Abstract
Theta (2–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta (12–35 Hz), and gamma (>35 Hz) rhythms are ubiquitous in the cortex. However, there is little understanding of whether they have similar properties and functions in different cortical areas because they have rarely been compared across them. We record neuronal spikes and local field potentials simultaneously at several levels of the cortical hierarchy in monkeys. Theta, alpha, beta, and gamma oscillations had similar relationships to spiking activity in visual, parietal, and prefrontal cortices. However, the frequencies in all bands increased up the cortical hierarchy. These results suggest that these rhythms have similar inhibitory and excitatory functions across the cortex. We discuss how the increase in frequencies up the cortical hierarchy may help sculpt cortical flow and processing.
INTRODUCTION
Theta (2–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta (12–35 Hz), and gamma (>35 Hz) are common across the cortex (Hari, Salmelin, Mäkelä, Salenius, & Helle, 1997; Gray, Engel, König, & Singer, 1990; Berger, 1929). Alpha/beta and gamma tend to be anticorrelated and have been associated with different functions. Increases in gamma power occur during sensory inputs/motor outputs, whereas increases in alpha/beta occur during top–down control and inhibition (Lundqvist et al., 2016; Bastos et al., 2015; van Kerkoerle et al., 2014; van Ede, de Lange, Jensen, & Maris, 2011; Fisch et al., 2009; Buschman & Miller, 2007; Jokisch & Jensen, 2007; Gray et al., 1990; Pfurtscheller & Aranibar, 1977). For example, in the motor cortex, beta power is high and gamma power is low when movements are inhibited, but this reverses when movement is released (Schmidt et al., 2019; Cheyne, Bells, Ferrari, Gaetz, & Bostan, 2008; Brovelli et al., 2004; Pfurtscheller, Stancák, & Neuper, 1996). In the visual cortex, gamma power is high and alpha power is low during sensory stimulation, and vice versa for representations outside focal attention (Bollimunta, Mo, Schroeder, & Ding, 2011; Buffalo, Fries, Landman, Buschman, & Desimone, 2011; Rohenkohl & Nobre, 2011; van Ede et al., 2011; Fisch et al., 2009; Jokisch & Jensen, 2007; Fries, Reynolds, Rorie, & Desimone, 2001; Klimesch, Doppelmayr, Russegger, Pachinger, & Schwaiger, 1998; Gevins, Smith, McEvoy, & Yu, 1997; Gray et al., 1990; Pfurtscheller & Aranibar, 1977). In PFC, bursts of gamma are associated with sensory information maintained in working memory (Bastos, Loonis, Kornblith, Lundqvist, & Miller, 2018; Lundqvist, Herman, Warden, Brincat, & Miller, 2018; Lundqvist et al., 2016). They are anticorrelated with alpha/beta bursts that decrease during encoding of sensory information in working memory and increase when working memory is cleared.
Given the ubiquity of these rhythms and their apparent push–pull relationship, it would be important to know how they compare across cortical areas. Any preserved attributes of the rhythms would suggest shared roles in cortical processing, whereas differences can provide insights into any functional differences across the cortical hierarchy. There is some evidence for differences. The lower rhythms skew toward alpha/low beta in the sensory and parietal cortex and toward higher beta in the motor cortex and PFC, albeit in comparisons across different studies and species (Lundqvist et al., 2016; Bollimunta et al., 2011; Buschman & Miller, 2007; Jokisch & Jensen, 2007). These differences could therefore be because of differences in task, species, and/or recording techniques. Few studies have compared multiple levels of the cortical hierarchy using multiple intracranial electrodes that allow good localization of local field potentials (LFPs) as well as their relationship to neuronal spiking.
We recorded spikes and LFPs simultaneously along the cortical visual hierarchy as monkeys performed a visual working memory task. We analyzed LFPs and multiunit activity (MUA). This revealed a similar functional relationship between spiking and alpha/beta and gamma power across cortical areas. Alpha/beta power was lower during encoding and retention of information and anticorrelated with spiking activity and gamma across time and recording sites. Both types of rhythms occurred in bursts, not sustained increases in power. Gamma was associated with spiking carrying sensory information; alpha/beta was negatively correlated with spiking. The peak frequencies of both hythms increased as the cortical hierarchy was ascended from sensory cortex to PFC. Moreover, theta (2–8 Hz) often couples with gamma rhythms (Schroeder & Lakatos, 2009; Tort, Komorowski, Manns, Kopell, & Eichenbaum, 2009; Canolty et al., 2006). We found theta power was most pronounced in the higher areas, correlated positively with spiking, and was also of a gradually higher frequency ascending the cortical hierarchy. This suggests that interactions between alpha/beta and gamma are preserved across the cortex and may play a general role in regulating expression of information by cortical neurons. We discuss the computational implications of the gradual increase of these frequencies up the cortical hierarchy.
METHODS
Experimental Design
Using positive reinforcement, we trained monkeys to perform a visual search task with a memory delay (Figure 1A). Monkeys fixated a point at the center of the screen (fixation window radius: 2–3 visual degrees) for a duration of 1 sec. Then, one of three sample objects was presented at the center of gaze for 1 sec. Then, monkeys maintained central fixation over a delay (between 0.5 and 1.2 sec; in some sessions (47/81), held fixed at 1.2 sec). A search array then appeared. It consisted of an object that matched the sample together with either one or two distractor objects presented at the same eccentricity (3°–8°) but in different visual quadrants as the sample. The position of the match and the distractors were always randomly chosen. Monkeys were rewarded if they made a direct saccade to the match. Monkeys were trained on this task using a library of 22 sample objects. For recordings, we used a subset of these objects (12), choosing three per session. The monkeys performed the task with blocks in which the sample was either randomly chosen trial-by-trial or held fixed. Only the data with trial-by-trial cuing, requiring engagement of working memory, were used, and only the 81 sessions with at least 70 correctly performed trials were included.
Animal Models
Two adult rhesus macaques (macaca mulatta) were used in this study (Monkey S: 6 years old, 5.0 kg; Monkey L: 17 years old, 10.5 kg). Both animals were pair-housed on 12-hr day/night cycles and maintained in a temperature-controlled environment (80°F). All procedures were approved by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and followed the guidelines of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and the U.S. National Institutes of Health.
Data Collection
All of the data were recorded through Blackrock headstages (Blackrock Cereplex M), sampled at 30 kHz, band-passed between 0.3 Hz and 7.5 kHz, and digitized at a 16-bit, 250 nV/bit. All LFPs were recorded with a low-pass 250-Hz Butterworth filter, sampled at 1 kHz, and alternating current–coupled.
We implanted the monkeys with three recording wells placed over visual/temporal, parietal, and frontal cortices. We kept 64 sessions with multicontact (“U probes” and “V probes” from Plexon) probes and 17 sessions with acute single-tip electrodes. In each session using multicontact probes, we inserted between one and three laminar probes into each recording chamber with either 100- or 200-μm intersite spacing and either 16 or 32 total electrodes per probe. Between three and seven probes in total per session were used, with a total channel count ranging between 48 and 128 electrodes per session. The recording reference was the reinforcement tube, which made metallic contact with the entire length of the probe (total probe length from connector to tip was 70 mm).
We used a number of physiologic indicators to guide our electrode placement, as previously described (Bastos et al., 2018). First, the presence of a slow 1-to 2-Hz signal, a heartbeat artifact, was often found as we pierced the pia mater and just as we entered the gray matter. Second, as the first contacts of the electrode entered the gray matter, the magnitude of the LFP increased, and single units and/or neural hash became apparent, both audibly and visually with spikes appearing in the online spike threshold crossing. Once the tip of the electrode transitioned into the gray matter, electrodes were lowered slowly by an additional ~2.5 mm. At this point, we retracted the probe by 200–400 μm and allowed the probe to settle for between 1 and 2 hr before beginning the task. We left between one and three contacts out of gray matter in the overlying cerebral spinal fluid. These contacts were not used in the analysis.
Preprocessing
For the analysis of the analog MUA, we band-pass filtered the raw, unfiltered, 30-kHz sampled data into a wide band between 500 and 5000 Hz, the power range dominated by spikes. The signal was then low-pass filtered at 250 Hz and resampled to 1000 kHz. The advantage of this signal is that it captures all nearby units, including those with a low signal-to-noise ratio that would not be captured with a strict threshold. A subset of contacts had apparent artifacts. These contacts were automatically removed (16/768 in lateral intraparietal cortex [LIP], 12/2252 in PFC, and 120/1538 in V4 [most from the same session]) from the analysis by setting a threshold (100% above the mean) on the power between 1 and 5 Hz.
Analysis and Statistical Tests
All analysis was performed using MATLAB (The MathWorks). We estimated power at all frequencies from 2 to 150 Hz using Morlet wavelets (five cycles, estimated each millisecond), with 81 frequencies (six octaves) of interest distributed on a logarithmic scale.
We correlated MUA and power using neighboring contacts to avoid spike bleed-through to drive correlations in the gamma band. Correlations were calculated for each frequency of interest, and we used Bonferroni correction to take this into account when calculating statistics. This was also the case for multiple pairwise t tests between area pairs.
To determine stimulus selectivity, we used bias-corrected percentage explained variance (PEV; Lundqvist et al., 2016; Olejnik & Algina, 2003). MUA was smoothed using 100-msec rectangular windows. For each 1-msec time point, a one-way ANOVA was performed with trials grouped based on the sample cue identity. Nonbiased PEV was used to avoid nonzero means for small sample sizes.
We correlated the time course of trial-averaged signals (power vs. MUA or power vs. PEV in MUA) for each electrode. For correlations over time, we used the last 500 msec of fixation, the 1000 msec of sample presentation, and the first 700 msec of the delay (not using trials with less than 700-msec delays). We also correlated, across electrodes, the change in MUA to the change in power from one epoch to another to determine relationships at the single electrode level. For this, we used Spearman’s ranked correlations using the epoch averages per electrode. For power modulation by task (Figure 2), −700 to −300 msec was used for prestimulation and 200–1000 msec for stimulation, where 0 msec is the time of stimulation onset, to avoid frequency smoothing cross talk between the epochs in the frequencies of interest.
To find spike–power relationships that did not depend on shared epoch preferences between the two measures, we calculated correlations between neighboring pairs of MUA and power and compared it to the correlations between random pairs of MUA and power (taken across all sessions but within each area). The correlations between random pairs were carried out 1000 times to estimate a distribution that was then compared to the original correlations.
Burst Detection
Power displayed strong fluctuations on a single-trial level, only exceeding baseline levels during brief burst events. To extract these, we used a procedure developed earlier (Lundqvist et al., 2016). In short, we used the fixation period as baseline to calculate mean and standard deviation in each band of interest (beta, 6–35 Hz; Gamma 1, 40–65Hz; Gamma 2, 55–90 Hz; Gamma 3, 70–100 Hz). We used a sliding window of 10 trials to calculate the mean and standard deviation used for each trial. In a first step, candidate bursts were extracted if the average power within a given band exceeded the threshold (set at 2 SDs above the mean of 10 trial average) for at least two cycles (based on the central frequency of each band).
Furthermore, we extracted the time–frequency representation of the signal in the spectrotemporal neighborhood of each burst using the wavelets. We resorted to fitting 2-D Gaussian function to the local time–frequency map to specify the aforementioned neighborhood. Finally, we defined the burst length as a time subinterval where the average instantaneous power was higher than half of the local maximum (half-power point). The burst intervals were extracted for the alpha/beta band (6–35 Hz) and three gamma subband oscillations (40–65, 55–90, and 70–100 Hz) from each trial, along with the central frequency of each burst and the frequency width. The frequency width of bursts was estimated analogously to their duration: the frequency range where the spectral power component did not fall below 50% of the local maximum (epicenter of the burst’s power). We could then select bursts within a more narrow range than described by the original bands. The central frequency of each burst was used when correlating duration of bursts with their frequency. To determine burst durations of specific bands, we only included bursts with their central frequency within a certain range of the average peak for that frequency band and area. For alpha/beta, we used ±4 Hz; and for gamma, ±10 Hz. We used bursts from the first 70 trials for each session to get nonbiased estimates.
Data Availability
The data and code will be made available by reasonable request by contacting the corresponding author, Earl Miller (ekmiller@mit.edu).
RESULTS
Increase of Frequencies Up the Cortical Hierarchy
Two rhesus macaques performed a delayed-match-to-sample task (Figure 1A). We simultaneously recorded LFPs and MUA from V4 (n = 1418 recording sites), LIP (n = 752), and PFC (n = 2240).
The mean peak frequency of oscillatory power increased up the cortical hierarchy. Figure 1B shows the average LFP power spectrum of each cortical area during a baseline interval just before presentation of the sample stimulus (see Methods). Each area was dominated by oscillations in the alpha (8–12 Hz) and beta (12–35 Hz) ranges (Figure 1B). The lack of a gamma peak was because of the lack of bottom–up sensory inputs, which are associated with increases in gamma power (Gray et al., 1990). The peak frequency of alpha/beta increased from V4 (11 Hz) to LIP (15 Hz) to PFC (19 Hz; all across-areas comparisons had significantly different means at p < 10−30, unpaired t test corrected for multiple tests, using a frequency between 7 and 40 Hz with the maximum amplitude as peak at each recording site).
The modulation of oscillatory power by stimulus presentation also increased in peak frequency up the cortical hierarchy. We established the “functionally defined” frequency bands in each area by determining how each frequency was modulated by presentation of the sample stimulus. To do so, power per frequency during sample presentation was divided by the power per frequency during the baseline. The result showed that sample presentation suppressed alpha/beta power and increased gamma power (Figure 2). Note that the frequency of maximum suppression of alpha/beta matched the baseline peak frequency for each area. As such, it also increased from V4 (11 Hz) to LIP (16 Hz) to PFC (20 Hz; all across-areas comparisons had significantly different means at p < 10−15, unpaired t test corrected for multiple tests on the frequency of maximal decrease per electrode). The peak of average gamma power also increased in frequency from V4 (65 Hz) to LIP (72 Hz) to PFC (80 Hz; Figure 2). We defined the transition between beta and gamma as the first frequency with positive modulation by stimulus presentation. This transition also occurred at gradually higher frequencies as the cortical hierarchy was ascended (Figure 2B, center; V4: 26 Hz, LIP: 40 Hz, PFC: 47 Hz, all across-areas comparisons had significantly different means at p < 10−15 using unpaired t test corrected for multiple tests).
Spiking Was Correlated with Changes in Oscillatory Power
Increases in spiking were associated with increases in gamma and theta power and decreases in alpha/beta power. We examined the correlation between changes in MUA and oscillatory power across time (over three task epochs: baseline, sample presentation, and memory delay). The level of MUA was positively correlated with changes in functionally defined (i.e., stimulus modulated; see above) gamma power and anticorrelated with changes in functionally defined alpha/beta power (Figure 3A; note that MUA and LFPs from neighboring, not the same, electrodes were used to avoid bleed-through effects). In LIP and PFC, but not V4, there was also a positive correlation between changes in theta power and level of MUA (Figure 3A).
The peak frequency of maximal negative correlation between LFP power in the alpha/beta band and MUA gradually shifted toward higher frequencies from V4 to LIP to PFC (Figure 3A). Likewise, we defined the frequency of transition between gamma and beta as the lowest frequency above 15 Hz with a significant positive correlation with spiking (see Methods). This also increased in frequency up the cortical hierarchy from V4 (26 Hz), LIP (33 Hz), to PFC (39 Hz; all means significantly different at p < 10−15 using unpaired t test, corrected for multiple tests). As a result of the changes in frequencies, the frequencies of (positively correlated) gamma in V4 overlapped with (negatively correlated) beta in PFC and PFC (positively correlated) theta overlapped with V4 (negatively correlated) alpha/beta (Figure 3B). Note that the correlation between spiking and gamma did not increase monotonically with frequency. Instead, it peaked below the maximum frequency tested (150 Hz). This suggests that the positive correlations were not because of spikes bleeding into the power spectrum (along with the fact that we used only spikes from neighboring electrodes). Similar results were obtained when we correlated stimulus information in MUA (instead of spike rate) and power (Supplementary Figure S11).
The correlation between changes in MUA and oscillatory power was not indirect, a by-product of both changing in response to external events (e.g., stimulus onset or offset). Rather, the correlation was direct. To show this, we performed the same analysis but using MUAs and LFPs from random pairs of electrodes from each area instead of neighboring pairs. Any correlation between such random pairs was attributed to correlation because of them both responding to external events, not a direct correlation. To determine direct correlation, we computed when the average correlation between MUA and LFPs in neighboring electrode pairs exceeded that of the average correlation between random pairs (permuted 1000 times). This revealed significant direct correlations (p < .01), albeit more narrow-band in the alpha/beta domain (Figure 3A, red/blue/black bars). The negative correlations between LFP and spiking were now seen around the peak frequency of the alpha/beta power for each region, whereas gamma remained broadly correlated with spiking. Overall, the patterns of correlations suggested that functionally similar frequency bands (excitatory gamma and theta, inhibitory alpha/beta) in all areas gradually shifted toward higher frequencies up the cortical hierarchy (Figure 3A; but no theta in V4).
We also compared, between electrodes, the correlations between the change in LFP power and the change in MUA from fixation to the memory delay period (Figure 4, using Spearman’s ranked correlation; bars show significance at p < .01, Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons). This showed that the recording sites with the largest decreases in alpha/beta power during the memory delay also had the highest increases in spiking during that time. In this analysis, theta was positively correlated with spiking in V4 (unlike the other analyses, see above). A similar pattern was seen if the correlation was performed between change in power and change in spiking from baseline to sample stimulus presentation (instead of change from fixation to delay; Figure S2).
Differences in Delay Activity across the Cortical Hierarchy
During sample presentation, alpha/beta power was suppressed relative to baseline in all areas but, toward the end of the presentation, less in V4 than in LIP and PFC (Figure 5A, using 12–26 Hz for all electrodes as this was the alpha/beta range shared by all areas). During the memory delay, alpha/beta power was highest in V4, second highest in LIP, and lowest in PFC (all areas significantly different at each time point at p < .01, corrected for multiple comparisons, using nonpaired t test). Gamma power during sample presentation was highest in V4 (which, similar to the suppression in alpha/beta, showed a sharper transient to sample presentation), second highest in LIP, and lowest in PFC (Figure 5B). During the memory delay, gamma power level was similar across all areas early in the memory delay (Figure 5C), but PFC and LIP showed a ramping up of gamma power toward the end of the memory delay (Figure 5C), whereas V4 showed a ramping down of gamma power. At the same time, alpha/beta power ramped up in V4 and ramped down in LIP and PFC (Figure 5A). Spiking activity showed similar dynamics and differences across areas as gamma power (Figure 5D).
Gamma Bursts Were Briefer in Higher Cortical Areas
An analysis of single-trial data (rather than trial-averaged data) showed that increases in gamma and alpha/beta power occurred in brief bursts. Figure S3 shows example electrodes from each area, demonstrating that the peak frequency differences in alpha/beta were driven by brief high-power events around those peaks. To quantify this, we estimated, on each trial, the duration of the bursts and the central frequency at which they occurred. We did so by first setting a threshold at 2 SDs above the mean power for each frequency. Around the events where power exceeded the threshold for at least one oscillatory cycle, we fitted 2-D Gaussians to estimate their frequency and time (Lundqvist et al., 2016; see Methods).
This revealed alpha/beta and gamma occurred in brief and narrow-band bursts in all areas (Figure 6; Figure S3; see Methods). Figure 6A shows the duration of the bursts of the stimulus-evoked gamma (i.e., bursts with their central frequency within ±10 Hz of the peak of stimulus-induced power; see Methods and Figure 2). The burst durations were significantly shorter in PFC (mean = 37 msec, n = 28,403) versus LIP (mean = 42 msec, n = 16,177), in PFC versus V4 (mean = 47 msec, n = 89,911), and in LIP versus V4 (p < 10−15 for all comparisons, Wilcoxon rank sum test for equal medians). This was partly, but not entirely, a consequence of the changes in gamma frequency peak across cortical areas. The duration of the bursts inversely scaled with their frequency (combining bursts from all areas; r = −.52 for beta, p < 10−30; r = −.21 for gamma, p < 10−30). However, selecting bursts from the same frequency range across areas (60–80 Hz) resulted in average gamma burst durations that were more similar but still significantly different across areas (Figure S4; Wilcoxon ranked sum test, p < 10−9). Note, for example, that bursts with a central frequency of 26 Hz, which was at the transition to gamma band in V4, had a longer duration (mean = 92 msec) than those at around the peak. Beta bursts, by contrast, although their peak frequencies increased along the cortical hierarchy, did not have a corresponding change in duration. Instead, they had a longer duration in LIP, next longest in V4, and shortest in PFC (Figure 6; LIP [mean = 240 msec] vs. V4 [mean = 220 msec], LIP vs. PFC [mean = 160 msec], and V4 vs. PFC, all significantly different medians using Wilcoxon rank sum test with p < 10−15).
In the alpha/beta band, there was a clear baseline peak in all areas (Figure 1). Comparisons of all bursts across a wide range (6–35 Hz, using a fixation period) suggested that the differences in average peak power between areas could be explained by differences in central frequency of bursts (p < 10−30 for all comparisons, Wilcoxon rank sum test for equal median central burst frequency; Figure S3). Potentially, differences in peak power could be because of distinct, nonsinusoidal wave shapes across compared areas. The power estimate of nonsinusoidal wave shapes contributes power also at higher frequencies than that of the principal period of the wave shape. If this occurred more so in, for instance, PFC, it would shift the estimated peak toward higher frequencies despite having the same underlying frequency of oscillation. To rule this out, we used the bottom (rather than central) frequency of each burst. This confirmed the gradually higher alpha/beta frequency up the hierarchy (p < 10−30 for all pairwise comparisons, Wilcoxon rank sum test for equal medians).
DISCUSSION
We observed a general motif as well as a trend across the cortical hierarchy. High-frequency (gamma band) power correlated positively with spiking, as did slower frequency (theta band) power. By contrast, alpha/beta band power correlated negatively with spiking. The peak frequency of all of these frequency bands became systematically higher as the cortical hierarchy was ascended from visual cortex to parietal cortex to PFC. Note that, because the peak frequencies of all frequency bands gradually increased up the cortical hierarchy, there was some overlap between “excitatory” (positive correlation with spiking) and “inhibitory” (negative correlation with spiking) frequencies across areas. For example, the excitatory theta in PFC overlapped with the inhibitory alpha/beta in V4. The fast excitatory rhythm (gamma) of V4 overlapped with the inhibitory rhythm (alpha/beta) of PFC. This suggests that the origin of the rhythms should be taken into account when determining the functional relevance of different frequency bands. The power increases occurred in bursts. The duration of the gamma bursts was shorter in higher areas. Finally, in the higher areas (PFC, LIP), there was a ramping up of gamma bursting (and associated spiking) toward the end of the memory delay (V4 instead ramped down). The ramping up has been linked to the readout of information from working memory (Lundqvist et al., 2016, 2018; Hussar & Pasternak, 2010).
The shorter duration of the gamma bursts higher in the hierarchy was partly because of the increase in gamma frequencies, but it may also be a consequence of increases of frequencies in the alpha/beta band. In the posterior cortex, gamma bursts are strongly coupled to the ongoing phase of alpha (Spaak, Bonnefond, Maier, Leopold, & Jensen, 2012; Voytek et al., 2010; Osipova, Hermes, & Jensen, 2008). In the frontal cortex, there is phase–amplitude coupling between beta and gamma (Bastos et al., 2018). This difference can be explained by the increases in alpha/beta frequencies from the posterior to anterior cortex. Gamma will naturally entrain to whatever frequency is most prominent in a given area. The higher frequency alpha/beta peak frequency in PFC may therefore partly explain the shorter gamma burst durations in PFC. The length of the average “duty cycle” (the depolarized or active phase of an oscillation) of a beta frequency is shorter than the average duty cycle length of an alpha oscillation (Figure 7).
Alpha/beta and gamma rhythms are common and anticorrelated across the cortex, but it has been unclear whether or not they have similar functional relationships in different areas. Frontal beta rhythms have been associated with top–down attention, inhibiting motor actions, and working memory storage (Schmidt et al., 2019; Lundqvist et al., 2016, 2018; Buschman & Miller, 2007). Occipital and parietal alpha has been linked to inhibition of unattended stimuli (Bollimunta et al., 2011; Rohenkohl & Nobre, 2011; van Ede et al., 2011; Jokisch & Jensen, 2007; Worden, Foxe, Wang, & Simpson, 2000; Klimesch et al., 1998). Gamma power increases (and alpha/beta power decreases) in the sensory cortex during sensory input and before/during movements in the motor cortex (Schmidt et al., 2019; Fisch et al., 2009; Cheyne et al., 2008; Brovelli et al., 2004; Fries et al., 2001; Gevins et al., 1997; Gray et al., 1990). Frontal beta and premotor alpha are correlated with reduced spike rates (Lundqvist et al., 2016, 2018; Haegens, Nácher, Luna, Romo, & Jensen, 2011). Our results suggest that these alpha-to-beta frequency differences are part of a continuum across the cortical hierarchy with similar general functions. Indeed, regardless of the exact frequencies of alpha, beta, or gamma, we found similar functional relationships at each cortical level. Alpha/beta and gamma were negatively and positively correlated with spiking, respectively, and negatively correlated with each other. Both alpha/beta and gamma were similarly modulated by stimulus processing and the WM task across the entire hierarchy. On a finer scale, we also observed differences with more alpha/beta during WM retention in the sensory areas, consistent with the proposed inhibitory function and more sustained delay activity in higher-order cortex. Theta was not as widely observed across the cortex, but in LIP and PFC, like gamma, it was positively correlated with spiking. In V4, a peak in the theta range positively correlated with spiking during the memory delay.
Modeling suggests that increased peak frequency of oscillatory rhythms in higher cortical areas can arise naturally because of inputs from earlier areas contributing to increased excitation in the higher-order areas (Lundqvist et al., 2013; Brunel & Wang, 2003). Increases in excitation up the cortical hierarchy have also been inferred from large-scale connectivity data (Chaudhuri, Knoblauch, Gariel, Kennedy, & Wang, 2015). At the same time, the autocorrelation of spiking suggests a slower time constant for spiking higher than the sensory cortex (Murray et al., 2014). This is not incompatible with our observations of higher, not slower, oscillatory dynamics in higher-order cortex. Rather, spiking versus oscillatory LFP rhythms could reflect different mechanisms with different functions. The slower dynamics for spiking can help higher-order cortex integrate information from lower areas (Chaudhuri et al., 2015). By contrast, the increased frequency in oscillatory rhythms may help sculpt information flow in the cortex. Below, we will elaborate on this.
Although there may be a shared inhibitory control function for alpha/beta rhythms across the cortex, increases in oscillatory frequencies can still be functionally relevant (Wutz, Melcher, & Samaha, 2018; Bosman et al., 2012). Spiking favoring specific oscillatory phases can group spiking into “packets” of information (Buschman & Miller, 2010; Schroeder & Lakatos, 2009). According to the “inhibition-timing” hypothesis, alpha oscillations reflect alternating periods of relative inhibition and excitability (Haegens et al., 2011; Klimesch, Sauseng, & Hanslmayr, 2007). The frequency of the oscillation dictates the duty cycle and thus the duration of nested gamma/spiking activity during the excitatory phases of the alpha rhythm. Different inputs arriving during the same duty cycle of a cortical alpha rhythm tend to be integrated (VanRullen & Koch, 2003). For example, the temporal resolution at which humans can discriminate two flashes in close succession is finer in subjects with a higher occipital alpha peak frequency (Wutz et al., 2018; Samaha & Postle, 2015). This is because the higher the occipital alpha frequency, the more likely that the two flashes will arrive at different duty cycles and therefore be perceptually segregated. Furthermore, a recent magnetoencephalography study showed that the alpha/beta rhythms are not fixed but can flexibly change with changes in task demands for perceptual segregation (Wutz et al., 2018). Alpha peak frequency increased when two successive flashes needed to be segregated and decreased when integration was demanded. Such flexible control over the speed of “alpha/beta clocks” might be achieved by adjusting top–down drive from areas higher in the hierarchy. Increased top–down drive would entrain the sensory alpha to a slightly higher frequency.
In the context of a hierarchy of connected areas, it also makes sense that the “alpha/beta clocks” increase in frequency up the cortical hierarchy. It allows higher areas to better segregate packets received from lower cortical areas and reduces the risk that two packets will be merged into one. Furthermore, whereas increases in alpha/beta power can suppress gamma/spiking by introducing periods of inhibition (Haegens et al., 2011), the increases in alpha/beta frequencies up the cortical hierarchy can help feed forward whatever spiking there is. If the receivers higher in the cortical hierarchy operate on a slightly higher time scale, it will make it more likely that the receiver will have some overlap between its duty cycle and incoming packets sent during the duty cycle of the sender, supporting the feedforward flow of spiking (Figure 7).
At the same time, the increase in frequencies up the cortical hierarchy may also bias the direction of rhythmic entrainment in the cortex in the feedback direction. If two oscillators with different harmonic frequencies are connected, the higher oscillator will be more effective at entraining the slightly slower oscillator than vice versa. The increase in frequencies in higher areas would thus naturally bias rhythmic entrainment in the feedback direction, down the cortical hierarchy. When there are no sensory inputs, alpha/beta rhythms predominate in the cortex. They have been associated with top–down control (Bastos et al., 2015; van Kerkoerle et al., 2014; Buschman & Miller, 2007). The frequency gradient we observed suggests that their entrainment has a feedback directional bias consistent with top–down control. Bottom–up inputs could overcome the top–down bias during periods of strong sensory drive. Because sensory stimuli drive gamma, it could explain the entrainment of gamma rhythms predominantly in the feedforward direction. In summary, the increase of oscillatory frequencies up the cortical hierarchy may provide another mechanism by which cortical processing and flow can be regulated.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Pawel Herman and Nancy Kopell for useful discussions. This work was supported by National Institutes of Mental Health grants R37MH087027 and 5K99MH116100-02, Office of Naval Research Multidisciplinary University Research Initiatives grant N00014-16-1-2832, the MIT Picower Institute Faculty Innovation Fund, Swedish Research Council Starting Grant 2018-04197, and the 2017 Young Investigator grant from the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation.
Footnotes
Supplementary figures for this paper can be retrieved from http://ekmillerlab.mit.edu/wp-content/uploads/2020/06/Supplementary-Material-Lundqvist-JOCN-2020.pdf.
REFERENCES
- Bastos AM, Loonis R, Kornblith S, Lundqvist M, & Miller EK (2018). Laminar recordings in frontal cortex suggest distinct layers for maintenance and control of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A, 115, 1117–1122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastos AM, Vezoli J, Bosman CA, Schoffelen J-M, Oostenveld R, Dowdall JR, et al. (2015). Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron, 85, 390–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berger H (1929). Über das elektroenkephalogramm des menschen. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 87, 527–570. [Google Scholar]
- Bollimunta A, Mo J, Schroeder CE, & Ding M (2011). Neuronal mechanisms and attentional modulation of corticothalamic alpha oscillations. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 4935–4943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosman CA, Schoffelen J-M, Brunet N, Oostenveld R, Bastos AM, Womelsdorf T, et al. (2012). Attentional stimulus selection through selective synchronization between monkey visual areas. Neuron, 75, 875–888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brovelli A, Ding M, Ledberg A, Chen Y, Nakamura R, & Bressler SL (2004). Beta oscillations in a large-scale sensorimotor cortical network: Directional influences revealed by Granger causality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A, 101, 9849–9854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunel N, & Wang X-J (2003). What determines the frequency of fast network oscillations with irregular neural discharges? I. Synaptic dynamics and excitation-inhibition balance. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90, 415–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buffalo EA, Fries P, Landman R, Buschman TJ, & Desimone R (2011). Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A, 108, 11262–11267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buschman TJ, & Miller EK (2007). Top–down versus bottom–up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science, 315, 1860–1862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buschman TJ, & Miller EK (2010). Shifting the spotlight of attention: Evidence for discrete computations in cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, Kirsch HE, et al. (2006). High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science, 313, 1626–1628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaudhuri R, Knoblauch K, Gariel M-A, Kennedy H, & Wang X-J (2015). A large-scale circuit mechanism for hierarchical dynamical processing in the primate cortex. Neuron, 88, 419–431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheyne D, Bells S, Ferrari P, Gaetz W, & Bostan AC (2008). Self-paced movements induce high-frequency gamma oscillations in primary motor cortex. Neuroimage, 42, 332–342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisch L, Privman E, Ramot M, Harel M, Nir Y, Kipervasser S, et al. (2009). Neural “ignition”: Enhanced activation linked to perceptual awareness in human ventral stream visual cortex. Neuron, 64, 562–574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, & Desimone R (2001). Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science, 291, 1560–1563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gevins A, Smith ME, McEvoy L, & Yu D (1997). High-resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: Effects of task difficulty, type of processing, and practice. Cerebral Cortex, 7, 374–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray CM, Engel AK, König P, & Singer W (1990). Stimulus-dependent neuronal oscillations in cat visual cortex: Receptive field properties and feature dependence. European Journal of Neuroscience, 2, 607–619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haegens S, Nácher V, Luna R, Romo R, & Jensen O (2011). α-oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A, 108, 19377–19382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hari R, Salmelin R, Mäkelä JP, Salenius S, & Helle M (1997). Magnetoencephalographic cortical rhythms. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 26, 51–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hussar C, & Pasternak T (2010). Trial-to-trial variability of the prefrontal neurons reveals the nature of their engagement in a motion discrimination task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A, 107, 21842–21847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jokisch D, & Jensen O (2007). Modulation of gamma and alpha activity during a working memory task engaging the dorsal or ventral stream. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 3244–3251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Russegger H, Pachinger T, & Schwaiger J (1998). Induced alpha band power changes in the human EEG and attention. Neuroscience Letters, 244, 73–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, Sauseng P, & Hanslmayr S (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Research Reviews, 53, 63–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lundqvist M, Herman P, Palva M, Palva S, Silverstein D, & Lansner A (2013). Stimulus detection rate and latency, firing rates and 1–40 Hz oscillatory power are modulated by infra-slow fluctuations in a bistable attractor network model. Neuroimage, 83, 458–471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lundqvist M, Herman P, Warden MR, Brincat SL, & Miller EK (2018). Gamma and beta bursts during working memory readout suggest roles in its volitional control. Nature Communications, 9, 394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lundqvist M, Rose J, Herman P, Brincat SL, Buschman TJ, & Miller EK (2016). Gamma and beta bursts underlie working memory. Neuron, 90, 152–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray JD, Bernacchia A, Freedman DJ, Romo R, Wallis JD, Cai X, et al. (2014). A hierarchy of intrinsic timescales across primate cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 1661–1663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olejnik S, & Algina J (2003). Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: Measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychological Methods, 8, 434–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osipova D, Hermes D, & Jensen O (2008). Gamma power is phase-locked to posterior alpha activity. PLoS One, 3, e3990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, & Aranibar A (1977). Event-related cortical desynchronization detected by power measurements of scalp EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 42, 817–826. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Stancák A Jr., & Neuper C (1996). Post-movement beta synchronization. A correlate of an idling motor area? Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 98, 281–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rohenkohl G, & Nobre AC (2011). Alpha oscillations related to anticipatory attention follow temporal expectations. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 14076–14084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samaha J, & Postle BR (2015). The speed of alpha-band oscillations predicts the temporal resolution of visual perception. Current Biology, 25, 2985–2990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt R, Ruiz MH, Kilavik BE, Lundqvist M, Starr PA, & Aron AR (2019). Beta oscillations in working memory, executive control of movement and thought, and sensorimotor function. Journal of Neuroscience, 39, 8231–8238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder CE, & Lakatos P (2009). Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection. Trends in Neurosciences, 32, 9–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spaak E, Bonnefond M, Maier A, Leopold DA, & Jensen O (2012). Layer-specific entrainment of γ-band neural activity by the α rhythm in monkey visual cortex. Current Biology, 22, 2313–2318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tort ABL, Komorowski RW, Manns JR, Kopell NJ, & Eichenbaum H (2009). Theta–gamma coupling increases during the learning of item–context associations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A, 106, 20942–20947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Ede F, de Lange F, Jensen O, & Maris E (2011). Orienting attention to an upcoming tactile event involves a spatially and temporally specific modulation of sensorimotor alpha- and beta-band oscillations. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 2016–2024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Kerkoerle T, Self MW, Dagnino B, Gariel-Mathis M-A, Poort J, van der Togt C, et al. (2014). Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A, 111, 14332–14341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- VanRullen R, & Koch C (2003). Is perception discrete or continuous? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 207–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voytek B, Canolty RT, Shestyuk A, Crone NE, Parvizi J, & Knight RT (2010). Shifts in gamma phase–amplitude coupling frequency from theta to alpha over posterior cortex during visual tasks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Worden MS, Foxe JJ, Wang N, & Simpson GV (2000). Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific α-bank electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, RC63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wutz A, Melcher D, & Samaha J (2018). Frequency modulation of neural oscillations according to visual task demands. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A, 115, 1346–1351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data and code will be made available by reasonable request by contacting the corresponding author, Earl Miller (ekmiller@mit.edu).