Abstract
Objectives.
To investigate the heterogeneous effects of education on sleep duration for Black and White adults and how the education effects changed between 2004 and 2018.
Methods.
251,994 adult participants in the 2004 to 2018 National Health Interview Survey were included in pooled cross-sectional data analyses. Separately for Black and White men and women, we calculated prevalence ratio and average marginal probability of short sleep (<7 hours) for each education level over the study period based on weighted logistic regression models.
Results.
Opposite educational gradients in short sleep were observed between Black and White adults. Greater educational attainment was associated with lower likelihood of short sleep among White adults but higher likelihood of short sleep among Black adults. Such heterogeneous educational gradients were robust after accounting for a set of socioeconomic, family, and health factors and persisted between 2004 and 2018.
Conclusions.
The health implications of education are not uniform in the US population, and heterogeneous education effects on sleep duration persisted over the past decade. More scholarly attention is needed to identify challenges and barriers that may be unique for race, sex, and education subpopulations to maintain healthy sleep.
Keywords: educational gradients, short sleep, racial disparity, socioeconomic disparity
1. Introduction
Sleep is a fundamental activity of human life and plays a vital role in human health. Growing evidence has linked short sleep duration, defined as less than seven hours per night on a regular basis1, with a wide range of mental and physical health conditions, including obesity, cardiovascular disease, and mental disorders2–4. While approximately a third of American adults sleep less than the current recommended minimum of seven hours per night1,5, the prevalence of short sleep is not uniformly distributed among sociodemographic groups6. In particular, previous studies have reported an educational gradient in sleep duration; that is, higher education levels are associated with lower likelihood of short sleep6–9.
However, little prior research has explored whether the educational gradient in sleep duration differs among sociodemographic groups. Black men and women may have different experiences in schools and work places than White men and women, so social factors such as education that prove to be protective for one race-sex group may not be protective or even counter-productive for other groups7,10,11. Race-related differences in sleep duration are already present in young adulthood12. Relatedly, prior studies reported that when compared to White individuals, Black individuals tend to exhibit weaker educational gradients in various health indicators including obesity and inflammation markers of chronic stress7,13. We thus first hypothesize that the protective effects of greater educational attainment for sleep are weaker for Black adults than for White adults (Hypothesis 1).
Second, prior studies have investigated temporal trends in sleep duration in the U.S. population, and some reported race differences in such trends 5,14–17. However, earlier studies did not examine the temporal trend in education effects on sleep duration among sociodemographic groups. While educational attainment has increased among all four race-sex groups over the past decade18, the implications of education for health differ among sociodemographic groups19,20. We are the first to explore how race and sex differences in educational gradients in sleep duration may have changed over the study period from 2004 to 2018.
Third, we assess the extent to which employment and work hours may account for the heterogeneities in educational gradients in sleep. Scholars suggested that work hours are the most important activity that competes with sleep time17. Race and sex minority groups have been historically underrepresented in higher education institutes and professional and management occupations21,22. In response to a hostile social environment that continuously exposes them to psychological stressors, as the John Henryism hypothesis posits, highly-educated Black individuals and other minority groups may develop a laborious work ethic that leads to more work hours23,24. We thus consider how employment status and work hours may account for the differential educational gradients. We expect race-sex differences in educational gradients in sleep to be reduced after adjusting for employment status and work hours (Hypothesis 2). Because education may be related to sleep through other aspects of socioeconomic status (SES) and health factors, we also examine the educational gradient after adjusting for income, occupation, and home ownership and a set of family- and health-related factors.
2. Data and Methods
We used the data from the National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) from 2004 to 2018, harmonized and distributed by IPUMS NHIS25. For every year since 2004, the NHIS asks respondents to report their usual or typical number of hours of sleep in a 24-hour period. Short sleep is defined as less than seven hours’ sleep1. We focused on the US-born adult population aged 25 to 69. Based on the “EDUC” variable that reports the respondents’ years of schooling and degree, we created four educational attainment categories including less than high school, high school graduate, some college, and college and above. We considered a set of socioeconomic covariates including household income (“INCFAM97ON2”: 1=$34,999 or less; 2=$35,000-$74,999; 3=$75,000 or above), occupation categories (“OCCUPN204”: 1=managerial or professional occupations; 2=supportive occupations; 3=laborer), home ownership (“OWNERSHIP”: 1=rent or other; 2=own a home), employment status (“EMPSTAT”: 1=out of labor force; 2=unemployed; 3=employed), and work hours (“HOURSWRK”: 1=zero hours worked, unemployed, or out of labor force; 2=1–34 hours; 3=35–49 hours; 4=50–80 hours). Other control variables include marital status, family size, and body mass index (BMI); see Table S2 for more details.
The sample consists of 16,662 Black men, 26,053 Black women, 97,355 White men, and 111,924 White women, and the total sample size is 251,994 (Model 1). Removing persons with missing information on employment or work hours for subsequent analyses reduces the sample size by about 1% for each race-sex group (Model 2). For later analyses that include all covariates, we deleted records with missing values on any variable, resulting in approximately 21.2%−25.1% and 17.0%−20.5% reduction for the Black and White adult samples, respectively (Model 3). We performed complete-case analysis because the missing occurs only in covariates and thus does not depend on the dependent variable in the logistic regression models26–29.
We examined the educational gradients in sleep among race-sex groups using weighted logistic regression models. The NHIS are household surveys, so for all our analyses of individual-level data we used the recommended weight “SAMPWEIGHT” as the sample weight in the “svyglm” function in R’s “survey” package. We began with a model that includes main effects of education levels and race-sex groups, and their interactions. Because interaction effects in nonlinear logistic regressions are not directly interpretable30–32, we conducted subsample analysis for each race-sex group33 to better illustrate sleep disparities and the heterogeneous educational gradients (Model 1). Specifically, we fit separate weighted logistic regression models for each race-sex group and calculated prevalence ratio34,35 of short sleep for three educational attainment group compared to high school graduates within each race-sex group. We then assessed how the aforementioned variables may explain the heterogeneity in educational gradients within race-sex groups by adding these variables (Models 2 and 3) to the main analytical model and to subsample analyses for each race-sex group.
Lastly, we investigated whether the educational gradients in short sleep changed over four periods of time: 2004–2007, 2008–2010, 2011–2014, and 2015–2018. Because sleep duration changes with age with middle-aged adults having less sleep than the younger and the older, all analyses were adjusted for nine age groups (25–29, 30–34, …, 60–64, and 65–69). Another dimension of time-related change is the cohort process, in which recently-born cohorts enter the population while the older cohorts exist36,37. We chose not to include cohort in the analysis for two reasons. First, there is little theoretical basis to expect cohort differences in short sleep. Second, to check robustness, we analyzed the data using the age-period-cohort-interaction model developed by Luo and Hodges38 and found little cohort variation in short sleep.
3. Results
Table 1 presents weighted prevalence rates in short sleep by educational attainment in each race-sex subsample. On average, Black adults had higher prevalence of short sleep (40.9% in men and 41.0% in women) than White adults (32.2% in men and 30.7% in women). When further stratifying by education, short sleep was most prevalent among Black men and women with some college education (45.5% and 44.4%, respectively), followed by college-educated Black adults (43.5% in men and 42.2% in women). Among White adults, high school graduates had the highest prevalence of short sleep (36.2% and 37.3% among men and women, respectively), while college-educated White adults had the lowest prevalence (26.8% and 24.6% among men and women, respectively).
Table 1.
Educ | Black Men |
Black Women |
White Men |
White Women |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Level | Short sleep | N | Short sleep | N | Short sleep | N | Short sleep | N |
Overall | 40.9% | 16,662 | 41.0% | 26,053 | 32.2% | 97,355 | 30.7% | 111,924 |
<HS | 37.9% | 5,614 | 37.3% | 7,220 | 34.7% | 26,084 | 33.4% | 27,165 |
HS | 33.9% | 2,801 | 37.5% | 4,149 | 36.2% | 8,538 | 37.3% | 8,735 |
SCol | 45.5% | 5,202 | 44.4% | 9,372 | 35.0% | 29,760 | 33.7% | 37,221 |
Col | 43.5% | 3,045 | 42.2% | 5,312 | 26.8% | 32,973 | 24.6% | 38,803 |
Note: N is the unweighted sample size for each race-sex and education group. Table figures are weighted prevalence rates of sleeping for less than seven hours per 24-hour period in the NHIS data. <HS: less than high school; HS: high school; SCol: some college; Col: college or more.
A whole-sample analysis that considers the interaction terms between education and race-sex groups reveals that the association between education and sleep duration differs between White and Black adults (F=42.675, p<.001; See Table S1 for logistic regression results). Figure 1 illustrates and Table 2 reports the heterogeneity in educational gradients based on subsample analysis for each of the four race-sex groups after adjusting for age and time periods. The symbols in the top panel represent predicted probability ratio of short sleep for those with less than high school education, some college, and college degree or more compared to high school graduates without adjusting for the potential impacts of other SES or health factors. Among White adults, the likelihood of short sleep decreases as education increases, although the some-college group does not appear to differ from those with a high school diploma. For example, compared to White men with high school education, the likelihood of short sleep is 5.7% (se=.023, p<.05) higher for White men without a high school diploma and 23.2% (se=.012, p<.001) lower among those with a college degree. For White women, the probability is 13.2% (se=.024, p<.001) higher for those without finishing high school and 28.8% (se=.011, p<.001) lower for the college educated. The educational gradients also appear more pronounced among White women than White men.
Table 2.
Educ | Black Men |
Black Women |
White Men |
White Women |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Level | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 |
<HS | 92.0% * | 93.4% | 93.0% | 101.9% | 101.6% | 99.8% | 105.7% * | 106.6% ** | 102.0% | 113.2% *** | 113.5% *** | 104.0% |
(0.035) | (0.036) | (0.041) | (0.031) | (0.031) | (0.035) | (0.023) | (0.023) | (0.025) | (0.024) | (0.024) | (0.026) | |
SCol | 119.8% *** | 116.7% *** | 115.0% *** | 117.7% *** | 117.3% *** | 113.5% *** | 100.3% | 99.5% | 101.2% | 98.8% | 98.3% | 101.2% |
(0.034) | (0.034) | (0.037) | (0.028) | (0.028) | (0.031) | (0.015) | (0.015) | (0.017) | (0.014) | (0.014) | (0.017) | |
Col | 113.5% *** | 108.7% * | 109.4% * | 110.5% *** | 109.3% ** | 109.6% * | 76.8% *** | 75.0% *** | 83.0% *** | 71.2% *** | 69.3% *** | 81.8% *** |
(0.037) | (0.037) | (0.047) | (0.030) | (0.031) | (0.041) | (0.012) | (0.012) | (0.017) | (0.011) | (0.011) | (0.016) |
Note: Table figures are predicted probability ratio (in percent) compared to high school graduates for each race-sex group based on weighted logistic regression models in Table S1. Numbers in parenthesis are standard errors. Model 1: adjusted for age and time periods. Model 2: adjusted for age, time periods, work hours, and employment status. Model 3: adjusted for age, time periods, socioeconomic variables, family- and health-related factors. <HS: less than high school; HS: high school; SCol: some college; Col: college or more.
p<0.5
p<0.1
p<0.001.
For Black adults, high school or less education does not appear to be associated with increased likelihood of short sleep. Among Black men, education seems positively associated with elevated likelihood of short sleep; the probability is 19.8% (se=.034, p<.001) and 13.5% (se=.037, p<.001) higher for those with some college and college degree, respectively, than Black men with a high school degree. Among Black women, the relative risk is 17.7% (se=.028, p<.001) and 10.5% (se=.030, p<.001) higher for the two more educated groups, respectively, compared to Black women with a high school degree.
Figure 2 presents period trends in predicted probabilities of short sleep based on subsample analysis for each education-race-sex group (see Table S3 for logistic regression coefficient estimates). For all education levels, the probability of short sleep increased from the 2000s to the 2010s, and this trend was similar across all race-sex groups. Most interestingly, the Black-White disparity in short sleep increases with educational attainment; the Black-White disparity is most pronounced among adults with a college degree, followed by the some-college-education group and the high school graduates. There seems relatively little race-sex variation in the least education group. Overall, these results suggest that race differences in the educational gradients in sleep duration remained stable over the past decade.
The middle panel in Fig 1 illustrates and Model 2 in Table 2 shows how these educational gradients in short sleep may (not) change after adjusting for employment status and work hours. The educational differences in Model 2 in Table 2 are largely comparable with those from Model 1, and the overall patterns of the educational gradients in short sleep shown in Fig 1 are largely similar in the upper and middle panels. These results suggest that the heterogeneous educational gradients in short sleep may not be explained by work-related factors.
The bottom panel in Fig 1 and Model 3 in Table 2 show that the higher likelihood of short sleep among White adults without a high school diploma is reduced (2.0% higher, p>.05 among men and 4.0% higher, p>.05 among women) after adjusting for income, occupation, home ownership, marital status, family size, and BMI, although the protective effect of college education against short sleep remains for White adults (17.0%lower, p<.001 among men and 18.2% lower, p<.001 among women). In contrast, the opposite educational gradients among Black adults are largely unchanged after controlling for the aforementioned variables.
4. Discussion
Using the 2004–2018 NHIS data, we found opposite educational gradients in sleep duration between Black and White adults. Specifically, greater educational attainment is associated with lower likelihood of short sleep among White adults, whereas Black adults with some college education or a college degree are more likely to report short sleep than Black adults with less than high school education or a high school degree. As a result, the Black-White difference in sleep duration is most pronounced among the college educated. Within each race, the overall patterns of educational gradients appear to be similar between men and women, although White women showed a steeper educational gradient than White men, whereas Black women showed a somewhat less steep gradient than Black men, suggesting that subtle sex-differences may exist within race. These race and sex differences in the educational gradients in sleep duration are robust after accounting for a set of socioeconomic, family, and health-related factors. Such heterogeneous educational gradients and the Black-White disparity in short sleep persisted over the study period from 2004 to 2018.
Education is believed to confer health benefits. We found such benefits of education among White adults: completing college education is associated with about 25% lower likelihood of short sleep than high school education. However, a college degree implies no protection against—in fact, higher likelihood of—short sleep for Black adults than their peers with high school or less education. Our finding echoes Jackson and colleagues’ work10 on race differences in the occupation-sleep association; they found that professional roles were associated with an elevated risk of short sleep duration among Black adults, whereas the opposite was observed among White adults. It is possible that the differential educational gradients in short sleep duration that we revealed in this study are driven by the occupational gradients because college graduates are more likely than the less educated to have professional and managerial jobs. However, the opposite educational gradients between Black and White adults remain largely unchanged after adjusting for occupation. This may be because education and occupation represent different aspects of socioeconomic positions and different socialization processes. Future research may investigate each factor’s unique implications for sleep.
Basner and colleagues17 showed that work activity is an important determinant that affects sleep. We investigated how employment status and work hours may explain the educational gradients in sleep duration. On the one hand, our main analysis showed that employment status or work hours do not directly account for the differential educational gradients between Black and White adults. On the other hand, the educational gradients appear to be more pronounced among the employed than the unemployed or out of labor force (results available upon request), suggesting that education and its related factors may interact with work environment to influence sleep time17. We also note that work hours are just one work-related factor that may affect sleep. Other characteristics including shifts and responsibilities merit future investigation. Moreover, a promising direction for future research is to use longitudinal data that follow participants’ work and time allocation pre, current, and post college years. With the rising estimated burden of educational loans39, it will be important to explore how acquired educational loans for some of the low-SES college-educated population may further strengthen their need to work at the potential cost of their sleep.
Why are college-educated Black adults more likely to sleep less even after accounting for other social, demographic, and health factors? On the one hand, our findings support the Minorities’ Diminished Returns (MDRs) theory, which suggests that SES indicators, such as educational attainment, do not provide Black individuals and other minorities (e.g., Hispanics) the same protective health benefits as non-Hispanic White individuals due to systematic barriers inflicted on minorities40–42. On the other hand, the results are also distinctly different from those in previous studies that supported the MDRs theory: in this study, greater educational attainment was not associated with a merely smaller benefit, but rather an opposite effect on sleep duration, suggesting that other mechanisms may be in play.
Scholars have hypothesized that chronic exposure to psychological stress may be a key mechanism underlying the racial and socioeconomic disparities in health behaviors and outcomes6,43. In particular, we argue that this could be reflective of Smith’s theoretical concept of “racial battle fatigue”44. This concept states that Black men with higher levels of education may be more susceptible than those with lower education to experiencing mundane, extreme, environmental stress (MEES) in social institutions such as school and workplace lacking racial and gender diversity. As result, Black men are likely to require additional energy and mental effort to process and cope with the MEES, which ultimately could elicit and exacerbate stress responses at psychological (e.g., apathy), physiological (e.g., sleep disturbances), and emotional/behavioral (e.g., overeating) levels.
Relatedly, Sellers and colleagues45 suggested that college-educated Black men are likely to perceive heightened MEES, particularly when personal aspirations and goals are not achieved. The negative impact of this goal-driving stress on mental health was significantly reduced for college-educated Black men who rationalized the challenge in achieving their goals to an external attribution orientation (e.g., lack of personal achievement perceived to be a result of societal discrimination) rather than an internal attribution orientation (e.g., lack of personal achievement perceived to be due self-image). Another study11 from the Sister Study on US working women provides additional support for the concept of “racial battle fatigue”. The authors revealed that greater likelihood for shorter sleep duration was associated with occupational discriminatory experiences attributed to racial identity. Unfortunately, the NHIS data lacks reliable measures of these culturally relevant stressors to test whether these prior empirical findings explain the current observations. Future research should explore whether Black men with higher educational levels may be susceptible to sleep distributions due to MEES and exposure to racial microaggressions, and whether attributional orientations (external/system-blame rather than internal/self-blame46) may be a protective of sleep health.
Among White adults, lower levels of education are associated with higher prevalence of short sleep, and White and Black adults with less than high school education have comparable probabilities of short sleep. A possible explanation for these findings is that White adults with lower levels of education are also experiencing social disadvantages and, consequently, psychosocial stressors or goal-driving stress. Prior evidence has suggested that White adults exposed to similar challenging social contexts as Black adults tend to exhibit poor health outcomes23. Specifically, in a sample of low-income, urban-dwelling White individuals (n=573), 38.9% had experienced discrimination, and their perceived discrimination was associated with higher depressive symptomology and anxiety47. The authors concluded that discriminatory experiences could be due to their SES and/or racial/ethnic background, particularly in environments where their identified racial/ethnic group is considered a minority population. Finally, it is speculated that low-income White adults may be particularly vulnerable to psychosocial stressors adversely impacting their health because social support networks and coping strategies, commonly utilized by other ethnic minority groups experiencing social challenges, may be less readily available47. Again, the current study does not include data to explore whether these prior findings could further explain our current observations. However, future research should attempt to disentangle these incongruent observations between the racial groups by identifying contextual and culturally relevant factors related to sleep health within the racial groups.
There are other socioeconomic parameters (e.g., reading literacy, financial strain, and neighborhood disadvantage) that are uniquely linked to sleep, health outcomes, and mortality even after accounting for education quantity. Thus, when exploring the education-sleep association, future research should look beyond strictly including education quantity as a comprehensive metric of SES. Meanwhile, SES including education is linked to health outcomes and mortality through an array of mechanisms including health knowledge, behaviors, and resources20. Furthermore, the dynamic sociopolitical climate (e.g. new local, state, or national legislation) could alter the relevance of existing mechanisms to explain the SES-health link or could elucidate new pertinent mechanisms. Thus, it is important for researchers to be cognizant that socioeconomic factors, such as education, may not be fully explained by one or a small number of mechanisms, particularly over time. This perspective further warrants more integration of an array of potential social determinants of health, so researchers and policy makers can better track what mechanisms are potential meaningful targets for health prevention/intervention.
It is important to note that our analysis is restricted to US-born Black and White adult samples in the NHIS, and the conclusions may not apply to the Hispanic or Asian population. The majority of the two ethnic groups consist of foreign-born respondents (65.4% among Hispanics and 80.7% among Asians). It has been shown that the foreign-born differ from the native-born in many health behaviors and outcomes7,48–51. Moreover, there is much heterogeneity among these ethnic groups in their health behaviors and outcomes that requires additional analyses 52–56. These complexities are beyond the scope of the current study and merit future research.
Our study has three other limitations. First, we used educational attainment based on self-reported years of education to investigate the education-sleep relationship. However, such measurement may be inadequate because the type, context, and implications of college education may differ between race and sex groups19. It is important to investigate the education-sleep association using other measures of education that considers the institutional, historical, and social contexts that shape the process of formal schooling within race and sex groups. Second, we relied on self-reported measures for sleep duration and explanatory variables that may be inaccurate and subject to recall bias. The heterogenous educational gradients are also observed using other definition of short sleep duration (e.g., less than six hours per 24-hour period or sleep hours; results are available upon request), but future studies may want to include objective measures. Another limitation is that we were unable to examine whether sleep quality is related to education in the same manner as sleep duration. Third, it is unclear whether the NHIS participants of different racial and/or socioeconomic backgrounds were recruited from similar residential areas. Because racial and gender minorities and/or lower socioeconomic individuals often reside in segregated residential neighborhoods that are susceptible to social disadvantages, sampling strategies used in many large national studies including the NHIS may result in enrolled racial groups with disparate social contexts, which can bias racial differences in health23. When participant samples are strategically selected from similarly challenging social contexts, racial disparities in health are often minimized. For example, Gamaldo and colleagues57 observed non-significant differences in sleep durations between Black and White adults enrolled in the HANDLS study, a longitudinal project strategically designed to recruit Black and White participants residing in similar socially disadvantaged neighborhoods of Baltimore City, MD. While racial differences were not observed in sleep disturbances, the authors did observe that sociodemographic correlates of sleep disturbance varied between Black and White individuals, which supports that identification of culturally-relevant psychosocial stressors are important for understanding adverse health within race-sex groups.
5. Conclusion
We caution against a simplistic view of health benefits of education. In particular, the high prevalence of short sleep in low education levels and among Black adults with college education observed in our study suggests that these populations may be at especially high risks for adverse health outcomes related to sleep deficiency such as cardiometabolic conditions and mental disorders3,4, and may need more rigorous surveillance and interventions tailored to their special needs. Moreover, although improving upstream social factors such as education generally improve public health, further research is needed to identify the unique challenges and barriers that minority groups including race, sex, and socioeconomic minority may have faced to maintain healthy sleep.
Supplementary Material
References
- 1.Watson NF, Badr MS, Belenky G, et al. Recommended Amount of Sleep for a Healthy Adult: A Joint Consensus Statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society. Sleep. 2015;38(6):843–844. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4716 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Wu Y, Zhai L, Zhang D. Sleep duration and obesity among adults: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Sleep Med. 2014;15(12):1456–1462. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.07.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cappuccio FP, Cooper D, D’Elia L, Strazzullo P, Miller MA. Sleep duration predicts cardiovascular outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Eur Heart J. 2011;32(12):1484–1492. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehr007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Monti JM, Monti D. Sleep disturbance in generalized anxiety disorder and its treatment. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2000;4(3):263–276. doi: 10.1053/smrv.1999.0096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sheehan CM, Frochen SE, Walsemann KM, Ailshire JA. Are U.S. adults reporting less sleep?: Findings from sleep duration trends in the National Health Interview Survey, 2004–2017. Sleep. 2019;42(2). doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Grandner MA, Patel NP, Gehrman PR, et al. Who gets the best sleep? Ethnic and socioeconomic factors related to sleep complaints. Sleep Med. 2010;11(5):470–478. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.10.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kimbro RT, Bzostek S, Goldman N, Rodríguez G. Race, ethnicity, and the education gradient in health. Health Aff (Millwood). 2008;27(2):361–372. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.27.2.361 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Jarrin DC, McGrath JJ, Silverstein JE, Drake C. Objective and Subjective Socioeconomic Gradients Exist for Sleep Quality, Sleep Latency, Sleep Duration, Weekend Oversleep, and Daytime Sleepiness in Adults. Behavioral Sleep Medicine. 2013;11(2):144–158. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2011.636112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Whinnery J, Jackson N, Rattanaumpawan P, Grandner MA. Short and Long Sleep Duration Associated with Race/Ethnicity, Sociodemographics, and Socioeconomic Position. Sleep. 2014;37(3):601–611. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3508 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jackson CL, Redline S, Kawachi I, Williams MA, Hu FB. Racial Disparities in Short Sleep Duration by Occupation and Industry. Am J Epidemiol. 2013;178(9):1442–1451. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwt159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lee S, Chang A-M, Sandler DP, Buxton OM, Jackson CL. Multiple Types of Perceived Job Discrimination and Sleep Health among Working Women: Findings from the Sister Study. American Journal of Epidemiology. Published online In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jones RD, Jackson WB, Mazzei A, Chang A-M, Buxton OM, Jackson CL. Ethnoracial sleep disparities among college students living in dormitories in the United States: a nationally representative study. Sleep Health: Journal of the National Sleep Foundation. 2020;6(1):40–47. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2019.10.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Fuller-Rowell TE, Curtis DS, Doan SN, Coe CL. Racial Disparities in the Health Benefits of Educational Attainment: A Study of Inflammatory Trajectories Among African American and White Adults. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2015;77(1):33. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Knutson KL, Van Cauter E, Rathouz PJ, DeLeire T, Lauderdale DS. Trends in the Prevalence of Short Sleepers in the USA: 1975–2006. Sleep. 2010;33(1):37–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Basner M, Dinges DF. Sleep duration in the United States 2003–2016: first signs of success in the fight against sleep deficiency? Sleep. 2018;41(4). doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Matricciani L, Bin YS, Lallukka T, et al. Past, present, and future: trends in sleep duration and implications for public health. Sleep Health. 2017;3(5):317–323. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2017.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Basner M, Fomberstein KM, Razavi FM, et al. American Time Use Survey: Sleep Time and Its Relationship to Waking Activities. Sleep. 2007;30(9):1085–1095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ryan CL, Bauman K. Educational Attainment in the United States: 2015. Population Characteristics. Current Population Reports. P20–578. US Census Bureau; 2016. Accessed April 4, 2020. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED572028 [Google Scholar]
- 19.Zajacova A, Lawrence EM. The relationship between education and health: reducing disparities through a contextual approach. Annu Rev Public Health. 2018;39:273–289. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031816-044628 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Phelan JC, Link BG, Tehranifar P. Social Conditions as Fundamental Causes of Health Inequalities: Theory, Evidence, and Policy Implications. J Health Soc Behav. 2010;51(1_suppl):S28–S40. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383498 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Welle PD, Graf HM. Effective Lifestyle Habits and Coping Strategies for Stress Tolerance Among College Students. American Journal of Health Education. 2011;42(2):96–105. doi: 10.1080/19325037.2011.10599177 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Diggins A, Woods-Giscombe C, Waters S. The association of perceived stress, contextualized stress, and emotional eating with body mass index in college-aged Black women. Eating Behaviors. 2015;19:188–192. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.LaVeist T, Pollack K, Thorpe R, Fesahazion R, Gaskin D. Place, not race: disparities dissipate in southwest Baltimore when Blacks and Whites live under similar conditions. Health Aff (Millwood). 2011;30(10):1880–1887. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0640 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Flaskerud JH. Coping and Health Status: John Henryism. Issues in Mental Health Nursing. 2012;33(10):712–715. doi: 10.3109/01612840.2012.673695 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Blewett LA, Drew JAR, King Mariam L, Williams KCW. IPUMS Health Surveys: National Health Interview Survey, Version 6.4 [dataset]. Published online 2019. 10.18128/D070.V6.4 [DOI]
- 26.Little RJA. Regression with Missing X’s: A Review. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1992;87(420):1227–1237. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1992.10476282 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.King G, Honaker J, Joseph A, Scheve K. Analyzing Incomplete Political Science Data: An Alternative Algorithm for Multiple Imputation. American Political Science Review. 2001;95(1):49–69. doi: 10.1017/S0003055401000235 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.White IR, Carlin JB. Bias and efficiency of multiple imputation compared with complete-case analysis for missing covariate values. Statistics in Medicine. 2010;29(28):2920–2931. doi: 10.1002/sim.3944 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Vach W Logistic Regression with Missing Values in the Covariates. Springer-Verlag; 1994. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-2650-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ai C, Norton EC. Interaction terms in logit and probit models. Economics Letters. 2003;80(1):123–129. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1765(03)00032-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Aiken LS, West SG, Reno RR. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. SAGE; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Brambor T, Clark WR, Golder M. Understanding Interaction Models: Improving Empirical Analyses. Political Analysis. 2006;14(1):63–82. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpi014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Moody DLB, Leibel DK, Darden TM, et al. Interpersonal-level discrimination indices, sociodemographic factors, and telomere length in African-Americans and Whites. Biol Psychol. 2019;141:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Muller CJ, MacLehose RF. Estimating predicted probabilities from logistic regression: different methods correspond to different target populations. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(3):962–970. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bender R, Kuss O. Methods to calculate relative risks, risk differences, and numbers needed to treat from logistic regression. J Clin Epidemiol. 2010;63(1):7–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Firebaugh G Where Does Social Change Come from? Estimating the Relative Contributions of Individual Change and Population Turnover. Population Research and Policy Review. 1992;11(1):1–20. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Alwin DF, McCammon RJ. Generations, Cohorts, and Social Change. In: Mortimer JT, Shanahan MJ, eds. Handbook of the Life Course. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Springer; US; 2003:23–49. doi: 10.1007/978-0-306-48247-2_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Luo L, Hodges JS. The Age-Period-Cohort-Interaction Model for Describing and Investigating Inter-cohort Deviations and Intra-cohort Life-course Dynamics: Sociological Methods & Research. Published online January 23, 2020. doi: 10.1177/0049124119882451 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Houle JN, Addo FR. Racial Disparities in Student Debt and the Reproduction of the Fragile Black Middle Class. Sociology of Race and Ethnicity. 2019;5(4):562–577. doi: 10.1177/2332649218790989 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Assari S Unequal Gain of Equal Resources across Racial Groups. Int J Health Policy Manag. 2018;7(1):1–9. doi: 10.15171/ijhpm.2017.90 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Assari S, Cobb S, Saqib M, Bazargan M. Diminished Returns of Educational Attainment on Heart Disease among Black Americans. Open Cardiovasc Med J. 2020;14:5–12. doi: 10.2174/1874192402014010005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Assari S, Chalian H, Bazargan M. Race, Ethnicity, Socioeconomic Status, and Chronic Lung Disease in the U.S. Res Health Sci. 2020;5(1):48–63. doi: 10.22158/rhs.v5n1p48 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Pieterse AL, Carter RT. An examination of the relationship between general life stress, racism-related stress, and psychological health among Black men. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2007;54(1):101–109. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.54.1.101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Smith WA. Black faculty coping with racial battle fatigue: The campus racial climate in a post-civil rights era. In: A Long Way to Go: Conversations about Race by African American Faculty and Graduate Students. Higher ed, 1523–9551 ; v. 14.; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sellers SL, Neighbors HW, Bonham VL. Goal-striving stress and the mental health of college-educated Black American men: the protective effects of system-blame. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2011;81(4):507–518. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2011.01116.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.LaVeist TA, Sellers R, Neighbors HW. Perceived racism and self and system blame attribution: Consequences for longevity. Ethnicity & disease. 2001;11(4):711–721. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bower KM, Thorpe RJ, LaVeist TA. Perceived racial discrimination and mental health in low-income, urban-dwelling Whites. Int J Health Serv. 2013;43(2):267–280. doi: 10.2190/HS.43.2.e [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Acevedo-Garcia D, Soobader M-J, Berkman LF. The differential effect of foreign-born status on low birth weight by race/ethnicity and education. Pediatrics. 2005;115(1):e20–30. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Angel JL, Buckley CJ, Sakamoto A. Duration or Disadvantage? Exploring Nativity, Ethnicity, and Health in Midlife. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2001;56(5):S275–S284. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.5.S275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Collins JW, Shay DK. Prevalence of Low Birth Weight among Hispanic Infants with United States-born and Foreign-born Mothers: The Effect of Urban Poverty. Am J Epidemiol. 1994;139(2):184–192. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116980 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Danso K Nativity and Health Disparities: Predictors of Immigrant Health. Social Work in Public Health. 2016;31(3):175–187. doi: 10.1080/19371918.2015.1099494 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Borella E, Ghisletta P, de Ribaupierre A. Age Differences in Text Processing: The Role of Working Memory, Inhibition, and Processing Speed. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2011;66(3):311–320. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cho Y, Frisbie WP, Hummer RA, Rogers RG. Nativity, Duration of Residence, and the Health of Hispanic Adults in the United States. International Migration Review. 2004;38(1):184–211. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-7379.2004.tb00193.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Oza-Frank R, Venkat Narayan KM. Masked heterogeneity in obesity between immigrant subgroups. Am J Public Health. 2008;98(6):967–968. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.134809 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Salant T, Lauderdale DS. Measuring culture: a critical review of acculturation and health in Asian immigrant populations. Social Science & Medicine. 2003;57(1):71–90. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00300-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Zsembik BA, Fennell D. Ethnic variation in health and the determinants of health among Latinos. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;61(1):53–63. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Gamaldo AA, McNeely JM, Shah MT, Evans MK, Zonderman AB. Racial differences in self-reports of short sleep duration in an urban-dwelling environment. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;70(4):568–575. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.