Abstract
Objective
To determine the impact of the affiliate stigma on mental well-being of relatives caring for a person with dementia.
Design
The study was conducted in a cross sectional design.
Setting
The study was conducted in a public setting, addressing relatives caring for a person with dementia.
Participants
Participants were relatives of patients with a formal diagnosis of dementia. Relatives were defined as caring or living closely to a patient. Participants were recruited with the help of care and welfare organizations.
Outcome Measures
The main outcome measure was the impact of the affiliate stigma on mental well-being of caring relatives.
Results
228 participants fully completed the survey. Women, relatives with a higher education and partners experienced more impact of the affiliate stigma on mental well-being than man, relatives with a lower education and relatives with another relationship to the person with dementia (resp. F-ratio = 15.67; p = 0.0001; F-ratio = 2.5865; p = 0.0381; F-ratio = 3.1131; p = 0.0099). The duration of dementia and the age of the caregiver had a clear significant effect on affiliate stigma (F-ratio = 4.9104; p = 0.0083) (F-ratio = 6.5515, p = 0.0112).
Conclusion
This study revealed that caregiver related features are predicting the presence of an affiliate stigma. Interventions to prevent or reduce the impact of this stigma might focus on these groups. Education about dementia and the impact on patients, relatives and the broader social context might alter the affiliate stigma surrounding dementia.
Keywords: dementia, family care, mental health, primary care, taboo
Introduction
Dementia is the collective name given to a group of symptoms that indicates a significant decline in a person's level of cognitive functioning. This syndrome has a global prevalence of about 7% in the population aged 65 years or older (Prince et al., 2013). Currently, more than 48 million people suffer from dementia worldwide. Without effective treatment, the World Health Organization estimates that this prevalence will continue to rise to 131 million people by 2050 (Alzheimer, 2016; Gale et al., 2018; Wolters and Ikram, 2018).
In addition to the decline in cognitive functioning, persons with dementia also experience a significant loss in social, occupational and/or domestic functioning. This makes them dependent on persons in their environment to carry out daily activities. This dependence puts a great physical, emotional and financial burden on the relatives of persons with dementia (Schoenmakers et al., 2010a; Chow et al., 2018). The assumptions made in our society about persons with dementia are often based on stereotypes. This stereotypical presentation is influenced by the later stages of the syndrome, where the person with dementia is fully care dependent (Van Gorp and Vercruysse, 2012). The result of these negative assumptions is that self-stigmatization occurs in both persons with dementia and their relatives, which can cause them feeling insecure in their dealing with the person with dementia (Greenwood et al., 2018; Jeong et al., 2020; Rewerska-Juśko and Rejdak, 2020). In certain cases, stigma negatively influences the opportunities of a timely diagnosis of dementia (Dubois et al., 2016). The affiliation stigma appears relatively independently of the type of dementia and seems rather related to other features and characteristics of patients, caregivers and community (Greenwood et al., 2018; Rewerska-Juśko and Rejdak, 2020).
In 2008, the WHO released a report defining the concept of stigma. With stigma, a distinction is made between the person who is stigmatized and others who attribute negative characteristics to the stigmatized person. In the event that stigma is present in the context of mental illness, it often results in social exclusion and discrimination, which in turn creates an additional emotional burden (Europe WHOROF, 2008). In addition to the emotional burden present in the stigmatized individual, several studies report that the family caregivers and relatives of individuals with mental illness also often experience stigma. This form of stigma present among family caregivers and relatives has been described as “courtesy stigma” or “associative stigma” (Goffman, 1963; Mehta and Farina, 1988). These terms point to the discrimination and prejudice that people may experience because they are associated with individuals who belong to a stigmatized group. In addition to “courtesy stigma,” other studies focused on the internalization of stigma that lives among family caregivers and relatives of individuals with mental illness. This internalization was described as “affiliate stigma.” The term “affiliate stigma” refers to the negative feelings that relatives of stigmatized individuals develop toward themselves, because they perceive the associative stigma that prevails in society. These negative feelings can cause attitude changes toward the person with dementia. Making less contact with the person with dementia, concealing the association with them from the public or generally less engaging in social contact are some examples (Mak and Cheung, 2008).
To date, only a limited number of studies have investigated the affiliate stigma associated with dementia. The purpose of this study is to identify the affiliate stigma present among the relatives of individuals with dementia and its effect on their mental well-being.
Methods
Design
This study examined the relationship between the affiliate stigma experienced by relatives of persons with dementia and the impact on their mental health well-being.
Data for this study were collected cross-sectionally over a 4-month period from August 2020 to November 2020. All participants were informed of the anonymity of the data handling and the purpose of the study prior to starting the survey.
Setting and Participants
Survey participants were relatives of patients with a formal diagnosis of dementia (diagnosed by a physician). No further specification of type or stage of dementia was required since caregiver and contextual characteristics are single predictors of care burden and affiliate stigma (Schoenmakers et al., 2010a; Rewerska-Juśko and Rejdak, 2020). Relatives were defined as caring or living closely to a patient without restriction in time spent caring or relationship with the care receiver (self-appointed carer). Participants were recruited with the help of care and welfare organizations. Participants who did not have a close relative with dementia, formal or professional caregivers and participants who did not complete the survey were excluded from the study or from further data analysis.
To calculate the sample size we started from a convenience sample of 500 participants to achieve a confidence interval between 1.5 and 2.5 for the score on mental well-being. The desired sample consisted of 217 participants.
Collection of Data
The survey was presented on a secured platform (server of the university KU Leuven) using Qualtrics Software. Study data were collected anonymously via a survey distributed through social media with a special focus on communities of family caregivers, relatives, and acquaintances of persons with dementia. The Expertise Centre Dementia Flanders was involved in the recruitment process. In addition, the survey was also shared through the site of a home nursing group that cares for patients with dementia and their relatives. Before starting the survey, the purpose of the study was explained. Surveys that were not completed in full were excluded from further processing.
The survey consisted of three parts: the first part contained questions about the background features of the participants (age, gender, and highest education degree), their relationship with the person with dementia, and how long ago the patient was diagnosed with dementia.
The questions in the second part of the survey were taken from the Affiliate Stigma Scale (ASS) (Mak and Cheung, 2008). The Affiliate Stigma Scale was originally developed to gauge the self-stigmatization that occurs among family caregivers, relatives, or acquaintances of patients with mental illness. The term “mental illness” was replaced with “dementia” in the Affiliate Stigma Scale. The scale consists of 22 questions that test for the internalization of stigma present in relatives. The scale addresses the cognitive, affective, and behavioral domain. The cognitive and the affective component contain 7 items and the behavioral component contains eight items. Participants answered all 22 items using a 4-point Likert scale ranging from “1 = Totally disagree” to “4 = Totally agree.” From these 22 items, the mean affiliate stigma score was calculated, which was a measure of reported stigma. The higher the mean score on the Affiliate Stigma Scale, the higher the internalized stigma. There is no cut off score.
The third section of the survey mapped the mental well-being of the participating relatives. This survey was composed based upon items from the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 and the 20-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Screening (Williams et al., 1999; Siu et al., 2016). Ten indicators of perceived mental well-being were included in the survey: helpless, failed, ashamed, concentration troubles, sleeping troubles, tiredness (little energy), less appetite, sadness, anxiety, anger. Answer options were distributed on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from “1 = Never” to “4 = Always.” A mean score was calculated from the values reported on these ten items and named the “mood score” in further analysis. The higher this mean score, the lower the mental well-being. There was no cut off score.
Data Analysis
The collected data were processed via Excel for descriptive analyses and from there imported into SAS9.4 for multivariate analyses using the General Linear Model procedure (GLM) with estimated effects.
The hypothesis assumed a significant effect of the affiliate stigma reported by the relatives of persons with dementia on their mental well-being (mood-score). The GLM-procedure was used to calculate the effect (by the estimated effect measure) of the affiliate stigma on mental well-being including all background features (independent variables). Second, the impact of the background features on affiliate stigma was analyzed, also by GLM-procedure (F-distribution).
Ethical Consideration
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008 (Puri et al., 2009). All procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the Medical Ethical Board of the University Hospitals of Leuven under the number MP015226 (May 2019) prior to data collection. Participants were comprehensively informed about the study and explicitly agreed to participate. By a simple opting out button, participants could withdraw from further participation.
Patient and Public Involvement
There was no patient or public involvement in the conception or dissemination of this study.
Results
Over a 4-month period, 504 relatives started the survey. During data processing, 40 participants were excluded because they did not have a relative with dementia. In addition, another 236 participants were excluded due to early termination of the survey. 228 participants were included in the final data analysis.
The mean age of the participants was 42.92 years, with a standard deviation (SD) of 15.75 years (Table 1). Most of the participants were women (193, 84.65%). Half of the participants earned a bachelor's degree (114, 50.00%) and about one-third earned a high school diploma (70, 30.70%). For the majority of those who completed the survey, the nearest person with dementia was a parent (97, 42.54%) or grandparent (73, 32.02%). Dementia was diagnosed more than 1 year ago in the majority of cases (199, 87.28%) (Table 1).
Table 1.
Feature | Number | % |
---|---|---|
Demographic features n = 228 | ||
Age in years | 42.92 (average) | Stand dev (15.75) |
Sex | ||
Male | 35 | 15.35% |
Female | 193 | 84.65% |
Education degree | ||
No degree | 5 | 2.19% |
Highschool | 70 | 30.70% |
Bachelor | 114 | 50.00% |
Master | 35 | 15.35% |
PhD | 4 | 1.75% |
Relation to the patient | ||
Partner | 14 | 6.14% |
Sibling | 7 | 3.07% |
Parent | 97 | 42.54% |
Grandparent | 73 | 32.02% |
Aunt/uncle | 8 | 3.51% |
Friend/neighbor | 24 | 10.53% |
No data | 5 | 2.19% |
Time since diagnosis | ||
<6 months | 8 | 3.51% |
6 months to 1 year | 19 | 8.33% |
> 1 year | 199 | 87.28% |
No data | 2 | 0.88% |
The mean affiliate stigma score of the participants was 1.63 [95% CI (1.57, 1.70)]. The mean mood-score of participants was 1.83 [95% CI (1.76, 1.89)]. The estimated effect of the affiliate stigma score on mental well-being, considering account age, gender, degree, relationship with the patient and duration of dementia was 0.66 [95% CI (0.55, 0.76), p <0.001] (Table 2).
Table 2.
Term | Estimate | Std error | t ratio | Prob>|t| | Lower 95% | Upper 95% |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 0.8655918 | 0.262209 | 3.30 | 0.0011* | 0.3485889 | 1.3825947 |
Affiliate Stigma Score mean | 0.65527 | 0.051272 | 12.78 | <0.0001* | 0.5541761 | 0.756364 |
Age | 0.0007379 | 0.002343 | 0.31 | 0.7531 | −0.003882 | 0.0053576 |
Sex (M) | −0.261222 | 0.065981 | −3.96 | 0.0001* | −0.391318 | −0.131126 |
Degree (Bachelor diploma) | 0.1786387 | 0.164229 | 1.09 | 0.2780 | −0.145175 | 0.5024525 |
Degree (PhD) | 0.2081076 | 0.239022 | 0.87 | 0.3850 | −0.263176 | 0.679391 |
Degree (Master diploma) | 0.20349 | 0.172063 | 1.18 | 0.2383 | −0.135769 | 0.542749 |
Degree (highschool) | 0.3304696 | 0.164059 | 2.01 | 0.0453 | 0.0069919 | 0.6539474 |
Relation (sibling) | −0.341489 | 0.164282 | −2.08 | 0.0389 | −0.665407 | −0.01757 |
Relation (grandparents) | −0.357473 | 0.130205 | −2.75 | 0.0066 | −0.614201 | −0.100745 |
Relation (parent) | −0.318028 | 0.104772 | −3.04 | 0.0027 | −0.524608 | −0.111447 |
Relation (aunt/uncle) | −0.600482 | 0.159235 | −3.77 | 0.0002* | −0.914449 | −0.286515 |
Relation (Friend/neighbor) | −0.346818 | 0.128372 | −2.70 | 0.0075 | −0.599932 | −0.093704 |
Time since diagnosis (<6 months) | −0.037392 | 0.128639 | −0.29 | 0.7716 | −0.291032 | 0.2162485 |
Time since diagnosis (1 year−6 months) | 0.0206053 | 0.088116 | 0.23 | 0.8153 | −0.153134 | 0.1943451 |
N = 228.
p <0.01.
Estimated effect as contrast with: Sex: female.
Education degree: no degree.
Relationship: partners.
Time since diagnosis: > 1 year.
There was a significantly different impact of affiliate stigma on mental well-being between men and women (F-ratio = 15.67; p <0.001) where women experienced a higher impact [0.26, 95% CI (0.13, 0.39), p <0.001]. Education also predicted the impact of affiliate stigma on mental well-being (F-ratio = 2.58; p = 0.038). There was a significant difference between individuals who did not obtain a DIPLOMA and those with a high school diploma experiencing less impact of [0.33, 95% CI (0.001, 0.65)] (p = 0.045) (Table 2).
The relationship between participants and the person with dementia affected the impact of affiliate stigma on mental well-being (F-ratio = 3.1131; p = 0.01). The partners experienced the largest impact of affiliate stigma on mental well-being as compared to all other relationships {mean mood score of 2.01 [95% CI (1.77, 2.25)]}. This impact significantly differs from the children's mean mood score of 1.69 [95% CI (1.54, 1.84), p = 0.00]. The aunts and uncles of persons with dementia experienced the lowest impact of affiliate stigma on their mental well-being and presented with a mean mood score of 1.41 [95% CI (1.14, 1.68)], with an estimated difference of 0.60 [95% CI (0.29, 0.91)] as compared to the partners (p = 0.0002) (Tables 2, 3).
Table 3.
Source | Nparm | DF | Sum of squares | F ratio | Prob > F |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 1 | 1 | 1.4436932 | 6.5515 | 0.0112 |
Sex | 1 | 1 | 0.0535956 | 0.2432 | 0.6224 |
Degree | 4 | 4 | 1.6324794 | 1.8521 | 0.1203 |
Relation | 5 | 5 | 2.4800308 | 2.2509 | 0.0507 |
Time since diagnosis | 2 | 2 | 2.1641102 | 4.9104 | 0.0083* |
N = 228.
p <0.01.
Nparm, non-parametric test for multivariate data; DF, degrees of freedom.
The impact of the demographic factors on the affiliate stigma score was analyzed secondarily. The relationship with the patient was a nearly significant predictor of the affiliate stigma (F-ratio = 2.251; p = 0.051) (Table 3). This analysis showed that the duration of dementia and the age of the care had a significant effect on affiliate stigma (F-ratio = 4.910; p = 0.008) (F-ratio = 6.5515, p = 0.011). Relatives of patients who had received the formal diagnosis of dementia for more than 1 year, presented with a mean affiliate stigma score of 1.65 [95% CI (1.48,1.82)], which was significantly higher {0.51 [95% CI (0.17, 0.85)]} than in relatives of patients who received the diagnosis less than 6 months ago (p = 0.0033). Older caregivers seemed also suffering more form affiliate stigma than their younger colleagues do [−0.08, 95%CI (−0.001 to 0.014)] (Table 4).
Table 4.
Term | Estimate | Std error | t ratio | Prob>|t| | Lower 95% | Upper 95% |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 1.350816 | 0.239517 | 5.64 | <0.0001* | 0.8785706 | 1.8230614 |
Age | −0.008061 | 0.003149 | −2.56 | 0.0112* | −0.01427 | −0.001852 |
Sex (M) | −0.044408 | 0.090046 | −0.49 | 0.6224 | −0.221949 | 0.1331326 |
Degree (Bachelor diploma) | −0.004354 | 0.074761 | −0.06 | 0.9536 | −0.151757 | 0.1430488 |
Degree (PhD) | −0.105318 | 0.247695 | −0.43 | 0.6711 | −0.593688 | 0.3830525 |
Degree (Master diploma) | −0.290234 | 0.223106 | −1.30 | 0.1948 | −0.730122 | 0.1496545 |
Degree (highschool) | 0.2014181 | 0.102802 | 1.96 | 0.0514 | −0.001273 | 0.4041095 |
Relation (sibling) | 0.2289657 | 0.220934 | 1.04 | 0.3013 | −0.206641 | 0.6645726 |
Relation (grandparents) | −0.016039 | 0.126772 | −0.13 | 0.8994 | −0.265992 | 0.2339129 |
Relation (parent) | 0.1901151 | 0.114073 | 1.67 | 0.0971 | −0.034797 | 0.4150277 |
Relation (aunt/uncle) | 0.4922047 | 0.171877 | 2.86 | 0.0046* | 0.1533216 | 0.8310877 |
Relation (Friend/neighbor) | 0.2829005 | 0.195482 | 1.45 | 0.1494 | −0.102523 | 0.6683242 |
Time since diagnosis (<6 months) | 0.3700052 | 0.2028 | 1.82 | 0.0695 | −0.029849 | 0.7698589 |
Time since diagnosis (1 year−6 months) | 0.5118241 | 0.171968 | 2.98 | 0.0033* | 0.1727613 | 0.8508869 |
p < 0.01.
Estimated effect as contrast with: Sex: female.
Education degree: no degree.
Relationship: partners.
Time since diagnosis: >1 year.
Discussion
Literature investigating affiliate stigma in the context of dementia is limited to date. This research shows that the affiliate stigma surrounding dementia negatively affects the mental well-being of close relatives. Female caregivers and partners are particularly affected. Furthermore, from this study, it appears that the longer the diagnosis of dementia exists and the older the caregiver, the higher the experience of the affiliate stigma.
Prior studies to date focused on the affiliate stigma in family caregivers of persons with dementia, whereas this study has also included other relatives. Caregivers who appoint themselves as a caregiver are considered as formal caregivers independently of their relationship with the care receiver or the time spent caring (Schoenmakers et al., 2010a, 2011).
This study shows that the impact of the affiliate stigma on mental well-being is higher in female relatives, which is in line with the findings in previous research (Kahn et al., 2016). More women than men take up the role of family caregiver and women often adopt different coping strategies than men (Schoenmakers et al., 2009; Werner et al., 2012). Coping strategies are more determined by caregivers characteristics than by care characteristics; female caregivers generally use a more emotional coping strategy while male caregivers tend to cope in a more problem solving way (Schoenmakers et al., 2009, 2010a). This might explain why woman suffer more form the affiliate stigma. Above, being a family caregiver implies a physical and emotional burden, which has an additional negative effect on mental well-being (Schulz and Martire, 2004; Papastavrou et al., 2007; Ask et al., 2014).
When considering the relationships with the person with dementia, it appears that partners and children report the highest impact of affiliate stigma on mental well-being, which was confirmed in previous research (Werner et al., 2012; Kahn et al., 2016). Partners generally are permanently on care duty and therefore more burdened (Schoenmakers et al., 2010a). Second, the intra-relation and intra-familial role switch is a source of stress for the caring partner and children (Schoenmakers et al., 2010a).
While previous research suggests that lower educational attainment is associated with a higher effect of stigma on mental well-being, this study reveals the opposite (Krajewski et al., 2013; Werner and AboJabel, 2020). In our study, the small number of participants without an education degree might affect the findings. On the other hand, being highly educated might also reduce the impact of feelings of shame as “it (dementia) appears to happen in the best families” (Schoenmakers et al., 2010a).
The results from this study indicate that if the formal diagnosis of dementia exists for a longer period, relatives report a higher affiliate stigma. An explanation for this might be that these relatives already experience a high burden, reinforcing the impact of negative feelings and in this case of the affiliate stigma (Schoenmakers et al., 2009). The relationship between reported affiliate stigma and duration since the formal diagnosis of dementia needs further investigation in the future.
This study has several limitations. First, this study included relatives of patients with all different types of dementia. Since dementia is defined as a syndrome with a broad range of etiologies, the presentation of behavioral changes of persons with dementia also differs. These different presentations might influence the onset of affiliate stigma in relatives. Second, this study was conducted in a selected population (recruited through care and welfare organizations), whereby the different groups were not equally represented. Above, participating relatives were already supported by professionals or at least “help seeking,” since they were recruited through organizations. Therefore, the resilience of these participants might differ from relatives caring without support (Schoenmakers et al., 2010b). Third, the items used to survey mental well-being were selected from existing surveys. We included items explicitly referring to mental well-being: sleep, cognitive functioning, social functioning, appetite, mood status. This selection was drawn by consensus among the researcher and we did not perform a validation of this survey. We thereby only focussed on the negative of mental well-being.
The strength of the study is the relatively large study population as compared to other studies. The use of the self-rating Affiliate Stigma Scale for this population is relatively unique. The affiliate stigma on dementia is well known but poorly studied. The use of the self-rating Affiliate Stigma Scale is therefore a strength. This scale has been reported in previous studies to have a broad scope, as it can be efficiently applied to both family caregivers and other family members of different levels of education (Saffari et al., 2019). We particularly opted not to use the Family Stigma in Alzheimer's Disease Scale (FS-ADS) as this is instrument focusses on Alzheimer's Disease while our premise is that the type of dementia does not play a major role in the presentation of affiliate stigma.
Conclusion
Only few studies are available that investigate affiliate stigma and its effect on mental well-being. From this study, we have learned that some groups are more prone to impact of the affiliate stigma on well-being. Women, partners and relatives with a higher education need particular attention and support to lower the impact of the affiliate stigma on mental well-being. In addition, the longer the diagnosis of dementia exists, the higher the affiliate stigma. Education about dementia and the impact on patients, relatives and the broader social context might alter the affiliate stigma surrounding dementia.
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary materials, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Medical Ethical Board of the University Hospitals of Leuven under the number MP015226. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
PV and BS: conception and design of the research, analysis of data, and revision the article. PV: acquisition of data and drafting the article. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
Our special thanks go to patients willing to participate in this emotionally loaded study.
References
- Alzheimer A.. (2016). Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 12, 459–509. 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ask H., Langballe E. M., Holmen J., Selbaek G., Saltvedt I., Tambs K. (2014). Mental health and wellbeing in spouses of persons with dementia: the Nord-Trondelag Health Study. BMC Public Health. 14, 413. 10.1186/1471-2458-14-413 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chow S., Chow R., Wan A., Lam H. R., Taylor K., Bonin K. (2018). National dementia strategies: what should Canada learn? Can. Geriatr. J. 21, 173–209. 10.5770/cgj.21.299 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois B., Padovani A., Scheltens P., Rossi A., Dell'Agnello G. (2016). Timely diagnosis for Alzheimer's disease: a literature review on benefits and challenges. J Alzheimers Dis. 49, 617–631. 10.3233/JAD-150692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Europe WHOROF (2008). Policies and Practices for Mental Health in Europe. Meeting the challenges. Report No.: ISBN 978 92 890 4279 6. [Google Scholar]
- Gale S. A., Acar D., Daffner K. R. (2018). Dementia. Am. J. Med. 131, 1161–1169. 10.1016/j.amjmed.2018.01.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goffman E.. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Greenwood N., Mezey G., Smith R. (2018). Social exclusion in adult informal carers: A systematic narrative review of the experiences of informal carers of people with dementia and mental illness. Maturitas. 112, 39–45. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2018.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeong J. S., Kim S. Y., Kim J. N. (2020). Ashamed caregivers: self-stigma, information, and coping among dementia patient families. J. Health Commun. 25, 870–878. 10.1080/10810730.2020.1846641 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahn P. V., Wishart H. A., Randolph J. S., Santulli R. B. (2016). Caregiver stigma and burden in memory disorders: an evaluation of the effects of caregiver type and gender. Curr. Gerontol. Geriatr. Res. 2016, 8316045. 10.1155/2016/8316045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krajewski C., Burazeri G., Brand H. (2013). Self-stigma, perceived discrimination and empowerment among people with a mental illness in six countries: Pan European stigma study. Psychiatry Res. 210, 1136–1146. 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mak W. W. S., Cheung R. Y. M. (2008). Affiliate stigma among caregivers of people with intellectual disability or mental illness. J. Appl. Res. Intellect. 21, 532–545. 10.1111/j.1468-3148.2008.00426.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mehta S. I., Farina A. (1988). Associative stigma—perceptions of the difficulties of college-aged children of stigmatized fathers. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 7, 192–202. 10.1521/jscp.1988.7.2-3.192 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Papastavrou E., Kalokerinou A., Papacostas S. S., Tsangari H., Sourtzi P. (2007). Caring for a relative with dementia: family caregiver burden. J. Adv. Nurs. 58, 446–457. 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04250.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prince M., Bryce R., Albanese E., Wimo A., Ribeiro W., Ferri C. P. (2013). The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dement. 9, 63–75.e2. 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puri K. S., Suresh K. R., Gogtay N. J., Thatte U. M. (2009). Declaration of Helsinki, 2008: implications for stakeholders in research. J. Postgrad. Med. 55, 131–134. 10.4103/0022-3859.52846 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rewerska-Juśko M., Rejdak K. (2020). Social stigma of people with dementia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 78, 1339–1343. 10.3233/JAD-201004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saffari M., Lin C. Y., Koenig H. G., O'Garo K. N., Brostrom A., Pakpour A. (2019). HAP version of the Affiliate Stigma Scale in caregivers of people with dementia. Health Promot. Perspect. 9, 31–39. 10.15171/hpp.2019.04 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenmakers B., Buntinx F., Delepeleire J. (2010a). Factors determining the impact of care-giving on caregivers of elderly patients with dementia. A systematic literature review. Maturitas. 66, 191–200. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2010.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenmakers B., Buntinx F., DeLepeleire J. (2010b). Supporting the dementia family caregiver: the effect of home care intervention on general well-being. Aging Ment. Health. 14, 44–56. 10.1080/13607860902845533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenmakers B., Buntinx F., Lepeleire D. J. (2009). The relation between care giving and the mental health of caregivers of demented relatives: a cross-sectional study. Eur. J. Gen. Pract. 15, 99–106. 10.1080/13814780903064455 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenmakers B., Buntinx F., Lepeleire D. J. (2011). The primary care physician and Alzheimer's disease: an international position paper. J. Nutr. Health Aging. 15, 595–596. 10.1007/s12603-011-0135-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulz R., Martire L. M. (2004). Family caregiving of persons with dementia: prevalence, health effects, and support strategies. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 12, 240–249. 10.1097/00019442-200405000-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siu A. L., Bibbins-Domingo K., Grossman D. C., Baumann L. C., Davidson K. W., Ebell M. (2016). Screening for depression in Adults: US preventive services task force recommendation statement. Obstet. Gynecol. Surv. 71, 283–285. 10.1097/01.ogx.0000482188.34232.33 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Gorp B., Vercruysse T. (2012). Frames and counter-frames giving meaning to dementia: a framing analysis of media content. Soc. Sci. Med. 74, 1274–1281. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.12.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werner P., AboJabel H. (2020). Who internalizes courtesy stigma and how? A study among Israeli Arab family caregivers of persons with dementia. Aging Ment Health. 24, 1153–1160. 10.1080/13607863.2019.1584790 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werner P., Mittelman M. S., Goldstein D., Heinik J. (2012). Family stigma and caregiver burden in Alzheimer's disease. Gerontologist 52, 89–97. 10.1093/geront/gnr117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams Jr J. W, Mulrow C. D., Kroenke K., Dhanda R., Badgett R. G., Omori D., et al. (1999). Case-finding for depression in primary care: a randomized trial. Am. J. Med. 106, 36–43. 10.1016/S0002-9343(98)00371-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolters F. J., Ikram M. A. (2018). Epidemiology of dementia: the burden on society, the challenges for research. Methods Mol. Biol. 1750, 3–14. 10.1007/978-1-4939-7704-8_1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary materials, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.