Skip to main content
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
. 2022 Jan 20;15:777799. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.777799

Roles for Sleep in Neural and Behavioral Plasticity: Reviewing Variation in the Consequences of Sleep Loss

Jacqueline T Weiss 1,2, Jeffrey M Donlea 1,*
PMCID: PMC8810646  PMID: 35126067

Abstract

Sleep is a vital physiological state that has been broadly conserved across the evolution of animal species. While the precise functions of sleep remain poorly understood, a large body of research has examined the negative consequences of sleep loss on neural and behavioral plasticity. While sleep disruption generally results in degraded neural plasticity and cognitive function, the impact of sleep loss can vary widely with age, between individuals, and across physiological contexts. Additionally, several recent studies indicate that sleep loss differentially impacts distinct neuronal populations within memory-encoding circuitry. These findings indicate that the negative consequences of sleep loss are not universally shared, and that identifying conditions that influence the resilience of an organism (or neuron type) to sleep loss might open future opportunities to examine sleep's core functions in the brain. Here, we discuss the functional roles for sleep in adaptive plasticity and review factors that can contribute to individual variations in sleep behavior and responses to sleep loss.

Keywords: sleep, plasticity, review, memory, Drosophila

Introduction

Sleep is a physiological state that has been conserved across evolution, even noted in invertebrates lacking a centralized brain (Hendricks et al., 2000; Shaw et al., 2000; Zhdanova et al., 2001; Raizen et al., 2008; Singh et al., 2014; Nath et al., 2017). Although sleep's physiological functions remain poorly understood, sleep loss has been associated with deleterious effects on health and cognition (Rechtschaffen and Bergmann, 1995; Dinges et al., 1997; Durmer and Dinges, 2005; Spiegel et al., 2005; Banks and Dinges, 2007; Knutson et al., 2007; Grandner et al., 2010). Sleep varies based on previous waking experience (Ganguly-Fitzgerald et al., 2006; Huber et al., 2007; Hanlon et al., 2009; Keene et al., 2010; Beckwith et al., 2017; Kirszenblat et al., 2019; Milinski et al., 2021) throughout the lifespan (Roffwarg et al., 1966; Kales et al., 1967; Feinberg and Carlson, 1968; Cauter et al., 2000; Backhaus et al., 2007; Dijk et al., 2010; Feinberg and Campbell, 2010; Carrier et al., 2011; Vienne et al., 2016; Mander et al., 2017), and between species (Lyamin et al., 2008, 2017, 2018; Siegel, 2008; Lesku et al., 2012), suggesting that sleep has multiple functions. However, because sleep coincides with broad changes in neurophysiology and necessitates a loss of consciousness with reduced responsiveness to external threats, it is likely that sleep evolved, at least in part, to support brain function (Rasch and Born, 2013; Tononi and Cirelli, 2014). Notably, sleep is often elevated during periods of synaptic reorganization, including early development (Roffwarg et al., 1966; Shaw et al., 2000; Kayser et al., 2014), recovery from neural injury (Singh and Donlea, 2020; Stanhope et al., 2020), and memory consolidation (Walker et al., 2002; Ganguly-Fitzgerald et al., 2006). These findings each suggest that sleep supports plastic remodeling in the brain. Synaptic plasticity allows behavioral flexibility in response to external stimuli, and enables the processing and storage of information (Hughes, 1958; Zucker and Regehr, 2002; Cooke and Bliss, 2006). However, the underlying cellular and molecular mechanisms that support plasticity during sleep remain an area of intense investigation.

The impacts of sleep loss, interestingly, vary widely depending on age, environmental conditions, and genotype. While organisms typically recover from acute sleep disruptions relatively quickly, early-life sleep disruptions can prevent developmental plasticity during critical periods and result in long-lasting changes in circuit connectivity and behavior (Frank et al., 2001; Seugnet et al., 2011; Kayser et al., 2014). Conversely, some individuals withstand sleep loss with few consequences depending on the physiological conditions or genetic factors (Viola et al., 2007, 2012; Lyamin et al., 2008; Keene et al., 2010; Thimgan et al., 2010; Donlea et al., 2012; Lesku et al., 2012). In some cases, sleep disruption even provides an opportunity to weaken maladaptive memories (Poe, 2017). Examining the variables that can influence an individual's sensitivity to sleep loss could provide new insights into the core mechanisms of sleep-dependent plasticity. In this review, we will discuss roles for sleep in the maintenance of neural and behavioral plasticity during development, and learning/memory. Finally, we outline ethologically relevant conditions in which organisms can maintain neural and behavioral plasticity in the face of sleep loss.

Development

Synaptic plasticity plays a crucial role in brain development, especially in the refining of neural connectivity through the process of pruning (Paolicelli et al., 2011). Defects in synaptic pruning during development are thought to contribute to atypical circuit function seen in neurodevelopmental disorders (Paolicelli et al., 2011; Konopaske et al., 2014; Tang et al., 2014; Cossío et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2017; Neniskyte and Gross, 2017). Daily sleep amounts peak in many species early in development, when the brain is undergoing significant plastic changes (Roffwarg et al., 1966; Jouvet-Mounier et al., 1970; Shaw et al., 2000; Kayser et al., 2014). Studies in humans have found that sleep disruption during development is associated with severe and lasting consequences for behavior and cognition (O'Brien et al., 2004; Halbower et al., 2006; Ednick et al., 2009). While these human studies provide a correlational link between impaired sleep and later cognition, several lines of animal studies described below indicate conserved roles for sleep in neurodevelopment of several species and begin to identify possible mechanisms by which sleep might influence brain development.

Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is thought to play a particularly important role in development. Infants spend as much as 50% of their time asleep in REM, compared to 25% in adults (Roffwarg et al., 1966; Jouvet-Mounier et al., 1970). This period of increased REM sleep coincides with heightened formation and elimination of synapses in the developing mouse brain (Marks et al., 1995). Previous work found that REM deprivation, but not non-REM (NREM) deprivation, prevents the elimination of newly-formed dendritic spines in layer V pyramidal neurons in the developing mouse motor cortex (Li et al., 2017). Further, elimination of recent spines during REM facilitates the development of new spines at nearby sites. While most newly formed spines are eliminated, persistent spines are strengthened by REM sleep. Notably, similar findings were observed in the adult mouse brain following motor learning (Li et al., 2017).

A unique feature of REM sleep is the occurrence of myoclonic twitches, or spontaneous, discrete, spastic movements of the limbs (Tiriac et al., 2012; Blumberg et al., 2013; Sokoloff et al., 2020). These twitches occur throughout the mammalian lifespan, but are particularly abundant in infancy (Tiriac et al., 2012; Blumberg et al., 2013; Sokoloff et al., 2020, 2021). The development of myoclonic twitches depends on sensory feedback; the spatiotemporal organization of twitches is disrupted in newborn ErbB2 muscle-specific knockout mice which lack muscle spindles and exhibit impaired proprioception in adulthood (Blumberg et al., 2015). Muscle spindles are sensory receptors that relay changes in the length of muscles to the central nervous system and are necessary for intact proprioception (Kröger and Watkins, 2021). These findings suggest that twitches during sleep provide the developing brain with opportunities to refine immature sensorimotor maps and better coordinate limb movements. Twitching during early-life REM episodes, therefore, could facilitate the transformation of uncoordinated movements during infancy to the fine-tuned sensorimotor maps of an adult. Sensory feedback from twitching limbs are thought to contribute to motor learning and sensorimotor integration (Blumberg et al., 2013, 2020; Sokoloff et al., 2015; Rio-Bermudez and Blumberg, 2018; Glanz et al., 2021), as reafference from myoclonic twitches selectively activates brain regions such as the thalamus, cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum in infant rats (Khazipov et al., 2004; Mohns and Blumberg, 2010; Tiriac et al., 2012; Sokoloff et al., 2015). Because reafference signals from self-movement are gated during waking, sleep disruptions that interfere with twitching, and their corresponding neuronal activity may disrupt sensorimotor maturation (Tiriac and Blumberg, 2016). While these studies provide an important and promising link between early-life sleep episodes and the development of mature sensorimotor representations, the underlying synaptic mechanisms and long-term consequences of myoclonic twitch disruptions remain to be characterized in detail.

A vital role for sleep in early life plasticity is shared across sensory circuits. The study of ocular dominance plasticity (ODP) induced by monocular deprivation (MD) in cats, for example, is a canonical model of critical period plasticity during development that is reliant upon sleep. During an early critical period for visual development, occluding one eye leads to enhanced visual cortex responses to inputs from the non-deprived eye (Hubel and Wiesel, 1970). Sleep enhances ODP; NREM sleep deprivation prevents enhancement of cortical plasticity, suggesting that sleep is vital for consolidating experience-dependent changes in ocular dominance following MD (Frank et al., 2001). More recent work has found that REM deprivation disrupts cortical plasticity after MD as well, perhaps by disrupting replay-like patterns of activity in the visual cortex (Bridi et al., 2015). Additionally, REM sleep following MD is sufficient to prevent reversal of ODP following subsequent manipulations such as further SD (Bridi et al., 2015), cortical inactivation (Jha et al., 2005), and inhibition of NMDA receptors (Aton et al., 2009). The dependence of ODP on REM sleep parallels studies of sensorimotor development described above, suggesting a vital role for REM sleep in permitting developmental refinement across sensory systems. The consolidation of ODP is also reminiscent of hippocampal memory consolidation during sleep (Diekelmann and Born, 2010; Rasch and Born, 2013). These studies suggest that sleep during development is necessary for the consolidation of plastic changes induced by waking experience, which likely guide appropriate behavioral adaptations to a changing environment. Since ODP (along with other forms of developmental plasticity) occurs during a tightly restricted critical period of development, sleep disruptions early in life could have long-lasting effects on neurophysiology and behavior.

Ontogenetic changes in sleep are conserved; sleep amount and intensity are increased early in life for invertebrates, such as the fruit fly, just as they are in mammals (Jouvet-Mounier et al., 1970; Shaw et al., 2000). In Drosophila, 24 h of sleep deprivation following eclosion leads to long-term learning deficits, whereas adults recover from the same duration of sleep loss after one night of recovery sleep (Seugnet et al., 2011). These chronic learning impairments are likely connected with altered dopamine signaling, and can be dampened either by blocking D1 receptor activity during early life sleep loss or by elevating dopamine signaling during the days after developmental sleep deprivation (Seugnet et al., 2011). Additionally, young sleep-deprived male flies, but not mature flies, show deficits in courtship behavior as adults (Seugnet et al., 2011; Kayser et al., 2014). These courtship deficits are accompanied by decreased size of an olfactory glomerulus associated with perception of social pheromones, caused by impaired developmental growth (Kayser et al., 2014). Similarly, 1 week of early life sleep disruption impairs later social bonding in adult prairie voles (Jones et al., 2019). In this study, sleep disruption occurred during the third and fourth weeks of life, which likely falls during a critical period for maturation of GABAergic circuits that contribute to sensory integration (Gogolla et al., 2014). Notably, early life sleep deprivation in prairie voles leads to an increase in parvalbumin immunoreactivity in the primary sensory cortex, a brain region relevant to social bonding (Jones et al., 2019). Chronic changes in parvalbumin signaling could disrupt sensory processing and social behavior by altering excitatory/inhibitory balance (Yizhar et al., 2011). Together, these studies demonstrate that early life sleep is vital for developmental growth of rapidly growing brain regions across many species, and that disrupted sleep during development can result in lasting effects on adult circuitry and behavior.

While human studies have not yet revealed a mechanistic understanding of how sleep promotes neural and cognitive development, animal models indicate that sleep's role in neurodevelopment is evolutionarily ancient. Model system studies, such as those in flies and mice discussed above, have begun to examine how sleep modulates synaptic connectivity in a variety of developing sensory circuits. Further studies in these systems may reveal interventions that facilitate healthy development during insufficient sleep (Seugnet et al., 2011; Kayser et al., 2014; Jones et al., 2019).

Learning and Memory

In a variety of species, sleep is required for several stages of memory formation and processing (Walker et al., 2002; Graves et al., 2003; McDermott et al., 2003; Ganguly-Fitzgerald et al., 2006; Seugnet et al., 2008; Krishnan et al., 2016). Indeed, sleep deprivation leads to impaired encoding (Walker et al., 2002; Yoo et al., 2007; Seugnet et al., 2008), consolidation (Graves et al., 2003; Diekelmann and Born, 2010), and retrieval (Gais et al., 2007; Lo et al., 2016; Montes-Rodríguez et al., 2019; Heckman et al., 2020) of recent associations. While even a brief nap restores memory in some assays (Seugnet et al., 2008; Ong et al., 2020), other learning and memory impairments persist after days of recovery sleep (Havekes et al., 2016; Yamazaki et al., 2020; Wu et al., 2021). While it is not clear why recovery from sleep loss varies between these conditions, studies have detected several types of longer-lasting cellular and molecular changes that persist after recovery sleep, including altered gene expression (Gaine et al., 2021), protein synthesis (Tudor et al., 2016; Lamon et al., 2021), and circuit connectivity (Weiss and Donlea, 2021). Interestingly, some types of memories seem to be more vulnerable to sleep loss than others. For example, procedural memories and memories acquired with a conscious motivation or reward benefit from sleep more than declarative or unmotivated memories (Stickgold and Walker, 2007; Diekelmann and Born, 2010). In Drosophila, sleep deprivation disrupts consolidation of appetitive sugar reward memories in fed flies, but in not starved flies (Chouhan et al., 2021). Together, these studies indicate that sleep deprivation likely does not have a universal effect on learning and memory, but varies based on physiological, environmental, and behavioral factors.

While the negative impacts of sleep loss on memory formation are typically detrimental, it is possible that targeted sleep disruption could be used to prevent the consolidation of maladaptive memories. Some studies, for instance, suggest that sleep deprivation could be used following trauma to degrade fear memories in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Studies by Vanderheyden et al. (2015) compared sleep patterns of rats that were susceptible to developing PTSD-like symptoms after trauma to those that were resilient. While susceptible rats exhibited an increase in REM sleep in the hours following the traumatic event, resilient rats slept little during this period (Vanderheyden et al., 2015). Heightened REM sleep following trauma could lead to consolidation and reactivation of the trauma memory, preventing fear extinction, and resulting in generalization of the fear memory (Poe, 2017). Traumatic events drive activation of the mammalian locus coeruleus (LC) (Passerin et al., 2000; Naegeli et al., 2018), a collection of noradrenergic cells that promote long-term potentiation (LTP) (Izumi et al., 1992; Thomas et al., 1996; Izumi and Zorumski, 1999) and are generally quiescent during REM sleep (Foote et al., 1980). Elevated LC activity during REM sleep following a traumatic event can contribute to enhancement of recently formed emotional memories as seen in PTSD (Wassing et al., 2019). Therefore, behavioral sleep deprivation or pharmacological REM suppression following a traumatic event could lead to interventions to prevent the development of PTSD (Vanderheyden et al., 2014, 2015; Poe, 2017). Conversely, given the importance of sleep in memory consolidation (Rasch and Born, 2013) and emotional processing (Palmer and Alfano, 2017; Tempesta et al., 2018), sleep loss following a traumatic event could prevent consolidation of fear extinction memory in other conditions (Pace-Schott et al., 2015). Recent human studies have produced mixed results (Porcheret et al., 2015; Kleim et al., 2016; Cohen et al., 2017), indicating that the role for sleep in consolidating and/or maintaining traumatic memories varies with context or time elapsed since trauma. Further studies will be required to examine the therapeutic potential of sleep manipulations more clearly.

Synaptic Plasticity and Homeostasis

Although the primary function or functions of sleep are not understood, evidence suggests a strong relationship between sleep and plasticity (Frank et al., 2001; Tononi and Cirelli, 2014). Sleep loss leads to impairments in the plastic processes of learning and memory (Diekelmann and Born, 2010; Rasch and Born, 2013). One prominent hypothesis posits that sleep's function is the renormalization of synaptic strength via downscaling of synapses that are potentiated during wake, thereby constraining excitability and restoring signal-to-noise ratios for neuronal firing (Tononi and Cirelli, 2014). Learning about the environment during waking experience requires strengthening of synapses (Clem and Barth, 2006; Gruart et al., 2006; Tye et al., 2008). According to this synaptic homeostasis hypothesis, sleep deprivation leads to cognitive deficits due to saturation of synaptic connections (Tononi and Cirelli, 2014). Evidence supporting the role of synaptic downscaling during sleep exists in a variety of species (Gilestro et al., 2009; Vyazovskiy et al., 2009; Bushey et al., 2011). At the molecular level, synaptoneurosomes from the cortex and hippocampus of adult rats display increased protein levels of GluA1-containing AMPA receptors after spontaneous and forced wake than after sleep (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008). Sleep has been found to promote synaptic downscaling in the mouse forebrain by internalizing AMPA receptors via the immediate early gene Homer1 (Diering et al., 2017). In addition, the size of the axon-spine-interface, an ultrastructural measure of synaptic strength, increases after several hours of wake compared to sleep in several mouse brain regions (Vivo et al., 2017, 2019; Spano et al., 2019). At the electrophysiological level, amplitude and/or frequency of miniature excitatory postsynaptic currents in several regions of the rodent brain increase during wake and after sleep loss, and decline following spontaneous sleep and recovery sleep (Liu et al., 2010; Bjorness et al., 2020; Khlghatyan et al., 2020). Additionally, firing rates of hippocampal and cortical neurons have been shown to increase with wake and decrease with sleep (Lubenov and Siapas, 2008; Vyazovskiy et al., 2008, 2009; Huber et al., 2013; Norimoto et al., 2018). Studies in Drosophila have also found increases in abundance of presynaptic and postsynaptic markers following sleep loss, consistent with the hypothesis of net potentiation during wake (Gilestro et al., 2009; Bushey et al., 2011; Huang et al., 2020; Weiss and Donlea, 2021). Additional work in the fruit fly has found that acute sleep induction is sufficient to reduce abundance of transcripts (Dissel et al., 2015) or protein (Weiss and Donlea, 2021) of synaptic components.

While evidence clearly suggests a role for sleep in synaptic downscaling in some circumstances, other studies have reported synaptic potentiation during sleep (Frank et al., 2001; Aton et al., 2013, 2014). Short periods of sleep loss decrease the number of dendritic spines in the CA1 region of the hippocampus due to increased activity of the actin-binding protein cofilin (Havekes et al., 2016). Suppressing cofilin activity in hippocampal neurons prevents spine loss and cognitive deficits following sleep deprivation, suggesting that disruption of synaptic potentiation during sleep deprivation can lead to defects in memory consolidation (Havekes et al., 2016). Similarly, sleep deprivation leads to decreased spine density in the dentate gyrus (Raven et al., 2019), and disrupts the formation of new spines following learning (Yang et al., 2014). These data indicate that, although evidence supports a general trend for synaptic downscaling during sleep, it is likely that different classes of synapses undergo different forms of plasticity during sleep or that sleep alters synaptic organization differently depending on the organism's developmental state and recent experience.

Several recent studies have sought to understand whether sleep loss differentially affects distinct classes of neurons within a single circuit or brain region. The Drosophila mushroom body (MB), which encodes olfactory associative memories, provides an ideal opportunity to examine the local effects of sleep loss on synapse organization. Heroic efforts have untangled the organization of the fly MB with the development of genetic drivers to label each cell type, often with single-cell resolution (Aso et al., 2014a,b) and serial reconstruction of electron micrographs have led to a detailed connectome of the MB circuitry (Li et al., 2020; Scheffer et al., 2020). These studies show that the Drosophila MB is an associative learning center that is divided into 15 zones defined by non-overlapping arborization of several cell types, including cholinergic Kenyon Cells (KCs), reinforcing dopaminergic neurons (DANs), and mushroom body output neurons (MBONs) which mediate behavioral valence output (Aso et al., 2014a). Associative engrams can be localized to individual zones of the MB lobes, where plasticity in the connections between odor-encoding KCs and valence-driving MBONs determines the fly's behavioral response to odorant stimuli (Aso et al., 2014b; Hige et al., 2015; Owald et al., 2015). Since sleep loss prior to training can impair acquisition/short-term memory and disrupting sleep after training prevents memory consolidation (Ganguly-Fitzgerald et al., 2006; Seugnet et al., 2008), it is likely that sleep deprivation alters either synaptic connectivity or plasticity in MB circuits. Overnight sleep deprivation selectively upscales synapses of cholinergic memory-encoding KCs, but not other cell types in the MB, including DANs or large, inhibitory interneurons (Weiss and Donlea, 2021). Further, not all types of KC output synapses are equally impacted by sleep loss; output connections from KCs to different classes of post-synaptic target neurons show wide variations in abundance following sleep loss.

Interestingly, studies by Chouhan et al. (2021), found that flies housed without food did not require sleep after appetitive conditioning to form new memories, unlike fed flies. While appetitive memory is encoded in the KC>MBON-γ2α'1 circuit in fed flies and is sensitive to sleep loss, appetitive memory is encoded in KC>MBON-γ1pedc circuitry in starved flies, and remains intact with sleep loss (Chouhan et al., 2021). Additionally, Weiss and Donlea (2021) found that sleep loss led to decreased connectivity between KCs and MBON-γ2α'1, necessary for sleep-dependent memory consolidation, while KC>MBON-γ1pedc connections, dispensable for sleep-dependent memory consolidation, were unaffected. Sleep loss could therefore disrupt consolidation of recent appetitive memories in fed flies by reducing overall connectivity between KCs and MBON-γ2α'1 (see Figure 1). Because plasticity rules can differ widely between MB sub-circuits (Hige et al., 2015), environmental conditions during learning likely influence the strength, retention, and/or decay time of a particular association. These results suggest that different zones of the MB exhibit distinct plasticity rules during sleep, likely based on learning paradigm, internal state, and other previous experience.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Schematic of local plasticity in Drosophila mushroom body after sleep loss. (A) Schematic illustration of Drosophila mushroom body. The γ lobe (light blue) contains the γ1 compartment, outlined in blue, and the γ2 compartment, outlined in red. Arrows represent changes in connectivity from Kenyon cells to MBON-γ1pedc (left, blue) and MBON-γ2α'1 (right, red). Appetitive memory encoded at KC>MBON-γ1pedc synapses is resilient to sleep loss, but appetitive memory encoded at KC>MBON-γ2α'1 synapses is impaired by sleep loss. (B) Schematic of connectivity between neuronal cell types in MB in rested (left) and sleep deprived brains (right). KC axons innervate tiled zones that each receive input from distinct DANs and provide input to unique MBONs. After SD, KC>MBON-γ1pedc connectivity is unchanged, but KC>MBON-γ2α'1 connectivity decreases. Based on findings from Weiss and Donlea (2021) and Chouhan et al. (2021).

Supporting the idea of region and circuit specific changes in plasticity with SD, Puentes-Mestril et al. (2021) examined the effects of sleep loss on ribosome-bound transcripts for activity-dependent regulators of plasticity in excitatory pyramidal neurons and inhibitory parvalbumin-expressing interneurons. While both classes of neurons show increases in plasticity-mediating transcripts in the cortex following sleep loss, SD has little effect on abundance of these transcripts in both cell types in the hippocampus (Puentes-Mestril et al., 2021). Additional work suggests that certain cell types in the mouse hippocampus likely have privileged roles in memory consolidation during sleep (Delorme et al., 2021). Sleep deprivation leads to activation of inhibitory somatostatin-expressing (Sst+) interneurons in the hippocampus, likely due to inputs from increasingly active cholinergic neurons (Delorme et al., 2021). Both pharmacological activation of cholinergic neurons and chemogenetic activation of Sst+ cells in the dorsal hippocampus in the absence of SD leads to deficits in sleep-dependent memory consolidation (Delorme et al., 2021). Notably, both Delorme et al. (2021) and Weiss and Donlea (2021) found that sleep deprivation enhances cholinergic signaling onto GABAergic interneurons in learning/memory-related circuits, which likely increases inhibition onto memory-encoding neurons (see Figure 2). Enhanced hippocampal inhibition due to increased Sst+ activity during SD correlates with impairment of memory consolidation by disrupting LTP (Vecsey et al., 2009; Havekes et al., 2016), the reactivation of memory-encoding cells (Stefanelli et al., 2016; Clawson et al., 2021), or hippocampal oscillations (Puentes-Mestril et al., 2019). Similarly, while some inhibition from the Drosophila APL interneurons onto KCs is necessary to maintain spatial and temporal sparseness of odor encoding (Lei et al., 2013; Lin et al., 2014), excess inhibition would likely prevent encoding of new odor associations and reactivation of existing memory traces. Interestingly, GABAergic signaling from dorsal paired medial (DPM) and anterior paired lateral (APL) promotes sleep at night, suggesting that these interneurons may be recruited by increased KC activity during SD to promote sleep and sparsen KC representations (Haynes et al., 2015). These studies in both mice and Drosophila suggest that increased cholinergic signaling disrupts learning and memory after sleep deprivation, and that inhibitory drive onto memory-encoding neurons could be recruited to compensate. While these studies find complementary effects of sleep loss in the fly and mouse, these results use different approaches; Weiss and Donlea (2021) measure synaptic active zone reporters in the fly MB while Delorme et al. (2021) and Puentes-Mestril et al. (2021) quantify hippocampal transcript levels of activity-dependent immediate early genes. Additional studies will be required to directly test the relationship between connectivity changes and cell-type specific changes in activity. Ultimately, characterizing the subsets of synapses, cell types, and circuits that are most sensitive to sleep loss will help elucidate the mechanisms by which SD impairs behaviors such as learning and memory.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Cell type specific effects of sleep loss on memory-encoding circuits. (A) Schematic of connectivity between memory-encoding KCs and APL/DPM interneurons in the Drosophila MB in rested (left) and sleep deprived (right) flies. Cholinergic KCs activate GABAergic interneurons, which provide feedback inhibition onto KCs. KCs also synapse back onto other KCs. After SD (right), KC>APL connectivity strengthens, presumably increasing inhibition back onto KCs. KC>KC synapses may also strengthen, further contributing to increased KC>APL connectivity. Increased inhibition from APL/DPM after SD could dampen KC>KC excitation and promote recovery sleep. Based on findings from Weiss and Donlea (2021). (B) Schematic of hippocampal circuitry including cholinergic neurons in the medial septum to the mouse hippocampus in rested (left) and sleep deprived (right) mice. Cholinergic neurons activate GABAergic SST+ interneurons in the hippocampus, which inhibit memory-encoding pyramidal neurons/granule cells (principal neurons). After SD (right), enhanced cholinergic signaling increasingly activates SST+ interneurons, thereby heightening inhibition and reducing activity of hippocampal pyramidal neurons and granule cells. Based on findings from Delorme et al. (2021).

Sleep not only balances synaptic connectivity, but also influences neuronal firing patterns. In the rodent frontal cortex, fast spiking pyramidal cells show decreased activity during NREM sleep, while slow firing neurons increase their firing rate (Watson et al., 2016). Similar findings were observed in the mouse primary visual cortex, and these changes in firing rates were disrupted by a period of brief sleep deprivation (Clawson et al., 2018). Pyramidal neurons that are active during sleep spindles, oscillatory activity that promotes plasticity underlying memory formation (Schabus et al., 2006; Rasch and Born, 2013; Cairney et al., 2018), are increasingly active over the course of slow-wave sleep (SWS), whereas spindle-inactive pyramidal neurons show decreased activity during SWS (Niethard et al., 2021). These results indicate that sleep can increase the signal-to-noise ratio of neuronal responses by increasing the activity of sparsely firing neurons with the highest selectivity while reducing noise by decreasing activity of faster spiking, less selective neurons (Clawson et al., 2018). Interestingly, sleep during early-life ODP in mice is vital for firing rate homeostasis, indicating a potential life-long role for sleep in normalizing neuronal activity (Hengen et al., 2016; Pacheco et al., 2021).

Resilience to Sleep Loss

Ethological Context

While sleep contributes to many forms of experience-dependent plasticity as described above, individuals can show a wide variation in their responses to sleep loss. Sleep is homeostatically regulated across many species, but both extrinsic and intrinsic factors can influence the responses of an organism to specific sleep challenges. Food-deprived Drosophila, for instance, typically reduce their sleep, presumably to maximize foraging opportunities (Keene et al., 2010; Thimgan et al., 2010; Yurgel et al., 2019). While acute sleep-deprivation is typically accompanied by impaired memory and a homeostatic increase in sleep, flies that lose sleep overnight during food deprivation can retain intact memory formation and show little, if any, sleep rebound (Thimgan et al., 2010). Similarly, socially naïve male flies will also forego sleep when paired overnight with a female fly (Beckwith et al., 2017; Machado et al., 2017). This effect can be replicated by activating pheromone sensing neurons or courtship control circuits and, like starvation-induced arousal, is not followed by a sleep rebound. Similarly, the ability to temporarily offset the need for sleep has also been found in vertebrate species. Fur seals suppress REM sleep for days or weeks when foraging in seawater, accompanied by little to no REM rebound (Lyamin et al., 2018). Migratory frigate birds can reduce the time that they spend asleep by over 90% for ~10 days while continuously in flight over the Pacific Ocean compared to their sleeping patterns on land (Rattenborg et al., 2016). Similarly, Arctic male sandpipers suppress sleep for a roughly 3 week period annually while they compete for mating partners (Lesku et al., 2012). During mating season, the sun never sets in the high Arctic, allowing males to engage in unlimited visual courtship displays. Because mating success is correlated with the amount of time that male sandpipers spend awake, there is likely selective pressure for genetic factors that can allow male sandpipers to withstand prolonged sleep loss without accruing cognitive deficits or sleep drive. Constant sunlight during this period likely interacts with social and reproductive cues, enabling males to forego sleep for an extended period. Social behaviors can also drive contexts in which mammals can delay the need for sleep. Whales and dolphins, for example, can nearly fully suppress sleep for up to a month after giving birth with no recorded physiological consequences (Lyamin et al., 2005). Importantly, vertebrate sleep stages are characterized by electrophysiological signatures measured with electroencephalography (EEG) (two process model), whereas Drosophila sleep is defined by behavioral criteria such as quiescence and increased arousal threshold (Hendricks et al., 2000; Shaw et al., 2000). Recent work has begun to investigate whether sleep in Drosophila is composed of distinct stages (Yap et al., 2017; Raccuglia et al., 2019; Tainton-Heap et al., 2021), which may account for variations in plasticity and responses to sleep loss discussed above. While mechanistic studies are not feasible in many of the species mentioned here, the range of contexts in which sleep need can be temporarily offset provides exciting opportunities to understand when sleep is required for plasticity (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Summary of experimental or ethologically-relevant conditions that reduce sleep in several species.

Species Manipulation Sleep response Behavioral response References
Drosophila melanogaster Sleep deprivation Decreased sleep, homeostatic rebound Impaired learning, STM and LTM Ganguly-Fitzgerald et al., 2006; Seugnet et al., 2008; Li et al., 2009
Starvation Decreased sleep, no rebound Intact memory Keene et al., 2010; Thimgan et al., 2010; Yurgel et al., 2019
Stimulants Decreased sleep Not measured Hendricks et al., 2000; Shaw et al., 2000; Andretic et al., 2005
Courtship Decreased sleep, no rebound Not measured Beckwith et al., 2017; Machado et al., 2017
Frigatebirds Migration Decreased sleep in flight, rebound on land Not measured Rattenborg et al., 2016
Sandpipers Mating season Decreased sleep Mating success positively correlated with amount of sleep loss Lesku et al., 2012
Cetaceans Postpartum Little to no sleep Not measured Lyamin et al., 2005, 2007
Fur seals In seawater Greatly reduced REM, no REM rebound Not measured Lyamin et al., 2018

Intrinsic Factors

Resilience to sleep loss can also be influenced by intrinsic factors that vary between individuals. Human subjects exhibit reliable, stable responses to repeated episodes of sleep loss, suggesting that sensitivity to sleep loss can be a durable trait over time (Dennis et al., 2017; Yamazaki and Goel, 2019). Naturally occurring genetic polymorphisms coincide with an individual's response to sleep loss in flies and humans (Viola et al., 2007, 2012; Donlea et al., 2012; Satterfield et al., 2015). In two of these studies, the same genetic alleles correlated with reduced cognitive impairments and dampened homeostatic sleep pressure after prolonged waking, indicating that the identified loci could contribute to protecting neural functions during sleep loss (Viola et al., 2007; Donlea et al., 2012). Interestingly, the identified human alleles in per3 and tnfα that protected individuals from the consequences of sleep loss did not predominate in the subject populations, consistent with the possibility that these alleles are accompanied with susceptibility to other physiological challenges. Brain structure can also influence sensitivity to sleep loss; variation in functional connectivity between brain regions and hippocampal structure can predict the cognitive impact of sleep loss in human subjects (Yeo et al., 2015; Saletin et al., 2016). While the neural and molecular mechanisms that connect these variations with susceptibility to sleep are not yet known, studies of model systems provide some insights into pathways that might provide protection from insufficient sleep. Drosophila and mouse studies have identified genetic pathways, including circadian rhythm (Mang et al., 2016; Ehlen et al., 2017), and metabolic factors (Thimgan et al., 2010, 2015), that can be manipulated to prevent rebound sleep following extended waking. It is important to note that each of these interventions can temporarily delay the accumulation of sleep debt, but it is unclear how long their protection persists and whether other consequences build as a result. Nonetheless, further examination of the external contexts and internal factors that can confer resilience to sleep loss may provide new insight into the neural functions of sleep and identify controllable interventions to facilitate rapid recovery from sleep loss.

Conclusion

In many contexts, sleep is vital for individuals to learn and adapt their behavior to best fit their environmental conditions. Sleep facilitates brain development and circuit refinement, and early life disruptions in sleep can result in long-lasting behavioral changes. Throughout the lifespan, sleep also impacts whether new memories can be effectively acquired and consolidated. While understanding the mechanisms that contribute to sleep-dependent plasticity remain an area of intense interest, many studies have already identified molecular and synaptic connectivity changes that occur during sleep to facilitate memory formation. More clearly identifying these mechanisms and developing strategies to manipulate them could open opportunities to support cognitive processing during sleep loss. Finally, individuals exhibit varying responses to sleep loss due to intrinsic and environmental factors. Understanding the benefits and detriments of variations in sleep, as well as the biological basis for inter-individual differences, will help resolve the function(s) of sleep and elucidate how sleep patterns affect future behavior.

Author Contributions

JW and JD wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work had support from NIH/NINDS R01NS105967 to JD and a Jessamine K. Hilliard UCLA Neurobiology Graduate Student Grant to JW.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank members of the Donlea lab for helpful suggestions and comments.

References

  1. Andretic R., Swinderen B. V., Greenspan R. J. (2005). Dopaminergic modulation of arousal in drosophila. Curr. Biol. 15, 1165–1175. 10.1016/j.cub.2005.05.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aso Y., Hattori D., Yu Y., Johnston R. M., Iyer N. A., Ngo T.-T., et al. (2014a). The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. Elife 3:e04577. 10.7554/elife.04577 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aso Y., Sitaraman D., Ichinose T., Kaun K. R., Vogt K., Belliart-Guérin G., et al. (2014b). Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. Elife 3:e04580. 10.7554/elife.04580 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aton S. J., Broussard C., Dumoulin M., Seibt J., Watson A., Coleman T., et al. (2013). Visual experience and subsequent sleep induce sequential plastic changes in putative inhibitory and excitatory cortical neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 3101–3106. 10.1073/pnas.1208093110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aton S. J., Seibt J., Dumoulin M., Jha S. K., Steinmetz N., Coleman T., et al. (2009). Mechanisms of sleep-dependent consolidation of cortical plasticity. Neuron 61, 454–466. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Aton S. J., Suresh A., Broussard C., Frank M. G. (2014). Sleep promotes cortical response potentiation following visual experience. Sleep 37, 1163–1170. 10.5665/sleep.3830 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Backhaus J., Born J., Hoeckesfeld R., Fokuhl S., Hohagen F., Junghanns K. (2007). Midlife decline in declarative memory consolidation is correlated with a decline in slow wave sleep. Learn Mem. 14, 336–341. 10.1101/lm.470507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Banks S., Dinges D. F. (2007). Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 03, 519–528. 10.5664/jcsm.26918 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Beckwith E. J., Geissmann Q., French A. S., Gilestro G. F. (2017). Regulation of sleep homeostasis by sexual arousal. Elife 6:e27445. 10.7554/elife.27445 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bjorness T. E., Kulkarni A., Rybalchenko V., Suzuki A., Bridges C., Harrington A. J., et al. (2020). An essential role for MEF2C in the cortical response to loss of sleep in mice. Elife 9:e58331. 10.7554/elife.58331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Blumberg M. S., Coleman C. M., Gerth A. I., McMurray B. (2013). Spatiotemporal structure of REM sleep twitching reveals developmental origins of motor synergies. Curr. Biol. 23, 2100–2109. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.08.055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Blumberg M. S., Coleman C. M., Sokoloff G., Weiner J. A., Fritzsch B., McMurray B. (2015). Development of twitching in sleeping infant mice depends on sensory experience. Curr. Biol. 25, 656–662. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Blumberg M. S., Dooley J. C., Sokoloff G. (2020). The developing brain revealed during sleep. Curr. Opin. Physiol. 15, 14–22. 10.1016/j.cophys.2019.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bridi M. C. D., Aton S. J., Seibt J., Renouard L., Coleman T., Frank M. G. (2015). Rapid eye movement sleep promotes cortical plasticity in the developing brain. Sci. Adv. 1:e1500105. 10.1126/sciadv.1500105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bushey D., Tononi G., Cirelli C. (2011). Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: structural evidence in Drosophila. Science 332, 1576–1581. 10.1126/science.1202839 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cairney S. A., Guttesen A. Á. V., Marj N. E., Staresina B. P. (2018). Memory consolidation is linked to spindle-mediated information processing during sleep. Curr. Biol. 28, 948–954.e4. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Carrier J., Viens I., Poirier G., Robillard R., Lafortune M., Vandewalle G., et al. (2011). Sleep slow wave changes during the middle years of life. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 758–766. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07543.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cauter E. V., Leproult R., Plat L. (2000). Age-related changes in slow wave sleep and REM sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy men. JAMA 284, 861–868. 10.1001/jama.284.7.861 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chouhan N. S., Griffith L. C., Haynes P., Sehgal A. (2021). Availability of food determines the need for sleep in memory consolidation. Nature 589, 582–585. 10.1038/s41586-020-2997-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Clawson B. C., Durkin J., Suresh A. K., Pickup E. J., Broussard C. G., Aton S. J. (2018). Sleep promotes, and sleep loss inhibits, selective changes in firing rate, response properties and functional connectivity of primary visual cortex neurons. Front. Syst Neurosci. 12, 40. 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Clawson B. C., Pickup E. J., Ensing A., Geneseo L., Shaver J., Gonzalez-Amoretti J., et al. (2021). Causal role for sleep-dependent reactivation of learning-activated sensory ensembles for fear memory consolidation. Nat. Commun. 12, 1200. 10.1038/s41467-021-21471-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Clem R. L., Barth A. (2006). Pathway-specific trafficking of native AMPARs by in vivo experience. Neuron 49, 663–670. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.01.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cohen S., Kaplan Z., Zohar J., Cohen H. (2017). Preventing sleep on the first resting phase following a traumatic event attenuates anxiety-related responses. Behav. Brain Res. 320, 450–456. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.10.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cooke S. F., Bliss T. V. P. (2006). Plasticity in the human central nervous system. Brain 129, 1659–1673. 10.1093/brain/awl082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cossío L. F., de Guzmán, A., Veldt S., van der, Luheshi G. N. (2017). Prenatal infection leads to ASD-like behavior and altered synaptic pruning in the mouse offspring. Brain Behav. Immun. 63, 88–98. 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.09.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Delorme J., Wang L., Kuhn F. R., Kodoth V., Ma J., Martinez J. D., et al. (2021). Sleep loss drives acetylcholine- and somatostatin interneuron–mediated gating of hippocampal activity to inhibit memory consolidation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118:e2019318118. 10.1073/pnas.2019318118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dennis L. E., Wohl R. J., Selame L. A., Goel N. (2017). Healthy adults display long-term trait-like neurobehavioral resilience and vulnerability to sleep loss. Sci. Rep. 7, 14889. 10.1038/s41598-017-14006-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Diekelmann S., Born J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 114–126. 10.1038/nrn2762 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Diering G. H., Nirujogi R. S., Roth R. H., Worley P. F., Pandey A., Huganir R. L. (2017). Homer1a drives homeostatic scaling-down of excitatory synapses during sleep. Science 355, 511–515. 10.1126/science.aai8355 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dijk D.-J., Groeger J. A., Stanley N., Deacon S. (2010). Age-related reduction in daytime sleep propensity and nocturnal slow wave sleep. Sleep 33, 211–223. 10.1093/sleep/33.2.211 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dinges D. F., Pack F., Williams K., Gillen K. A., Powell J. W., Ott G. E., et al. (1997). Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. Sleep 20, 267–277. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dissel S., Angadi V., Kirszenblat L., Suzuki Y., Donlea J., Klose M., et al. (2015). Sleep restores behavioral plasticity to Drosophila mutants. Curr. Biol. 25, 1270–1281. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Donlea J. M., Leahy A., Thimgan M. S., Suzuki Y., Hughson B. N., Sokolowski M. B., et al. (2012). Foraging alters resilience/vulnerability to sleep disruption and starvation in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 2613–2618. 10.1073/pnas.1112623109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Durmer J. S., Dinges D. F. (2005). Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin. Neurol. 25, 117–129. 10.1055/s-2005-867080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ednick M., Cohen A. P., McPhail G. L., Beebe D., Simakajornboon N., Amin R. S. (2009). A review of the effects of sleep during the first year of life on cognitive, psychomotor, and temperament development. Sleep 32, 1449–1458. 10.1093/sleep/32.11.1449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ehlen J. C., Brager A. J., Baggs J., Pinckney L., Gray C. L., DeBruyne J. P., et al. (2017). Bmal1 function in skeletal muscle regulates sleep. Elife 6:e26557. 10.7554/elife.26557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Feinberg, Carlson V. R. (1968). Sleep variables as a function of age in man. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 18, 239–250. 10.1001/archpsyc.1968.0174002011101425996397 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Feinberg I., Campbell I. G. (2010). Sleep EEG changes during adolescence: an index of a fundamental brain reorganization. Brain Cogn. 72, 56–65. 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Foote S. L., Aston-Jones G., Bloom F. E. (1980). Impulse activity of locus coeruleus neurons in awake rats and monkeys is a function of sensory stimulation and arousal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 77, 3033–3037. 10.1073/pnas.77.5.3033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Frank M. G., Issa N. P., Stryker M. P. (2001). Sleep enhances plasticity in the developing visual cortex. Neuron 30, 275–287. 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00279-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gaine M. E., Bahl E., Chatterjee S., Michaelson J. J., Abel T., Lyons L. C. (2021). Altered hippocampal transcriptome dynamics following sleep deprivation. Mol. Brain 14, 125. 10.1186/s13041-021-00835-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gais S., Albouy G., Boly M., Dang-Vu T. T., Darsaud A., Desseilles M., et al. (2007). Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 18778–18783. 10.1073/pnas.0705454104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ganguly-Fitzgerald I., Donlea J. M., Shaw P. J. (2006). Waking experience affects sleep need in Drosophila. Science 313, 1775–1781. 10.1126/science.1130408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gilestro G. F., Tononi G., Cirelli C. (2009). Widespread changes in synaptic markers as a function of sleep and wakefulness in Drosophila. Science 324, 109–112. 10.1126/science.1166673 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Glanz R. M., Dooley J. C., Sokoloff G., Blumberg M. S. (2021). Sensory coding of limb kinematics in motor cortex across a key developmental transition. J. Neurosci. 41, 6905–6918. 10.1523/jneurosci.0921-21.2021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gogolla N., Takesian A. E., Feng G., Fagiolini M., Hensch T. K. (2014). Sensory integration in mouse insular cortex reflects GABA circuit maturation. Neuron 83, 894–905. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Grandner M. A., Hale L., Moore M., Patel N. P. (2010). Mortality associated with short sleep duration: the evidence, the possible mechanisms, and the future. Sleep Med. Rev. 14, 191–203. 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.07.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Graves L. A., Heller E. A., Pack A. I., Abel T. (2003). Sleep deprivation selectively impairs memory consolidation for contextual fear conditioning. Learn Mem. 10, 168–176. 10.1101/lm.48803 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gruart A., Muñoz M. D., Delgado-García J. M. (2006). Involvement of the CA3-CA1 synapse in the acquisition of associative learning in behaving mice. J. Neurosci. 26, 1077–1087. 10.1523/jneurosci.2834-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Halbower A. C., Degaonkar M., Barker P. B., Earley C. J., Marcus C. L., Smith P. L., et al. (2006). Childhood obstructive sleep apnea associates with neuropsychological deficits and neuronal brain injury. PLoS Med. 3:e301. 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hanlon E. C., Faraguna U., Vyazovskiy V. V., Tononi G., Cirelli C. (2009). Effects of skilled training on sleep slow wave activity and cortical gene expression in the rat. Sleep 32, 719–729. 10.1093/sleep/32.6.719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Havekes R., Park A. J., Tudor J. C., Luczak V. G., Hansen R. T., Ferri S. L., et al. (2016). Sleep deprivation causes memory deficits by negatively impacting neuronal connectivity in hippocampal area CA1. Elife 5:e13424. 10.7554/eLife.13424 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Haynes P. R., Christmann B. L., Griffith L. C. (2015). A single pair of neurons links sleep to memory consolidation in Drosophila melanogaster. Elife 4, R774. 10.7554/elife.03868 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Heckman P. R. A., Kuhn F. R., Meerlo P., Havekes R. (2020). A brief period of sleep deprivation negatively impacts the acquisition, consolidation, and retrieval of object-location memories. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 175, 107326. 10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hendricks J. C., Finn S. M., Panckeri K. A., Chavkin J., Williams J. A., Sehgal A., et al. (2000). Rest in Drosophila is a sleep-like state. Neuron 25, 129–138. 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80877-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Hengen K. B., Pacheco A. T., McGregor J. N., Hooser S. D. V., Turrigiano G. G. (2016). Neuronal firing rate homeostasis is inhibited by sleep and promoted by wake. Cell 165, 180–191. 10.1016/j.cell.2016.01.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Hige T., Aso Y., Modi M. N., Rubin G. M., Turner G. C. (2015). Heterosynaptic plasticity underlies aversive olfactory learning in Drosophila. Neuron 88, 985–998. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Huang S., Piao C., Beuschel C. B., Götz T., Sigrist S. J. (2020). Presynaptic active zone plasticity encodes sleep need in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 30, 1077–1091. 10.1016/j.cub.2020.01.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Hubel D. H., Wiesel T. N. (1970). The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. J. Physiol. 206, 419–436. 10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Huber R., Mäki H., Rosanova M., Casarotto S., Canali P., Casali A. G., et al. (2013). Human cortical excitability increases with time awake. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1–7. 10.1093/cercor/bhs014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Huber R., Tononi G., Cirelli C. (2007). Exploratory behavior, cortical BDNF expression, and sleep homeostasis. Sleep 30, 129–139. 10.1093/sleep/30.2.129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hughes J. R. (1958). Post-tetanic potentiation. Physiol. Rev. 38, 91–113. 10.1152/physrev.1958.38.1.91 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Izumi Y., Clifford D. B., Zorumski C. F. (1992). Norepinephrine reverses N-methyl-d-aspartate-mediated inhibition of long-term potentiation in rat hippocampal slices. Neurosci. Lett. 142, 163–166. 10.1016/0304-3940(92)90364-d [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Izumi Y., Zorumski C. F. (1999). Norepinephrine promotes long-term potentiation in the adult rat hippocampus in vitro. Synapse 31, 196–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Jha S. K., Jones B. E., Coleman T., Steinmetz N., Law C.-T., Griffin G., et al. (2005). Sleep-dependent plasticity requires cortical activity. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 25, 9266–9274. 10.1523/jneurosci.2722-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Jones C. E., Opel R. A., Kaiser M. E., Chau A. Q., Quintana J. R., Nipper M. A., et al. (2019). Early-life sleep disruption increases parvalbumin in primary somatosensory cortex and impairs social bonding in prairie voles. Sci. Adv. 5:eaav5188. 10.1126/sciadv.aav5188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Jouvet-Mounier D., Astic L., Lacote D. (1970). Ontogenesis of the states of sleep in rat, cat, and guinea pig during the first postnatal month. Dev. Psychobiol. 2, 216–239. 10.1002/dev.420020407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kales A., Wilson T., Kales J. D., Jacobson A., Paulson M. J., Kollar E., et al. (1967). Measurements of all-night sleep in normal elderly persons: effects of aging*. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 15, 405–414. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1967.tb02072.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Kayser M. S., Yue Z., Sehgal A. (2014). A critical period of sleep for development of courtship circuitry and behavior in Drosophila. Science 344, 269–274. 10.1126/science.1250553 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Keene A. C., Duboué E. R., McDonald D. M., Dus M., Suh G. S. B., Waddell S., et al. (2010). Clock and cycle limit starvation-induced sleep loss in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 20, 1209–1215. 10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Khazipov R., Sirota A., Leinekugel X., Holmes G. L., Ben-Ari Y., Buzsáki G. (2004). Early motor activity drives spindle bursts in the developing somatosensory cortex. Nature 432, 758–761. 10.1038/nature03132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Khlghatyan J., Evstratova A., Bozoyan L., Chamberland S., Chatterjee D., Marakhovskaia A., et al. (2020). Fxr1 regulates sleep and synaptic homeostasis. EMBO J. 39:e103864. 10.15252/embj.2019103864 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kim H.-J., Cho M.-H., Shim W. H., Kim J. K., Jeon E.-Y., Kim D.-H., et al. (2017). Deficient autophagy in microglia impairs synaptic pruning and causes social behavioral defects. Mol. Psychiatry 22, 1576–1584. 10.1038/mp.2016.103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kirszenblat L., Yaun R., Swinderen B., van (2019). Visual experience drives sleep need in Drosophila. Sleep 8, 269. 10.1093/sleep/zsz102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Kleim B., Wysokowsky J., Schmid N., Seifritz E., Rasch B. (2016). Effects of sleep after experimental trauma on intrusive emotional memories. Sleep 39, 2125–2132. 10.5665/sleep.6310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Knutson K. L., Spiegel K., Penev P., Cauter E. V. (2007). The metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Sleep Med. Rev. 11, 163–178. 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Konopaske G. T., Lange N., Coyle J. T., Benes F. M. (2014). Prefrontal cortical dendritic spine pathology in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Jama Psychiatry 71, 1323–1331. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Krishnan H. C., Gandour C. E., Ramos J. L., Wrinkle M. C., Sanchez-Pacheco J. J., Lyons L. C. (2016). Acute sleep deprivation blocks short- and long-term operant memory in aplysia. Sleep 39, 2161–2171. 10.5665/sleep.6320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Kröger S., Watkins B. (2021). Muscle spindle function in healthy and diseased muscle. Skelet. Muscle 11, 3. 10.1186/s13395-020-00258-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Lamon S., Morabito A., Arentson-Lantz E., Knowles O., Vincent G. E., Condo D., et al. (2021). The effect of acute sleep deprivation on skeletal muscle protein synthesis and the hormonal environment. Physiol. Rep. 9:e14660. 10.14814/phy2.14660 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Lei Z., Chen K., Li H., Liu H., Guo A. (2013). The GABA system regulates the sparse coding of odors in the mushroom bodies of Drosophila. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 436, 35–40. 10.1016/j.bbrc.2013.05.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Lesku J. A., Rattenborg N. C., Valcu M., Vyssotski A. L., Kuhn S., Kuemmeth F., et al. (2012). Adaptive sleep loss in polygynous pectoral sandpipers. Science 337, 1654–1658. 10.1126/science.1220939 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Li F., Lindsey J. W., Marin E. C., Otto N., Dreher M., Dempsey G., et al. (2020). The connectome of the adult Drosophila mushroom body provides insights into function. Elife 9:e62576. 10.7554/elife.62576 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Li W., Ma L., Yang G., Gan W.-B. (2017). REM sleep selectively prunes and maintains new synapses in development and learning. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 427–437. 10.1038/nn.4479 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Li X., Yu F., Guo A. (2009). Sleep deprivation specifically impairs short-term olfactory memory in Drosophila. Sleep 32, 1417–1424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Lin A. C., Bygrave A. M., Calignon A., de Lee T., Miesenböck G. (2014). Sparse, decorrelated odor coding in the mushroom body enhances learned odor discrimination. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 559–568. 10.1038/nn.3660 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Liu Z.-W., Faraguna U., Cirelli C., Tononi G., Gao X.-B. (2010). Direct evidence for wake-related increases and sleep-related decreases in synaptic strength in rodent cortex. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 30, 8671–8675. 10.1523/jneurosci.1409-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Lo J. C., Chong P. L. H., Ganesan S., Leong R. L. F., Chee M. W. L. (2016). Sleep deprivation increases formation of false memory. J. Sleep Res. 25, 673–682. 10.1111/jsr.12436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lubenov E. V., Siapas A. G. (2008). Decoupling through synchrony in neuronal circuits with propagation delays. Neuron 58, 118–131. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Lyamin O., Pryaslova J., Kosenko P., Siegel J. (2007). Behavioral aspects of sleep in bottlenose dolphin mothers and their calves. Physiol. Behav. 92, 725–733. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.064 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Lyamin O., Pryaslova J., Lance V., Siegel J. (2005). Continuous activity in cetaceans after birth. Nature 435, 1177–1177. 10.1038/4351177a [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Lyamin O. I., Kosenko P. O., Korneva S. M., Vyssotski A. L., Mukhametov L. M., Siegel J. M. (2018). Fur seals suppress REM sleep for very long periods without subsequent rebound. Curr. Biol. 28, 2000.e2–2005.e2. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Lyamin O. I., Manger P. R., Ridgway S. H., Mukhametov L. M., Siegel J. M. (2008). Cetacean sleep: an unusual form of mammalian sleep. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1451–1484. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Lyamin O. I., Mukhametov L. M., Siegel J. M. (2017). Sleep in the northern fur seal. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 44, 144–151. 10.1016/j.conb.2017.04.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Machado D. R., Afonso D. J., Kenny A. R., Öztürk-Çolak A., Moscato E. H., Mainwaring B., et al. (2017). Identification of octopaminergic neurons that modulate sleep suppression by male sex drive. Elife 6, 1717. 10.7554/elife.23130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Mander B. A., Winer J. R., Walker M. P. (2017). Sleep and human aging. Neuron 94, 19–36. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Mang G. M., Spada F. L., Emmenegger Y., Chappuis S., Ripperger J. A., Albrecht U., et al. (2016). Altered sleep homeostasis in Rev-erbα knockout mice. Sleep 39, 589–601. 10.5665/sleep.5534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Marks G. A., Shaffery J. P., Oksenberg A., Speciale S. G., Roffwarg H. P. (1995). A functional role for REM sleep in brain maturation. Behav. Brain Res. 69, 1–11. 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00018-o [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. McDermott C. M., LaHoste G. J., Chen C., Musto A., Bazan N. G., Magee J. C. (2003). Sleep deprivation causes behavioral, synaptic, and membrane excitability alterations in hippocampal neurons. J. Neurosci. 23, 9687–9695. 10.1523/jneurosci.23-29-09687.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Milinski L., Fisher S. P., Cui N., McKillop L. E., Blanco-Duque C., Ang G., et al. (2021). Waking experience modulates sleep need in mice. BMC Biol. 19:65. 10.1186/s12915-021-00982-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Mohns E. J., Blumberg M. S. (2010). Neocortical activation of the hippocampus during sleep in infant rats. J. Neurosci. 30, 3438–3449. 10.1523/jneurosci.4832-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Montes-Rodríguez C. J., Rueda-Orozco P. E., Prospéro-García O. (2019). Total sleep deprivation impairs fear memory retrieval by decreasing the basolateral amygdala activity. Brain Res. 1719, 17–23. 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.05.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Naegeli C., Zeffiro T., Piccirelli M., Jaillard A., Weilenmann A., Hassanpour K., et al. (2018). Locus coeruleus activity mediates hyperresponsiveness in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 83, 254–262. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.08.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Nath R. D., Bedbrook C. N., Abrams M. J., Basinger T., Bois J. S., Prober D. A., et al. (2017). The jellyfish Cassiopea exhibits a sleep-like state. Curr. Biol. 27, p2984.e3–2990.e3. 10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Neniskyte U., Gross C. T. (2017). Errant gardeners: glial-cell-dependent synaptic pruning and neurodevelopmental disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 658–670. 10.1038/nrn.2017.110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Niethard N., Brodt S., Born J. (2021). Cell-type-specific dynamics of calcium activity in cortical circuits over the course of slow-wave sleep and rapid eye movement sleep. J. Neurosci. 41, 4212–4222. 10.1523/jneurosci.1957-20.2021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Norimoto H., Makino K., Gao M., Shikano Y., Okamoto K., Ishikawa T., et al. (2018). Hippocampal ripples down-regulate synapses. Science 359, 1524–1527. 10.1126/science.aao0702 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. O'Brien L. M., Mervis C. B., Holbrook C. R., Bruner J. L., Klaus C. J., Rutherford J., et al. (2004). Neurobehavioral implications of habitual snoring in children. Pediatrics 114, 44–49. 10.1542/peds.114.1.44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Ong J. L., Lau T. Y., Lee X. K., Rijn E., van, Chee M. W. L. (2020). A daytime nap restores hippocampal function and improves declarative learning. Sleep 43:zsaa058. 10.1093/sleep/zsaa058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Owald D., Felsenberg J., Talbot C. B., Das G., Perisse E., Huetteroth W., et al. (2015). Activity of defined mushroom body output neurons underlies learned olfactory behavior in Drosophila. Neuron 86, 417–427. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Pace-Schott E. F., Germain A., Milad M. R. (2015). Effects of sleep on memory for conditioned fear and fear extinction. Psychol. Bull. 141, 835–857. 10.1037/bul0000014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Pacheco A. T., Bottorff J., Gao Y., Turrigiano G. G. (2021). Sleep promotes downward firing rate homeostasis. Neuron 109, 530.e6–544.e6. 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Palmer C. A., Alfano C. A. (2017). Sleep and emotion regulation: an organizing, integrative review. Sleep Med. Rev. 31, 6–16. 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Paolicelli R. C., Bolasco G., Pagani F., Maggi L., Scianni M., Panzanelli P., et al. (2011). Synaptic pruning by microglia is necessary for normal brain development. Science 333, 1456–1458. 10.1126/science.1202529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Passerin A. M., Cano G., Rabin B. S., Delano B. A., Napier J. L., Sved A. F. (2000). Role of locus coeruleus in foot shock-evoked fos expression in rat brain. Neuroscience 101, 1071–1082. 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00372-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Poe G. R. (2017). Sleep is for forgetting. J. Neurosci. 37, 464–473. 10.1523/jneurosci.0820-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Porcheret K., Holmes E. A., Goodwin G. M., Foster R. G., Wulff K. (2015). Psychological effect of an analogue traumatic event reduced by sleep deprivation. Sleep 38, 1017–1025. 10.5665/sleep.4802 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Puentes-Mestril C., Delorme J., Wang L., Donnelly M., Popke D., Jiang S., et al. (2021). Sleep loss drives brain region-specific and cell type-specific alterations in ribosome-associated transcripts involved in synaptic plasticity and cellular timekeeping. J. Neurosci. 41, 5386–5398. 10.1523/jneurosci.1883-20.2021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Puentes-Mestril C., Roach J., Niethard N., Zochowski M., Aton S. J. (2019). How rhythms of the sleeping brain tune memory and synaptic plasticity. Sleep 42:zsz095. 10.1093/sleep/zsz095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Raccuglia D., Huang S., Ender A., Heim M.-M., Laber D., Suárez-Grimalt R., et al. (2019). Network-specific synchronization of electrical slow-wave oscillations regulates sleep drive in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 29, 3611–3621. 10.1016/j.cub.2019.08.070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Raizen D. M., Zimmerman J. E., Maycock M. H., Ta U. D., You Y., Sundaram M. V., et al. (2008). Lethargus is a Caenorhabditis elegans sleep-like state. Nature 451, 569–572. 10.1038/nature06535 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Rasch B., Born J. (2013). About sleep's role in memory. Physiol. Rev. 93, 681–766. 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Rattenborg N. C., Voirin B., Cruz S. M., Tisdale R., Dell'Omo G., Lipp H.-P., et al. (2016). Evidence that birds sleep in mid-flight. Nat. Commun. 7:12468. 10.1038/ncomms12468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Raven F., Meerlo P., Zee E. A. V., der Abel T., Havekes R. (2019). A brief period of sleep deprivation causes spine loss in the dentate gyrus of mice. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 160, 83–90. 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.03.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Rechtschaffen A., Bergmann B. M. (1995). Sleep deprivation in the rat by the disk-over-water method. Behav. Brain Res. 69, 55–63. 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00020-t [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Rio-Bermudez C. D., Blumberg M. S. (2018). Active sleep promotes functional connectivity in developing sensorimotor networks. Bioessays 40:e1700234. 10.1002/bies.201700234 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Roffwarg H. P., Muzio J. N., Dement W. C. (1966). Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science 152, 604–619. 10.1126/science.152.3722.604 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Saletin J. M., Goldstein-Piekarski A. N., Greer S. M., Stark S., Stark C. E., Walker M. P. (2016). Human hippocampal structure: a novel biomarker predicting mnemonic vulnerability to, and recovery from, sleep deprivation. J. Neurosci. 36, 2355–2363. 10.1523/jneurosci.3466-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Satterfield B. C., Wisor J. P., Field S. A., Schmidt M. A., Dongen H. P. A. V. (2015). TNFα G308A polymorphism is associated with resilience to sleep deprivation-induced psychomotor vigilance performance impairment in healthy young adults. Brain Behav. Immun. 47, 66–74. 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.12.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Schabus M., Hödlmoser K., Gruber G., Sauter C., Anderer P., Klösch G., et al. (2006). Sleep spindle-related activity in the human EEG and its relation to general cognitive and learning abilities. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 1738–1746. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04694.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Scheffer L. K., Xu C. S., Januszewski M., Lu Z., Takemura S., Hayworth K. J., et al. (2020). A connectome and analysis of the adult Drosophila central brain. Elife 9:e57443. 10.7554/elife.57443 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Seugnet L., Suzuki Y., Donlea J. M., Gottschalk L., Shaw P. J. (2011). Sleep deprivation during early-adult development results in long-lasting learning deficits in adult Drosophila. Sleep 34, 137–146. 10.1093/sleep/34.2.137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Seugnet L., Suzuki Y., Vine L., Gottschalk L., Shaw P. J. (2008). D1 receptor activation in the mushroom bodies rescues sleep-loss-induced learning impairments in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 18, 1110–1117. 10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Shaw P. J., Cirelli C., Greenspan R. J., Tononi G. (2000). Correlates of sleep and waking in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 287, 1834–1837. 10.1126/science.287.5459.1834 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Siegel J. M. (2008). Do all animals sleep? Trends Neurosci. 31, 208–213. 10.1016/j.tins.2008.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Singh K., Ju J. Y., Walsh M. B., DiIorio M. A., Hart A. C. (2014). Deep conservation of genes required for both Drosophila melanogaster and Caenorhabditis elegans sleep includes a role for dopaminergic signaling. Sleep 37, 1439–1451. 10.5665/sleep.3990 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Singh P., Donlea J. M. (2020). Bidirectional regulation of sleep and synapse pruning after neural injury. Curr. Biol. 30, 1063.e3–1076.e3. 10.1016/j.cub.2019.12.065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Sokoloff G., Dooley J. C., Glanz R. M., Wen R. Y., Hickerson M. M., Evans L. G., et al. (2021). Twitches emerge postnatally during quiet sleep in human infants and are synchronized with sleep spindles. Curr. Biol. 31, 3426.e4–3432.e4. 10.1016/j.cub.2021.05.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Sokoloff G., Hickerson M. M., Wen R. Y., Tobias M. E., McMurray B., Blumberg M. S. (2020). Spatiotemporal organization of myoclonic twitching in sleeping human infants. Dev. Psychobiol. 62, 697–710. 10.1002/dev.21954 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Sokoloff G., Uitermarkt B. D., Blumberg M. S. (2015). REM sleep twitches rouse nascent cerebellar circuits: implications for sensorimotor development. Dev. Neurobiol. 75, 1140–1153. 10.1002/dneu.22177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Spano G. M., Banningh S. W., Marshall W., Vivo L., de Bellesi M., Loschky S. S., et al. (2019). Sleep deprivation by exposure to novel objects increases synapse density and axon-spine interface in the hippocampal CA1 region of adolescent mice. J. Neurosci. 39, 6613–6625. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0380-19.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Spiegel K., Knutson K., Leproult R., Tasali E., Cauter E. V. (2005). Sleep loss: a novel risk factor for insulin resistance and type 2 diabetes. J. Appl. Physiol. 99, 2008–2019. 10.1152/japplphysiol.00660.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Stanhope B. A., Jaggard J. B., Gratton M., Brown E. B., Keene A. C. (2020). Sleep regulates glial plasticity and expression of the engulfment receptor draper following neural injury. Curr. Biol. 30, 1092–1101.e3. 10.1016/j.cub.2020.02.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Stefanelli T., Bertollini C., Lüscher C., Muller D., Mendez P. (2016). Hippocampal somatostatin interneurons control the size of neuronal memory ensembles. Neuron 89, 1074–1085. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Stickgold R., Walker M. P. (2007). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Sleep Med. 8, 331–343. 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.03.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Tainton-Heap L. A. L., Kirszenblat L. C., Notaras E. T., Grabowska M. J., Jeans R., Feng K., et al. (2021). A paradoxical kind of sleep in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 31, 578.e6–590.e6. 10.1016/j.cub.2020.10.081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Tang G., Gudsnuk K., Kuo S.-H., Cotrina M. L., Rosoklija G., Sosunov A., et al. (2014). Loss of mTOR-dependent macroautophagy causes autistic-like synaptic pruning deficits. Neuron 83, 1131–1143. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Tempesta D., Socci V., Gennaro L. D., Ferrara M. (2018). Sleep and emotional processing. Sleep Med. Rev. 40, 183–195. 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Thimgan M. S., Seugnet L., Turk J., Shaw P. J. (2015). Identification of genes associated with resilience/vulnerability to sleep deprivation and starvation in Drosophila. Sleep 38, 801–814. 10.5665/sleep.4680 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Thimgan M. S., Suzuki Y., Seugnet L., Gottschalk L., Shaw P. J. (2010). The perilipin homologue, lipid storage droplet 2, regulates sleep homeostasis and prevents learning impairments following sleep loss. PLoS Biol. 8:e1000466. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000466 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Thomas M. J., Moody T. D., Makhinson M., O'Dell T. J. (1996). Activity-dependent β-adrenergic modulation of low frequency stimulation induced LTP in the hippocampal CA1 region. Neuron 17, 475–482. 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80179-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Tiriac A., Blumberg M. S. (2016). Gating of reafference in the external cuneate nucleus during self-generated movements in wake but not sleep. Elife 5:e18749. 10.7554/elife.18749 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Tiriac A., Uitermarkt B. D., Fanning A. S., Sokoloff G., Blumberg M. S. (2012). Rapid whisker movements in sleeping newborn rats. Curr. Biol. 22, 2075–2080. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Tononi G., Cirelli C. (2014). Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81, 12–34. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Tudor J. C., Davis E. J., Peixoto L., Wimmer M. E., Tilborg E., van Park A. J., et al. (2016). Sleep deprivation impairs memory by attenuating mTORC1-dependent protein synthesis. Sci. Signal 9:ra41. 10.1126/scisignal.aad4949 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Tye K. M., Stuber G. D., Ridder B., de Bonci A., Janak P. H. (2008). Rapid strengthening of thalamo-amygdala synapses mediates cue-reward learning. Nature 453, 1253–1257. 10.1038/nature06963 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Vanderheyden W. M., George S. A., Urpa L., Kehoe M., Liberzon I., Poe G. R. (2015). Sleep alterations following exposure to stress predict fear-associated memory impairments in a rodent model of PTSD. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 2335–2346. 10.1007/s00221-015-4302-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Vanderheyden W. M., Poe G. R., Liberzon I. (2014). Trauma exposure and sleep: using a rodent model to understand sleep function in PTSD. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 1575–1584. 10.1007/s00221-014-3890-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Vecsey C. G., Baillie G. S., Jaganath D., Havekes R., Daniels A., Wimmer M., et al. (2009). Sleep deprivation impairs cAMP signalling in the hippocampus. Nature 461, 1122–1125. 10.1038/nature08488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Vienne J., Spann R., Guo F., Rosbash M. (2016). Age-related reduction of recovery sleep and arousal threshold in Drosophila. Sleep 39, 1613–1624. 10.5665/sleep.6032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Viola A. U., Archer S. N., James L. M., Groeger J. A., Lo J. C. Y., Skene D. J., et al. (2007). PER3 polymorphism predicts sleep structure and waking performance. Curr. Biol. 17, 613–618. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.01.073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Viola A. U., Chellappa S. L., Archer S. N., Pugin F., Götz T., Dijk D.-J., et al. (2012). Interindividual differences in circadian rhythmicity and sleep homeostasis in older people: effect of a PER3 polymorphism. Neurobiol Aging 33, 1010.e17–27. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.10.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Vivo L., de Bellesi M., Marshall W., Bushong E. A., Ellisman M. H., Tononi G., et al. (2017). Ultrastructural evidence for synaptic scaling across the wake/sleep cycle. Science 355, 507–510. 10.1126/science.aah5982 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Vivo L., de Nagai H., Wispelaere N. D., Spano G. M., Marshall W., Bellesi M., et al. (2019). Evidence for sleep-dependent synaptic renormalization in mouse pups. Sleep 42:zsz184. 10.1093/sleep/zsz184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Vyazovskiy V. V., Cirelli C., Pfister-Genskow M., Faraguna U., Tononi G. (2008). Molecular and electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 200–208. 10.1038/nn2035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Vyazovskiy V. V., Olcese U., Lazimy Y. M., Faraguna U., Esser S. K., Williams J. C., et al. (2009). Cortical firing and sleep homeostasis. Neuron 63, 865–878. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Walker M. P., Brakefield T., Morgan A., Hobson J. A., Stickgold R. (2002). Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep-dependent motor skill learning. Neuron 35, 205–211. 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00746-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Wassing R., Lakbila-Kamal O., Ramautar J. R., Stoffers D., Schalkwijk F., Someren E. J. W. V. (2019). Restless REM sleep impedes overnight amygdala adaptation. Curr. Biol. 29, 2351.e4–8.e4. 10.1016/j.cub.2019.06.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Watson B. O., Levenstein D., Greene J. P., Gelinas J. N., Buzsáki G. (2016). Network homeostasis and state dynamics of neocortical sleep. Neuron 90, 839–852. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Weiss J. T., Donlea J. M. (2021). Sleep deprivation results in diverse patterns of synaptic scaling across the Drosophila mushroom bodies. Curr. Biol. 31, 3248.e3–61.e3. 10.1016/j.cub.2021.05.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Wu J., Zhou Q., Li J., Chen Y., Shao S., Xiao Y. (2021). Decreased resting-state alpha-band activation and functional connectivity after sleep deprivation. Sci. Rep. 11, 484. 10.1038/s41598-020-79816-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Yamazaki E. M., Antler C. A., Lasek C. R., Goel N. (2020). Residual, differential neurobehavioral deficits linger after multiple recovery nights following chronic sleep restriction or acute total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 44:zsaa224. 10.1093/sleep/zsaa224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Yamazaki E. M., Goel N. (2019). Robust stability of trait-like vulnerability or resilience to common types of sleep deprivation in a large sample of adults. Sleep 43:zsz292. 10.1093/sleep/zsz292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Yang G., Lai C. S. W., Cichon J., Ma L., Li W., Gan W.-B. (2014). Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning. Science 344, 1173–1178. 10.1126/science.1249098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Yap M. H. W., Grabowska M. J., Rohrscheib C., Jeans R., Troup M., Paulk A. C., et al. (2017). Oscillatory brain activity in spontaneous and induced sleep stages in flies. Nat. Commun. 8:1815. 10.1038/s41467-017-02024-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Yeo B. T. T., Tandi J., Chee M. W. L. (2015). Functional connectivity during rested wakefulness predicts vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Neuroimage 111, 147–158. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Yizhar O., Fenno L. E., Prigge M., Schneider F., Davidson T. J., O'Shea D. J., et al. (2011). Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature 477, 171–178. 10.1038/nature10360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Yoo S.-S., Hu P. T., Gujar N., Jolesz F. A., Walker M. P. (2007). A deficit in the ability to form new human memories without sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 385–392. 10.1038/nn1851 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Yurgel M. E., Kakad P., Zandawala M., Nässel D. R., Godenschwege T. A., Keene A. C. (2019). A single pair of leucokinin neurons are modulated by feeding state and regulate sleep–metabolism interactions. PLoS Biol. 17:e2006409. 10.1371/journal.pbio.2006409 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Zhdanova I. V., Wang S. Y., Leclair O. U., Danilova N. P. (2001). Melatonin promotes sleep-like state in zebrafish11 published on the world wide web on 6 April 2001. Brain Res. 903, 263–268. 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02444-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Zucker R. S., Regehr W. G. (2002). Short-term synaptic plasticity. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 64, 355–405. 10.1146/annurev.physiol.64.092501.114547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES