Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Feb 3;17(2):e0263383. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0263383

Green returns to education: Does education affect pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors in China?

Qi Wang 1, Geng Niu 2, Xu Gan 3,*, Qiaoling Cai 2
Editor: Shihe Fu4
PMCID: PMC8812898  PMID: 35113928

Abstract

Using microdata from the Chinese General Social Survey (CGSS 2010), this paper investigates whether there are green returns to education in China, where educational attainment promotes pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. We establish causality by exploiting the exogenous variation induced by the implementation of the Compulsory Schooling Law (CSL) in China. We find evidence that educational attainment is associated with higher levels of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, and these estimates are robust to various robustness checks. Further analysis reveals that the acquisition of environmental knowledge is the channel that drives the effect of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Finally, the effects of education are heterogeneous across individuals.

1. Introduction

China faces severe environmental problems such as air pollution, depletion of resources, extreme weather events and food contaminations in recent years [13]. Environmental problems not only significantly increase the morbidity and mortality rates of various diseases but also generate a heavy medical burden and large disutility [4]. The health loss erodes average life expectancy and human-capital accumulation, thereby jeopardizing economic development [5]. These environmental problems are, at least partly, rooted in human behavior [6,7], and can thus be mitigated by changing the relevant behaviors so as to improve environmental quality. Therefore, understanding the determinants of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors is of high academic and policy relevance in China. Nevertheless, the research in this direction is still in its infancy. Most of the existing studies have focused on the harmful effects of pollutions produced by firm-level manufacturing activity in China [8]. In fact, an important part of the environmental contamination comes from the daily practices of the households, which has not received sufficient scholarly attention [9].

This paper undertakes an in-depth examination of the role that education plays in affecting individual pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors in China. The association between education and many positive economic, social and health outcomes is well documented in the economic literature [1014]. In principal, education may affect household environmental behaviors in several ways. First, formal schooling is the primary way individuals acquire knowledge and skills [15]. Second, education helps people to obtain access to more sources and types of information [16], which further facilitates knowledge acquisition. Knowledge and information about environmental issues might increase individuals’ sense of environmental responsibility. In addition, education might increase the marginal utility of pro-environmental behaviors by enabling people to pursue a higher level of demand [17]. Alternately, education might decrease people’s environmental awareness if the education system directs people to be obsessed with competition and thus makes people focus on self-enhancement values [18].

In this paper, we rely on data from the Chinese General Social Survey (CGSS) conducted in wave 2010 for empirical analysis. A major advantage of this dataset lies in the provision of questions concerning a wide range of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. We find a strong positive association between education and pro-environmental attitudes as well as behaviors. To alleviate the potential endogenous concern, we instrument education with personal exposure to the implementation of the Compulsory Schooling Law (henceforth, CSL) in China. The two-stage least squares (2SLS) regression results confirm that education imposes a positive influence on both pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. In particular, an additional year of schooling results in an increase of 0.012 and 0.011 in the pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, which are approximately 2.1% and 2% of the corresponding sample means. To test the validity of our analysis, we conduct several robustness checks. It turns out that our results are robust to placebo tests, alternative measures of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, and sample restrictions. We also find that the effects of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors are more pronounced for males, rural residents, and low-income individuals.

In addition, we examine a potential mechanism underlying the effect of schooling education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, namely environmental knowledge. The willingness to alleviate environmental problem is a partial function of risk perception, which is reflected in environmental knowledge [5]. The acquisition of environmental knowledge makes individuals more informed about the situation of local and global environment, negative health effects of environmental problems, and public environmental policies. Indeed, education facilitates a better understanding of complex environmental messages such as climate change [15,19,20]. Hence, the environmental knowledge may play a major role in forming people’s pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Thanks to the richness of the CGSS 2010 dataset, we are able to measure the level of environmental literacy among Chinese households and test whether environmental knowledge is an underlying mechanism through which formal education influences pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. In particular, the CGSS 2010 asks ten true or false questions concerning fundamental concepts of environment to assess the respondents’ environmental knowledge. We observe a lack of environmental literacy among Chinese people. The proportion of correct answers to most of questions is below 62%, while the proportion of the corresponding incorrect responses or “do not know” answers is quite sizable. We also discover that people with a higher level of formal education are more likely to get higher accuracy rate in answering environmental questions. Furthermore, a mediation analysis is conducted to investigate whether environmental knowledge serves as a mediator. We find that the effect of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors attenuates substantially after controlling for environmental knowledge.

The contributions of this paper are three-fold. First, we add to the small yet burgeoning literature on household environment-related behaviors [5,7,21] by identifying the important role that formal education plays in shaping pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Furthermore, we examine the pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors of households in the context of the severe environmental problems in China. While there are a large number of papers studying the pollutions associated with firm-level manufacturing activities in China [22,23], the literature on Chinese households’ pro-environmental behaviors is rather underdeveloped. In particular, with the increase in social responsibility for environmental well-being and the sustainable development of economy, Chinese households are encouraged to take pro-environmental behaviors. For example, following the practice in Shanghai, many local governments in China are considering introducing new garbage-sorting regulations. Thus, it is important to understand the determinants of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors in China, which is the theme of our study.

Second, despite the ample evidence on the association between education and pro-environmental behaviors including consumption, conservation and lifestyle [1,24,25], most of the existing studies fail to deal with the potential endogeneity of education. The only exceptions are Meyer [26] and Chankrajang and Muttarak [15], both of which employ changes in compulsory education laws to establish a causal relation between education and pro-environmental behaviors in Europe and in Thailand, respectively. Building on their works, we are among the first to provide causal estimates of the effects of general education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors in China. Studying the Chinese context is interesting and important. As the cost of fast industrial growth and rapid urbanization, China has suffered a series of environmental problems [27]. As the largest developing country in the world, the environmental problems of China have strong global impacts. Environmental attitudes and behaviors of Chinese residents play an important role in affecting the country’s future environmental scenarios. In addition, we identify that the acquisition of environmental knowledge partly mediates the causal effect of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. The existing studies have found that education has speeded up the acquisition and the diffusion of knowledge [28]. Our study therefore contributes to the literature by shedding direct lights on the channel behind the nexus between education and pro-environmental behaviors.

Third, our study contributes to the literature that explores the social returns to education. Previous works have investigated the impact of schooling education on economic, social and health outcomes such as labor market returns [29], risk-takings in financial market [14], climate adaptation [28], adult civic engagements and attitudes [10], cognitive abilities at old age [12], health capital [11] and mortality [13]. In particular, utilizing the firm level data, Amore et al. [21] show that CEO education forms managerial styles, giving rise to greater sustainability in corporate actions. By providing a detailed assessment at individual-level of how formal education influences pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors in China, we lend further support to those who stress the importance of education’s positive outcomes. Our results indicate that the improvement of educational attainment promotes pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, which in turn are likely to have a positive influence on economic sustainability.

Our study also has important policy implications. As in most industrialized and industrializing nations, Chinese government is seeking to set the country on a path to more sustainable development, with a focus on the effectiveness administrative and regulatory instruments [30]. This paper offers an alternative solution by stressing the efficiency of increasing access to general education as an essential way of motivating pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors among a broad population.

The rest of the paper proceeds as follows: Section 2 introduces the related literature and the background of the CSL in China. Section 3 describes the data source and estimation strategy. Section 4 presents the main results and the robustness of our results. Section 5 provides the possible mechanism analysis. Section 6 summarizes the results and provides some concluding thoughts.

2. Literature review and institutional background

It is well documented that education is associated with a wide range of positive outcomes [31]. While early studies focus on financial outcomes such as income and wealth, the non-pecuniary returns to education, in various domains such as job satisfaction, health, marriage, and parenting, are receiving increasing scholarly attention [31]. Moreover, education not only brings private returns but might also generate externalities by affecting social outcomes such as economic growth, innovation, regional crime, and tax revenue [3234].

