Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Jan 31;17(1):e0262351. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262351

Cleaner wrasse Labroides dimidiatus perform above chance in a “matching-to-sample” experiment

Mélisande Aellen 1,*, Ulrike E Siebeck 2, Redouan Bshary 1
Editor: Adrian G Dyer3
PMCID: PMC8803161  PMID: 35100297

Abstract

Concept learning have been studied widely in non-human animal species within or not an ecological context. Here we tested whether cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus, which show generalised rule learning in an ecologically relevant context; they generalise that any predator may provide protection from being chased by other fish; can also learn a general concept when presented with abstract cues. We tested for this ability in the matching-to-sample task. In this task, a sample is shown first, and then the subject needs to choose the matching sample over a simultaneously presented different one in order to obtain a food reward. We used the most general form of the task, using each stimulus only once in a total of 200 trials. As a group, the six subjects performed above chance, and four individuals eventually reached learning criteria. However, individual performance was rather unstable, yielding overall only 57% correct choices. These results add to the growing literature that ectotherms show the ability of abstract concept learning, though the lack of stable high performance may indicate quantitative performance differences to endotherms.

Introduction

Abstract concept learning show the ability to categorize/classify objects based on similar shape, association or relation equities and then being able to transfer this knowledge to new conditions/situations [1]. Four main different types of abstract concept learning are described, differing in the way individuals have to discriminate between stimuli (reviewed in [2]). Discrimination can be based on forming classes, such as chair or flower, known as perceptual concept learning [3]; discrimination to form categories by associating stimuli to another, such as an object with the word for that object, known as associative concept learning (reviewed in [4]); discrimination via the relationship between or among stimuli, such as same/different, known as the relational concept learning [5]; and finally the discrimination via analogy, i.e. develop the relations between relations, such as a set of five same icons is similar to a set of five same but different icons and dissimilar to a set of five different icons, known as the analogical reasoning [68]. Overall, abstract concepts learning are represented by the relation of taught rules with training stimuli to novel stimuli. Because such cognitive flexibility is supposedly more complex than standard reinforcement learning [912], it has attracted considerable attention in animal cognition research [13].

As the ability to form abstract concepts is considered to be cognitively demanding, only large-brained animal species, like primates, were initially taken into consideration [10, 1417]. However, the ability to learn abstract concepts eventually turned out to be widespread in the animal kingdom, like in primates, dolphins, seals, birds, rodents, octopus, mollusks, insects and fish [2, 1820]. Experiments on abstract concept learning test for the animals’ ability to generalise. Generalisation requires the ability to conceptualise a new environmental input based on available knowledge, i.e. relating a new situation with information acquired from previous experience [21]. A standard approach is to offer subjects in succession diverse pairs of stimuli, where only one is rewarding. Each single combination can be learned by operant conditioning, i.e. the subject associating its choice of stimulus with either a positive or a negative reinforcement [22, 23]. However, if subjects are able to extract the general rule that “in any pair of stimuli, one is rewarding while the other one is not”, they can solve any new stimulus combination from the second trial onwards. This is because while the choice during the first trial is necessarily random, a reward shows that the chosen stimulus is the correct one, while no reward shows that the non-chosen stimulus must be the correct one [24]. Another commonly used task is the same/different discrimination where individuals have to discriminate if two stimuli are the same or if they are different based on their physical pattern [25].

Yet another paradigm employed to determine the relational, i.e. same/different, concept competence in animals is the matching-to-sample task (hereafter MTS) [2630]. Subjects are shown a stimulus (the sample), and then they can choose between simultaneously presented matching and differing stimuli. Only the choice of the matching stimulus is rewarded. In the simultaneous MTS, the sample stimulus is presented and then both, the matched and the non-matched stimuli are presented beside it [3136]. In the delayed MTS, the sample is removed before the two choice stimuli are presented [36]. Crucially, the sample varies from trial to trial, forcing subjects to comprehend the concept of ‘same’ and ‘different’ by relating the choice stimuli to the sample in order to succeed [1, 2]. Hitherto, two main experimental paradigms have been used to test animals. In the most commonly used paradigm, subjects are confronted during an initial learning phase repeatedly with the same two choice items but each is presented as sample stimulus in a counterbalanced way [1, 15, 32, 36, 37]. Once subjects reach learning criterion (often only after running hundreds of trials), a transfer test is conducted: two new stimuli are introduced and it is tested whether subjects reach learning criterion faster with the new combination (evidence for transfer of acquired knowledge). In the other paradigm, experimenters operate from the beginning with a larger number of samples and corresponding pairs of stimuli [37].

Studies using the transfer task paradigm showed variation in the performance of the different taxa tested, with several studies yielding positive results (bottlenose dolphins [38, 39], California sea lions [40], capuchin monkeys [15, 33], chimpanzees ([36, 37]; cited by [41]), hens [42], honeybees [43], pigeons [29, 32, 37, 44], and rats [31]; summarized in S1 Table). Different senses were used in those diverse studies, such as olfaction, audition, and vision. Overall, only a few studies have tested fish so far: goldfish [45, 46], Malawi cichlids [47], archerfish [35], and zebrafish [36]. In those studies, all fish were tested with visual stimuli using either coloured light or forms and shapes. Evidence for simultaneous MTS was found in goldfish [46] and zebrafish [36], but neither in cichlids [47] nor in archerfish [35]. No fish species has yet been tested in the second paradigm which uses a large number of different samples over successive trials. This is unfortunate as the second paradigm is more indicative of generalised rule learning, as the transfer task paradigm may yield high performance with subjects being able to ‘learn-how-to-learn’ [44, 48] rather than using a general rule.

Given the mixed evidence for fishes regarding their capacity for conceptual learning in the simultaneous MTS task, we tested cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus, using different shapes and colour 2D images as stimuli, where any stimulus was used only in one trial. Cleaner fish are a suitable study species for an MTS task using abstract stimuli as there is evidence that they use generalised rule learning in an ecologically relevant context [49]. The ecologically relevant context is linked to conflicts between cleaners and ‘client’ reef fish that arise when cleaner feed on client mucus rather than on client ectoparasites [50]. Clients sometimes respond to cleaners eating mucus with aggressive chasing [51]. In such situations, cleaners may seek a passing-by predatory client, which functions as a social tool as its mere proximity makes the punishing individual stop its pursuit [52]. Within this context, Wismer et al. (2016) showed evidence for generalised rule learning in controlled laboratory experiments. In these experiments, cleaners were offered preferred and non-preferred food items on a Plexiglas plate. In response to cleaners eating a preferred item, the experimenter would start chasing the cleaner with the plate (manoeuvring a lever attached to the plate). The chasing would push the cleaners in the direction of two laminated pictures at the end of the aquarium, one showing a predatory client and one showing a harmless client. If the cleaner approached the picture of the predator, the pursuit with the plate stopped, while the pursuit continued if the cleaner moved towards the image of the harmless client. Once individual cleaners showed a significant preference for the predator picture, the two pictures were exchanged. In these follow-up tasks, cleaners reached learning criteria significantly faster, which can interpreted as evidence for generalised rule learning [49], albeit with the caveat that ‘learning to learn’ offers an alternative explanation.

The results fit the prediction that animals typically excel in tasks that are presented in an ecologically relevant context [53, 54]. Solving ecologically relevant tasks may be achieved by any animal species, while more abstract presentations supposedly warrant relatively large brains that allow flexibility beyond concise situations [5558]. We, therefore, wanted to test if cleaner fish are capable of demonstrating the ability of concept learning even when the task has no ecological relevance, i.e. in an abstract MTS paradigm. In order to avoid any ‘learn-how-to-learn’ [44, 48] explanations as in the Wismer et al. (2016) study, and indeed all previous studies providing positive results on the MTS task, we opted against any repeated use of stimuli. Studies using multiple sample stimuli suggest that if anything the use of more stimuli (up to 76 in pigeons) may enhance generalised rule learning [32, 33, 59, 60]. Thus, in contrast to previous studies on MTS, we ran no training trials prior to testing. Instead, each symbol was used only once per fish. Under these circumstances, any performance above chance must be based on the formation of a general concept. The experiment should answer the question of whether cleaner fish show evidence for generalised rule learning only within a narrow ecologically relevant context or whether they can form concepts also in more abstract circumstances. In the latter case, we would conclude that concept learning is a domain-general cognitive capacity of cleaner fish.

Materials and methods

This study was conducted from April until May 2017 at the Gump research station on Moorea (French Polynesia). Six cleaner fish (Labroides dimidiatus) between 7.5 and 8.6 cm were caught with a barrier net (2m long, 1.5m high, mesh size 0.5cm) and using hand-nets in lagoons around Moorea. Cleaners were accustomed to plastic aquaria of a size of 69 x 50 x 43 cm (S1 Fig), the water level at 34 cm, with a small PVC pipe as a shelter for at least 19 days before experiments started. A pipe from the lagoon provided a continuous seawater flow.

Acclimation and pre-training

After seven days in captivity, cleaners were first trained to feed small items of mashed prawn off small dots painted on small Plexiglas plates (10 x 7 cm). For the experiment, they were habituated to approach the experimental plate (20 x 20 cm) with a random training symbol in the middle (symbol drawn on a white square piece of paper of dimension 3 x 3 cm), and when they would touch or bite the training symbol they would receive a food reward, i.e. a piece of mashed prawn, by inserting the reward plate (3 cm width). As a first training step, a food item was placed directly on the training symbol itself. Once fish were comfortable to approach the training symbol, the reward plate was used. Furthermore, subjects were familiarized with a dashed see-through barrier placed 10 centimetres from the aquarium wall with a sliding door inserted in the middle. The barrier created an “experimental” compartment into which the plate was introduced and a “resting” compartment to which the cleaner was initially confined. The door was opened once the experimental plate had been put into place (Fig 1). All cleaners readily approached the experimental plate before the trials began. The time it took each individual to be accustomed to the experimental setup and hence to start the experiment varied between 11 and 17 days. Consequently, not all individuals had the same amount of training days, and thus they did not start the experiment on the same day.

Fig 1.

Fig 1

Exposition 1 (a) and 2 (b) of the experimental plate. i. Set up, ii. Fish view. We used eight different positions around the central sample symbol as emplacements for the matched and the non-matched symbols (c).

Experimental procedure

Cleaners were tested in their home aquarium. Once the fish was in the “resting” area, we fixed a GoPro camera using a clamp to clip it at the edge of the aquarium wall such that it filmed the trial. After 60 seconds, we introduced the experimental plate with the “sample” symbol placed in the middle of a 3x3 square matrix with 5 cm between symbols emplacement, for about 10 seconds (giving the fish time to look at the “sample” symbol; Fig 1A). The symbols varied in shape, colour, and were of a dimension of 3x3 cm and were created for the purpose of this study. The experimental plate, with hooks glued on the back, was hanged on the aquarium wall avoiding hand movement that might interfere with the fish’s choice. The plate was then removed to add the “match” and “non-match” symbols. To avoid the development of side bias, these two symbols could be placed on any of the eight remaining squares (Fig 1C), with the exact locations pre-determined to balance positions across trials. After 25–30 seconds, the experimental plate was re-inserted with the three symbols (Fig 1B). The door was opened 3–5 seconds later so that the cleaner could make its choice. The fish had to be in physical contact with a symbol (either by giving tactile stimulation with its pelvic fins or by touching it with the mouth) so that we considered that a choice had been made. Only if the choice was the matching symbol, the cleaner received a small piece of mashed prawn as a food reward on the introduced reward plate. When it had finished eating, we gently removed both the rewarding and the experimental plate. If the “non-match” symbol was chosen, no reward plate was introduced but instead, the experimental plate was quickly removed. We completed one trial with each fish before we conducted the next round of trials. As a result, the intertrial interval (ITI) was about twenty minutes for each fish. Each individual fish was exposed to a maximum of 200 trials, each one presenting two new symbols, so that the fish was confronted with a maximum of 400 different symbols. The same set of 400 symbols was used for all fish but combinations, locations, and function (“match” and “not-match” symbols) were randomised (see S2 Fig for the first 20 symbols used, the rest of the symbols utilized can be found on 10.6084/m9.figshare.13522100).

Ten trials were conducted per individual per day. As long as cleaners did not reach the confirmation criteria (explained in the next paragraph), they were continuing in the experiment until the completion of 200 trials, i.e. 20 experimental days. On a few occasions, fish did not make a proper choice, i.e. they randomly touched both symbols with their body by swimming over them without stopping, and swimming back and forth in the aquarium until the 60s trial was timed out. These trials were discarded but not replaced. As a consequence, few individual daily data sets contained only nine data points. We directly recorded the individuals’ choices (correct/incorrect) but all trials were also video recorded and can be requested. Fish were fed for five minutes 20 minutes prior to the start of the experiment. As it stands cleaner fish eat regularly over eleven hours per day, and hence are likely hungry in the morning after 13 hours of fasting. Feeding them prior to the experiment aimed at reducing the probability that they make random choices because of high hunger levels. During the trials, cleaners did not obtain much food (ten small prawn items max if all choices had been correct). Therefore, when the experiment was finished for the day, the food plate was introduced for about two hours so that subjects could obtain the caloric needs for the day.

Learning and confirmation criterion

An individual was considered to have learned the task if it performed significantly above chance levels (based on a sign test table/ binomial tests) in either one session (9 or 10 out of 10 trials correct), two successive sessions (twice at least 8 out of 10 trials correct) or three consecutive sessions (three times at least 7 out of 10 trials correct). All these learning criteria yield a p < 0.05 that performance was due to chance. In order to be conservative, we exposed any individual that reached a learning criterion to another 10 trials and only considered the individual to have succeeded if it chose correctly at least 7 out of 10 trials. Failure to achieve at least 7 out of 10 correct choices meant that the individual continued the simultaneous MTS experiment.

Statistical analyses

Apart from the individual learning criteria, we also asked whether our six cleaners as a group performed above chance levels, i.e. > 50%. We ran a fit Bayesian Generalized Linear Mixed-Effects Models with the statistical program Rstudio © (R Version 1.3.1093, © 2009–2019 RStudio, PBC) [61] using the package ‘lme4’ [62] and ‘blme’ [63]. The individuals’ choice was set as a binary response variable. In order to verify whether the session had an effect in our model, we set it as a fixed-effect variable. The individual was set as the random-effect variable (both at intercept and at slope level with respect to trial) of the ‘bglmer’ model. All data and the R-scripts to reproduce the analyses are available on 10.6084/m9.figshare.13522100.

Ethics approval

Our research study adheres to the ASAB/ABS Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research. The study was approved by the French Polynesian authorities responsible of the program ‘Délégation à la Recherche’ and by the Gump Research Station where the study was conducted in accordance with their rules and regulations for animal research. We acknowledge that catching is a stressful event for the fish. Afterwards, the fishes adapt well to captive conditions and lose their shyness towards human experimenters. The cognitive experiments can be seen as behavioural enrichment. All fish were released at their site of capture.

Results

Four out of the six cleaners eventually reached the learning criterion. Two of these four cleaners also reached the confirmation criterion in the next session, while the other two failed and never reached the learning criterion again until the completion of the 200 trials (Fig 2). Of the two successful cleaners, one completed the task in 40 trials (including the confirmation session), while the other one needed in total 180 trials.

Fig 2. Simultaneous matching-to-sample experiment.

Fig 2

The percentage when the matched symbol was chosen is represented as function of the session number (1 session = 10 trials; n = 6). The red dashed horizontal line indicates chance expectation (50% correct choices).

Combining individual performances, cleaners succeeded significantly above chance as a group (estimate = 0.18, std. error = 0.07, z-value = 2.51, p = 0.01; Fig 2). The effect size is small (n = 6, mean = 57.08%). Individual mean performance varied between 48.5% and 75% (Table 1). This latter high value was due to the individual reaching criterion within only 40 trials. Even without that individual, cleaners still performed above chance as a group (estimate = 0.15, std. error = 0.07, z-value = 2, p = 0.046). Nevertheless, we note that overall mean performance (57%) was only slightly above chance level, and there was no significant improvement with experience as the factor ‘session’ was not significant (estimate = - 0.0002, std. error = 0.01, z-value = - 0.01, p = 0.99).

Table 1. Mean percentage of correct choice (= success) and standard deviation of each individual throughout all sessions tested.

  Bouchon Chip Georges Leon Merlyn Titi
Mean % success 48.5% 56.3% 52.61% 75% 54.08% 56%
Standard deviation 17.3 13.5 14.6 5.8 18.9 19.8

Discussion

We had asked to what extent cleaner wrasses, Labroides dimidiatus, were able to learn an abstract general rule without experiments that allow the acquisition of intermediate steps, using the MTS task. According to our results, cleaners have the potential to learn an abstract rule as they performed above chance as a group. Four out of six cleaners eventually reached learning criteria that were set such that individuals performed better than expected by chance ([64]; described in [65, 66]), and two of these cleaners even passed a more conservative criterion. However, the cleaners’ performance was also not consistent, with two individuals returning to chance levels after reaching the initial criterion. This contrasts with other experiments in which cleaners generally continued to perform well in sessions after reaching learning criteria ([67]; Salwiczek et al., unpublished data). It also contrasts with the cleaners’ consistent ability to use generalised rule learning in an ecologically relevant task [49]. Taken together, as a species, cleaners expressed at best some limited understanding, with only a minority performing above chance. Single individuals excelling at specific tasks is a widespread phenomenon in cognitive studies [6871], highlighting the importance to understand intraspecific variation in cognitive performance [72].

To the best of our knowledge, the procedure that we used, i.e. the simultaneous MTS task in its most general form, was never tested before in non-human animals. Most previous studies started with a training phase that consisted of using two stimuli that alternated as matched and non-matched symbols, referred to as the ‘standard MTS paradigm’. When an individual reached learning criterion, another set of stimuli known as the transfer stimuli was presented. This transfer task was introduced to rule out the possibility that subjects had learned two configurations rather than the rule ‘match to sample’. If individuals had learned the rule, the number of trials needed to reach criterion with those second transfer stimuli should decrease compared to the training stimuli. While this approach yielded many positive results (S1 Table), some authors proposed the alternative explanation that subjects potentially merely learned how to learn rather than understand the concept [2, 18, 20, 44, 48]. There is some evidence that the general version may be more challenging than the standard version of the MTS task. Training pigeons with 76 trials per day, Wright et al. (1988) exposed two subjects to the standard version (2 stimuli alternating roles) and 2 subjects to 76 fixed pairs of stimuli (that would be used again over consecutive days, alternating roles as well). The two pigeons exposed to the standard version reached criteria after > 1000 trials, while it took the other two pigeons > 27000 trials. On the other hand, the latter performed very well in transfer trials while the former did not. Given that small differences in experimental design may cause major differences in performance, we consider it important that more studies will use our design on a variety of species belonging to different major vertebrate clades. In return, future research on cleaner fish should use the transfer task paradigm for the sake of obtaining data that are comparable to existing studies on MTS.

The fact that we obtained overall performance slightly above chance levels despite using new stimuli at every trial, as well as the significant performance of two individuals, favours the interpretation that cleaner wrasse are indeed capable of abstract concept learning [73, 74]. However, we note that the fish did not improve as a function of session their performance from the first 100 trials to the second 100 trials, which would be expected if they learned the task. One possibility is that the increment of the number of stimuli used during the experiment increased the information load that the fish were exposed to [73]. The constant use of new stimuli may have caused an overload of information, which increased the cognitive difficulty of the task and prevented a continued improvement of performance over the trials. It has been documented before that more information does not necessarily lead to better performance [74, 75]. Alternatively, cleaners might have a small perception bias that causes them to slightly favour the symbol that they see twice. To rule out that potential explanation, future experiments should reward the symbol that is the ‘Oddity from sample’ [76]. If cleaners perform above chance level in this opposite reward scheme, perception biases could be ruled out as an explanation for cleaner performance. The available literature suggests that choosing the odd option is simpler than choosing the matching option [76, 77], even more in the delayed version of the task [7880].

Until now, positive evidence for abstract rule learning by fishes in the MTS task was restricted to goldfish [45, 46] and zebrafish [36], the proper controls conducted with a negative reinforcer (electric shock) [46]. In contrast, archerfish [35] and a cichlid (Pseudotropheus sp.) failed at MTS tasks [47]. The current study with cleaner fish thus provides some additional evidence that fish (at least some individuals) can learn an abstract concept. We cannot propose an explanation for the positive results for goldfish and cleaners and the negative results for the other two species. The key idea of the MTS task is that it tests for abstract concept learning in a context that lacks ecological relevance. Neither goldfish nor cleaners appear to have particularly large brains compared to other fish species (Allen et Kuiter, 1999; Cuvier et Valenciennes, 1839; Fowler et Bean, 1928; Lacépède, 1801; Randall, 1981; Bleeker, 1855; all cited in [81, 82]). At least for cleaner fish there is evidence that they are able to use generalized rule learning in an ecologically relevant context [49, 52].

Conclusions

Cleaner wrasses, L. dimidiatus, can solve a MTS task but individual performance is not consistent, at least in the general version we used. We cannot compare their performance to other species because of the new design. In future studies, it would be interesting to compare the standard design (training with limited set of stimuli prior to continuously novel stimuli) with our design (continuously novel stimuli only) in a diversity of species, including cleaners. Compared to the data showing that pigeons require many thousands of trials with novel stimuli until they learned the task, the performance of cleaners within the 200 trials presented here was rather good. Thus, generalised rule learning in cleaners does not seem to be restricted to ecologically relevant contexts, indicating that abstract concept learning is a general cognitive tool present in this species.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Experimental aquarium design.

The “resting” area is on the left where the fish is waiting behind a see-through grid barrier with a door in the middle. On the right is the “experimental” compartment where the experimental plate is displayed during the trial.

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Representation of the twenty first symbols used in the matching-to-sample task.

The symbols were drawn on a 3 x 3cm white paper sheet.

(TIF)

S1 Table. Main results of MTS studies.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Gump research station in Moorea (French Polynesia) for letting us work in their station. We are particularly thankful to the directors and the staff for their unconditional help and great friendship. We thank Léonore Bonin for fish capturing. We acknowledge Dr. Christèle Borgeaud for English corrections and valuable comments. We thank Radu Alexandru Slobodeanu for statistical help and support.

Data Availability

All data and the R-scripts to reproduce the analyses are available at https://figshare.com/s/4ed4d7c5bcc750500de9.

Funding Statement

The study was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant numbers 31003A_153067 and 310030B_173334/1) to R.B. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Truppa V, Piano Mortari E, Garofoli D, Privitera S, Visalberghi E. Same/Different Concept Learning by Capuchin Monkeys in Matching-to-Sample Tasks. Santos L, editor. PLoS ONE. 2011. Aug 16;6(8):e23809. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023809 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Zentall TR, Wasserman EA, Lazareva OF, Thompson RKR, Rattermann MJ. Concept Learning in Animals. Comp Cogn Behav Rev [Internet]. 2008. [cited 2019 Oct 1];3. Available from: http://comparative-cognition-and-behavior-reviews.org/2008/vol3_zentall_wasserman_lazareva_thompson_rattermann [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Goldstone RL. Influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1994;123(2):178–200. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.123.2.178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Zentall TR, Wasserman EA, Urcuioli PJ. Associative concept learning in animals: ASSOCIATIVE CONCEPT LEARNING. J Exp Anal Behav. 2014. Jan;101(1):130–51. doi: 10.1002/jeab.55 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Thompson RK. Natural and relational concepts in animals. Comp Approaches Cogn Sci. 1995;175:224. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.French RM. The subtlety of sameness: A theory and computer model of analogy-making. MIT press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gentner D, Medina J. Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition. 1998. Jan;65(2–3):263–97. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(98)00002-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Vosniadou S, Ortony A. Similarity and analogical reasoning. Cambridge University Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Katz JS, Wright AA, Bodily KD. Issues in the comparative cognition of abstract-concept learning. Comp Cogn Behav Rev. 2007;2:79. doi: 10.3819/ccbr.2008.20005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Premack D. On the abstractness of human concepts: Why it would be difficult to talk to a pigeon In: Hulse SH, Fowler H, Honig WK, editors. Cognitive processes in animal behavior. 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Thomas RK. Evolution of Intelligence: an Approach to Its Assessment. Brain Behav Evol. 1980;17(6):454–72. doi: 10.1159/000121814 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Thomas RK. Investigating cognitive abilities in animals: unrealized potential. Cogn Brain Res. 1996. Jun;3(3–4):157–66. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(96)00003-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wynne CDL, Udell MAR. Animal cognition: evolution, behavior and cognition. Second edition. Houndsmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire; New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan; 2013. 346 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.D’Amato MR, Salmon DP. Cognitive processes in cebus monkeys. Anim Cogn. 1984;149–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.D’Amato MR, Salmon DP, Colombo M. Extent and limits of the matching concept in monkeys (Cebus apella). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1985;11(1):35–51. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.11.1.35 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Premack D. Animal Cognition. Annu Rev Psychol. 1983. Jan;34(1):351–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Premack D. The codes of man and beasts. Behav Brain Sci. 1983. Mar;6(1):125–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Penn DC, Holyoak KJ, Povinelli DJ. Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behav Brain Sci. 2008. Apr;31(2):109–30. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08003543 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Perry CJ, Barron AB, Cheng K. Invertebrate learning and cognition: relating phenomena to neural substrate: Invertebrate learning and cognition. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2013. Sep;4(5):561–82. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1248 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wasserman EA, Young ME. Same–different discrimination: The keel and backbone of thought and reasoning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2010;36(1):3–22. doi: 10.1037/a0016327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Emery NJ, Clayton NS. The Mentality of Crows: Convergent Evolution of Intelligence in Corvids and Apes. Science. 2004. Dec 10;306(5703):1903–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1098410 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Jenkins HM. Animal learning and behavior theory. In: The first century of experimental psychology. Routledge; 2019. p. 177–230. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Thorndike EL. Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychol Rev Monogr Suppl. 1898;2(4):i. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wynne CD. Animal cognition: The mental lives of animals. 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Thompson RK, Oden DL. Categorical perception and conceptual judgments by nonhuman primates: The paleological monkey and the analogical ape. Cogn Sci. 2000;24(3):363–96. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Clark HB, Sherman JA. Effects of a conditioned reinforcer upon accuracy of match-to-sample behavior in pigeons1. J Exp Anal Behav. 1970. May;13(3):375–84. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1970.13-375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ferster CB. Intermittent reinforcement of matching to sample in the pigeon1. J Exp Anal Behav. 1960. Jul;3(3):259–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Skinner BF. Are theories of learning necessary? Psychol Rev. 1950;57(4):193–216. doi: 10.1037/h0054367 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Blough DS. Delayed matching in the pigeon. J Exp Anal Behav. 1959. Apr;2(2):151–60. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1959.2-151 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Konorski JA. A New Method of Physiological Investigation of Recent Memory in Animals. Bull Acad Pol Sci. 1959;7:115–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Iversen IH. Acquisition of matching-to-sample performance in rats using visual stimuli on nose keys. J Exp Anal Behav. 1993. May;59(3):471–82. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1993.59-471 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bodily KD, Katz JS, Wright AA. Matching-to-sample abstract-concept learning by pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2008;34(1):178–84. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.34.1.178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Truppa V, Garofoli D, Castorina G, Piano Mortari E, Natale F, Visalberghi E. Identity concept learning in matching-to-sample tasks by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Anim Cogn. 2010. Nov;13(6):835–48. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0332-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gierszewski S, Bleckmann H, Schluessel V. Cognitive Abilities in Malawi Cichlids (Pseudotropheus sp.): Matching-to-Sample and Image/Mirror-Image Discriminations. Warrant EJ, editor. PLoS ONE. 2013. Feb 20;8(2):e57363. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Newport C, Wallis G, Siebeck UE. Concept learning and the use of three common psychophysical paradigms in the archerfish (Toxotes chatareus). Front Neural Circuits [Internet]. 2014. Apr 24 [cited 2017 Jan 24];8. Available from: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fncir.2014.00039/abstract [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bloch S, Froc C, Pontiggia A, Yamamoto K. Existence of working memory in teleosts: Establishment of the delayed matching-to-sample task in adult zebrafish. Behav Brain Res. 2019. Sep;370:111924. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.111924 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wright AA, Cook RG, Rivera JJ, Sands SF, Delius JD. Concept learning by pigeons: Matching-to-sample with trial-unique video picture stimuli. Anim Learn Behav. 1988. Dec;16(4):436–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Herman LM, Gordon JA. Auditory delayed matching in the bottlenose dolphin1. J Exp Anal Behav. 1974. Jan;21(1):19–26. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1974.21-19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Herman LM, Hovancik JR, Gory JD, Bradshaw GL. Generalization of visual matching by a bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): Evidence for invariance of cognitive performance with visual and auditory materials. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1989;15(2):124. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Pack AA, Herman LM, Roitblat HL. Generalization of visual matching and delayed matching by a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Anim Learn Behav. 1991. Mar;19(1):37–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Oden DL, Thompson RK, Premack D. Spontaneous transfer of matching by infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1988;14(2):140. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Foster TM, Temple W, Mackenzie C, DeMello LR, Poling A. Delayed matching-to-sample performance of hens: Effects of sample duration and response requirements during the sample. J Exp Anal Behav. 1995. Jul;64(1):19–31. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1995.64-19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Giurfa M, Zhang S, Jenett A, Menzel R, Srinivasan MV. The concepts of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ in an insect. Nature. 2001. Apr;410(6831):930–3. doi: 10.1038/35073582 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Nakamura T, Wright AA, Katz JS, Bodily KD, Sturz BR. Abstract-concept learning carryover effects from the initial training set in pigeons (Columba livia). J Comp Psychol. 2009;123(1):79–89. doi: 10.1037/a0013126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Goldman M, Shapiro S. Matching-to-sample and oddity-from-sample in goldfish. J Exp Anal Behav. 1979. Mar;31(2):259–66. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1979.31-259 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Zerbolio DJ, Royalty JL. Matching and oddity conditional discrimination in the goldfish as avoidance responses: Evidence for conceptual avoidance learning. Anim Learn Behav. 1983. Sep;11(3):341–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Gierszewski S, Bleckmann H, Schluessel V. Cognitive Abilities in Malawi Cichlids (Pseudotropheus sp.): Matching-to-Sample and Image/Mirror-Image Discriminations. Warrant EJ, editor. PLoS ONE. 2013. Feb 20;8(2):e57363. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Harlow HF. The formation of learning sets. Psychol Rev. 1949;56(1):51. doi: 10.1037/h0062474 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wismer S, Grutter A, Bshary R. Generalized rule application in bluestreak cleaner wrasse (Labroides dimidiatus): using predator species as social tools to reduce punishment. Anim Cogn. 2016. Jul;19(4):769–78. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-0975-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Grutter AS, Bshary R. Cleaner wrasse prefer client mucus: support for partner control mechanisms in cleaning interactions. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2003. Nov 7;270(Suppl_2):S242–4. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bshary R, Grutter AS. Experimental evidence that partner choice is a driving force in the payoff distribution among cooperators or mutualists: the cleaner fish case. Ecol Lett. 2002;5(1):130–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Bshary R, Wickler W, Fricke H. Fish cognition: a primate’s eye view. Anim Cogn. 2002. Mar 1;5(1):1–13. doi: 10.1007/s10071-001-0116-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kamil AC. On the Proper Definition of Cognitive Ethology. In: Animal Cognition in Nature [Internet]. Elsevier; 1998. [cited 2019 Jun 6]. p. 1–28. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/B9780120770304500532 [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Shettleworth SJ. The evolution of comparative cognition: Is the snark still a boojum? Behav Processes. 2009. Mar;80(3):210–7. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Burkart JM, van Schaik CP. Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates? Anim Cogn. 2010. Jan;13(1):1–19. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0263-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Burkart JM, Schubiger MN, van Schaik CP. The evolution of general intelligence. Behav Brain Sci [Internet]. 2017. [cited 2019 Oct 23];40. Available from: https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0140525X16000959/type/journal_article [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Deaner RO, Isler K, Burkart J, van Schaik C. Overall Brain Size, and Not Encephalization Quotient, Best Predicts Cognitive Ability across Non-Human Primates. Brain Behav Evol. 2007;70(2):115–24. doi: 10.1159/000102973 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.van Schaik CP, Isler K, Burkart JM. Explaining brain size variation: from social to cultural brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012. May;16(5):277–84. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Katz JS, Wright AA. Same/different abstract-concept learning by pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2006;32(1):80–6. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.1.80 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Katz JS, Wright AA, Bachevalier J. Mechanisms of same-different abstract-concept learning by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2002;28(4):358–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.TEAM, R. Core. TEAM, R. Core. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016. 2017. 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J Stat Softw [Internet]. 2015. [cited 2018 Oct 29];67(1). Available from: http://www.jstatsoft.org/v67/i01/ [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Chung Y, Rabe-Hesketh S, Dorie V, Gelman A, Liu J. A Nondegenerate Penalized Likelihood Estimator for Variance Parameters in Multilevel Models. Psychometrika. 2013. Oct;78(4):685–709. doi: 10.1007/s11336-013-9328-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Fisher RA. Statistical methods for research workers. 14th ed., revised and enlarged. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd; 1970. 362 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Salwiczek LH, Prétôt L, Demarta L, Proctor D, Essler J, Pinto AI, et al. Adult Cleaner Wrasse Outperform Capuchin Monkeys, Chimpanzees and Orang-utans in a Complex Foraging Task Derived from Cleaner–Client Reef Fish Cooperation. PLoS ONE. 2012. Nov 21;7(11):e49068. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Triki Z, Bshary R. Cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus discriminate numbers but fail a mental number line test. Anim Cogn [Internet]. 2017. Nov 13 [cited 2017 Dec 7]; Available from: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10071-017-1143-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Wismer S, Pinto AI, Triki Z, Grutter AS, Roche DG, Bshary R. Cue-based decision rules of cleaner fish in a biological market task. Anim Behav. 2019. Dec;158:249–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Aellen M, Dufour V, Bshary R. Cleaner fish and other wrasse match primates in their ability to delay gratification. Anim Behav. 2021. Jun;176:125–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Beran MJ, Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Pate JL, Rumbaugh DM. Delay of gratification in chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes). Dev Psychobiol. 1999. Mar;34(2):119–27. doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Herrmann E, Call J. Are there geniuses among the apes? Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2012. Oct 5;367(1603):2753–61. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0191 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Leonardi RJ, Vick S-J, Dufour V. Waiting for more: the performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) on exchange tasks. Anim Cogn. 2012. Jan;15(1):107–20. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0437-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Thornton A, Lukas D. Individual variation in cognitive performance: developmental and evolutionary perspectives. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2012. Oct 5;367(1603):2773–83. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0214 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Augier L, Thibaut J-P. The benefits and costs of comparisons in a novel object categorization task: Interactions with development. Psychon Bull Rev. 2013. Dec;20(6):1126–32. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0436-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Thibaut J-P, Witt A. Young children’s learning of relational categories: multiple comparisons and their cognitive constraints. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2015. May 19 [cited 2020 Apr 14];6. Available from: http://www.frontiersin.org/Cognition/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00643/abstract [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Thibaut J-P, French R, Vezneva M. Cognitive load and semantic analogies: Searching semantic space. Psychon Bull Rev. 2010. Aug;17(4):569–74. doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.4.569 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Wright AA, Delius JD. Learning Processes in Matching and Oddity: The Oddity Preference Effect and Sample Reinforcement. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2005;31(4):425–32. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.31.4.425 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Muszynski NM, Couvillon PA. Category difference facilitates oddity learning in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J Comp Psychol. 2020. Aug;134(3):266–79. doi: 10.1037/com0000228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Mishkin M, Delacour J. An analysis of short-term visual memory in the monkey. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1975;1(4):326–34. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.1.4.326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Overman WH. Performance on Traditional Matching to Sample, Non-Matching to Sample, and Object Discrimination Tasks by 12- to 32-Month-Old Children.: A Developmental Progression. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1990. Dec;608(1 The Developme):365–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48903.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Overman WH, McLain C, Ormsby GE, Brooks V. Visual recognition memory in squirrel monkeys.Anim Learn Behav. 1983. Dec;11(4):483–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Chojnacka D, Isler K, Barski JJ, Bshary R. Relative Brain and Brain Part Sizes Provide Only Limited Evidence that Machiavellian Behaviour in Cleaner Wrasse Is Cognitively Demanding. Iwaniuk A, editor. PLOS ONE. 2015. Aug 11;10(8):e0135373. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135373 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Kotrschal A, Rogell B, Bundsen A, Svensson B, Zajitschek S, Brännström I, et al. Artificial Selection on Relative Brain Size in the Guppy Reveals Costs and Benefits of Evolving a Larger Brain. Curr Biol. 2013. Jan;23(2):168–71. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.11.058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Adrian G Dyer

14 Jul 2021

PONE-D-21-17431

Cleaner wrasse Labroides dimidiatus perform above chance in a "Matching-to-sample" experiment

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Aellen,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

So far I have had one independent expert on animal behaviour review the manuscript, and I have also carefully read the work. I agree with R1 that the work is potentially of interest to publish, but clarity of explanations and writing can be improved a lot (see reveiwer report). In particular, I agree with R1 that readers need a more complete explanation of MTS (matching to sample)for which you use simultaneous presentation, and how delayed MTS (ie DMTS) experiments has been more frequently used in other animal species, at least in recent times. DMTS requires buth longer term memory formation of rule, and working memory (WM) of samples in a particular instance. The simultaneous MTS thus is not directlly comparable to DMTS; at least you need to clearly explain differences for readers and how such differences influence your experimental interpretation(s).

I also found the line "Fish were fed ad libitum for five minutes 20 minutes prior to the start of the experiment." confusing as to what this meant.........please use a lot more care so a general readership can fully understand your paper. I also was confused by s3 Figure which I cant see mentioned in main manuscript. Can you please take much more care in paper presentation and message? I have allowed time for a major revision to enable this. If you can do a good job I will then ask R1 and another expert to review the revised paper.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Aug 28 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Adrian G Dyer, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure: 

 [The study was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant numbers 31003A_153067 and 310030B_173334/1) to R.B. 

NO]. 

a) Please provide an amended Funding Statement that declares *all* the funding or sources of support received during this specific study (whether external or internal to your organization) as detailed online in our guide for authors at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submit-now.  

b) Please state what role the funders took in the study.  If any authors received a salary from any of your funders, please state which authors and which funder. If the funders had no role, please state: "The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript." 

Please send your amended statements by return email; we will change the online submission form on your behalf. 

3. Thank you for stating the following in the Acknowledgments Section of your manuscript: 

[We are grateful to the Gump research station in Moorea (French Polynesia) for letting us work in their station. We are particularly thankful to the directors and the staff for their unconditional help and great friendship. We thank Léonore Bonin for fish capturing. We acknowledge Dr. Christèle Borgeaud for English corrections and valuable comments. We thank Radu Alexandru Slobodeanu for statistical help and support. The study was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant numbers 31003A_153067 and 310030B_173334/1) to R.B.]

We note that you have provided funding information that is currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form. 

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. 

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information. 

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: In this study, the authors tested the ability of cleaner fish to succeed in a simultaneous matching-to-sample task. Such task is classically used to test for rule learning based on a ‘same’ abstract relational concept. This cognitive ability has been established in many animal species from mammals to insects. However, while some fish species appeared able to master this task (zebrafish and goldfish), others were not (e.g. archerfish). The current study thus proposes to further explore fish concept learning capacities by testing an additional species. This question is scientifically valid and of interest as I consider that the comparative cognition field certainly gains from more model species to be tested for key cognitive tasks. The results demonstrate great individual variability but with success of a few individuals. These results support the existence of an ability for solving matching-to-sample task in this species in particular as the procedure chosen is not classic and may have had detrimental effect on performance. Indeed, usually, the sample is not longer present when the subject has to make a choice between a stimulus identical to the sample and an alternative stimulus. In addition, only a limited number of samples are used in standard procedures at least during the acquisition phase while in the current study, each trial presented a novel stimulus. These key differences may have induced attentional bias and noise which prevented most subjects to extract the common rule between trials. Proposing alternative procedures is of high interest to better understand the underlying processes but I regret that the authors did not test in parallel the classical procedure for comparison. With the current data, it is indeed impossible to compare the cleaner fish performances with those of other species.

Other comments:

Introduction: The introduction needs quite a substantial editing. Some sentences are difficult to read, some others are incorrect or imprecise. I recommend the authors to invest more effort in drafting the introduction in a novel version of the manuscript.

A few examples:

l. 35 : do you mean a shaping procedure ? It is not clear what you mean by operant conditioning. Your description does not allow to understand what is the procedure to study the ability of animals to learn abstract concepts. Please revise the lines 34-37

l. 46 : I do not understand this sentence. ‘concepts are represented by the relation of abstract rules to novel stimuli’ ??

l. 49-53 : Difficult to follow, please revise for improved clarity

l.57 : The reference 27 seems fully out of context here. Could you please provide references of studies using simultaneous MTS to test for same/difference concept learning ?

l. 58: Incorrect references as they used a DMTS procedure and not a simultaneous MTS

Methods

l. 164 : Why did you feed the fish ab libitum before the experiment. This should induce a lack of motivation for the food reward.

l. 165 : Why did you let the food plate in place for 2 hours ?

I suggest to provide a video as supporting information to help the reader to better picture the whole procedure.

Discussion

l.228 : There is at least one study, the one in pigeons which is described a few sentences later (ref 12)

Bibliography : please check the references formatting as not consistent between references and does not follow the journal guidelines.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Jan 31;17(1):e0262351. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262351.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


28 Sep 2021

CC: adrian.dyer@rmit.edu.au

PONE-D-21-17431

Cleaner wrasse Labroides dimidiatus perform above chance in a "Matching-to-sample" experiment

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Aellen,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

So far I have had one independent expert on animal behaviour review the manuscript, and I have also carefully read the work. I agree with R1 that the work is potentially of interest to publish, but clarity of explanations and writing can be improved a lot (see reveiwer report). In particular, I agree with R1 that readers need a more complete explanation of MTS (matching to sample)for which you use simultaneous presentation, and how delayed MTS (ie DMTS) experiments has been more frequently used in other animal species, at least in recent times. DMTS requires buth longer term memory formation of rule, and working memory (WM) of samples in a particular instance. The simultaneous MTS thus is not directlly comparable to DMTS; at least you need to clearly explain differences for readers and how such differences influence your experimental interpretation(s).

I also found the line "Fish were fed ad libitum for five minutes 20 minutes prior to the start of the experiment." confusing as to what this meant.........please use a lot more care so a general readership can fully understand your paper. I also was confused by s3 Figure which I cant see mentioned in main manuscript. Can you please take much more care in paper presentation and message? I have allowed time for a major revision to enable this. If you can do a good job I will then ask R1 and another expert to review the revised paper.

Dear Dr Dyer, we would like to thank you for your comments. We considered each input and hope that the new version will be more suitable and reach your journal standards in PLOS ONE.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Aug 28 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

• A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

• A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

• An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Adrian G Dyer, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

Thank you, we will carefully follow the journal guidelines.

2. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure:

[The study was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant numbers 31003A_153067 and 310030B_173334/1) to R.B.

NO].

a) Please provide an amended Funding Statement that declares *all* the funding or sources of support received during this specific study (whether external or internal to your organization) as detailed online in our guide for authors at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submit-now.

We took good notes of it and will do the necessary, thank you.

b) Please state what role the funders took in the study. If any authors received a salary from any of your funders, please state which authors and which funder. If the funders had no role, please state: "The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript."

Please send your amended statements by return email; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

The amended statements have been returned by email, thank you.

3. Thank you for stating the following in the Acknowledgments Section of your manuscript:

[We are grateful to the Gump research station in Moorea (French Polynesia) for letting us work in their station. We are particularly thankful to the directors and the staff for their unconditional help and great friendship. We thank Léonore Bonin for fish capturing. We acknowledge Dr. Christèle Borgeaud for English corrections and valuable comments. We thank Radu Alexandru Slobodeanu for statistical help and support. The study was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant numbers 31003A_153067 and 310030B_173334/1) to R.B.]

We note that you have provided funding information that is currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form.

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement.

Thank you for your comment. We did remove the funding-related text from the manuscript and updated the Acknowledgments section accordingly on L. 314 - 319.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

We did add the captions for the Supporting Information at the end of the manuscript and updated them in the text accordingly, thank you.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Preamble to our replies to the specific referee comments below: we would like to thank the referee for his/her highly useful comments. The different comments have led to substantial changes in the introduction as demanded, and we trust that our revised version make the paper clearer.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

________________________________________

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

________________________________________

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Sorry we do not understand, under the ‘Statistical analyses’ part of the manuscript, it is stipulated that ‘All data and the R-scripts to reproduce the analyses are available at https://figshare.com/s/4ed4d7c5bcc750500de9.’ On L. 213-214.

________________________________________

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

________________________________________

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: In this study, the authors tested the ability of cleaner fish to succeed in a simultaneous matching-to-sample task. Such task is classically used to test for rule learning based on a ‘same’ abstract relational concept. This cognitive ability has been established in many animal species from mammals to insects. However, while some fish species appeared able to master this task (zebrafish and goldfish), others were not (e.g. archerfish). The current study thus proposes to further explore fish concept learning capacities by testing an additional species. This question is scientifically valid and of interest as I consider that the comparative cognition field certainly gains from more model species to be tested for key cognitive tasks. The results demonstrate great individual variability but with success of a few individuals. These results support the existence of an ability for solving matching-to-sample task in this species in particular as the procedure chosen is not classic and may have had detrimental effect on performance. Indeed, usually, the sample is not longer present when the subject has to make a choice between a stimulus identical to the sample and an alternative stimulus. In addition, only a limited number of samples are used in standard procedures at least during the acquisition phase while in the current study, each trial presented a novel stimulus. These key differences may have induced attentional bias and noise which prevented most subjects to extract the common rule between trials. Proposing alternative procedures is of high interest to better understand the underlying processes but I regret that the authors did not test in parallel the classical procedure for comparison. With the current data, it is indeed impossible to compare the cleaner fish performances with those of other species.

We would like to thank you for your comments. We agree that we cannot compare our study with others on the simultaneous matching-to-sample task as we did change the procedure substantially. However, we believe that this new procedure might be of great interest for further studies. Indeed, it is known that the use of novel stimuli over trials is the only way to determine whether animals achieved abstract concept learning . According to this view, using the same stimuli repeatedly over trials may only yield indicative results. While some studies have supplemented the initial solving with new stimuli afterwards, on an individual level it could still be a case of ‘learning to learn’. Furthermore, it has been shown that the fact to utilize an increased number of various stimuli enhance the gain of relational ability . We propose that our procedure avoids all these potential shortcomings; if subjects perform above chance it must be based on abstract concept learning. We do agree that to study cleaner fish in the ‘standard’ procedure of simultaneous MTS might be a nice future study. We say so explicitly in the new version in the discussion.

Other comments:

Introduction: The introduction needs quite a substantial editing. Some sentences are difficult to read, some others are incorrect or imprecise. I recommend the authors to invest more effort in drafting the introduction in a novel version of the manuscript.

Thank you for your input. We revised the introduction and hopefully made our writing clearer. Indeed, we changed the order of points and wrote more explicitly about various aspects.

A few examples:

l. 35 : do you mean a shaping procedure ? It is not clear what you mean by operant conditioning. Your description does not allow to understand what is the procedure to study the ability of animals to learn abstract concepts. Please revise the lines 34-37

Thank you for your comment. We are now much more explicit about the experimental paradigm, which should make things clear on L. 57 – 63.

l. 46 : I do not understand this sentence. ‘concepts are represented by the relation of abstract rules to novel stimuli’ ??

Thank you for your comment. We chance the sentence accordingly on L. 50 - 53.

l. 49-53 : Difficult to follow, please revise for improved clarity

We change this sentence accordingly to clarify the procedure on L. 78 – 80. Thank you

l.57 : The reference 27 seems fully out of context here. Could you please provide references of studies using simultaneous MTS to test for same/difference concept learning ?

We apologize, the right references were added on L. 75. Thank you

l. 58: Incorrect references as they used a DMTS procedure and not a simultaneous MTS

Thank you for your remark. We change the reference accordingly on L. 75.

Methods

l. 164 : Why did you feed the fish ab libitum before the experiment. This should induce a lack of motivation for the food reward.

Thank you for your comment. ‘We changed the word ‘ad libitum’ as we simply offered food for 5 min. Cleaner fish eat 11 hours during the day and hence have an adapted digestion. This implies that they are hungry in the morning, after 13h without food. The goal of giving them some food prior to the start of the trials was to avoid that they are so hungry that they do not pay attention to the stimuli. The 10 trials per day were spread over 5-6 h, so any lack of motivation due to the feeding would have affected at best the first trial (L. 191 – 197).

l. 165 : Why did you let the food plate in place for 2 hours ?

I suggest to provide a video as supporting information to help the reader to better picture the whole procedure.

Thank you for your comment. We left the plate for about 2 hours after the trials to make sure that fish did eat enough for the day (L. 191 - 197). Food intake during trials was quite low.

Discussion

l.228 : There is at least one study, the one in pigeons which is described a few sentences later (ref 12)

Thank you for your remark. In the study with the pigeons, they did use 76 stimuli but those stimuli were used every day. Here in our study, no same stimuli were shown over the 200 trials (so 400 stimuli were used in total without being used twice) (L. 269 – 271).

Bibliography : please check the references formatting as not consistent between references and does not follow the journal guidelines.

Thank you for your comment. The bibliography was carefully checked and the formatting respected to follow the journal guidelines (Vancouver style) on L. 323 and so on.

________________________________________

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Adrian G Dyer

22 Dec 2021

Cleaner wrasse Labroides dimidiatus perform above chance in a "Matching-to-sample" experiment

PONE-D-21-17431R1

Dear Dr. Aellen,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Adrian G Dyer, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Appologies for slow response; 2021 is a difficult year for obtaining reviewers as many people are disrupted. The manuscript has now been reviewed twice by a world expert on the topic, and I have also carefully read the manuscript versions. As the expert reviewer now recommends acceptance; I am recommending acceptance of the manuscript.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Acceptance letter

Adrian G Dyer

21 Jan 2022

PONE-D-21-17431R1

Cleaner wrasse Labroides dimidiatus perform above chance in a “Matching-to-sample” experiment

Dear Dr. Aellen:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Adrian G Dyer

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Experimental aquarium design.

    The “resting” area is on the left where the fish is waiting behind a see-through grid barrier with a door in the middle. On the right is the “experimental” compartment where the experimental plate is displayed during the trial.

    (TIF)

    S2 Fig. Representation of the twenty first symbols used in the matching-to-sample task.

    The symbols were drawn on a 3 x 3cm white paper sheet.

    (TIF)

    S1 Table. Main results of MTS studies.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All data and the R-scripts to reproduce the analyses are available at https://figshare.com/s/4ed4d7c5bcc750500de9.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES