Abstract
Older children with online schooling requirements, unsurprisingly, were reported to have increased screen time during the first COVID-19 lockdown in many countries. Here, we ask whether younger children with no similar online schooling requirements also had increased screen time during lockdown. We examined children’s screen time during the first COVID-19 lockdown in a large cohort (n = 2209) of 8-to-36-month-olds sampled from 15 labs across 12 countries. Caregivers reported that toddlers with no online schooling requirements were exposed to more screen time during lockdown than before lockdown. While this was exacerbated for countries with longer lockdowns, there was no evidence that the increase in screen time during lockdown was associated with socio-demographic variables, such as child age and socio-economic status (SES). However, screen time during lockdown was negatively associated with SES and positively associated with child age, caregiver screen time, and attitudes towards children’s screen time. The results highlight the impact of the COVID-19 lockdown on young children’s screen time.
Subject terms: Psychology, Human behaviour
In March 2020, the COVID-19 outbreak led to lockdowns and social distancing measures around the globe: Social interactions were restricted and many parents had to work from home while taking care of their children, because childcare facilities and schools were (partially) closed. The move to online schooling led school-aged children to spend many hours a day in front of screens, interacting with their teachers and classmates. Unsurprisingly then, older children (3- to 17-year-olds) in many countries (Australia, China, France, Germany, Italy, Netherlands, South Korea, Spain, UK, USA; see Table 1) spent extended periods engaged with digital media during lockdown. What about the youngest members of our society, toddlers and pre-schoolers, who did not attend online schooling? What was their experience with screen time during this lockdown period? And what were the factors that shaped their screen time exposure during lockdown? Were they exposed to increased periods of screen time during lockdown as their caregivers battled to keep them occupied while working from home, and was there an association between potential increases in screen time during lockdown and children’s language development? The current study addresses these questions by examining screen time during lockdown in 8–36 month-olds using data from 15 labs across 12 countries (Canada, France, Germany, Israel, Norway, Poland, Russia, Saudi Arabia, Switzerland, Turkey, UK, USA).
Table 1.
Country | Age | Screen time effect |
---|---|---|
Canada | 5- to 11-years | 95% of children not meeting guidelines for physical activity due to sedentary behaviour including screen time1 |
China | 6- to 17-years | 30 h more screen time per week2 |
France | 6- to 10-years | 62% of children had increased screen time3 |
Germany | 4- to 17-years | One hour more screen time per day4 |
Italy | 6- to 18-years | Almost 5 h more screen time per day in children with obesity5 |
Netherlands | 6- to 14-years | Self-reported screen time increased by 59–62 min per day6 |
South Korea | age not reported | 81% of children had increased screen time7 |
Spain | 8- to 16-years | 2 h more screen time per day8 |
USA | < 18 years | ‘Dramatic’ increase in screen time9 |
Multi-country | 3- to 7-years | 50 min more screen time per day10 |
Factors associated with screen time in early childhood
Children start using digital media devices early in life11–13, with US data suggesting a 32% increase in children’s screen time over the last two decades14. Children’s screen time also increases with age11,13,15–18 with two-year-olds in the US having less than an hour of daily screen time and two- to four-year-olds having on average 2.5 h of daily screen time13. However, there appear to be differences in children’s screen time across countries19–21. For example, four to six-year-olds from medium–high SES families in Germany and Spain were reported to have 20–30 min of daily TV time, whereas this estimate was between 30–90 min in Greece, and 60–240 min in Belgium, Poland, and Bulgaria20. Thus far, most studies have focused on children from English-speaking countries22,23. For example, of the 29 studies concerning screen time in children aged ≤ three years reviewed by Duch and colleagues22, 25 were conducted in the US, while only four were conducted in Europe and East Asia. Furthermore, most of the studies reviewed in Duch and colleagues22 focused on TV viewing rather than mobile media use. Moreover, available survey estimates of children’s screen time tend to focus on older children: For example, EU Kids Online or the UK’s Office of Communications, which publishes annual survey results of children’s screen time, does not provide data for children under the age of three. Thus, there is a gap in the literature with regards to a more global perspective on toddlers’ screen time, especially given the widespread availability of mobile media devices.
Screen time is strongly embedded in the familial context, with older siblings often showing their younger siblings how to use media devices, helping them choose content, and co-using digital devices or co-viewing17,24, but see 22,25 for null results]. This sibling influence on screen time entails that toddlers with older siblings are often exposed to content that is targeted at older children26,27. Furthermore, there are differences in co-viewing and co-using patterns across early development, with some studies finding that the rate of parent–child co-use of mobile media is highest in children younger than two years and decreases dramatically as the child grows older13,18.
Exposure to screen time may be particularly exacerbated in children from lower socio-economic status (SES) families13,20,23,28–32. Reports suggest that 97% of children aged six months to four years from low-income backgrounds in the US used mobile devices, and that most children started using them before their first birthday30. Caregiver education is also strongly associated with screen time given findings that, among 0–8-year-olds, the average daily screen time varies from 3 h 12 min for children whose caregivers have a high school diploma (or less) to 2 h 24 min for children whose caregivers have some college experience, and 1 h 38 min for children whose caregivers have a college degree13. In line with this, a recent large-scale multinational study, on which the present study builds, also reported a negative association between maternal education and young children’s screen time during the first COVID-19 lockdown33.
Caregiver behaviours and attitudes also play an important role in young children’s screen time. Caregivers of children under the age of three who reported having longer screen time themselves reported allowing their children more time with smart devices17. Caregivers with stronger beliefs about the educational benefits of screen time were more likely to co-use mobile media with their child than to have the child use mobile media alone34. One- to four-year-olds were also more likely to have access to their caregiver’s smartphone if caregivers believed that it was important for their child to familiarize themselves with technology35.
Taken together, the studies reviewed above suggest that children have access to screens from the first year of life and that screen time is linked to socio-demographic factors such as child age, socioeconomic status of the family, the number of siblings, as well as factors such as caregiver’s screen time and attitudes towards screen time, although these effects appear to vary cross-nationally.
Children’s screen time during COVID-19 lockdown
Children’s screen time also appears to be modulated by disruptive external influences. For example, several studies reported more screen time among hospitalized children36,37. More recently, in addition to pre-existing differences in children’s physical activity and screen time across countries38, the COVID-19 pandemic and the associated lockdown measures have triggered an increase in screen time in many regions. In Spain, two out of three children under 48 months used smartphones and tablets daily during COVID-19 lockdown16. Table 1 presents the key findings of studies examining lockdown-related increases in screen time among older children across several countries. Taken together, these studies suggest a widespread, immediate, and potentially adverse impact on children’s screen time as a consequence of the COVID-19 lockdown measures in older children between 3 to 18 years of age. But, to our knowledge, no single study has examined the effects of the COVID-19 lockdown and associated measures (i.e., lockdown severity and duration) on younger children’s screen time across countries.
Lockdown severity across countries
The duration and implementation of lockdowns and social distancing measures introduced to contain the pandemic differed widely across countries, resulting in various degrees of lockdown severity. To account for lockdown severity, the Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker (OxCGRT) project suggested a Stringency Index, based on the following factors: school and childcare closures, workplace closures, cancellation of public events, social contact restrictions, public transport closures, and movement restrictions39. The 12 countries participating in this study imposed various degrees of lockdowns for different periods of time (see Table 1, Supplementary Information). Most countries started implementing lockdown measures in March 2020. The two countries that implemented the strictest measures according to the COVID-19 Government Stringency Index were Saudi Arabia and Israel. Although some countries allowed outdoor activities for children whilst maintaining social distancing practices (e.g., Norway and the Netherlands), others did not allow children under the age of 15 to leave the house, even after lockdown measures were eased later in 2020 (e.g., Turkey and Saudi Arabia). Against the background of studies suggesting disruptive environmental influences on children’s screen time36,37, differences in lockdown measures are likely associated with children’s screen time, for example, because they limit the activities children can engage in.
Children’s screen time and language development
Health agency reports suggest that excessive screen time in the early years can be detrimental to early development, e.g.,40–45. However, such reports also stress that there is limited work examining the impact of screen time in young children and highlight children’s learning from digital media when caregiver support and scaffolding is provided.
The results of the few studies examining the impact of frequency and duration of screen time on children’s development are mixed. On the one hand, population-based studies suggested a negative association between excessive screen time in early childhood and children’s language development27, especially with regards to children’s expressive (but not receptive) vocabulary46 (but see33 for similar results), as well as a negative association between screen time and children’s receptive vocabulary47. On the other hand, a recent meta-analysis found that while increased screen time was associated with lower language skills, quality screen time (educational programs) and caregiver scaffolding during screen time was associated with stronger language skills in children under twelve years of age48. Thus, there is a need for further examination of the association between children’s screen time and language development during lockdown.
The current study
The current study addresses the following three limitations of previous research on young children’s screen time. First, the literature currently lacks a systematic investigation of the factors associated with screen time in children under the age of three. Second, to our knowledge, there is no study of young children’s screen time during lockdown, especially with regards to potential increases in screen time during lockdown relative to pre-lockdown. Third, the literature lacks a global record of young children’s screen time. Thus, more diverse data collected through similar sampling methods are necessary to begin to understand cross-national differences in young children’s screen time. Such comparative research will shed new light on the factors that influence screen time both globally and nationally, and will allow informed recommendations for young children’s screen time. We address this gap by examining screen time during lockdown in a large international sample of 8-to-36-month-olds.
In particular, this study has the following aims: To examine (i) the factors associated with young children’s screen time during the first COVID-19 lockdown, (ii) whether there was an increase in young children’s screen time during lockdown relative to before the first COVID-19 lockdown and (iii) the association between potential increases in screen time during lockdown and vocabulary development.
Predictions
We predicted that caregiver’s screen time, caregiver’s beliefs about the positive impact of screen time, maternal education level (as a proxy of SES), children’s age, and the severity of the lockdown in the country would be positively associated with children’s screen time. We also predicted an increase in children’s screen time during lockdown, compared to before lockdown. Finally, we predicted that an increase in children’s screen time during lockdown would be negatively associated with vocabulary gains during lockdown.
Results
Factors associated with young children’s screen time during lockdown
First, we examined young children’s screen time during lockdown using the COVID-language dataset (33, https://osf.io/ty9mn/, (see Model 1 specification and additional model parameters in Supplementary Information (Table 2), n = 1292). The full model was compared to a null model excluding all fixed effects predictors except SES as preregistered.
The full-null model comparison was significant, χ2 = 41.51, df = 6, p < 0.001, suggesting that at least one (or a combination of one or more) predictors lacking in the null model improved model fit. Table 2 shows the resulting parameter estimates with p-values examining the individual contribution of each of the predictors entered into the model. Note that this model does not include the interaction between lockdown.severity and lockdown.duration (which was included in the original model) due to this interaction not being significant, χ2 = 2.83, df = 1, p = 0.093 (see Table 3, Supplementary Information). We, therefore, report here the results of a reduced model excluding this interaction to examine potential main effects of lockdown.severity and lockdown.duration.
Table 2.
Term | Estimate | SE | Lower CI | Upper CI | LRT | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
0|3 | − 0.896 | 0.279 | − 1.416 | − 0.352 | |||
3|4 | − 0.416 | 0.278 | − 0.934 | 0.126 | |||
4|5 | 0.530 | 0.278 | 0.017 | 1.084 | |||
5|6 | 1.854 | 0.283 | 1.335 | 2.456 | |||
6|7 | 3.343 | 0.300 | 2.832 | 3.970 | |||
7|8 | 4.634 | 0.342 | 4.046 | 5.408 | |||
8|9 | 6.219 | 0.498 | 5.468 | 7.514 | |||
Lockdown.severity | − 0.230 | 0.248 | − 0.754 | 0.247 | 0.833 | 1 | 0.361 |
Lockdown.duration | 0.029 | 0.084 | − 0.137 | 0.215 | 0.117 | 1 | 0.732 |
Caregiver.screentime | 0.270 | 0.062 | 0.154 | 0.380 | 10.560 | 1 | 0.001 |
Siblings | − 0.080 | 0.059 | − 0.207 | 0.043 | 1.421 | 1 | 0.233 |
Age | 1.008 | 0.096 | 0.839 | 1.219 | 25.449 | 1 | 0.000 |
SES | − 0.424 | 0.095 | − 0.615 | − 0.237 | 10.689 | 1 | 0.001 |
Bold formatting highlights significant predictors in the models presented.
The model summary presented in Table 2 suggests a positive association between caregiver screen time and children’s screen time, with caregivers who reported having more screen time themselves also reporting their children having more screen time. There was also a positive association between age of the child and screen time, with older children having more screen time than younger children (see Fig. 1). Finally, there was a negative association between SES and screen time, with caregivers from lower SES families reporting their children having more screen time (see Fig. 1). We found no evidence for associations between screen time and lockdown.severity, lockdown.duration or the number of siblings a child had.
Next, we fitted an additional model (see Model 2 specification and additional model parameters in Table 4 in Supplementary Information, n = 951) including only participants from the COVID-screen dataset who provided information on caregivers’ affective response to children’s screen time (caregiver.affect) based on their responses to statements about potential positive or negative side effects of their children’s screen time. We did not include SES or siblings as a predictor in this model (due to data loss). However, a separate model including SES and siblings as predictors revealed very similar results to those reported here (see Table 6 in Supplementary Information, Model 2.SES, n = 622). The full model was compared to a null model excluding all predictors as preregistered. The full-null model comparison was significant, χ2 = 26.31, df = 6, p < 0.001. Table 3 shows the resulting parameter estimates together with p-values. Note that this model does not include the interaction between lockdown.severity and lockdown.duration due to this interaction not being significant, χ2 = 0.079, df = 1, p = 0.375, see Table 5 in Supplementary Information). We, therefore, report here the results of a reduced model excluding this interaction to examine potential main effects of lockdown.severity and lockdown.duration.
Table 3.
Term | Estimate | SE | Lower CI | Upper CI | LRT | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
0|1 | − 0.632 | 0.363 | − 1.346 | 0.034 | |||
1|2 | 0.162 | 0.362 | − 0.546 | 0.861 | |||
2|3 | 1.367 | 0.365 | 0.652 | 2.085 | |||
3|4 | 2.405 | 0.371 | 1.703 | 3.125 | |||
4|5 | 3.216 | 0.380 | 2.504 | 3.964 | |||
5|6 | 4.104 | 0.401 | 3.372 | 4.990 | |||
Lockdown.severity | 0.304 | 0.372 | − 0.464 | 1.059 | 0.617 | 1 | 0.432 |
Lockdown.duration | 0.005 | 0.135 | − 0.346 | 0.361 | 0.001 | 1 | 0.971 |
Caregiver.screentime | 0.268 | 0.079 | 0.078 | 0.445 | 6.150 | 1 | 0.013 |
Age | 0.608 | 0.104 | 0.419 | 0.806 | 9.229 | 1 | 0.002 |
caregiver.affect | 0.665 | 0.119 | 0.437 | 0.905 | 8.477 | 1 | 0.004 |
Bold formatting highlights significant predictors in the models presented.
The model summary presented in Table 3 suggests a positive association between caregiver screen time and children’s screen time (see Fig. 2), as well as age of the child and screen time. As in the previous models, we found no obvious associations between lockdown.severity, lockdown.duration and siblings on children’s screen time – while the model including SES (see Tables 6 and 7 in Supplementary Information, Model 2.SES) replicated the negative association between SES and children’s screen time. Importantly, we found a positive association between caregiver’s affective response to screen time and children’s screen time (caregiver.affect), with caregivers who reported being more positively inclined towards children’s screen time also reporting their child having more screen time (see Fig. 2).
Differences in young children’s screen time prior to and during lockdown
We fitted an additional model including only participants from the COVID-screen dataset who provided information on how much screen time their children had access to prior to and the lockdown as well as during the lockdown (see Model 3 specification and additional model parameters in Table 8 in Supplementary Information, n = 953). The full model was compared to a null model excluding all fixed effects predictors except caregiver.affect as preregistered.
The full-null model comparison was significant, χ2 = 32.95, df = 8, p < 0.001. Tests of individual effects suggested a significant interaction between lockdown.stage and lockdown.duration, χ2 = 4.59, df = 1, p = 0.032. There were no interactions between lockdown.stage and any of the other predictor variables. Model 3 in Supplementary Information (Table 8) shows the resulting parameter estimates together with p-values.
As Fig. 3 suggests, children had more access to screen time during lockdown relative to before the lockdown. Figure 4 depicts the data and the fitted model to illustrate the interaction between lockdown.stage (T1, top panel; T2, bottom panel) with lockdown.duration and shows that there is a small, but in our models, significant interaction effect (see Table 3) such that longer lockdown durations were associated with a greater increase in screen time. There was no evidence that increases in screen time during lockdown were associated with the other predictors included. We note that as the model is implemented on a reduced dataset, including SES and siblings as predictors failed to converge.
Lockdown-related surge in screen time and vocabulary development
Finally, we examined whether lockdown-related increases in children’s screen time impacted children’s vocabulary development, such that those children who were reported to have had more screen time during lockdown were also reported to show smaller gains in vocabulary development during lockdown, separately for children’s receptive and expressive percentile scores (see Model 4a/b specification and additional model parameters in Table 9 and 11 in Supplementary Information, n = 117 and 156 respectively). The full models were compared to null models excluding all fixed effects predictors except SES as preregistered.
The full-null model comparison for the receptive model was not significant, χ2 = 13.02, df = 7, p = 0.072, while the full-null model comparison for the expressive model was significant, χ2 = 14.92, df = 7, p = 0.037. We further fitted reduced models including only the interaction between lockdown.stage and screen.time given that this interaction was near-significant in the full model for expressive vocabulary, χ2 = 3.52, df = 1, p = 0.060 (see Table 12 in Supplementary Information). In this reduced model, the interaction between lockdown.stage and screen.time was significant for expressive vocabulary, χ2 = 4.63, df = 1, p = 0.031. While the results with regards to the significant interaction between lockdown.stage*screen.time should be treated with caution due to the marginally non-significant interaction in the full model, they suggest that those children who had smaller increases in screen time during lockdown relative to prior to lockdown were reported to have larger increases in expressive vocabulary during lockdown (see Fig. 5).
Discussion
This study examined 8–36-month-old children’s screen time during the first COVID-19 lockdown. With regards to the factors associated with young children’s screen time during lockdown, we found that toddlers had exposure to screens from very early on: From as early as eight months of age, some children appeared to have regular daily exposure to screens. Nevertheless, we found that screen time increased with age, with older children reported to have more screen time than younger children. This effect was consistent across the two datasets and the literature11,13,15–18. Socioeconomic status (SES), as indexed by maternal education, was negatively associated with screen time, with caregivers from lower SES families reporting that their children had more screen time than caregivers from higher SES families, although we note that our sample was biased towards higher SES families. Therefore, this finding may not generalize to the wider population.
We also found a positive association between children’s and caregivers’ screen time consistent with the literature17. Probing this relation further, caregivers’ beliefs about the impact of screen time (e.g., that children’s screen time allows them contact with others outside the family, or leads to fights between siblings) was associated with children’s screen time, such that caregivers who were more positively inclined to screen time also reported allowing their children more screen time. This finding may suggest that caregivers’ attitudes towards children’s screen time predict the extent of children’s screen time exposure. However, we note that an alternative explanation of this association may be that those caregivers who were more positively inclined towards children’s screen time were also more likely to report that their child had access to screen time, due to their not viewing this activity negatively. Our results are unable to tease apart these two explanations.
Furthermore, as Fig. 3 suggests, we found that even toddlers were reported to have had more screen time during lockdown relative to the period before lockdown. While this echoes previous results with older children (see Table 1), increases in screen time during lockdown in school-aged children are likely related to enforced online schooling. Here, we found that, despite toddlers having no online schooling requirements, even these youngest members of our societies had increased exposure to screens during lockdown. There was some evidence that this increase in screen time during lockdown was related to the duration of lockdown in specific countries, such that children from countries who enforced longer lockdowns had increased screen time relative to children from countries with shorter lockdowns. This finding highlights how differences in environmental factors, such as, for instance, restrictions during lockdown on activities that families typically engaged in, can impact children’s screen time early in development. Thus, the longer caregivers were at home caring for their children while also working from home - with limited access to other activities to occupy their charges - the more screen time their children reportedly had access to. However, we found no evidence that increases in screen time during lockdown were associated with other sociodemographic characteristics such as SES or the age of the child.
Finally, we discuss the potential impacts of increased screen time on children’s development during lockdown, particularly with regard to children’s vocabulary development. A study on children33 at the same age as those reported here found that children’s gains in expressive vocabulary size during lockdown were negatively associated with children’s screen time. Extending these findings, we found tentative evidence for the hypothesis that children who experienced larger increases in screen time during lockdown relative to before lockdown showed smaller increases in their expressive vocabulary during lockdown, such that their language development was on par with, but not exceeding, expected growth. While this finding suggests that abrupt changes in children’s daily lives may have consequences for their language development, we note that these results should be treated with caution due to the exploratory nature of the analyses. Nevertheless, these findings raise questions regarding why screen time, especially with regards to suddenly increased screen time during the lockdown, is negatively associated with language and other developmental milestones. While some explanations for similar findings target the difficulties toddlers face when learning words from screens, others target potential negative effects of screen time with regards to the fact that screen time may displace time spent on other enriching activities. For instance, children who have increased exposure to television, spend less time reading or being read to49,50, and have fewer books at home31. Our findings could have implications for public health and practice, given the suggestion that increased sedentary screen time during lockdown is negatively associated with children’s language outcomes. However, we advise caution with regards to this suggestion given the tentative nature of the association between language development and increased screen time reported here.
Furthermore, it should be noted that the variables measured in this study, such as screen time and vocabulary size, are subject to self-report biases. During lockdown, many caregivers worked from home and thus had increased opportunities to observe their child, and this increase might have caused the overestimation of screen time and vocabulary size (see33 for a similar discussion). In addition, given our use of a 10-point scale to measure screen time, our data cannot speak for the exact number of hours children spent in front of the screen. Finally, we note that we do not report on the content and context of screen time in the current manuscript. This is especially important given that some findings suggest that the quality of screen time may be more strongly associated with developmental outcomes than the quantity of screen time. Indeed, we did collect data on the content (educational, entertainment) of children’s screen time as well as the context of their screen time (alone, with siblings, with caregivers). However, this data was only collected in a handful of countries and there was considerable variability in the exact questions included across countries. Planned country-specific examination of these issues will address these questions further, and may provide further detail into the influence of screen time on early development.
Conclusions
This study highlights the consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic on early development. On the one hand, we found that toddlers with no online schooling requirements were exposed to more screen time during lockdown relative to prior to lockdown. We also found that this may have been particularly exacerbated in countries with longer lockdowns. On the other hand, we found that factors previously associated with screen time before the lockdown in the literature were associated with screen time during lockdown in the current study (i.e., the age of the child, SES, caregiver screen time and caregiver attitude to screen time). We interpret this in terms of the continuity of the presence of screens in young children’s lives. The COVID-19 pandemic provided us a unique window to explore the changes in children’s lives, and to examine sources of individual as well as cultural differences in screen time in young children. These findings shed light on the way different families view, use, and are affected by screens in both their normal and disrupted lives.
Methods
This study’s predictions and analyses were pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/4h7sw/) following data collection and prior to data preprocessing, visualisation and analysis. All materials, anonymized data, and analysis codes are available on the project’s OSF. Deviations from the preregistration and exploratory analyses are highlighted below.
Participants
Families with children aged between 8 and 36 months were recruited between March and September 2020 by 15 labs across 12 countries (Canada, France, Germany, Israel, Norway, Poland, Russia, Saudi Arabia, Switzerland, Turkey, UK, USA). Participants were recruited through online advertisements on social media, and contacting caregivers registered to babylab databases. In total, data from 2209 children (and their caregivers) were entered into the models reported in this manuscript, 1292 of which were collected in the context of a larger COVID-lockdown study, henceforth referred to as the COVID-language dataset (33, see data exclusion details below).
In a subset of countries, henceforth referred to as the COVID-screen dataset (Germany, Israel, Switzerland, UK), additional data were collected which explicitly aimed at examining children’s screen time during lockdown (n = 1323, n after exclusions = 992, see details below; gender information not available for this sample). Not all of the participants who were in the COVID-screen dataset were included in COVID-language dataset (and vice versa) due to these participants not providing data for mandatory questions in that study.
Some of the analyses reported here focus solely on the COVID-screen dataset (n = 1323). Of these, 331 participants were excluded from the analysis for the following reasons: (a) older or younger than the specified age limit in the study (i.e., younger than 8 months or older than 36 months; n = 261), (b) participant information not available or conflicting participant information across datasets (n = 66) or (c) participants completed the study after the end of data collection (n = 4), leaving a total of 992 participants whose data could be entered into the different models. Across all models, we excluded participants who did not provide data for all variables included. The number of participants whose data were entered into each model are highlighted in the results section as well as presented with details of model specification and model output in Supplementary Information B.
Separate analyses examine the participants analysed in the COVID-language dataset (23, n = 1742, 888 girls), following exclusion of 450 of the original 1742 participants due to conflicts in the data provided (date at which childcare facilities shut down for lockdown later than date of filling in the last questionnaire, n = 67), providing responses that deviated from the monotonous scale adopted for the current study (n = 353) and/or not providing responses for all variables entered into the model (n = 29). Finally, we report analyses including those participants from the COVID-language dataset33 for whom we were able to obtain data on both their vocabulary development during lockdown as well as additional data on their screen time during lockdown from the COVID-screen dataset (n = 176, 117 of which provided data for the receptive analyses and 156 of which provided data for the expressive vocabulary analyses due to some participants providing data on both children’s receptive and expressive vocabulary size).
Procedure
As part of a larger global COVID-lockdown study33, participants were asked to complete an online questionnaire at the beginning of the first lockdown in March 2020 (T1) and at the end or easing of the lockdown (T2) in their respective countries (between May and September 2020). Some of the participants were only presented with questionnaires at T2 (n = 615) and were asked to complete a compiled version of the T1 and T2 questionnaires that included all relevant questions for the current study at this time. T1 and T2 questionnaires also included other variables which are not investigated in this study. The entire study was conducted online. This research was carried out in accordance with the provisions of the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. The project was approved by the ethics committee of the Georg-Elias-Müller Institute for Psychology at the University of Göttingen. Collaborating labs obtained ethical approval from their institutions and each lab followed the ethical guidelines and ethics-review-board protocols of their own institution. All labs obtained informed consent from the legal guardians of the children whose data was included in the study before proceeding with the study. Central data analyses exclusively used depersonalized data.
Materials
Time 1 questionnaire
The T1 questionnaire included questions relating to children’s socio-demographic characteristics (i.e., child age, exposure to [different] languages in daily life, number of siblings, caregivers’ native language, caregiver education) and the child’s vocabulary development at T1 (see33 for further details). Based on research highlighting the influential role of maternal education on children’s later achievement51, we used maternal education as a proxy for socioeconomic status (SES; see Figs. 1 and 2 in the Supplementary Information for distribution of maternal education across the two datasets), measured on a scale from 1 to 6 as follows: 1 (primary school), 2 (high school), 3 (college/University), 4 (Bachelor degree), 5 (Master degree), 6 (Doctoral degree).
Vocabulary measure
Children’s receptive and expressive vocabularies were measured using age-appropriate Communicative Development Inventories (CDIs) and their adaptations for the relevant language (or regional variant) of the child. Caregivers were asked to indicate whether their child understands (receptive vocabulary) and/or says (expressive vocabulary) each word in the inventory. Kartushina et al.33 transformed the number of words on CDIs to daily percentiles for each language using data from wordbank.stanford.edu52 or from norming data collected by authors of that study for this explicit purpose when available. These percentile scores, calculated as described in33 constituted our vocabulary measure at T1 in the current study.
Time 2 questionnaire
The T2 questionnaire included a range of questions examining children’s screen time, vocabulary size at T2 (percentile score similar to T1), caregivers’ screen time and caregivers’ attitude towards children’s screen time, described in more detail below. Some labs included other questions not included here.
Children’s screen time
With some variation between labs, questions targeted the quantity, quality and context of children’s screen time. The current study focused on the quantity of children’s screen time due to considerable data loss with regards to the other descriptives. All labs asked caregivers to rate the amount of time their child spent in watching baby cartoons and shows on any device (e.g., TV, DVD, smartphone) and playing digital baby games. Some questions also separately targeted the quantity of screen time prior to and during lockdown with differences between labs in whether the questions on screen time prior to lockdown were asked at T1 or at T2. Given differences in the specific scales used across different labs, we harmonised the data to a seven-point scale (ranging from “My child never uses these devices” to “More than 4 h per day”) that collapsed across lab-specific scales or time-estimates (see Fig. 6 for distribution of screen time across the participating countries).
Caregiver screen time
Caregivers were also asked to report their own screen use on a 10-point scale ranging from “I do not use this type of device” to “More than 6 h per day”. Some labs asked about caregivers’ general use of screens whilst other labs asked about the use of specific devices separately, for example, phones, laptops, and tablets, as well as whether this was in the presence of their child or not, and whether their media use was work related or not. This was harmonised to generate a single value of caregiver screen time on the same seven-point scale as children's screen time (see Figs. 2 and 3 in the Supplementary Information for visualisation of variation in caregiver screen time across the two datasets).
Caregiver attitude to children’s screen time
Caregivers were asked to select any perceived positive or negative impacts of their child’s screen use from a list of five positive and six negative possibilities (see Supplementary Information D for options presented to parents and visualisation of caregivers’ perception of children’s screen time). For example, a potential positive impact of media use may be “this allows my child to have contact with family/friends” while a potential negative impact may be “screens lead to sibling fights”.
Vocabulary measure
As at T1, we measured children’s receptive and expressive vocabularies using age-appropriate CDIs. Percentile scores based on caregiver responses on the CDIs calculated as described in33, constituted our vocabulary measure at T2 in the current study.
Country-specific lockdown characteristics
Lockdown severity
Due to variation in COVID-19 transmission rates and government response, the lockdown restrictions and dates varied between countries. Subsequently, the dates for T1 and T2 data collection also varied across countries. Adapting the Oxford COVID-19 lockdown Stringency Index39, we calculated a simple additive lockdown severity index for each country on a three-point scale with one point awarded for each of the following: (1) playground closures, (2) social contact restrictions and (3) restrictions on going outside (see Table 1 in Supplementary Information). We also collected data on whether childcare facilities were shut and whether leisure and eating facilities were closed down but these data were uniform across the countries included here and were excluded from the three-point scale.
Lockdown duration
We also calculated the duration of lockdown until the T2 questionnaire was completed in each country or region. Where available, this was based on the number of days between the date on which the T2 questionnaire was filled (the end of lockdown for that family or the end of data collection if lockdown was not yet complete) and the date on which nurseries, preschools and childcare facilities shut down in that region or country. This ranged from 35 to 151 days with a mean of 77 days (see Figs. 5 and 6, Supplementary Information).
Pre-processing
Following import of the data from the different labs, we identified subtle differences in the scales used by the different labs. We therefore converted the scales provided by the labs to hours and minutes and then reconverted the data to a harmonised scale across labs. This was done by taking the midpoint of the time-range for each value on the scale. Also, as specified in the preregistration, since the response variable was measured on a non-monotonous 0 to 9 scale (varying from number of times per week the child has access to screen time to how many minutes per day the child has access to screen time), we excluded those values (1 and 2) that were on a different scale (number of times per week) relative to the other responses (duration per day). For labs which collected data on screen time across a range of devices or across a variety of digital content (age-appropriate, age-inappropriate), we summed the midpoint time estimates across different devices and/or content to calculate total screen time for each participant. The final harmonised scale across countries entered into the models was the following seven-point scale: 0 (not at all), 1 (0–30 min a day), 2 (30–60 min a day), 3 (1–2 h a day), 4 (2–3 h a day), 5 (3–4 h a day) and 6 (more than 4 h a day). Figure 6 visualizes these data separately for participants from the different countries contributing to the COVID-language dataset and the COVID-screen dataset. With regards to caregiver screen time, we also excluded those values (1 and 2) that were on a different scale (per week) and only retained responses that indicated how much screen time caregivers had per day (see Figs. 3 and 4, Supplementary Information). Finally, with regards to caregivers’ attitude to children’s screen time, caregivers were asked to indicate which of six potentially positive and five potentially negative side-effects of children’s screen time use they perceived: For example, whether screen time led to them having more time for themselves, or to siblings fighting amongst each other. We first examined whether caregivers’ responses to the positive side-effects were correlated with caregivers’ responses to the negative side-effects of screen time. Due to the significant correlation between these two variables, r = 0.167, p < 0.001, we collapsed the two measures as proposed in the preregistration. In particular, we calculated the percentage of positive side-effects caregivers indicated they agreed with as well as the percentage of negative side-effects caregivers indicated their agreement and then computed the difference between these two as an index of caregiver’s overall view of children’s screen time. Thus, if this measure (henceforth, caregiver affect) were positive, it indicates that caregivers indicated they agreed with a greater proportion of the positive side-effects of screen time and vice versa if this measure were negative.
All predictor variables entered into the model were scaled (using the default scale function in R) by calculating the mean and standard deviation of all the values and then scaling each value by subtracting the mean from each value and dividing it by the standard deviation. After this, they had a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one, which eases model convergence.
Data analysis
First, we examined young children’s screen time during lockdown using the COVID-language dataset33 (https://osf.io/ty9mn/). Fixed effects entered into the model were lockdown.severity (on a scale of 0 to 3) and lockdown.duration (calculated as the number of days since the shutdown of childcare facilities in the city where participants were located and the date the questionnaire was filled in). We also included as fixed effects the number of siblings, age of the child (in days), and SES (as indexed by maternal education) as well as the amount of caregiver screen time. We fitted an ordinal model53 using the ordinal package (version 2019–12-10, see references for list of package citations) in R (Version 4.0.3). We included a random intercepts effect for country and all theoretically identifiable random slopes (see Model 1 specification including full random effects structure in Table 2 in Supplementary Information54). We initially tried to include parameters for the correlations among random intercepts and slopes, but removed the correlations due to issues with model convergence. All models were fitted on complete datasets. The model reported here was compared to a null model excluding all predictors except SES as preregistered. Such a full-null model comparison aims at avoiding “cryptic multiple testing”55. The sample analysed with this model comprised a total of 1292 cases from 11 countries.
In addition, using the subset of data which included information on whether caregivers agreed with statements about potential positive or negative side effects of their children’s screen time, we fitted an additional ordinal model (see Model 2 specification including full random effects structure in Table 4 in Supplementary Information) including participants from the COVID-screen dataset who provided information on caregivers’ affective response to children’s screen time (caregiver.affect). We did not include SES or number of siblings as a predictor in this model (due to data loss). The sample analysed with this model comprised a total of 951 cases from 4 countries. However, a separate model including SES and siblings as predictors revealed very similar results to those reported here (see Table 6, Supplementary Information). The model reported here was compared to a null model excluding all predictors as preregistered. While we had originally preregistered including whether the child asked for access to screens to the model we did not include this predictor variable due to almost no countries providing data for this variable. The sample analysed with this model comprised a total of 622 cases from 3 countries.
Countries contributing to the COVID-screen dataset (Germany, Israel, Switzerland and the UK) also asked caregivers to provide additional information on how much screen time their children had access to prior to the lockdown as well as during the lockdown. We, therefore, fitted an additional model including all participants who provided information on quantity of screen time prior to and during lockdown (see Model 3 specification including full random effects structure in Table 8 in Supplementary Information). The factor lockdown.stage coded for whether the values indicated for the response variable were for the time prior to the lockdown or during lockdown, with the time prior to lockdown as the reference level. The model reported here was compared to a null model excluding all predictors except caregiver.affect as preregistered. The sample analysed with this model comprised a total of 953 individuals from 4 countries.
Finally, we examined whether lockdown-related increases in children’s screen time impacted children’s vocabulary development, such that those children who were reported to have had more screen time during lockdown were also reported to show smaller gains in vocabulary development during lockdown. The response variables entered into separate receptive and expressive vocabulary models were children’s receptive and expressive percentile scores respectively (see Model 4a/b specification including full random effects structure in Table 9 and 11 in Supplementary Information). The models reported here were compared to null models excluding all predictors except SES as preregistered. The models on vocabulary development were fitted with a beta error distribution and logit link function56,57 and with the function glmmTMB of the equally named package (version 1.1.1;58). The samples analysed with these models comprised a total of 234 percentile scores for 132 individuals from two countries (comprehension model) and a total of 312 percentile scores for 172 individuals from three countries (production model). Neither of the two percentile scores was overdispersed (dispersion parameters, comprehension model: 0.588; production model: 0.609).
For all models we determined their stability by dropping levels of the random effects, one at a time, fitting the full model to each of the subset data sets, and finally comparing the range of model estimates obtained for the subsets those obtained for the full data set. This revealed the fixed effects model estimates to be of moderate to good stability (see full model results in Supporting Information C). We determined confidence limits of fixed effects model estimates and fitted values by means of parametric bootstraps (N = 1000). In case of the ordinal models we implemented these with a function written by RM, and for models fitted with the function glmmTMB we used the function simulate of the respective package. We determined the significance of individual fixed effects by dropping them from the respective model, one at a time, and comparing the resulting reduced model with the one from which the fixed effect had been dropped. These comparisons as well as full-null model comparisons utilized a likelihood ratio test58. Furthermore, ordinal models make the proportional odds assumption. In essence, this states that effects of the predictors on the probability of the response to exceed a certain value should be the same for all values of the response. We checked whether this assumption was fulfilled and found, in part, indecisive results (see Supplementary Information E for details). Finally, we are aware that, in most of our models, the number of levels associated with the random effects factor country was likely too low to reliably estimate its effect. However, with regard to the ordinal models, including country as a fixed effect was not possible since lockdown severity did not vary within countries. Hence, we decided to retain the models as preregistered as the best likely approach to estimate the effect of lockdown.severity while at the same time controlling for country level variation in toddlers’ screen time. With regard to the models with vocabulary size as the response, we fitted corresponding models into which we included country as a fixed rather than a random effect. We did not include any interactions with country in these models in order to allow comparability with the original models. The models revealed results very similar to those of the original models (see Supplementary Information F). Furthermore, Supplementary Information G provides details of the random effects by country for all of the full models which quantifies the variance attributable to country.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
Data collected in the UK was funded by the UKRI rapid response to COVID-19 ES/V004085/1.
Author contributions
N.D. and N.M. conceived and planned the study in the context of a larger study on COVID-19 and language development (Kartushina et al., 2021, see details on author contributions there). N.D. and N.M. created the materials for the questionnaire on screen time. C.B., N.D. and N.M. managed the project. N.M. wrote the preregistration with comments from S.A., H.A., M.B., C.B., T.H.K., J.M. and J.S. N.D., K.A., A.A., H.A., S.A., C.D., C.K., N.G.G., S.G., N.H., T.H.K., J.K., N.K., J.M., J.S. and N.M. collected data for the manuscript and N.D., S.A., C.D., C.K., N.G.G., S.G., N.H., T.H.K., J.S. and N.M. collected additional data on screen time for the manuscript. C.B. and R.M. preprocessed the data. C.B., J.M., R.M. and N.M. analysed the data and created visualizations. N.M. took the lead in writing the manuscript with significant contributions from N.D., K.A., H.A., S.A., M.B., C.B., C.D., N.G.G., S.G. and J.K. A.A., H.A., K.A., C.B., J.K. and N.M. edited and prepared the manuscript for submission. All authors provided critical feedback and commented on the manuscript. C.B., S.A., H.A., J.K., R.M. and N.M. revised the manuscript after the first round of reviews.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Data availability
This study’s predictions and analyses were pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/4h7sw/) following data collection and prior to data preprocessing, visualisation and analysis. All materials, anonymized data, and analysis code are available on the project’s OSF. Deviations from the preregistration and exploratory analyses are highlighted. Identifiable data are securely archived at the Language Archive of the Max Planck Institute in Nijmegen, Netherlands.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Christina Bergmann and Nevena Dimitrova.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-05840-5.
References
- 1.Moore SA, Faulkner G, Rhodes RE, Brussoni M, Chulak-Bozzer T, Ferguson LJ, Raktim Mitra R, O’Reilly N, Spence JC, Vanderloo LM, Tremblay MS. Impact of the COVID-19 virus outbreak on movement and play behaviours of Canadian children and youth: a national survey. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2020;17(1):85. doi: 10.1186/s12966-020-00987-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Xiang M, Zhang Z, Kuwahara K. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on children and adolescents' lifestyle behavior larger than expected. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 2020;63(4):531–532. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2020.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chambonniere C, Lambert C, Fearnbach N, Tardieu M, Fillon A, Genin P, Larras B, Melsens P, Bois J, Pereira B, Tremblay A, Thivel D, Duclos M. Effect of the COVID-19 lockdown on physical activity and sedentary behaviors in French children and adolescents: new results from the ONAPS national survey. Eur. J. Integr. Med. 2021;43:101308. doi: 10.1016/j.eujim.2021.101308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schmidt S, Anedda B, Burchartz A, Eichsteller A, Kolb S, Nigg C, Niessner C, Oriwol D, Worth A, Woll A. Physical activity and screen time of children and adolescents before and during the COVID-19 lockdown in Germany: a natural experiment. Sci. Rep. 2020;10(1):21780. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-78438-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pietrobelli A, Pecoraro L, Ferruzzi A, Heo M, Faith M, Zoller T, Antoniazzi F, Piacentini G, Fearnbach SN, Heymsfield SB. Effects of COVID-19 lockdown on lifestyle behaviors in children with obesity living in Verona, Italy: a longitudinal study. Obesity. 2020;28(8):1382–1385. doi: 10.1002/oby.22861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.ten Velde, G., Lubrecht, J., Arayess, L., van Loo, C., Hesselink, M., Reijnders, D., & Vreugdenhil, A. (2021). Physical activity behaviour and screen time in Dutch children during the COVID-19 pandemic: Pre-, during and post-school closures. Pediatric Obesity, e12779. 10.1111/ijpo.12779 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 7.Guan H, Okely AD, Aguilar-Farias N, del Pozo Cruz B, Draper CE, El Hamdouchi A, Florindo AA, Jáuregui A, Katzmarzyk PT, Kontsevaya A, Löf M, Park W, Reilly JJ, Sharma D, Tremblay MS, Veldman SLC. Promoting healthy movement behaviours among children during the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health. 2020;4(6):416–418. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30131-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Medrano, M., Cadenas-Sanchez, C., Oses, M., Arenaza, L., Amasene, M., & Labayen, I. (2020). Changes in lifestyle behaviours during the COVID-19 confinement in Spanish children: A longitudinal analysis from the MUGI project. Pediatric Obesity, e12731. Advance online publication. 10.1111/ijpo.12731 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 9.Hartshorne JK, Huang Y, Aulestia PMLP, Oppenheimer K, Robbins PT, Molina MDV. Screen time as an index of family distress. PsyArXiv. 2020 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/zqc4t. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ribner AD, Coulanges L, Friedman S, Libertus M. Screen time in the COVID era: International trends of increasing use among 3- to 7-year-old children. PsyArXiv. 2021 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/nx9ew. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bedford R, Saez de Urabain IR, Celeste CH, Karmiloff-Smith A, Smith TJ. Toddlers’ fine motor milestone achievement is associated with early touchscreen scrolling. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:1–8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ribner AD, McHarg G. Screens across the pond: findings from longitudinal screen time research in the US and UK. Infant Behav. Dev. 2021;63(February):101551. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rideout V, Robb MB. The Common Sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight, 2020. Common Sense Media; 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Goode JA, Fomby P, Mollborn S, Limburg A. Children’s technology time in two US cohorts. Child Indic. Res. 2020;13(3):1107–1132. doi: 10.1007/s12187-019-09675-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Alroqi H, Serratrice L, Cameron-Faulkner T. The home literacy and media environment of Saudi toddlers. J. Child. Media. 2021 doi: 10.1080/17482798.2021.1921819. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Cartanyà-Hueso À, Lidón-Moyano C, Cassanello P, Díez-Izquierdo A, Martín-Sánchez JC, Balaguer A, Martínez-Sánchez JM. Smartphone and tablet usage during COVID-19 pandemic confinement in children under 48 months in Barcelona (Spain) Healthcare. 2021;9(1):96. doi: 10.3390/healthcare9010096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Paudel S, Jancey J, Subedi N, Leavy J. Correlates of mobile screen media use among children aged 0–8: a systematic review. BMJ Open. 2017;7(10):e014585. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Nevski E, Siibak A. The role of parents and parental mediation on 0–3-year-olds’ digital play with smart devices: Estonian parents’ attitudes and practices. Early Years. 2016;36(3):227–241. doi: 10.1080/09575146.2016.1161601. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Chaudron, S., Di Gioia, R., & Gemo, M. (2018). Young children (0–8) and digital technology: A qualitative study across Europe; EUR 29070. 10.2760/294383
- 20.De Decker E, De Craemer M, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Wijndaele K, Duvinage K, Koletzko B, Grammatikaki E, Iotova V, Usheva N, Fernández-Alvira JM, Zych K, Manios Y, Cardon G, ToyBox-study group, Influencing factors of screen time in preschool children: An exploration of parents’ perceptions through focus groups in six European countries. Obes. Rev. 2012;13:75–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00961.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Santos A, Silva-Santos S, Andaki A, Mendes EL, Vale S, Mota J, Santos A, Silva-Santos S, Andaki A, Mendes EL, Vale S, Model Mota J. Screen time between Portuguese and Brazilian children: A cross-cultural study. Motriz Revista de Educação Física. 2017 doi: 10.1590/s1980-6574201700020006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Duch H, Fisher EM, Ensari I, Harrington A. Screen time use in children under 3 years old: A systematic review of correlates. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2013;10(1):102. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-10-102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hoyos Cillero I, Jago R. Systematic review of correlates of screen-viewing among young children. Prev. Med. 2010;51(1):3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Domoff SE, Radesky JS, Harrison K, Riley H, Lumeng JC, Miller AL. A naturalistic study of child and family screen media and mobile device use. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2019;28(2):401–410. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1275-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hinkley T, Salmon J, Okely AD, Trost SG. Correlates of sedentary behaviours in preschool children: a review. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2010;7(1):66. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bentley GF, Turner KM, Jago R. Mothers’ views of their preschool child’s screen-viewing behaviour: A qualitative study. BMC Public Health. 2016;16(1):718. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3440-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Television and DVD/video viewing in children younger than 2 years. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2007;161(5):473–479. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.5.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.De Lepeleere S, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Van Stappen V, Huys N, Latomme J, Androutsos O, Manios Y, Cardon G, Verloigne M. Parenting practices as a mediator in the association between family socio-economic status and screen-time in primary schoolchildren: A Feel4Diabetes study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2018;15(11):2553. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15112553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hutton JS, Huang G, Sahay RD, DeWitt T, Ittenbach RF. A novel, composite measure of screen-based media use in young children (ScreenQ) and associations with parenting practices and cognitive abilities. Pediatr. Res. 2020;87(7):1211–1218. doi: 10.1038/s41390-020-0765-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kabali HK, Irigoyen MM, Nunez-Davis R, Budacki JG, Mohanty SH, Leister KP, Bonner RL. Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics. 2015;136(6):1044–1050. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Linebarger DL, Moses A, Garrity Liebeskind K, McMenamin K. Learning vocabulary from television: Does onscreen print have a role? J. Educ. Psychol. 2013;105(3):609–621. doi: 10.1037/a0032582. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Supanitayanon S, Trairatvorakul P, Chonchaiya W. Screen media exposure in the first 2 years of life and preschool cognitive development: a longitudinal study. Pediatr. Res. 2020;88(6):894–902. doi: 10.1038/s41390-020-0831-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kartushina N, Mani N, Aktan-Erciyes A, Alaslani K, Aldrich NJ, Almohammadi A, Alroqi H, Anderson L, Andonova E, Aussems S, Babineau M, Barokova MD, Bergmann C, Cashon C, Custode S, de Carvalho A, Dimitrova N, Dynak A, Farah R, Mayor J. COVID-19 first lockdown as a unique window into language acquisition: What you do (with your child) matters. PsyArXiv. 2021 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/5ejwu. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Levine LE, Waite BM, Bowman LL, Kachinsky K. Mobile media use by infants and toddlers. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019;94:92–99. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.12.045. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Roy, R., & Paradis, G. (2015, March 19–21). Smartphone use in the daily interactions between parents and young children [Poster session]. Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, Philadelphia, PA, United States.
- 36.Guttentag DN, Albritton WL, Kettner RB. Daytime television viewing by hospitalized children: The effect of alternative programming. Pediatrics. 1983;71(4):620–625. doi: 10.1542/peds.71.4.620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Simon TD, Berry J, Feudtner C, Stone BL, Sheng X, Bratton SL, Dean JM, Srivastava R. Children with complex chronic conditions in inpatient hospital settings in the United States. Pediatrics. 2010;126(4):647–655. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-3266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Whiting S, Buoncristiano M, Gelius P, Abu-Omar K, Pattison M, Hyska J, Duleva V, Musić Milanović S, Zamrazilová H, Hejgaard T, Rasmussen M, Nurk E, Shengelia L, Kelleher CC, Heinen MM, Spinelli A, Nardone P, Abildina A, Abdrakhmanova S, Aitmurzaeva G, Breda J. Physical activity, screen time, and sleep duration of children aged 6–9 years in 25 countries: An analysis within the WHO European childhood obesity surveillance initiative (COSI) 2015–2017. Obesity Facts. 2020 doi: 10.1159/000511263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hale T, Angrist N, Goldszmidt R, Kira B, Petherick A, Phillips T, Webster S, Cameron-Blake E, Hallas L, Majumdar S, Tatlow H. A global panel database of pandemic policies (Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker) Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021;5:529–538. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01079-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.AAP Council on Communications and Media Media and young minds. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5):e20162591. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Australian Department of Health. (2019). 24-hour movement guidelines for the early years (0 to 5 years): An integration of physical activity, sedentary behaviour, and sleep. https://www1.health.gov.au/internet/main/publishing.nsf/Content/ti-0-5years [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 42.Canadian Paediatric Society. (2017). Screen time and young children: Promoting health and development in a digital world. Paediatr Child Health 2017;22(8):461–468 https://www.cps.ca/en/documents/position/screen-time-and-young-children [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 43.German Federal Ministry of Health. (2016). National recommendations for physical activity and physical activity promotion. https://opus4.kobv.de/opus4-fau/files/7827/Bewegungsempfehlung_OPUS.pdf
- 44.Public Health England (2013). How Healthy Behaviour Supports Children’s Wellbeing, PHE Publications Gateway Number: 2013146. Available at: https://www.gov.uk/government/news/sedentary-lifestyles-and-too-much-screen-time-affect-childrens-wellbeing (accessed May 21, 2021).
- 45.World Health Organization. (2019). Guidelines for physical activity, sedentary behavior, and sleep: For children under 5 years of age. Geneva. ISBN: 978–92–4–155053–6
- 46.Dynia JM, Dore RA, Bates RA, Justice LM. Media exposure and language for toddlers from low-income homes. Infant Behav. Dev. 2021;63:101542. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Pagani LS, Fitzpatrick C, Barnett TA. Early childhood television viewing and kindergarten entry readiness. Pediatr. Res. 2013;74(3):350–355. doi: 10.1038/pr.2013.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Morim C, Tough S. Association between screen time and children’s performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(3):244–250. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Khan KS, Purtell KM, Logan J, Ansari A, Justice LM. Association between television viewing and parent-child reading in the early home environment. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2017;38(7):521–527. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Vandewater EA, Bickham DS, Lee JH, Cummings HM, Wartella EA, Rideout VJ. When the television is always on: Heavy television exposure and young children’s development. Am. Behav. Sci. 2005;48(5):562–577. doi: 10.1177/0002764204271496. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Davis-Kean PE, Tighe LA, Waters NE. The role of parent educational attainment in parenting and children’s development. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2021;30(2):186–192. doi: 10.1177/0963721421993116. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Frank MC, Braginsky M, Yurovsky D, Marchman VA. Wordbank: an open repository for developmental vocabulary data. J. Child Lang. 2017;44:677–694. doi: 10.1017/S030500009160000209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Agresti A. Categorical Data Analysis. 2. John Wiley and Sons, Inc.; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. J. Mem. Lang. 2013;68:255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Forstmeier W, Schielzeth H. Cryptic multiple hypothesis testing in linear models: overestimated effect sizes and the winner’s curse. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2011;65:47–55. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1038-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.McCullagh P, Nelder JA. Generalized Linear Models. Chapman and Hall; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Bolker BM. Ecological Models and Data in R. Princeton University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Brooks ME, Kristensen K, van Benthem KJ, Magnusson A, Berg CW, Nielsen A, Skaug HJ, Maechler M, Bolker BM. glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R J. 2017;9(2):378–400. doi: 10.32614/RJ-2017-066. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
This study’s predictions and analyses were pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/4h7sw/) following data collection and prior to data preprocessing, visualisation and analysis. All materials, anonymized data, and analysis code are available on the project’s OSF. Deviations from the preregistration and exploratory analyses are highlighted. Identifiable data are securely archived at the Language Archive of the Max Planck Institute in Nijmegen, Netherlands.