As environmental problems are closely related to human behavior [6,7], individuals’ pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors are expected to bring positive externalities to the society. Education might promote pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors in several ways. First, formal schooling is the primary way individuals acquire knowledge and skills [15]. As environmental concepts and topics can be complicated, knowledges and skills accumulated through education make individuals familiar with scientific terms in environment issues [15,19]. Second, and relatedly, people with a higher level of formal education have access to more sources and types of information [16]. An increased exposure to information further increases peoples’ environmental knowledge and environmental awareness. Third, based on the hierarchy of needs theory [17,35], education might help individuals meet their basic needs, and thereby enabling them to adopt a pro-environmental lifestyle to pursue a higher level of demand [9,36]. In another word, education might increase the marginal utility of pro-environmental behaviors relative to the marginal utility of money.

While some prior studies have explored the relation between education and pro-environmental behaviors [1,15,2426], causal evidence in the context of China is still lacking. A causal analysis can provide important public policy implications. China also offers an interesting context to study this topic. China is facing many severe environmental problems [13]. As the second largest economy in the world, China’ environmental problems can have global impacts. For example, China has been ranked first of world top economies in terms of carbon dioxide emissions [37].

Since the 1980s, China began a process of education reform that aimed to gradually align the educational system with the newly emerging marketization of the economy. The Compulsory Schooling Law (CSL) of the People’s Republic of China was passed on April 12, 1986 and officially went into effect on July 1, 1986. This was the first formal law to specify education policies for the entire country [3840]. The CSL stipulated that a nine-year compulsory education should be mandatory and free for school-age children in China and across all ethnicity groups. All children at age of six (or sometimes age seven) were required to enroll in schools. A nine-year compulsory education was divided into two stages: six-year primary education and three-year junior middle school education. After primary school, graduates can be directly admitted into junior middle schools without general selection tests. Additionally, it was illegal to employ school-age children or adolescents for any organization or individual. Since local governments are allowed to autonomously determine the steps to implement the CSL in accordance with the economic development of the region [41], the timing of the actual implementation of the CSL varies significantly across provinces. S1 Table in S1 Appendix shows the time when the CSL was introduced at the province level. Some coastal provinces or municipalities were among the first to enforce the reform (e.g., Shanghai, Beijing and Zhejiang), while other provinces did not implement the new system until the early or mid-1990s (e.g., Gansu, Guangxi, Hainan, Hunan and Tibet). The overall year gap is six years between the earliest provinces and latest provinces in the sample.

The CSL in China notably raised the average education level of a broad population. By 2000, China basically realized the goal of universal compulsory education, covering 85% of its population. As illustrated in Fig 1, the junior secondary school enrollment rate dramatically increased from 68.4% in 1985 to 98.4% in 2005. Meanwhile, the illiteracy rate fell from 23.5% in the 1982 to 6.72% in the 2000 according to national census. In the meantime, the implementation of the CSL is a great exogenous factor with respect to personal educational attainment in China, because it is independent of individual preferences and economic conditions and cannot be adjusted in advance. Hence, we utilize this shock to construct an instrumental variable for education, which will be explained in detail in the identification strategy section.

Fig 1. School enrollment rate in China 1981–2006.

Fig 1

Note: This figure illustrates the school enrollment rate in China over the period between 1981 and 2006. The solid line represents the primary school enrollment rate and the dashed line represents the junior high school enrollment rate. Data source is National Bureau of Statistics of China.

3. Data, variables, and models

3.1. Data

Our main source of data is the Chinese General Social Survey (CGSS), which is a comprehensive database that aims to systematically monitor the changing relationship between social structure and quality of life in both urban and rural China. Initiated in 2003, the CGSS is one of the earliest national representative cross-sectional survey projects in China. Since 2010, the survey has been conducted annually by the National Survey Research Center (NSRC) at Renmin University of China. The respondents are required to be age 18 and above.

We use the 2010 round of CGSS data to explore whether there are green returns to education in China, because it is the only wave that contains information about respondents’ pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors as well as environmental knowledge, and is in the meantime publicly available. In 2010, the survey covers 11,783 respondents, residing in 134 cities across 31 of China’s provinces. To shorten questionnaire length and improve survey compliance, some modules in CGSS 2010 are randomly assigned to a subset of respondents. For example, for questions on environmental attitudes and behaviors, only those born in February, September, November, or December are asked. Thus, our study can only use approximately one third of the sample. We dropped the observations with missing values for variables that are used in the analysis and end up with 3,661 observations. We conduct a balancing test based on a range of observational variables between the regression sample and the dropped sample to check whether there exists a severe sample selection issue. The results, shown in S2 Table in S2 Appendix, suggest that the two samples are similar in observational characteristics, which gives us further confidence in the representativeness of our regression sample.

3.2 Variables

3.2.1 Pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors

The CGSS 2010 asks respondents three questions regarding the willingness of their pro-environmental attitudes (Question 12 in L part of the survey) and six questions about the frequency of their pro-environmental behaviors (Question 20 in L part of the survey). For three attitude questions, respondents are asked to rate their willingness to pay higher prices, higher taxes and to lower their standard of living for environmental protection based on five ordinal scales: (i) “Extremely unwilling to”; (ii) “Reluctant”; (iii) “Do not care”; (iv) “Willing to” and (v) “Extremely willing to”. As for the six pro-environmental behaviors, taking waste recycling for example, the respondents are asked whether they have taken any actions to separate wastes for recycling, and they need to indicate how often they carry out such actions given three ordinal options: (i) “Not at all”, (ii) “Occasionally” and (iii) “Frequently”. The exact wording of other environmental behaviors is provided in Table 1.

Table 1. Summary of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors.
% of respondents
Panel A: Pro-environmental attitudes
Extremely unwilling to Reluctant to Do not care Willing to Extremely willing to
To protect the environment, are you willing to pay higher prices? 7.8 25.4 20.2 37.3 9.4
To protect the environment, are you willing to pay higher taxes? 9.8 31.2 21.1 31.7 6.2
To protect the environment, are you willing to lower your standard of living? 12.6 33.7 19.6 28.6 5.4
Panel B: Pro-environmental behaviors
Not at all Occasionally Frequently
How often do you separate wastes for recycling? 23.8 32.6 43.5
How often do you buy locally produced products or groceries without chemical fertilizers or pesticides? 32.0 37.7 30.3
How often do you buy environmentally friendly products marked with an environmental label? 34.1 41.6 24.3
How often do you reduce car usage for environmental protection? 28.8 44.4 26.9
How often do you cut down energy consumption for environmental protection? 27.1 40.3 32.7
How often do you cut down water consumption for environmental protection? 17.0 34.0 49.0

Note: Panel A reports the proportion of respondents providing “Extremely unwilling”, “Reluctant to”, “Do not care”, “Willing to” and “Extremely willing to” answers to each of the three pro-environmental attitudes questions. Panel B reports the proportion of respondents providing “Not at all”, “Occasionally”, and ‘‘Frequently” answers to each of the six pro-environmental behaviors questions. Percentages may not sum up to 100 due to rounding.

Table 1 summarizes the pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Specifically, Panel A reports the proportion of respondents providing “Extremely unwilling”, “Reluctant to”, “Do not care”, “Willing to” and “Extremely willing to” answers to each of the three pro-environmental attitudes questions, while Panel B shows the distribution of respondents providing “Not at all”, “Occasionally”, and “Frequently” answers to each of the six pro-environmental behaviors questions. Overall, there are significant variations in respondents’ pro-environmental attitudes. To protect the environment, respondents are more willing to pay higher prices than paying higher taxes and lowering living standard. Panel B of Table 1 shows that the majority of Chinese respondents carry out pro-environmental behaviors on a regular basis.

Following the previous literature [26], we aggregate the three attitudes questions into one variable. For each of the three attitude questions, respondents are asked to provide answers on a five-point scale from “Extremely unwilling to” (i) to “Extremely willing to” (v), with a higher value indicating a stronger pro-environmental attitude. By summing answers to the three questions together, we obtain a variable with a maximum score of 15. We divide this variable by 15 to construct the variable Pro-environmental attitudes, which has a maximum score of one. Similarly, we aggregate the six behavior questions into one variable. For each of the six behavior questions, respondents provide answers on a three-point scale, with a higher value indicating a higher intensity of pro-environmental behaviors. We construct the variable Pro-environmental behaviors by summing answers to the six behavior questions and divide the result by 18, which makes the variable has a maximum value of one. All else being equal, individuals who have higher values of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors are likely to think and behave in a more environmentally friendly manner.

3.2.2 Other variables

The main explanatory variable for our analysis is Education, which is defined as the years of schooling that the respondent receives as in the literature [38,42]. Since the CGSS does not directly ask for the years of schooling, we quantify Education by assigning zero to being without schooling, six years to a primary school education, nine years to a junior high school education, twelve years to a senior high school education, thirteen years to a vocational school education, fifteen years to a junior college education, sixteen years to a college level, eighteen years to a graduate school education and beyond. Moreover, the CGSS data set collects detailed demographic information, which allows us to control for a range of confounding factors. Specifically, we control for Male, Married, Income, Employed, Han nationality, Rural, cohort fixed effect, and city fixed effect. Table 2 provides the definitions of variables of interest.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics.
Variables Definition Obs. Mean Std. Dev. Median Min Max
Pro-environmental attitudes The intensity of an individual’s pro-environmental attitudes 3,458 0.569 0.210 0.6 0.066 1
Pro-environmental behaviors The intensity of an individual’s pro-environmental behaviors 3,661 0.539 0.199 0.555 0.055 1
Education Years of schooling 3,661 8.933 4.607 9 0 18
Age Age in number of years 3,661 47.31 15.73 47 18 85
Male = 1 if male, = 0 otherwise 3,661 0.473 0.499 0 0 1
Married = 1 if married, = 0 otherwise 3,661 0.807 0.395 1 0 1
Income Ln (Personal income+1) 3,661 9.298 4.093 9.473 0 16.118
Han nationality = 1 if Han nationality, = 0 otherwise 3,661 0.918 0.275 1 0 1
Employed = 1 if employed, = 0 otherwise 3,661 0.629 0.483 1 0 1
Rural = 1 if rural, = 0 otherwise 3,661 0.488 0.500 0 0 1
CSL Exposure to implementation of the CSL 3,661 0.222 0.386 0 0 1
Environmental knowledge Number of correct responses to ten environmental knowledge questions 3,601 5.154 2.762 5 0 10

Table 2 reports the descriptive statistics of the variables outlined above. The mean educational attainment is 8.933 years, which suggests that on average the final degree that the respondents in our sample receive is merely junior high school. The majority of respondents are married, employed and belong to Han ethnic nationality. 47.3% of respondents are men and 48.8% live in rural areas.

To have a preliminary view on how education relates to pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, we plot the average educational attainment and the average pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors against the birth-year level in Figs 2 and 3. Obviously, the educational level displays an upward trend; it increases from 6.66 years in 1930 to 12.23 years in 1990. More importantly, the average pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors move very closely with the level of education, suggesting strong positive correlations between education and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, respectively.

Fig 2. Average years of schooling and pro-environmental attitudes across year of birth.

Fig 2

Note: This figure illustrates the relationships between education and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors across year of birth. The solid line represents the average years of schooling and the dashed lines represent the average pro-environmental attitudes (2a) and behaviors (2b) respectively. Data source is CGSS 2010.

Fig 3. Average years of schooling and pro-environmental behaviors across year of birth.

Fig 3

Note: This figure illustrates the relationships between education and pro-environmental behaviors across year of birth. The solid line represents the average years of schooling and the dashed lines represent the average pro-environmental behaviors. Data source is CGSS 2010.

To address the possibility that education is correlated with other individual characteristics and to gauge the impact of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors more precisely, we proceed with a regression framework, which is specified below.

3.3 Model specifications

We estimate the relationship between education and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors using the following multivariate model:

Yij=α+βEducationij+δXij+γCh+ρCityk+εij, (1)

where Yij is the pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors for individual i in province j, Educationij is the years of schooling, Xij is a vector of control variables, α is the intercept, Ch is a vector of cohort dummies, and εij is the error term. Given the distinct regional segregation in China, we add city dummies Cityk in all regressions to capture city fixed effects. We are mainly interested in the coefficient of Educationij, β, which directly captures the relationship between educational attainment and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. We use OLS for estimation and report standard errors clustered at the cohort-city level to adjust for the potential correlation of observations within the same cluster.

Based on the above OLS regression, however, we cannot give a causal interpretation for the relationship between educational attainment and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors for the omitted variable bias due to the unobservable confounding factors. For example, some individual characteristics that can affect pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors are hard to measure, e.g., sense of responsibility. Those with higher sense of responsibility are more likely to achieve higher educational attainment and in the meantime care more about environmental issues. If this is the case, the OLS estimates would be biased.

To address the endogeneity problem, we adopt an instrumental variable approach to estimate the causal effects of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Valid instruments need to be correlated with the endogenous variable and not to be linked to the dependent variable through channels other than the independent variable. We exploit the enactment of the CSL in 1986 as an exogenous source of changes in individual schooling levels. On the one hand, as discussed above, the implementation of the CSL in China made a great achievement in increasing the average education level of the population as a whole. This implies that individuals that are more affected by the CSL are likely to end up with higher educational attainment. On the other hand, the enactment and implementation of the CSL are exogenously determined by governments and are unlikely to be correlated with personal characteristics and other potentially confounding factors that influence pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Moreover, the CSL would not directly affect pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, because the development of pro-environmental concerns hardly started at that time.

Following Liang and Dong [42], we construct the instrumental variable, CSL, by measuring individual exposure to the implementation of compulsory schooling law. We define it as a continuous variable that falls between zero and one. Specifically, CSL equals one if the individual is fully affected by the compulsory schooling law (i.e. aged 6 or below) and equals zero if he/she is completely unaffected by the law (i.e. aged 16 or above). For those whose ages were between 6 and 16 when the law was enforced, we define CSL as (16 –year of implementation + year of birth)/9, a decreasing linear function of age. Fig 4 illustrates the construction of the instrumental variable. Note that in constructing the instrumental variable, we take into account the fact that the year of implementation varies across provinces as shown in S1 Table in S1 Appendix. In other words, for individuals of the same age, their exposures to the CSL may differ due to the differences in the timing of the CSL in the provinces they lived.

Fig 4. Construction of instrumental variable.

Fig 4

Note: This figure illustrates the construction of instrumental variable. The X-axis is the individual age at the implementation of the CSL and the Y-axis is the individual exposure to CSL, which equals to one if the individual is fully affected by CSL (i.e. aged 6 or below) and equals to zero if the individual is unaffected by CSL (i.e. age 16 or above). A linear function is assumed for the ages in between.

Using the instrument CSL, we re-estimate the effect of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors by the two-stage least squares (2SLS) approach:

Educationij=α0+α1CSLij+α2Xij+α3Ch+α4Cityk+ηij, (2)
Yij=β0+β1Education^ij+β2Xij+β3Ch+β4Cityk+eij, (3)

where Eqs (2) and (3) are the first stage and second stage regressions, respectively. Education^ij represents the fitted value of Educationij from the first stage regression, and the other variables are the same as in Eq (1). Again, we cluster standard errors at the cohort-city level.

4. Regression results

4.1. Benchmark results

We first examine the relationship between educational attainment and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors by estimating Eq (1) using OLS models. Table 3 presents the regression results. Columns (1) and (2) report the estimated coefficients on pro-environmental attitudes, while columns (3) to (4) report the estimated coefficients on pro-environmental behaviors. As explained above, each model includes a constant, cohort fixed effects, and city dummies, the coefficients of which are not reported for brevity. At the bottom of the table, we report the number of observations and adjusted R-squared. When no control variables are included in columns (1) and (3), we find that education has a significantly positive coefficient estimate. As expected, the estimate of education is still positive and significant at the 1% level when we control for a set of background characteristics in columns (2) and (4), suggesting that more educated individuals are more environmentally friendly.

Table 3. The effects of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, baseline results.

Pro-environmental attitudes Pro-environmental behaviors
(1) (2) (3) (4)
Education 0.009*** 0.008*** 0.011*** 0.008***
(0.001) (0.001) (0.001) (0.001)
Male 0.005 -0.001
(0.008) (0.005)
Married 0.009 0.011
(0.009) (0.008)
Income 0.001 0.001
(0.001) (0.001)
Employed -0.008 -0.010
(0.007) (0.007)
Han nationality -0.058 -0.017
(0.022) (0.016)
Rural -0.012 -0.063***
(0.011) (0.010)
Cohort FE Yes Yes Yes Yes
City FE Yes Yes Yes Yes
Observations 3,458 3,456 3,664 3,661
R-Square 0.177 0.194 0.301 0.316

Note: The table reports the estimates from OLS regressions of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Columns (1)-(2) report the results for pro-environmental attitudes, while columns (3)-(4) report the results for pro-environmental behaviors. All regressions include a constant term, cohort fixed effects, and city dummies, but their coefficients are suppressed for brevity. Robust standard errors are clustered at the cohort-city level and are reported in parentheses.

***, ** and * indicate 1%, 5% and 10% significance levels, respectively.

Inspecting the control variables yields some interesting observations. Rural residents display less pro-environmental behaviors than people in urban areas. This is probably because urban residents have more opportunities to perceive the importance of environmental issues, such as the information from media publicity, and therefore have higher incentives to conduct pro-environmental behaviors.

To solve the potential endogeneity problem, we also perform an instrumental variable analysis. As shown in Table 4, the coefficients on the instrumental variable, CSL, in the first-stage regression are positive and highly significant. This is consistent with our expectation that individuals that are more affected by the CSL have higher educational attainment. The F-test statistics of the first-stage regressions are 129.41 and 143.44 respectively, which exceed the weak instrument critical value established by Staiger and Stock [43]. Taken together, these results indicate that the weak identification is unlikely an issue in our study. More importantly, education remains highly significant in the second-stage regression. The magnitude of the coefficient is not negligible. Examining the economic significance of the education coefficient in column (1), we find that an extra year of schooling is associated with an increase of 0.012 in the pro-environmental attitudes, which is equivalent to 2.1% (= 0.012/0.569) of the sample mean. The coefficient estimate in column (2) implies that an additional year of schooling results in an increase of 0.011 in the pro-environmental behaviors, which is approximately 2% (= 0.011/0.539) of the corresponding sample means. Therefore, the impact of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors is economically sizable on annual basis. Our results show that the IV estimates are larger than the corresponding OLS estimates for regressions of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. The downward bias of OLS estimates might be driven by measurement error in survey response. For example, some studies suggest that measurement errors in schooling can lead to downward bias in the OLS estimate of the effect of schooling [29,44].

Table 4. The effects of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, 2SLS results.

Pro-environmental attitudes Pro-environmental behaviors
(1) (2)
Education 0.012** 0.011**
(0.005) (0.004)
Controls Yes Yes
Observations 3,456 3,661
First-stage estimation
CSL 2.937*** 3.141***
(0.258) (0.262)
F-statistics 129.41 143.44

Note: The table reports the estimates from 2SLS regressions of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Columns (1) and (2) report the results for pro-environmental attitudes and for pro-environmental behaviors, respectively. All regressions have the same controls as in column (2) of Table 3, but their coefficients are suppressed for brevity. Robust standard errors are clustered at the cohort-city level and are reported in parentheses.

***, ** and * indicate 1%, 5% and 10% significance levels, respectively.

4.2. Heterogeneity analysis

The benchmark results show that educational attainment has strong and positive impacts on the pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors of the whole sample. However, these impacts might differ across different groups of people. In this section, we examine the heterogeneity in the effects of education in the following three aspects. Specifically, we split our sample based on a given individual characteristic (e.g., gender) and then within each subsample, regress pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors on education, controlling for other observable characteristics.

First, we examine whether the education effects on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors differ by gender. In Table 5, the results in columns (1) and (7) are for male group, while those in columns (2) and (8) are for female group. For pro-environmental attitudes, the estimated coefficient on education is significant for males, but insignificant for females, suggesting that men are more likely to have pro-environmental attitudes through education. This might be attributed to the gender differences in pro-environmental attitudes that females are found to be more environmentally friendly than males [26]. For pro-environmental behaviors, however, the effects of education attainment do not display gender differences.

Table 5. Heterogeneity analysis.

Pro-environmental attitudes Pro-environmental behaviors
Male Female Urban Rural High income Low income Male Female Urban Rural High income Low income
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) (12)
Education 0.024*** 0.004 0.003 0.035*** 0.011 0.023** 0.002** 0.015** 0.006 0.023** 0.013 0.012*
(0.009) (0.005) (0.007) (0.011) (0.012) (0.011) (0.001) (0.006) (0.006) (0.009) (0.011) (0.007)
Controls Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Observations 1,646 1,810 1,807 1,649 718 656 1,734 1,928 1,876 1,786 753 712
First-stage estimation
CSL 2.831*** 3.148*** 2.658*** 3.040*** 2.076*** 3.620*** 2.831*** 3.468*** 2.828*** 3.349*** 2.346*** 3.812***
(0.499) (0.375) (0.330) (0.535) (0.622) (0.837) (0.469) (0.372) (0.342) (0.555) (0.593) (0.811)
F-statistics 32.12 70.50 64.72 32.19 11.12 18.67 36.55 86.92 68.25 36.34 15.62 22.08

Note: The table reports the results of heterogeneity analysis. The sample is split into two subsamples according to whether the individual is male in columns (1)-(2) and in columns (7)-(8); whether the individual lives in urban areas in columns (3)-(4) and in columns (9)-(10); and whether the individual earns high income in columns (5)-(6) and in columns (11)-(12). All regressions use 2SLS models for estimation and have the same controls as in column (2) of Table 3 except that the variable used as the criteria for splitting the sample is not included. The coefficients on the control variables are suppressed for brevity. Robust standard errors are clustered at the cohort-city level and are reported in parentheses.

***, ** and * indicate 1%, 5% and 10% significance levels, respectively.

Second, given the huge urban-rural divide in China, it is interesting to study whether the education effects are different between these two areas. Columns (3) and (9) present the results for urban residents, while columns (4) and (10) present the results for rural residents. The coefficient on education is positive and significant for rural residents but insignificant for urban residents. There are two possible explanations. First, rural residents exhibit less pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors than those urbanites, as found in our benchmark analysis. Second, rural residents are less exposed to the discussion about environmental issues, e.g. less access to social media. As a result, education plays a bigger role for these individuals.

Finally, we investigate whether the education effects on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors vary with income levels. To explore this possible heterogeneity, we divide our sample into two distinct income groups, namely the groups of respondents whose income is in the top quintile and bottom quintile of the income distribution. Then we re-run the regressions separately for these two income groups. The results are presented in columns (5)-(6) and (11)-(12) of Table 5. The estimate of schooling years is positive and significant for people in low-income groups, but insignificant for those with high income. Prior studies show that people’s demand for public goods such as clean environment will increase with income [45]. Accordingly, high income people might already have a high level of environmental awareness, so that the effect of additional schooling years become negligible. Put it in another word, our finding suggests that income can substitute for the effect of education.

4.3. Robustness checks

4.3.1. Placebo test

The validity of our instrument relies on the assumption that the CSL influences pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors only through its impact on education rather than through other pathways. Put differently, our instrument should not be systematically correlated with the omitted variables that also affect pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Otherwise, it may just reflect some province-specific structural changes other than the implementation of the CSL that are correlated with pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. To check the validity of this assumption, we conduct a placebo test. Specifically, we assume that the CSL had been implemented five years earlier than the actual implementation year of each province and re-run the regressions as Eqs (2) and (3) using the false reform exposure as the instrument. We expect that the false reform exposure has no impact on individual education levels and, more importantly, no impact on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors.

Columns (1) and (2) of Table 6 show the results of the placebo test. Obviously, the coefficients on the false reform exposure are insignificant in both stages of regressions. This finding substantiates the validity of our instrumental variable and indicates that the benchmark results are reliable.

Table 6. Robustness checks.
Placebo test Alternative pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors Sample with shorter birth cohort span
Pro-environmental attitudes Pro-environmental behaviors Pro-environmental attitudes Pro-environmental behaviors Pro-environmental attitudes Pro-environmental
behaviors
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
Education 0.146 0.145 0.043** 0.042** 0.022** 0.009**
(1.104) (0.108) (0.017) (0.018) (0.008) (0.004)
Controls Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Observations 3,337 3,542 3,456 3,661 2,403 2,511
First-stage estimation
CSL 1.052 1.052 2.937*** 3.141*** 1.794*** 1.841***
(0.693) (0.693) (0.258) (0.262) (0.283) (0.273)
F-statistics 2.31 2.31 129.41 143.44 40.07 45.31

Note: The table reports the estimates from three robustness checks. 2SLS models are used for estimation. In columns (1)-(2), two placebo tests are conducted based on the assumption that the CSL had been implemented five years earlier than the actual implementation year of each province. In columns (3)-(4), alternative pro-environmental attitudes and pro-environmental behaviors obtained from factor analysis are used as the dependent variables. In columns (5)-(6), the sample is restricted to individuals born less than 16 year earlier than the first CSL-affected cohort. All regressions have the same controls as in column (2) of Table 3, but their coefficients are suppressed for brevity. Robust standard errors are clustered at the cohort-city level and are reported in parentheses. ***, ** and * indicate 1%, 5% and 10% significance levels, respectively.

4.3.2. Alternative pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors

We further verify whether our benchmark results are robust to alternative measure of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors constructed by factor analysis. Factor analysis takes into account the correlations between answers to different attitudes/behaviors questions and the random error in measurement [46]. Specifically, we obtain an index of pro-environmental attitudes by performing a factor analysis on the three attitudes questions and an index of pro-environmental behaviors by performing a factor analysis on the six behaviors questions. The two indexes serve as alternative measures of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors.

We run 2SLS regressions with the alternative measure of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors as the dependent variable and present the results in columns (3) and (4) of Table 6. Obviously, in both regressions, the estimates of education are positive and highly significant, suggesting that the positive impacts of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors are robust to alternative measure of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors.

4.3.3. Restricted sample

Given that our sample covers a long span of birth cohorts, we check the robustness of our results by considering a sample with shorter birth cohort span. In particular, we restrict our sample to those born less than 16 years earlier than the first CSL-affected cohort. Columns (5) and (6) of Table 6 report the results in the restricted sample. It turns out that the effects of education remain significantly positive.

5. Mechanism analysis

After establishing the casual link between education and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, it would be interesting to explore the mechanism that drives this strong relationship. In particular, we focus on one important channel, namely environmental knowledge.

The willingness to alleviate environmental problem is a partial function of risk perception, which is reflected in environmental knowledge [5]. The acquisition of environmental knowledge makes individuals more informed about the situation of local and global environment, negative health effects of environmental problems, and public environmental policies. Indeed, previous literature has established that education facilitates a better understanding of complex environmental messages such as climate change [15,19,20]. In other words, the environmental knowledge may play a major role in forming people’s pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Hence, we postulate that the environmental knowledge may be a channel underlying the relationship between education and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. We test this mechanism in the following.

Environmental knowledge refers to people’s knowledge of environmental protection and climate literacy [15]. In the CGSS 2010, the respondents are asked ten true or false questions covering fundamental environmentally-related concepts to evaluate their environmental literacy. These questions are expressed as they would be present in daily life and media publicity. The exact wording of questions is provided in Table 7.

Table 7. Summary of environmental knowledge.

% of respondents
Correct Incorrect Do not know
Automobile exhaust does not pose a threat to human health. True or False? 81.2 12.4 6.4
Abuse of fertilizers and pesticides damages the environment. True or False? 83.6 9.6 6.8
Use of phosphorus-containing washing powder does not cause water pollution. True or False? 62.1 12.9 25.0
Fluoride emissions from fluorinated refrigerators damages the ozone layer in the atmosphere. True or False? 51.7 9.7 38.6
Acid rain has nothing to do with burning coal. True or False? 44.3 10.8 44.8
Species depend on each other, and the disappearance of one species has a chain reaction. True or False? 52.4 5.8 41.7
In the domestic air quality reports, air of third-level quality is better than air of first-level quality. True or False? 26.2 11.0 62.7
A single species of forest is more susceptible to pests and diseases. True or False? 44.4 9.2 46.3
In the domestic water pollution reports, water of Ⅴ (5) quality is better than water of Ⅰ (1) quality. True or False? 16.4 7.9 75.7
The increase in carbon dioxide in the atmosphere is a contributor of global warming. True or False? 53.2 4.9 41.9

Note: This table reports the proportion of respondents providing correct, incorrect, and ‘‘do not know” answers to each of the ten environmental knowledge questions. Percentages may not sum up to 100 due to rounding.

Table 7 summaries the responses to the environmental knowledge questions. In particular, it reports the proportion of respondents providing correct, incorrect and “do not know” answers to each of the ten environmental literacy questions. Overall, we observe a lack of environmental literacy among Chinese people. Most respondents could provide correct answers to the questions of automobile exhaust (81.2%) and abuse of fertilizers (83.6%), while the proportions of correct answers to the questions of acid rain, single species, domestic water pollution reports and air quality reports fail to exceed 50%. The question of domestic water pollution reports turns out to be the most difficult question for respondents, as the percentage of correct answer to that question (16.4%) is the lowest among the ten questions. Then, we construct an environmental knowledge score by awarding one point for each correct answer. Thereby the resulting score, Environmental knowledge, lies between 0 and 10.

We relate this environmental knowledge score to educational attainment and report the results in Table 8. The coefficients on education are significantly positive, indicating that the level of education has a positive impact on environmental knowledge. This supports the notion that the education effects pass through environmental knowledge.

Table 8. The effects of education on environmental knowledge.

Environmental knowledge
OLS 2SLS
(1) (2)
Education 0.191*** 0.340***
(0.013) (0.065)
Controls Yes Yes
Observations 3,601 3,601
First-stage estimation
CSL 3.157***
(0.260)
F-statistics 147.00

Note: The table reports the estimates from regressions of education on environmental knowledge. Column (1) uses OLS model for estimation, while column (2) uses 2SLS model for estimation. All regressions have the same controls as in column (2) of Table 3, but their coefficients are suppressed for brevity. Robust standard errors are clustered at the cohort-city level and are reported in parentheses. ***, ** and * indicate 1%, 5% and 10% significance levels, respectively.

Furthermore, we investigate the extent to which this mediating variable contributes to the effects of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. To achieve this goal, we follow the literature [14,39] and perform a standard mediation analysis in which we assess how the coefficient of education changes when the mediating variable is added as controls to the regressions. Specifically, we compute the percent change in the magnitude for the coefficient of education when a particular mediator is added and interpret it as the proportion of effects of education that are explained by that mediator.

Table 9 reports the results of the mediation analysis. We keep the sample in which all the variables are consistently available to ensure that the changes in the estimated coefficients across regressions are not driven by changes in the sample. Consequently, we end up with 3,401 observations for pro-environmental attitudes and 3,601 observations for pro-environmental behaviors. We report the corresponding benchmark estimates without the mediator in the columns (1) and (3) for the purpose of comparison. Columns (2) and (4) show the results when the variable Environmental knowledge is included as an explanatory variable. The variable Environmental knowledge has significant and positive effects on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. The addition of the environmental knowledge score in column (2) causes the coefficient of education to decrease from 0.012 to 0.007, indicating that 41.6% of the effect of education is explained by environmental knowledge. Similarly, we add the environmental knowledge score in column (4), resulting in a decrease in the magnitude of the coefficient of education by 63.6%. Moreover, the estimated coefficients of education in columns (2) and (4) even become insignificant after controlling for the environmental knowledge. These results reveal that the mediator of environmental knowledge accounts for a large proportion of the effect of education. Hence, improved environmental knowledge is the important pathway through which education influences pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors.

Table 9. The part of education effects mediated by environmental knowledge.

Pro-environmental attitudes Pro-environmental behaviors
Base Base+ Environmental knowledge Base Base+ Environmental knowledge
2SLS 2SLS 2SLS 2SLS
(1) (2) (3) (4)
Part of effects can be explained 41.6% 63.6%
Education 0.012** 0.007 0.011** 0.004*
(0.004) (0.005) (0.004) (0.001)
Environmental 0.013*** 0.019***
knowledge (0.003) (0.003)
Controls Yes Yes Yes Yes
Observations 3,401 3,401 3,601 3,601
First-stage estimation
CSL 2.949*** 2.448*** 3.154*** 2.625***
(0.255) (0.257) (0.260) (0.261)
F-statistics 133.44 90.61 146.58 101.50

Note: The table reports the results of mediation analysis. 2SLS models are used for estimation. As the potential mediator, environmental knowledge is added to the regressions in column (2) and in column (4), respectively. For the purpose of comparison, the results of base model without environmental knowledge are reported in column (1) and in column (3). All regressions also control for the covariates in the basic model as in column (2) of Table 3. The coefficients on the control variables are suppressed for brevity. Robust standard errors are clustered at the cohort-city level and are reported in parentheses. ***, ** and * indicate 1%, 5% and 10% significance levels, respectively.

6. Conclusions

In this paper, we investigate whether educational attainment promotes pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors in China, using microdata from CGSS 2010. We first construct an attitude score and a behavior score using the responses to three questions regarding the willingness of their pro-environmental attitudes and six questions about the frequency of their pro-environmental behaviors, respectively. We establish causality by exploiting the exogenous variation induced by the implementation of the Compulsory Schooling Law (CSL) in China. We find evidence that educational attainment is associated with higher levels of both pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. The estimates from 2SLS regressions reveal that an additional year of schooling results in an increase of 0.012 and 0.011 in the pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, which are approximately 2.1% and 2% of the corresponding sample means. Moreover, we show that the positive effects of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors are robust to various robustness checks and are heterogeneous across individuals. Furthermore, we focus on environmental knowledge as the major pathway underlying the relationship between education and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. We find that the acquisition of environmental knowledge is the channel that drives the effect of education on pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors.

Our study offers crucial policy implications as well. Chinese government has been seeking to set the country on a path to more sustainable development, focusing on the effectiveness administrative and regulatory instruments [30]. Our study provides an alternative perspective. Specifically, increasing access to general education could be an essential and effective measure to motivate pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors among a broad population. In particular, the effect of general education in promoting environmental knowledge is crucial for developing countries such as China, where many people have low levels of environmental knowledge.

Our study might have some limitations. First, our study is based on survey responses, which can suffer from measurement error problems. For example, well-educated people may be more likely to over-claim their pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors for keeping a good image. If this is the case, the effect of education on actual pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors might be overstated. In addition, partly due to data availability, we focus on the knowledge acquisition channel. There might exist other (coexisting) channels underlying the relationship between education and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. For example, given the strong relationship between educational attainment and labor market outcomes, people with a higher educational attainment might be more optimistic about their future income, and thus have less concern about insufficient material needs when following a pro-environmental lifestyle. Moreover, as in general the exclusion restrictions in IV models are not testable [47], other research methods (e.g., lab and field experiments) can be useful to validate our findings. Furthermore, our study focuses on the effect of schooling education but is salient on the effect of other types of education. We leave these issues to future research.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Year of implementation of compulsory schooling law across provinces.

(DOCX)

S2 Appendix. Balancing test.

(DOCX)

Data Availability

Data cannot be shared publicly because it is owned by a third party and authors do not have permission to share the data. Data are available from the Chinese General Social Survey (CGSS) database (contact via http://cgss.ruc.edu.cn/English/Home.htm) for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Interested researchers can replicate our study findings in their entirety by directly obtaining the data from the third party listed above and following the protocol. In addition, we confirm that the authors of this paper did not have any special access privileges that others would not have.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the 111 Project of China (No. B16040) and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 71904160). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Xu P, Zeng Y, Fong Q, Lone T, Liu Y. Chinese consumers’ willingness to pay for green-and eco-labeled seafood. Food control. 2012;28(1):74–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Zhang L, Wu Y. Market segmentation and willingness to pay for green electricity among urban residents in China: The case of Jiangsu Province. Energy Policy. 2012;51:514–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Yu X, Lei X, Wang M. Temperature effects on mortality and household adaptation: Evidence from China. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management. 2019;96:195–212. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Chen S, Guo C, Huang X. Air pollution, student health, and school absences: Evidence from China. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management. 2018;92:465–97. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Tu M, Zhang B, Xu J, Lu F. Mass media, information and demand for environmental quality: Evidence from the “Under the Dome”. Journal of Development Economics. 2020;143:102402. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Vlek C, Steg L. Human Behavior and Environmental Sustainability: Problems, Driving Forces, and Research Topics. Blackwell Publishing Inc; Malden, USA; 2007. p. 1–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Steg L, Bolderdijk JW, Keizer K, Perlaviciute G. An integrated framework for encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: The role of values, situational factors and goals. Journal of Environmental psychology. 2014;38:104–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wang C, Wu J, Zhang B. Environmental regulation, emissions and productivity: Evidence from Chinese COD-emitting manufacturers. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management. 2018;92:54–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ponce P, Alvarado R, Ponce K, Alvarado R, Granda D, Yaguana K. Green returns of labor income and human capital: Empirical evidence of the environmental behavior of households in developing countries. Ecological economics. 2019;160:105–13. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dee TS. Are there civic returns to education? Journal of public economics. 2004;88(9–10):1697–720. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Albouy V, Lequien L. Does compulsory education lower mortality? Journal of health economics. 2009;28(1):155–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2008.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Banks J, Mazzonna F. The effect of education on old age cognitive abilities: evidence from a regression discontinuity design. The Economic Journal. 2012;122(560):418–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2012.02499.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Clark D, Royer H. The effect of education on adult mortality and health: Evidence from Britain. American Economic Review. 2013;103(6):2087–120. doi: 10.1257/aer.103.6.2087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Black SE, Devereux PJ, Lundborg P, Majlesi K. Learning to take risks? The effect of education on risk-taking in financial markets. Review of Finance. 2018;22(3):951–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Chankrajang T, Muttarak R. Green returns to education: Does schooling contribute to pro-environmental behaviours? Evidence from Thailand. Ecological Economics. 2017;131:434–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wen LM, Rissel C, Baur LA, Lee E, Simpson JM. Who is NOT likely to access the Internet for health information? Findings from first-time mothers in southwest Sydney, Australia. International Journal of Medical Informatics. 2011;80(6):406–11. doi: 10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2011.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Liere KDV, Dunlap RE. The social bases of environmental concern: A review of hypotheses, explanations and empirical evidence. Public opinion quarterly. 1980;44(2):181–97. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lu S, Au W-T, Jiang F, Xie X, Yam P. Cooperativeness and competitiveness as two distinct constructs: Validating the Cooperative and Competitive Personality Scale in a social dilemma context. International Journal of Psychology. 2013;48(6):1135–47. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2012.743666 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bybee RW. Scientific literacy, environmental issues, and PISA 2006: The 2008 Paul F-Brandwein lecture. Journal of Science Education and Technology. 2008;17(6):566–85. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.McCright AM. The effects of gender on climate change knowledge and concern in the American public. Population and Environment. 2010;32(1):66–87. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Amore MD, Bennedsen M, Larsen B, Rosenbaum P. CEO education and corporate environmental footprint. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management. 2019;94:254–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zhang P, Deschenes O, Meng K, Zhang J. Temperature effects on productivity and factor reallocation: Evidence from a half million Chinese manufacturing plants. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management. 2018;88:1–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Chen X, Yang L. Temperature and industrial output: Firm-level evidence from China. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management. 2019;95:257–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ngobo PV. What drives household choice of organic products in grocery stores? Journal of retailing. 2011;87(1):90–100. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mannberg A, Jansson J, Pettersson T, Brännlund R, Lindgren U. Do tax incentives affect households׳ adoption of ‘green’cars? A panel study of the Stockholm congestion tax. Energy Policy. 2014;74:286–99. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Meyer A. Does education increase pro-environmental behavior? Evidence from Europe. Ecological economics. 2015;116:108–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Zheng S, Kahn ME. Understanding China’s urban pollution dynamics. Journal of Economic Literature. 2013;51(3):731–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lutz W, Muttarak R, Striessnig E. Universal education is key to enhanced climate adaptation. Science. 2014;346(6213):1061–2. doi: 10.1126/science.1257975 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Card D. Estimating the return to schooling: Progress on some persistent econometric problems. Econometrica. 2001;69(5):1127–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ma G, Andrews-Speed P, Zhang J. Chinese consumer attitudes towards energy saving: The case of household electrical appliances in Chongqing. Energy Policy. 2013;56:591–602. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Oreopoulos P, Salvanes KG. Priceless: The nonpecuniary benefits of schooling. Journal of Economic perspectives. 2011;25(1):159–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Moretti E. Human capital externalities in cities. Handbook of regional and urban economics. 4: Elsevier; 2004. p. 2243–91. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.McMahon WW. The social and external benefits of education. International handbook on the economics of education. 2004;211:259. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lange F, Topel R. The social value of education and human capital. Handbook of the Economics of Education. 2006;1:459–509. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Maslow AH. New introduction: Religions, values, and peak-experiences. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology. 1970;2(2):83–90. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Inglehart R. Public support for environmental protection: Objective problems and subjective values in 43 societies. PS: Political Science & Politics. 1995;28(1):57–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Xu C, Yang J, He L, Wei W, Yang Y, Yin X, et al. Carbon capture and storage as a strategic reserve against China’s CO2 emissions. Environmental Development. 2021;37:100608. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Xie S, Mo T. The impact of education on health in China. China Economic Review. 2014;29:1–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Huang W. Understanding the effects of education on health: evidence from China. 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cui Y, Liu H, Zhao L. Mother’s education and child development: Evidence from the compulsory school reform in China. Journal of Comparative Economics. 2019;47(3):669–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Fang H, Eggleston KN, Rizzo JA, Rozelle S, Zeckhauser RJ. The returns to education in China: Evidence from the 1986 compulsory education law. National Bureau of Economic Research, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Liang Y, Dong Z. Has education led to secularization? Based on the study of compulsory education law in China. China Economic Review. 2019;54:324–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Staiger DO, Stock JH. Instrumental variables regression with weak instruments. National Bureau of Economic Research Cambridge, Mass., USA; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Griliches Z. Estimating the returns to schooling: Some econometric problems. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society. 1977:1–22.12278790 [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kotchen MJ, Boyle KJ, Leiserowitz AA. Willingness-to-pay and policy-instrument choice for climate-change policy in the United States. Energy Policy. 2013;55:617–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Rummel RJ. Applied factor analysis: Northwestern University Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Angrist JD, Krueger AB. Instrumental variables and the search for identification: From supply and demand to natural experiments. Journal of Economic perspectives. 2001;15(4):69–85. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Shihe Fu

12 Jul 2021

PONE-D-21-14017

Green Returns to Education: Does Education Affect Pro-Environmental Attitudes and Behaviors in China?

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Gan,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Both qualified reviewers have provided constructive comments for a major revision. Please try to address their concerns as much as you can. I also have two additional suggestions. (1) If the data contain city code, you could include city fixed effects instead of province fixed effects as environmental policies and qualities are very city-specific. (2) In table 9, columns 2 and 4, you should report the coefficient of environmental knowledge variable.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Aug 26 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Shihe Fu, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

3. PLOS requires an ORCID iD for the corresponding author in Editorial Manager on papers submitted after December 6th, 2016. Please ensure that you have an ORCID iD and that it is validated in Editorial Manager. To do this, go to ‘Update my Information’ (in the upper left-hand corner of the main menu), and click on the Fetch/Validate link next to the ORCID field. This will take you to the ORCID site and allow you to create a new iD or authenticate a pre-existing iD in Editorial Manager. Please see the following video for instructions on linking an ORCID iD to your Editorial Manager account: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_xcclfuvtxQ

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This paper aims to estimate the green return to education in China. Using individual data from CGSS 2010, the authors link years of schooling to pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. To alleviate the endogeneity issue, the authors instrument education attainment with personal exposure to the implementation of the Compulsory Schooling Law in China. Based on a 2SLS model, they find that educational attainment has a moderately positive impact on individual pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. They further provide evidence that the acquisition of environmental knowledge is one channel that explains the education effect.

Below, I outline my suggestions and comments, which hopefully will improve this paper.

1. The sample selection problem. The authors use the 2010 round of CGSS data originally containing 11,783 respondents residing in 134 cities across 31 of China’s provinces. However, due to the missing values of some variables (what variables? Demographic factors or pro-environmental attitude/behaviors?), the authors drop roughly 70% of the observations without show the geographic distribution of the remaining sample, raising the concern of sample representativeness. Besides, the missing rate of outcome variables may be related to respondents’ education level. For example, surveyed individuals with lower education levels may be less likely to answer questions related to pro-environmental attitudes/behaviors. If this is the case, then the analysis is derived from a selected sample and subject to external validity issues. I would suggest the authors conduct balancing checks based on all observational variables between the regression sample and the sample the author dropped. By doing this, the reader could better understand the representativeness of the regression sample.

2. The potential measurement error in pro-environmental attitude/behaviors. Well-educated people may be more likely to over-claim their pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors for keeping a good image. Then the effect of education may be overstated. The authors may include this point in the discussion/caveat section. Also, in section 4.3.2, the authors use an alternative measure of pro-environmental attitudes/behaviors to conduct a robustness check. However, without a detailed elaboration of how to construct these new measurements, the readers are hardly convinced by this piece of evidence.

3. Specification. The current sample contains a long span of birth cohorts. Apart from the province fixed effects, I would suggest the authors include the cohort fixed effects, which captures the time-varying factors related to the pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. For perspective, people from different cohorts may have different reporting styles, either over-claim or under-claim. They may have different preferences for cleaner air at the cost of income. They also have different techniques for learning new knowledge. Given a substantial difference among various cohorts, I feel the necessity of including the cohort fixed effects in the OLS as well as the 2SLS regressions.

4. Exclusion restrictions. The authors instrument years of schooling with personal exposure to the implementation of the Compulsory Schooling Law in China (CSL, hereafter). This IV passes the weak instrument test. However, it may not pass the requirement of exclusion restrictions. For example, given a strong relationship between educational attainment and labor outcome, treated people may have a higher expectation of their future income. People who are optimistic about their future without fear of insufficient material needs may pay more attention to environmental protection and pursuit higher quality of life. If this is the case, then CSL may affect people’s pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors through multiple channels, failing to address the endogeneity problem.

5. Heterogeneity. The authors split the sample into top quintile and bottom quintile of the income groups and check the heterogeneity by income. I am wondering whether the observations from the high-income group are mainly coming from urban areas, while those in the low-income group are from rural areas. If that is the case, then the results presented in columns (5)-(6) and (11)-(12) are explained by urban-rural status rather than income level. I would suggest the authors check the heterogeneity by income conditioning on gender or urban-rural status, and these results may provide more insight into the influence of income.

6. Typo: It should be Table 5 rather than Table 6 on page 19 paragraph 2.

Reviewer #2: This paper examines how educational attainment affects pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors in China. It uses cross-section data from CGSS 2010 and exploits the exogenous variation of implementing Compulsory Schooling Law (CSL) in China to identify a causal relationship. The major issue is endogeneity of educational attainment. Its identification strategy is to instrument education attainment by a variable measuring one’s exposure to CSL. The identifying assumption is that the implementation of CSL is exogenously determined and would not directly affect pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors at adulthood. The estimates show that an additional year of schooling results in an increase of 0.084 in the pro-environmental attitudes and 0.219 in the pro-environmental behaviors.

The manuscript is well-polished, but I have a number of comments which may improve this paper.

1. Contribution and the introduction part

There is abundant literature documenting the relationship between education attainment and social attitudes or behaviors related to environmental protection. The marginal contribution of this article is to deal with the endogeneity of education in this context. How can we compare the findings of this paper with previous literature? Are the estimates of education attainment different with studies which fail to deal with endogeneity problem or use data from other countries? It is important to understand how different or unique the findings are when we focus on China. A more comprehensive literature review could be helpful.

Moreover, is there any theory to predict the relationship between education attainment and pro-social behaviors? Can education attainment negatively affect pro-environmental behaviors or they have a non-linear relationship? The authors could refer to a broader scope of literature and motivate the research question more carefully.

2. Birth cohort fixed effects

The age range of respondents in the sample is large. Respondents from different cohorts vary greatly in their social attitudes. By including the age variable may not be enough to control unobserved heterogeneity across birth cohorts. As individuals exposed to CSL are from younger sample, I’m a bit concerned that the major finding that more education attainment promotes pro-environmental attitudes is actually driven by birth cohort variations. I suggest that the authors control cohort fixed effects in the regression, for example, constructing dummies for every 10-year age interval.

3. Interpretation of OLS and IV estimates

According to Table 3 and Table 4, we know that if we ignore the endogeneity problem of education OLS will overestimate the effect on pro-environmental attitudes but underestimate that on pro-environmental behaviors. How can we interpret this? Please carefully discuss how OLS estimates and IV estimates are different and why.

4. Alternative mechanism of the findings

To interpret the major findings, the authors test the environmental knowledge mechanism among others. The mediation analysis shows that more years of education improves environmental knowledge, hence promoting pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. But an alternative mechanism could be that individuals with more education attainment have higher incomes, so the marginal utility of pro-social behaviors relative to marginal utility of money is larger for them than that for the poor. As a matter of fact, the authors already did a heterogeneity analysis by splitting the high-income and the low-income. From Table 5 we can know that the positive effect of education attainment is mainly driven by the low-income group. This implies that it is environmental knowledge which plays a role, not marginal utility of alleviating environmental problems. The authors could discuss the mechanisms in more detail here.

5. Scale of measures

It is not very clear why and how the authors construct the attitudes questions into a variable ranging from 1 to 15 and the behaviors questions into a variable ranging from 1 to 18. Why not construct two variables of the same range, or just use the original survey questions as dependent variables? Are the findings robust against methods of constructing dependent variables?

In addition, I suggest that maximum and minimum of all variables could also be reported in Table 2.

6. Missing sample

The sample used in this analysis is 3,661 observations, around 30% of the full CGSS 2010 sample. Are the respondents randomly chosen to answer the pro-environmental questions? More explanation how the sample is constructed could be included.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Referee report_05.pdf

PLoS One. 2022 Feb 3;17(2):e0263383. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0263383.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


13 Oct 2021

We have uploaded our response to specific reviewer and editor comments as a separate file labeled "Response to Reviewers". Please find this attached file.

Attachment

Submitted filename: response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Shihe Fu

26 Nov 2021

PONE-D-21-14017R1Green Returns to Education: Does Education Affect Pro-Environmental Attitudes and Behaviors in China?PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Gan,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Both the reviewers still raised some minor concerns. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 10 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Shihe Fu, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: The current manuscript addresses my comments well by providing a more comprehensive literature review, including richer background information and conducting more rigorous regression analysis. However, there are several comments that the authors can take into further account before making it to a publishable version.

1. page 8, line 6: “…but also because China’s unique culture and institution help validate...”

page 11, line 18: “In addition, China has its own unique culture and ideology, …”

It is not very clear which part of China’s unique cultural or institutional features would help validate the relationship between education and environmental attitudes. The authors should avoid too general arguments without further elaboration in a scientific research paper. Instead, the authors could briefly discuss China’s environmental problems as a consequence of fast industrial growth as part of motivation. That is a unique feature of the Chinese context.

2. page 11, line 5: “…in particular, China, schooling education is examination-oriented and emphasizes competition… are therefore less caring for the environment”

page 23, line 8: “people that are obsessed with personal achievement might care less for the environment.”

There is no solid evidence to support the negative association between education and pro-environmental attitudes. The authors should elaborate very carefully how willingness to compete conflicts with pro-environmental attitudes, and why this is unique in China. As a matter of fact, I think there is no need to raise this point. The downward bias of OLS estimates might be driven by measurement error. I would prefer not to mention this channel.

3. Page 14: why are only those born in February, September, November or December asked about environmental attitudes and behaviors in CGSS? It is difficult to understand.

4. typos

page 7, line 6: “While there are large number of papers…” should be “there are a large number of …”

page 10, line 13: “… skills accumulated through education make individuals be familiar with…”, delete “be”

page 10, line 15: “… people with higher level of formal education…”, should be “with a higher level”

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: PLOS report.pdf

PLoS One. 2022 Feb 3;17(2):e0263383. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0263383.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


3 Dec 2021

We have revised the manuscript to carefully address the editor's and reviewers’ insightful comments. Please see our revised manuscript and responses to editor's and reviewers’ comments for details.

Attachment

Submitted filename: response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 2

Shihe Fu

19 Jan 2022

Green Returns to Education: Does Education Affect Pro-Environmental Attitudes and Behaviors in China?

PONE-D-21-14017R2

Dear Dr. Gan,

I see you have addressed the referees' comments well in this second-round revision and there is no need to send out for review again. We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Shihe Fu, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Shihe Fu

26 Jan 2022

PONE-D-21-14017R2

Green Returns to Education: Does Education Affect Pro-Environmental Attitudes and Behaviors in China?

Dear Dr. Gan:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Shihe Fu

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Appendix. Year of implementation of compulsory schooling law across provinces.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Appendix. Balancing test.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Referee report_05.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: response to reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PLOS report.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: response to reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    Data cannot be shared publicly because it is owned by a third party and authors do not have permission to share the data. Data are available from the Chinese General Social Survey (CGSS) database (contact via http://cgss.ruc.edu.cn/English/Home.htm) for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Interested researchers can replicate our study findings in their entirety by directly obtaining the data from the third party listed above and following the protocol. In addition, we confirm that the authors of this paper did not have any special access privileges that others would not have.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES