dog, behaviour

1. Introduction

There are many definitions of environmental enrichment (EE) within the literature, but EE is commonly described as a technique designed to enhance the quality of life in captive and domestic animals by providing additional and temporary environmental stimuli to promote psychological and physiological wellbeing [1,2,3]. EE has been shown to be beneficial to the wellbeing of species including rats [4], pigs [5], cats [6] and geckos [7].

EE is used as a standard part of dog husbandry across a range of environments including the home [8], kennels [9,10,11,12,13] and laboratories [14]. The reported benefits of EE for dogs include reduced stress [10,15], decreases in stereotypic and abnormal behaviours [10,12,14], increased relaxation [16], improved cognitive abilities [3,13] and reduced barking or vocalisations [13,15,16]. Recently there has also been an increase in the number of studies reporting or reviewing the use of different EE types (e.g., the use of olfactory [16,17] and auditory EE [16,18]) to improve dog welfare, particularly in rehoming or shelter environments [19].

To support the development of best practice guidance for clinicians, practitioners and pet dog owners, a pilot study was undertaken to evaluate the impact of seven different EE activities on the behaviour of 10 training dogs in an office environment within an assistance dog charity. The aim of this study was to examine the behaviour of dogs pre- and post-enrichment.

2. Materials and Methods

A group of assistance dogs in training were the subjects of this study. A total of 10 neutered dogs (4M/6F) aged 12 to 14 months from two breeds (nine Golden Retriever Sire × Labradors Dam, and one German Shepherd Dog Sire × Golden Retriever Dam) were bred and reared as part of an assistance dog training programme. Prior to entering training, the dogs were raised by volunteer puppy raisers from eight weeks of age. Throughout training, the dogs were housed in an office environment when not undertaking formal training, and during the evenings and weekends were housed in volunteer homes.

Data were collected over a period of eight weeks. Individual dogs were exposed to each of the seven EE activities (Table 1) twice during the study in a randomised order. Activities were performed in an allocated activity room (approximately 5 m × 4 m in size) familiar to the dogs, with the exception of the stuffed food toy which was given to subjects in their pen. Dogs were supervised at all times during the activities by the assessor. Behavioural data were collected using continuous video recordings of the dogs in their individual pen for two 15-min time periods; one prior to the activity (pre-EE) and another post-activity (post-EE). The time of 15 minutes was chosen to align with the time (approximately one-hour slots) that dogs would spend in a pen when not undertaking training.

Table 1.

A description of the EE activities and handling protocol for assessment.

EE Activity Handling Protocol
Bonding Handler sits in room providing dog with physical contact, stroking and grooming for 15 min if dog chooses to engage in the interaction
Bubble machine Bubble machine blowing bacon flavour bubbles placed in room with dog for 15 min
Conspecific play Dog has supervised interaction for 15 min with a known conspecific
Interactive toy Food puzzle game provided until all treats retrieved from toy or after 15 min
Playhouse Playhouse consisting of tunnels, slides and platforms provided for the dogs to explore for 15 min with handler providing encouragement when needed
Stuffed food toy Stuffed food toy provided until all food removed by subject or after 15 min
Tug play Known handler engages in 15 min play with the dog through tug and fetch games using soft toys and rope knots

Behavioural frequencies were recorded using an ethogram. Behaviours that were displayed for up to 15 s were included as one count. For any behaviour that was continuously displayed beyond 15 s additional counts were recorded. Related behaviours were grouped under six behavioural categories (maintenance, play, locomotion, relaxation, alert and stress; see Supplementary Table S1 for behaviours). The same observer (the corresponding author) was used for the creation of the ethogram and all behavioural analyses to exclude assessor bias. Frequencies of behaviours were averaged from the two observations to create a single pre and post EE value for each category of behaviour per dog. An overall behavioural change score for each behaviour category was then calculated by subtracting the pre-EE value from the post-EE value for each dog. As such, a negative behavioural change score was associated with a decrease in the frequency of the behaviour post-EE activities. A positive behavioural change score was associated with an increase in the frequency of the behaviour post-EE activities.

To establish the impact of EE type on each behavioural category, general linear mixed effect models were run with EE type as a fixed effect and dog as a random effect. LSD post hoc pairwise analyses were used to compare the behavioural change scores between each EE type following a significant result. Frequencies were reported as mean ± one standard error. Results were considered significant when p < 0.05. All analyses were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows version 22.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA).

The research project was approved by The University of Worcester’s Ethics Committee. All procedures adhered to the assistance dog organisation’s welfare and ethical policies. Dog behaviour was continuously monitored throughout all EE activities with a plan for any activity to be stopped should a dog show any signs of distress.

3. Results

Due to the infrequent display of maintenance (with the exception of drinking post-EE), play and locomotion behaviours during pre- and post-EE videos (see Supplementary Table S2), the behaviours were not included in further statistical analyses. EE activities resulted in a significant increase in the frequency of relaxation behaviours (d.f. = 6, F = 6.48, p < 0.01; Figure 1a) and a significant reduction in alert (d.f. = 6, F = 4.16, p < 0.01; Figure 1b) and stress behaviours (d.f. = 6, F = 2.79, p = 0.02; Figure 1c). For mean behavioural change scores for each EE activity and behaviour category, see Supplementary Table S2.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Figure 1

Mean behavioural change scores for (a) relaxation (b) alert and (c) stress behaviours for the seven EE activities. 95% CI bars are shown.

A visual representation of the significant behaviour changes for each EE activity when compared to the other activities is shown in Table 2. Relaxation behavioural change scores were significantly higher for all activities when compared to the food-based EE activities (Stuffed food toy and Interactive toy). Alert behavioural change scores were significantly lower for Conspecific play when compared to all other EE except for the Bubble machine. There was also a significant reduction in alert behaviours for Bonding, Bubble machine, Conspecific play and Playhouse when compared to the Stuffed food toy. Stress behavioural change scores were significantly lower for Conspecific play, Playhouse and Tug play when compared to the Stuffed food toy. Additionally, the Playhouse activity resulted in significantly lower stress behavioural change scores compared to Bonding. There were no other significant differences between the EE types and behavioural change scores. For significance values for all comparisons, see Supplementary Table S3.

Table 2.

Summary of behaviour change scores when comparing each EE activity to one another using LSD post hoc pairwise analyses for alert (A), relaxation (R) and stress (S) behaviour categories. A green square indicates a significant increase for relaxation behaviours or a significant decrease for alert and stress behaviours. A red square indicates a significant decrease for relaxation behaviours or a significant increase for alert and stress behaviours. A black square indicates the same activity, so no comparisons were undertaken.

EE Activity Bonding Bubble Machine Conspecific play Interactive toy Playhouse Stuffed Food Toy Tug Play
Behaviour Category A R S A R S A R S A R S A R S A R S A R S
Compared to Bonding
Compared to Bubble machine
Compared to Conspecific play
Compared to Interactive toy
Compared to Playhouse
Compared to Stuffed food toy
Compared to Tug play

4. Discussion

For an environmental intervention to be considered enriching, the changes it produces must be linked to an improvement in an animals’ state. Therefore, measuring behaviours is essential when examining whether environmental interventions can be considered successful [20]. Relaxation, alert and stress behaviours in dogs are widely used indicators of psychological welfare [21,22,23]. Despite the importance of EE, there are few controlled studies assessing the effect of multiple enrichment activities on dog welfare in different environments [24]. This pilot study presents the first results examining the impact of individual EE activities on dog behaviour in an office environment within an assistance dog training programme. Findings from this study suggested that Conspecific play and Playhouse activities resulted in the greatest behaviour change compared with all other activities. The smallest behaviour change overall was observed for food-based activities (Stuffed food toy and Interactive toy).

The impact of EE activities on dog behaviour varies dependent upon the type of activity and the animal’s mental state. Food-based EE activities are frequently provided for dogs [10,13,25], yet the relative welfare benefits reported are varied. A recent study reported feeding enrichment to result in reduced stereotypies and increased activity levels for kennelled dogs [26]. Similarly, Schipper et al. [13] found stuffed food toys and treat games reduced barking frequencies and increased activity levels for dogs housed in kennels. However, for some working dogs it would appear that the feeding enrichment is not as impactful. Gaines et al. [25] suggested a stuffed food toy had minimal impact on the behaviour and working performance of military dogs and so was of limited welfare value. Similarly, this study found the food-based EE activities (Stuffed food toy and Interactive toy) to have the least behaviour impacts overall for a group of assistance dogs when compared to the other activities. The food-based EE items were removed from the dogs when all food had been eaten so the toys were not chewed. As such, the time spent interacting with the EE was variable for individual dogs depending on how quickly the food was eaten. The 15-min pre and post EE behaviour change remained consistent for all EE assessed. The dogs in this study were not permanently housed in kennels which may explain the reduced impact of food-based EE when compared to dogs housed permanently in kennels during training. The results for food-based EE may also vary by breed or individual dogs based on food motivation levels.

There are reported benefits for dogs when provided with social contact, either conspecifics or humans [27,28]. Social contact EE activities are associated with decreased stereotypy, greater sociability, reduced periods of inactivity, decreased cortisol concentrations and increased relaxation [29,30,31,32]. Wells and Hepper [27] found social stimulation had a greater positive impact on dog behaviour compared to the provision of toys, suggesting that providing a range of EE activities is likely to produce the greatest benefit. This study also supports these findings as the Conspecific play activity had the greatest overall behavioural changes for any EE activities (see Table 2 and Table S2). The variation in behavioural responses to different activities suggests that a combination of EE activities enables dogs to display a wider range of natural behaviours.

The Playhouse had the highest mean behavioural change score in stress behaviours and the Bubble machine had the second highest mean behavioural change scores for relaxation and alert behaviours (see Table S2) The Playhouse and Bubble machine were completely novel EEs provided to the dogs as part of the study. Providing new EE activities and rotating them is important to maximise benefits and prevent habituation [1]. It has also been suggested that if a break is provided from EE presentation, the effect of any habituation can recover, and the animal can successfully reinvestigate the EE [1]. The Bubble machine also added scent EE in the form of bacon scented bubbles. Studies have shown the benefits of utilising scent such as lavender [16,17] and rabbit scent [17]. EE activities that influence multiple senses could have additional positive impacts on behaviour.

There were some limitations to this study as it was a small pilot study including 10 dogs and therefore results should be interpreted with caution. As the study was a pilot, inter-rater reliability was not included due to data being collected by one assessor. It is recommended that future studies include inter-rater reliability measures. The method for collecting data on behaviour change scores could have been improved if the duration of behaviour display had been recorded. The authors suggest future studies on EE record the proportion of time spent displaying each behaviour and the time the dog spent engaging with each activity.

5. Conclusions

These results suggest there is a variation in the impact of EE type on behavioural displays. Therefore, by providing a range of different activities and rotating the activities in a random order, dogs are able to display a wider range of behaviours and potentially reduce the occurrence of habituation. However, more studies are required to compare the impact of specific EE activities on individual behaviours and thus determine their welfare value.

Acknowledgments

A summary of this work for practitioners has previously been shared through the International Association of Animal Behavior Consultants (IAABC) professional publication, Issue 15, February 2020.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani12020141/s1, Table S1: The behaviours coded within each behavioural category; Table S2: Mean behaviour change scores for relaxation, alert and stress behaviour for each EE activity; Table S3: Significance values for LSD post hoc pairwise analyses when comparing each EE activity to one another for alert (A), relaxation (R) and stress (S) behaviour categories.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.L.H. and H.W.; methodology, R.L.H., H.W. and S.P.; formal analysis, R.L.H. and H.W.; writing—original draft preparation, R.L.H.; writing—review and editing, H.W. and S.P., supervision, S.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were given by the University of Worcester on 11 June 2015.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author subject to the agreement of the assistance dog organisation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Tarou L.R., Bashaw M.J. Maximizing the effectiveness of environmental enrichment: Suggestions from the experimental analysis of behavior. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007;102:189–204. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.026. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Newberry R.C. Environmental enrichment: Increasing the biological relevance of captive environments. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1995;4:229–243. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(95)00616-Z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Shepherdson D.J. Environmental enrichment: Past, present and future. Int. Zoo Yearb. 2003;38:118–124. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1090.2003.tb02071.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Mármol F., Sánchex J., Torres M.N., Chamizo V.D. Environmental enrichment in the absence of wheel running produces beneficial behavioural and anti-oxidative effects in rats. Behav. Processes. 2017;144:66–71. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Van der Staay J.F., van Zutphen J.A., de Ridder M.M., Nordquist R.E. Effects of environmental enrichment on decision-making behaviour in pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017;194:14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.05.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Rehnberg L.K., Robert K.A., Watson S.J., Peters R.A. The effects of social interaction and environmental enrichment on the space use, behaviour and stress of owned housecats facing a novel environment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015;169:51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2015.06.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bashaw M.J., Gibson M.D., Schowe D.M., Kucher A.S. Does enrichment improve reptile welfare? Leopard geckos (Eublepharis macularius) respond to five types of environmental enrichment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016;184:150–160. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.08.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Heath S., Wilson C. Canine and Feline Enrichment in the Home and Kennel: A Guide for Practitioners. Vet. Clin. N. Am. Small Anim. Pract. 2014;4:427–449. doi: 10.1016/j.cvsm.2014.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Döring D., Haberland B.E., Bauer A., Dobenecker B., Hack R., Schmidt J., Erhard M.H. Behavioral observations in dogs in 4 research facilities: Do they use their enrichment? J. Vet. Behav. 2016;13:55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.04.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Herron M.E., Kirby-Madden T.M., Lord L.K. Effects of environmental enrichment on the behavior of shelter dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2014;244:687–692. doi: 10.2460/javma.244.6.687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kogan L.R., Schoenfeld-Tacher R., Simon A.A. Behavioral effects of auditory stimulation on kenneled dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2012;7:268–275. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2011.11.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pullen A.J., Merrill R.J.N., Bradshaw J.W.S. Preferences for toy types and presentations in kennel housed dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010;125:151–156. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.04.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Schipper L.L., Vinke C.M., Schilder M.B.H., Spruijt B.M. The effect of feeding enrichment toys on the behaviour of kennelled dogs (Canis familiaris) Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008;114:182–195. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.01.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hubrecht R.C. A comparison of social and environmental enrichment methods for laboratory housed dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1993;37:345–361. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(93)90123-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wells D.L. A review of environmental enrichment for kennelled dogs, Canis familiaris. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004;85:307–317. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2003.11.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Amaya V., Paterson M.B.A., Phillips C.J.C. Effects of Olfactory and Auditory Enrichment on the Behaviour of Shelter Dogs. Animals. 2020;10:581. doi: 10.3390/ani10040581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Murtagh K., Farnworth M.J., Brilot B.O. The scent of enrichment: Exploring the effect of odour and biological salience on behaviour during enrichment of kennelled dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2020;223:104917. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104917. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lindig A.M., McGreevy P.D., Crean A.J. Musical Dogs: A Review of the Influence of Auditory Enrichment on Canine Health and Behavior. Animals. 2020;10:127. doi: 10.3390/ani10010127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Perry P.J., Scarlett J.M., Houpt K.A., Erb H.N. A comparison of four environmental enrichments on adoptability of shelter dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2020;35:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2019.07.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ellis S.L. Environmental enrichment: Practical strategies for improving feline welfare. J. Feline Med. Surg. 2009;11:901–912. doi: 10.1016/j.jfms.2009.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Beerda B., Schilder M.B.H., van Hooff J.A.R.A.M., de Vries H.W., Mol J.A. Behavioural and hormonal indicators of enduring environmental stress in dogs. Anim. Welf. 2000;9:49–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Part C., Kiddie J., Hayes W., Mills D., Neville R., Morton D., Collins L. Physiological, physical and behavioural changes in dogs (Canis familiaris) when kennelled: Testing the validity of stress parameters. Physiol. Behav. 2014;133:260–271. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Serpell J.A., Coppinger R., Fine A.H. Handbook on Animal Assisted Therapy, Chapter 20: Welfare Considerations in Therapy and Assistance Animals. 2nd ed. Elsevier; San Diego, CA, USA: 2006. pp. 453–474. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Taylor K.D., Mills D.S. The effect of the kennel environment on canine welfare: A critical review of experimental studies. Anim. Welf. 2007;16:435–447. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gaines S.A., Rooney N.J., Bradshaw J.W. The effect of feeding enrichment upon reported working ability and behavior of kenneled working dogs. J. Forensic. Sci. 2008;53:1400–1404. doi: 10.1111/j.1556-4029.2008.00879.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ibrahim S.A., Goma A.A., Alsenosy A.-W.A. Springing Bottles Enrichment Toy Effect on Behaviors and Cortisol Level of Kenneled Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) AJVS. 2020;66:20–29. doi: 10.5455/ajvs.129089. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Wells D.L., Hepper P.G. The Influence of environmental change on the behaviour of sheltered dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2000;68:151–152. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(00)00100-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Tuozzi A., Arhant C., Anderle K., Backes J., Cords C., Magierski V., Rault J.-L., Windschnurer I. Effects of Human Presence and Voice on the Behaviour of Shelter Dogs and Cats: A Preliminary Study. Animals. 2021;11:406. doi: 10.3390/ani11020406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Belpedio C., Buffington L., Clusman S., Prete F., Sadler A., Whittemore L., Mungre S. Effect of Multidog Play Groups on Cortisol Levels and Behavior of Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) Housed in a Humane Society. J. Appl. Compan. Anim. Behav. 2010;4:15–27. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Coppola C.L., Grandin R., Enns R.M. Human interaction and cortisol: Can human contact reduce stress for shelter dogs? Physiol. Behav. 2006;87:537–541. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hubrecht R.C., Serpell J.A., Poole T.B. Correlates of pen size and housing conditions on the behaviour of kennelled dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1992;24:365–383. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(05)80096-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Menor-Campos D.J., Molleda-Carbonell J.M., Lopez-Rodriguez R. Effects of exercise and human contact on animal welfare in a dog shelter. Vet. Rec. 2011;169:388–392. doi: 10.1136/vr.d4757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author subject to the agreement of the assistance dog organisation.


Articles from Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES