Abstract
Social behavior is a key component of pig welfare on farms, but little is known on the development of social behaviors in piglets. This study aimed to explore social behaviors and identify early social styles in suckling piglets. Social behaviors of 68 piglets from 12 litters were scored continuously for 8 h per day at 21 and 42 days of age, and were included in a Hierarchical Clustering on Principal Components analysis to identify clusters of pigs with similar social styles. Social nosing represented 78% of all social interactions given. Three social styles were identified: low-solicited inactive animals (inactive), active animals (active), and highly-solicited avoiders (avoiders). Belonging to a cluster was independent of age, but was influenced by sex, with females being more represented in the ‘inactive’ cluster, and males in the ‘active’ cluster, whereas both sexes were equally represented in the ‘avoider’ cluster. Stability of piglets’ allocation to specific clusters over age was high in the ‘inactive’ (59%) and ‘active’ (65%) clusters, but low in the ‘avoider’ cluster (7%). Haptoglobin and growth rate were higher in ‘active’ than ‘inactive’ pigs, and intermediate in ‘avoiders’. Our findings suggest the existence of transient social styles in piglets, likely reflective of sexual dimorphism or health status.
Subject terms: Behavioural methods, Animal behaviour
Introduction
The pig (Sus scrofa) is a highly gregarious species, which lives in highly stable social groups1,2 resting on a linear hierarchy based on dominant-subordinate relationships between group members3,4. These dominant-subordinate relationships and agonistic behaviors have received extensive research attention, whereas the relevance of socio-positive behaviors for pig welfare on farms remains poorly understood2,5,6. Yet, in stable groups of pigs, the incidence of agonistic interactions is low compared to that of positive social behaviors, such as ‘social nosing’7–9, and recent research suggests that (putative) positive social behaviors, also sometimes referred to as ‘affiliative’ or ‘prosocial’ behaviors, may be key components for welfare of animals on farms5. For instance, social nosing, a common contact behavior in pigs comparable to the licking and grooming behavior described in other farm species2 is assumed to contribute to individual recognition10–12, to participate in the maintenance of social relationships and group cohesion2,6,13, and to generate positive affective states11,14. Social play is another putative positive social behavior that is generally assumed to be an indicator of welfare in juvenile pigs15–17. Indeed, social play helps piglets to develop social skills necessary for conflict resolution and individual recognition18,19, and allows littermates to develop social bonds that remain quite strong even after regrouping with other litters, thus participating in group cohesion2,10.
Furthermore, while previous work has focused heavily on dominance relationships and agonistic behaviors, dominance rank cannot fully explain the large individual differences in social behavior and physiological reactions to social situations often shown by animals20,21. These individual differences in behavior and physiology have been explained by personality traits, also sometimes referred to as individual ‘strategies’ or ‘styles’, that can be defined as “a coherent set of strategies, as well as behavioral and morphological adaptations, which are consistent over time and contexts”22. The study of personality traits is a major challenge for animal welfare research because it may help to understand the consistent differences in behavioral and physiological responses of farm animals to various challenges and conditions, such as different housing systems, management practices or veterinary interventions22. In gregarious animals, sociability and aggressiveness are among the most commonly measured personality traits22,23. In this context, recent behavioral research has explored social personality traits, or individual social strategies or styles, in a variety of farm animals, including dairy calves24, cows25, goats26,27, ewes28, and pigs20,29,30. These social styles have been found to be associated with a variety of individual features. For instance, avoider, affiliative, aggressive and pragmatic ewes differed in their body length, body weight and/or thorax circumference26–28, while aggressive and non-aggressive or affiliative animals showed contrasted cortisol levels in ewes28, goats27 and cows25. Research, however, has focused almost exclusively on adult individuals, and little is known on the development of early social styles in farm animals. Yet, identifying early individual social styles integrating both (putative) affiliative and agonistic behaviors may help to predict the adaptability of animals to various challenges or husbandry practices they may encounter later in life, such as social rearrangements5. A better understanding of early social behavior may also help to identify putative positive social behaviors important for welfare. For instance, some social behaviors, such as rough-and-tumble play or ‘playfight’, are assumed to help piglet developing the motor and social skills needed for successful fighting18,19,31, thus favoring optimal integration into future hierarchical social groups18.
Our study aimed first to describe the social behaviors of 3- to 6-week-old suckling piglets raised in an enriched environment, and explore the existence of early social styles throughout the suckling period based on these social interactions. Next, we aimed to assess the stability of these social styles with age, and the association between these social styles and a variety of individual features, including non-social behaviors (time spent lying, standing or eating) and morphological traits (sex, weight, growth). Because health status can influence behavior, including social behaviors32, we also investigated the association between these social styles and health-related blood parameters (markers of inflammation: haptoglobin33, and markers of oxidative stress: hydrogen peroxide and plasma antioxidant capacity). Furthermore, because peripheral (and central) serotonin (5-HT) levels have been shown to be related to affiliative behaviors (for reviews, see34,35) and aggressive behaviors36,37 in a variety of species, including dogs, pigs and humans, the association between blood 5-HT levels and social behaviors and styles at both ages was explored38.
Results
Descriptive analysis of social (and non-social) behaviors
We collected a total of 3795 (emitted and received) social interactions from 68 piglets from 12 litters at 21 of age, and 6630 (emitted and received) interactions from 69 piglets from 12 litters at day 42 of age. In total, 78% of all behaviors emitted (social nosing, agonistic and other social behaviors) were social nosing (nosing nose, head and body) at both ages, while 8 and 12% were agonistic behaviors, and 14% and 9% were other social behaviors (mounting and nudging) at 21 and 42 days of age, respectively. Social play, on the other hand, represented 22% of all (emitted and received) social interactions at both ages.
Piglets were involved in more social interactions at 42 (63.4 ± 3.9 interactions/pig [range: 4–149]) than at 21 days of age (35.7 ± 2.6 [1–83], F(1,66) = 52.6, p < 0.001). Notably, piglets emitted more agonistic and social nosing behaviors, received more agonistic behaviors, and showed more avoidance and social play episodes at 42 than at 21 days of age (Fig. 1). Males were involved in more social interactions than females (males: 57.9 ± 3.3 interactions/pig; females: 39.9 ± 3.0; F(1,58) = 14.7, p < 0.001). Notably, males emitted more agonistic and other social behaviors (i.e. nudging and mounting), were involved in more social play episodes, but showed fewer avoidance behaviors than females (Fig. 2).
When considering non-social behaviors, piglets spent less time lying (proportion of total observations, d21: 0.58 ± 0.01; d42: 0.49 ± 0.01; F(1,72) = 24.9, p < 0.001), and more time standing (d21: 0.22 ± 0.008; d42: 0.34 ± 0.01; F(1,73) = 84.4, p < 0.001) at 42 than at 21 days of age. No effect of age was found on any other behavioral activities (time spent suckling, eating or drinking; p > 0.10 for all). No effect of sex was found on non-social behavioral activities, including the proportion of observations spent lying, standing, ingesting and suckling.
Identification of individual social styles
Principal components analysis (PCA)
A total of 3 PC explaining 58% of the total variance were extracted from the PCA performed on the 60 piglets observed on both days (Table 1). The first PC accounted for 25% of the total variance. The number of social nosing given (loading: 0.70), other social behaviors given (0.75), and social play episodes (0.66) loaded positively on the component, which was thus labelled ‘social exploration & social play’. The second PC accounted for 18% of the total variance. The number of avoidance behaviors (0.77) and other received social behaviors (0.66) loaded positively, while the number of agonistic behaviors given (− 0.54) loaded negatively on the second component, which was thus labelled ‘avoidance & receipt of exploration vs aggression’. The third PC accounted for 15% of the total variance. The number of received social nosing behaviors (0.77) loaded positively on the third component, which was thus labelled ‘receipt of social nosing’.
Table 1.
Dimension 1 ‘Social exploration & social play’ |
Dimension 2 ‘Avoidance & receipt of social exploration vs aggression’ |
Dimension 3 ‘Receipt of social nosing’ |
|
---|---|---|---|
Variance explained | 25% | 18% | 15% |
Agonistic behaviors given | 0.39 | − 0.54 | 0.48 |
Social nosing behaviors given | 0.70 | − 0.10 | − 0.32 |
Other social behaviors given | 0.75 | − 0.04 | 0.04 |
Agonistic behaviors received | 0.43 | − 0.03 | − 0.38 |
Social nosing behaviors received | 0.21 | 0.31 | 0.77 |
Other social behaviors received | 0.29 | 0.66 | 0.26 |
Social play | 0.66 | − 0.23 | 0.09 |
Avoidance | 0.29 | 0.77 | − 0.23 |
aHigh to moderate (> 0.50 or < − 0.50) coordinates are in bold.
Hierarchical clustering analysis on principal components (HCPC)
The hierarchical clustering analysis was performed on the 3 extracted PC to identify clustered groups of pigs based on social behaviors at both ages. The HCPC suggested the existence of 3 social clusters (Table 2 and Fig. 3a,b). Of the 120 observations included in the PCA (i.e. 60 piglets at both ages), 58 were in cluster 1 (29 and 29 piglets observed at 21 and 42 days, respectively; 48% of observations), 24 piglets in cluster 2 (14 and 10 piglets observed at 21 and 42 days, respectively; 20% of the observations), and 38 piglets in cluster 3 (17 and 21 piglets observed at 21 and 42 days, respectively; 32% of the observations). Clusters were not significantly associated with the supplemental qualitative variable ‘day’, suggesting that the distribution of piglets observed at 21 or 42 days of age was balanced between clusters (χ2 test, p > 0.10; Fig. 3a,b).
Table 2.
Principal component and variables | Coordinatea | Cluster 1 ‘Low-solicited inactive animals’ |
Cluster 2 ‘Highly-solicited avoiders’ |
Cluster 3 ‘Socially active animals’ |
---|---|---|---|---|
n = 58 (d21: 29; d42: 29) |
n = 24 (d21: 14; d42: 10) |
n = 38 (d21: 17; d42: 21) |
||
v.testb | v.testb | v.testb | ||
Principal component 1 | – | − 8.45*** | 3.45*** | 6.11*** |
Agonistic given | 0.39 | – | – | – |
Social nosing given | 0.70 | − 5.26*** | – | 4.96*** |
Other social given | 0.75 | − 5.62*** | – | 5.14*** |
Agonistic received | 0.43 | − 3.72*** | – | 2.35* |
Social nosing received | 0.21 | − | – | – |
Other social received | 0.29 | − | – | – |
Social play | 0.66 | − 5.70*** | – | 5.16*** |
Avoidance | 0.29 | − | – | − |
Total (supplementary quantitative) | 0.92 | − 7.52*** | – | 6.69*** |
Principal component 2 | – | – | 6.76*** | − 5.31*** |
Agonistic given | − 0.54 | − 4.01*** | – | 5.16*** |
Social nosing given | − 0.10 | – | – | – |
Other social given | − 0.04 | – | – | – |
Agonistic received | − 0.03 | – | – | |
Social nosing received | 0.31 | – | – | – |
Other social received | − 0.66 | − 3.80*** | 6.94*** | – |
Social play | − 0.23 | – | – | – |
Avoidance | 0.77 | − 2.17* | 4.87*** | – |
Total (supplementary quantitative) | − 0.17 | – | – | – |
Principal component 3 | – | – | 2.70** | |
Agonistic given | 0.48 | – | – | |
Social nosing given | − 0.32 | – | – | – |
Other social given | 0.04 | – | – | – |
Agonistic received | − 0.38 | – | – | – |
Social nosing received | 0.77 | − 2.85** | 5.11*** | – |
Other social received | 0.26 | – | – | – |
Social play | 0.09 | – | – | – |
Avoidance | − 0.23 | – | – | – |
Total (supplementary quantitative) | − 0.08 | – | – |
The table presents coordinates and characterizes clusters with active and supplementary variables.
aCoordinates (loadings) of the variable on the principal component. Variables with high coordinates on the principal component (> 0.50 or < − 0.50, in bold) are positively and negatively correlated to the principal component, respectively;
bOnly significant v.test values (v.test > 1.96 or v.test < − 1.96; * p value < 0.05, ** p value < 0.01, *** p value < 0.001) are shown. Test values for variables are shown within the principal component section for which the variable had the highest coordinate. Principal components and variables with significant v.test values described the clusters obtained from the hierarchical clustering analysis. The sign of the v.test value indicates if the mean of the cluster is significantly lower or greater than the overall mean of all clusters.
Compared to all piglets, animals from cluster 1 had lower coordinates on PC 1 (‘social exploration & social play’, p < 0.001), and were characterized by fewer social play episodes, social nosing, agonistic behaviors, and other social behaviors given (p < 0.01 for all). Animals in cluster 1 also received fewer agonistic behaviors, social nosing and other social behaviors (p = 0.01), and exhibited fewer avoidance behaviors (p = 0.03). Overall, animals from cluster 1 were involved in fewer social interactions compared to all piglets (p < 0.001), and were thus labelled ‘low-solicited inactive animals’.
Compared to all piglets, animals from cluster 2 had higher coordinates on PC 2 (‘avoidance & receipt of exploration vs aggression’, p < 0.001), PC 1 (‘social exploration & social play’, p < 0.001) and, to a lesser extent, PC 3 (‘receipt of social nosing’, p = 0.007) Cluster 2 was characterized by more social nosing and other social behaviors received (p < 0.001 for both), and more avoidance behaviors (p < 0.001). Therefore, animals from cluster 2 were labelled ‘highly-solicited avoiders’.
Compared to all piglets, animals from cluster 3 had higher coordinates on PC 1 (‘social exploration & social play’, p < 0.001) and lower coordinates on PC 2 (‘avoidance & receipt of exploration vs aggression’, p < 0.001). Cluster 3 was characterized by more social play episodes, and social nosing, agonistic behaviors and other social behaviors given (p < 0.001 for all), and by more agonistic behaviors received (p = 0.02). Overall, animals from cluster 3 were also involved in more social interactions (p < 0.001) than all other piglets, and were thus labelled ‘socially active animals’.
Characterization of social clusters with individual characteristics
Analyses of the association between the clusters and supplementary qualitative variables revealed that clusters were significantly associated with sex (χ2 test, p = 0.003). A total of 31 males and 29 females on both days were included in the multivariate analyses, that is 52% are males and 48% are females. The cluster of ‘low-solicited inactive animals’ was typically composed of females (v-test = 2.88, p = 0.004), with 62% of pigs in this cluster being females (22 males vs 36 females). On the contrary, 76% of animals in the cluster of ‘socially active animals’ were males (v-test = 3.67, p < 0.001, 29 males vs 9 females). In the cluster of ‘highly-solicited avoiders’, the sex ratio was balanced (11 males vs 13 females).
Effects of clusters, and their interaction with sex, on quantitative variables are presented in Table 3. Significant cluster × sex interaction effects were found for non-social behaviors . Indeed, females in the ‘low-solicited inactive’ cluster spent more time lying than females from the ‘socially active’ cluster (post-hoc, p = 0.02) and less time standing than females from the ‘socially active’ (p < 0.001) and ‘highly-solicited avoiders’ clusters (p = 0.002), whereas no differences were found for males. Other non-social behaviors, including time spent eating, drinking, or suckling, were not influenced by cluster, sex or their interactions (p > 0.05). Irrespective of sex, weight and averaged daily gain (ADG) were associated with cluster. Indeed, ‘low-solicited inactive animals’ were lighter (7.3 ± 2.9 kg vs 9.0 ± 2.9 kg, p < 0.001) and gained fewer weight (209 ± 48.8 g/d vs 263 ± 49.8 g/d, p = 0.006) than ‘socially active animals’. ‘Low-solicited inactive animals’ also had higher haptoglobin levels (2.35 ± 0.75 mg/mL vs 1.32 ± 0.78 mg/mL, p = 0.02) than ‘socially active animals’. Other blood parameters, including platelet-rich plasma serotonin (5-HT), and markers of oxidative stress (FRAP and dROM), were not influenced by cluster, sex or their interactions (p > 0.05 for all).
Table 3.
n | Females | Males | p values5 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cluster 1 ‘Inactive’ | Cluster 2 ‘Avoiders’ | Cluster 3 ‘Active’ |
Cluster 1 ‘Inactive’ |
Cluster 2 ‘Avoiders’ |
Cluster 3 ‘Active’ |
Cluster | Sex | Cluster × Sex | |
36 | 13 | 9 | 22 | 11 | 29 | ||||
Behavioral traits2 | |||||||||
Time lying | 0.58 ± 0.03a | 0.50 ± 0.04ab | 0.45 ± 0.04b | 0.54 ± 0.03ab | 0.58 ± 0.04ab | 0.54 ± 0.03ab | 0.09 | 0.08 | 0.03 |
Time standing | 0.23 ± 0.06a | 0.32 ± 0.06b | 0.36 ± 0.06b | 0.25 ± 0.06ab | 0.25 ± 0.06ab | 0.30 ± 0.06ab | < 0.001 | 0.02 | 0.02 |
Time ingesting | 0.02 ± 0.003 | 0.02 ± 0.004 | 0.02 ± 0.005 | 0.01 ± 0.004 | 0.01 ± 0.005 | 0.02 ± 0.003 | 0.13 | 0.11 | 0.79 |
Time suckling | 0.15 ± 0.02 | 0.14 ± 0.02 | 0.12 ± 0.03 | 0.15 ± 0.02 | 0.15 ± 0.03 | 0.17 ± 0.02 | 0.77 | 0.02 | 0.10 |
Production traits | |||||||||
Weight (kg) | 7.2 ± 2.9 | 7.6 ± 3.0 | 9.6 ± 3.0 | 7.5 ± 2.9 | 8.5 ± 3.0 | 8.4 ± 2.9 | < 0.001 | 0.95 | 0.08 |
ADG3 (g/d) | 211 ± 49 | 237 ± 52 | 276 ± 54 | 207 ± 50 | 247 ± 53 | 251 ± 50 | 0.006 | 0.66 | 0.69 |
Blood parameters | |||||||||
5-HT (µmol/L PRP4) | 14.0 ± 4.5 | 14.8 ± 4.9 | 11.7 ± 5.2 | 18.6 ± 4.7 | 15.2 ± 5.0 | 15.3 ± 4.6 | 0.35 | 0.10 | 0.60 |
Haptoglobin (mg/mL) | 2.3 ± 0.77 | 1.2 ± 0.84 | 1.4 ± 0.90 | 2.4 ± 0.80 | 2.5 ± 0.86 | 1.2 ± 0.78 | 0.02 | 0.22 | 0.19 |
dROM (CARRU) | 914 ± 43 | 842 ± 61 | 796 ± 74 | 902 ± 51 | 995 ± 66 | 868 ± 47 | 0.22 | 0.09 | 0.24 |
FRAP (µg/mL) | 67 ± 3.6 | 69 ± 4.2 | 68 ± 4.7 | 66 ± 3.9 | 67 ± 4.5 | 68 ± 3.7 | 0.69 | 0.69 | 0.84 |
1LS-Means ± S.E.M.;
2Proportion of total observation time;
3ADG, average daily gain;
4PRP, platelet-rich plasma;
5Letters indicate differences between groups as estimated by post-hoc test with Tukey adjustment for comparing multiple groups (p < 0.05).
Stability of social behaviors and social styles over time
Consistency of social behaviors
We found positive correlations between social behaviors at 21 and 42 days age. Notably, social play at 21 days were strongly associated to social play at 42 days (Fig. 4a, r = 0.51, p < 0.001, padj < 0.001). Moderate positive correlations between 21 and 42 days of age were also found for social nosing (Fig. 4b, r = 0.36, p = 0.005, padj = 0.09), agonistic behaviors (r = 0.37, p = 0.003, padj = 0.07), and other social behaviors given (r = 0.34, p = 0.009, padj > 0.10). However, the 2 latter correlations were strongly influenced by outliers, and did not resist adjustment for multiple comparisons after outliers were removed (agonistic behaviors given, 1 outlier, r = 0.25, p = 0.05, padj > 0.10; other social behaviors given, 2 outliers, r = 0.25, p = 0.05, padj > 0.10).
Consistency of individual positions (coordinates) on each PCA dimension
The coordinates of piglets at 21 days of age were positively correlated to the coordinates of piglets at 42 days for PC 1 ‘social exploration & social play’ (rs = 0.38, p = 0.003), but not for PC 2 ‘avoidance & receipt of exploration vs aggression’ (rs = 0.14, p > 0.10) or PC 3 ‘receipt of social nosing’ (rs = 0.04, p > 0.10).
Stability of piglets’ allocation to each cluster
Of the 60 piglets observed on each day, 48% of all piglets (29/60) remained in the same cluster on both observation days, but the stability of piglets’ allocation to a given cluster varied greatly between clusters. Indeed, 59% (17/29) of piglets classified in the cluster of ‘low-solicited inactive animals’ on day 21 and 65% (11/17) of piglets classified in the cluster of ‘socially active animals’ on day 21 remained in the same cluster on day 42, whereas, for the cluster of ‘highly-solicited avoiders’, only 7% (1/14) of the piglets remained in the same cluster on both days.
Associations between social behaviors and 5-HT
Blood levels of 5-HT at 21 days were positively correlated to 5-HT levels at 42 days, but the correlation was weak and did not resist adjustment for multiple comparisons (r = 0.29, p = 0.03, padj > 0.10). No significant correlations were found between 5-HT and social behaviors at either age (p > 0.05 for all), except for a weak negative correlation between 5-HT levels and the number of social play episodes at 21 days (r = − 0.25; p = 0.04, padj > 0.10).
Discussion
This study aimed to explore and characterize early social behaviors and social styles in suckling piglets raised with their littermates until 6 weeks of age, and to assess the stability of these social styles over time, and their associations with a variety of individual features, including non-social behaviors, morphological traits, and health-related blood parameters.
Descriptive analysis of social behaviors in suckling piglets
Descriptive analysis of behaviors showed that (putative) positive social behaviors, namely social nosing and social play, represented a large proportion of all behaviors, whereas the proportion of agonistic behaviors and other social behaviors were low at both ages. Accordingly, low incidence of agonistic interactions compared to non-agonistic social interactions has been reported in stable social groups of juvenile pigs7,8. Social nosing is assumed to promote social relationship maintenance and group cohesion and to play a role in the reduction of social tension through positive soothing effects5,6,11,14,39. Therefore, the high incidence of social nosing and the low incidence of agonistic behaviors reported in our study likely reflected strong social cohesion and affiliative relationships within litters in a calm and stable social context10,11,40, and was likely caused by optimal environmental conditions, including extra space and access to a foraging substrate7. Indeed, piglets in our study were housed with littermates, in large enclosure with outdoor access, which enabled them to escape fights, and likely resulted in fewer and less intense agonistic interactions.
Despite stable relative levels of social behaviors over time, the absolute levels of social activities increased with age, notably social play and social nosing. In line with our findings, an increase in time spent interacting with littermates (aggression, nose contact, mounting, and oral manipulation) with age has been reported in pre-weaning piglets41,42. More specifically, the greater number of social play episodes with age agrees with prior studies that showed a peak in social play between 2 and 6 weeks of age43, followed by a sharp decrease from 5 to 9 weeks of age in a semi-natural or enriched environment with saw bedding44. On the contrary, no effect of age was found on the frequency of social play in piglets raised in a poor environment characterized by restricted space16, suggesting that enriched environment may favor optimal play development. Social play, including rough-and-tumble play or ‘playfight’, is assumed to help piglets to develop a set of social skills that are important for optimal integration into future hierarchical social groups and for successful fighting and individual recognition18,19,31. Like social nosing2,6,13, early social play is thought to help maintain group cohesion2,10, and may thus be considered a putative positive social behavior important for long-term pig welfare on farms.
Remarkably, although males and females had the same activity levels in general, males were involved in more social interactions than females. Notably, males emitted more agonistic behaviors and other social behaviors, namely mounting and nudging, and were involved in significantly more social play episodes than females. Mounting is often considered a sexual behavior45, but is also assumed to play a role in the formation of dominance hierarchy46. In line with our findings, entire male pigs have been found to display more agonistic and sexual behaviors, such as mounting, than female pigs15,45, although contradictory findings are sometimes reported46. Furthermore, in a wide range of farm animals, including horses47, lambs48, and pigs19,49,50, males initiate more juvenile social play than females. Indeed, male pigs have been found to be involved more often in play fighting, pushing and nudging, than females during the pre-weaning period (7 to 27 days of age50, 14 to 26 days of age49), and after weaning (50 to 68 days of age19). These differences in play behaviors support the hypothesis that species in which males and females differ in their later-life social environment show sex differences in their early play behavior, especially fight-related play47,49,51. Altogether, our findings thus support the existence of sexual dimorphism in the early expression of social behaviors, notably play fight, mounting or aggression, in young piglets.
Identification and characterization of individual social styles
The multivariate analyses highlighted the existence of 3 distinct social styles. The largest cluster represented ‘low-solicited inactive animals’, which were involved in fewer social interactions than other animals in general. The diametrically opposed cluster was that of ‘socially-active animals’, which emitted more social play, social nosing, agonistic behaviors and other social behaviors than other animals. The smallest cluster represented ‘highly-solicited avoiders’, which was typically characterized by more avoidance behaviors in response to a high level of social solicitation, as suggested by great counts of received social-nosing, mounting and nudging.
The characterization of social styles differing in their levels of ‘social engagement’ (i.e. socially active vs inactive animals) agrees with prior studies in cattle which showed that high levels of socio-positive behaviors (social licking) are linked to greater levels of sociality overall, including agonistic behaviors25, suggesting that more affiliative or more aggressive animals are more social overall and vice versa. Remarkably, the social styles highlighted in our study were not only characterized by the level of emitted social behaviors, but also by the level of received social contacts (i.e. low-solicited vs highly-solicited animals). In line with our findings, ‘avoiders animals’ have been characterized by high levels of received aggression, while ‘passive animals’ received high levels of positive contacts in sheep and goats26–28. Altogether, these findings suggest that the amount and type of social solicitation received by an individual within a group influence its social responses and highlight the importance of considering both emitted and received interactions when studying social behavior. Furthermore, animals from low dominance status have been shown to have low participation in agonistic or non-agonistic interactions and to avoid conflicts in adult sows20, cows25, goats26,27 and ewes28, thus suggesting that dominance status may be an important determinant of social styles or strategies in farm animals. Further studies including assessment of dominance status should thus be considered to confirm whether ‘highly-solicited avoiders’ or ‘low-solicited inactive animals’ are primarily low-ranking animals.
In our study, the 3 distinct social styles were associated with a number of individual features. Notably, ‘low-solicited inactive animals’ were typically females, whereas ‘socially-active animals’, which exhibited high levels of play behavior, nudging and mounting, were predominantly males. Accordingly, males have been found to exhibit more social play, nudging, and mounting than females in suckling49,50 and weaned piglets19. Furthermore, as mentioned before, mounting in pigs has been described as both a sexual45 and a dominance behavior46 that is typically displayed by males. Therefore, the social styles highlighted in our study may be partly explained by sexual dimorphism in the expression of social behaviors associated with sex or dominance.
Besides sex, social styles were associated with behavioral and physiological parameters. Indeed, ‘low-solicited inactive animals’ spent less time standing, and more time lying than animals from other clusters, although this effect was only observed in the sub-groups of females. In line with our findings, inactivity has been associated with little social nosing in weaned piglets13, and a passive social style characterized by low incidence of emitted non-agonistic and agonistic interactions and a high proportion of time spent resting (lying) has been reported in goats26,27. Remarkably, ‘low-solicited inactive animals’ also had higher levels of haptoglobin compared to ‘socially active animals’. Concentration of serum haptoglobin, a major porcine acute phase protein, rises rapidly in response to infection, and is thus an important marker of health in pigs33. A rise in acute phase protein and inflammatory cytokine levels, paired with a decline in social motivation and general activity is assumed to be indicative of ‘sickness behavior’ in pigs, as evidenced by studies using lipopolysaccharide-induced inflammatory challenges32,52. The cluster of ‘low-solicited inactive animals’ may thus be typically composed of (female) animals of bad health status or suffering from sub-clinical infection. Alternatively, accumulated evidence suggest that acute phase proteins may be released not only following an infection, but also in response to physical or psychological stressors53–55, and may thus be a promising indicator of stress13. Therefore, clusters in our study may partly reflect individual differences in health status, stress level or both on a given day. Remarkably, we reported a relative instability in the piglets’ allocation to a given social cluster over time, with only 48% of piglets belonging to the same clusters at 3 and 6 weeks of age. Together with the association between clusters and inflammation markers, this instability supports the postulate that our clusters may not depict stable sociality traits, but rather transient social mood reflective of the individual’s psychological and physiological health at a specific time point.
The stability of piglets’ allocation to a given cluster varied greatly between clusters, with relatively high levels of stability for ‘low-solicited inactive animals’ (59%) and ‘socially active animals’ (65%), and very low stability (7%) for the ‘highly-solicited avoiders’. The low stability of the cluster of ‘highly-solicited avoiders’ may be explained by the “intermediate” position of animals from this cluster in terms of health-related parameters (haptoglobin, growth rate) or behaviors (agonistic behaviors given and received, other social behaviors given, social play), that were major discriminant factors of the two other clusters. Therefore, piglets from the ‘highly-solicited avoiders’ cluster may easily move from their cluster to others. Furthermore, when considering position of animals on each PCA axis, we reported positive correlations between ages for PC 1, that represented ‘social engagement’ or ‘sociability’, but not for other PCs which represented other social traits, such as ‘aggressiveness’ and ‘avoidance’, or passive social involvement. While supporting the postulate that different social behaviors reflect distinct dimensions of sociality, as suggested by others9,56, our results also suggest that the level of ‘sociability’, may be a stable social trait in young piglets, contrarily to ‘avoidance’ or ‘aggressiveness’. As mentioned earlier, appropriate responses to aggression (avoidance vs fight) required for successful conflict resolution later in life is likely acquired through early play fight experience18,19. We therefore posit that the development of the ‘avoidance’ and ‘aggressiveness’ traits in young piglets is under the influence of early life experience, thus explaining their instability in young, developing individuals.
Like individual position on the PCA axes, some social behaviors showed relative consistency with age when analyzed independently. Notably, animals involved in the greatest count of social play behaviors at 3 weeks were also the ones playing the most at 6 weeks, maybe because this behavior was highly influenced by the sex of the animals. In line with our findings, counts of play behaviors (jumping, turning and running) and time spent displaying these behaviors during a ‘play session’ have been found to be fairly consistent between 2 consecutive play sessions in 44- to 48-day-old piglets, suggesting that playfulness could be an inherent individual trait17. Furthermore, in our study, piglets that were nosing pen mates the most at 3 weeks were also the ones nosing pen mates the most at 6 weeks. As no strong intra-individual associations were found for other social behaviors, we posit that playfulness and social nosing may represent valid and stable indicators of early individual social styles in young piglets raised in enriched environments, and deserve more research effort.
Finally, and contrarily to our expectations, levels of 5-HT in platelet-rich plasma were not associated to any social styles or behaviors in our study, except for a very weak negative association with the count of social play episodes. The motivation to play during daily play sessions has been found to be negatively correlated to the rise in peripheral 5-HT levels before and after a play session in minipigs, suggesting that the more the piglets are willing to play, the less the 5-HT concentrations rise57. In line with this finding, our result may support the postulate of a negative association between play behavior and 5-HT levels in pigs. Further investigation in various contexts of play is needed to determine whether peripheral 5-HT level is a promising candidate to characterize social play, and positive affective states, in young piglets.
Conclusion
To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to characterize social styles in suckling piglets. Our findings suggest the existence of individual behavioral patterns that seem reflective of transient social styles, and that are behaviorally and physiologically distinct. Although individuals’ allocation to a given social style was not very stable over time, the ‘sociability’ trait and positive social interactions (social nosing and play) appeared to be stable social traits in young developing pigs, contrarily to dominance or sex-related social traits (mounting, avoidance and agonistic behaviors). Altogether, these findings bring innovative insights into early individual social styles in pigs. Further research should consider investigating to what extent piglets with divergent early social styles differ in their ability to adapt to weaning, a stressful social challenge on pig farms.
Methods
Ethical notes
The project was conducted at the French National Institute INRAE organic pig farm Porganic (Rouillé, France) from May to October 2020 in compliance with the current ethical standards of the European Community (Directive 2010/63/EU). The experimental procedures used in this study were approved by the Regional Ethics Committee in Animal Experiments of Poitou Charente (n°084, December 18, 2019) and by the French Ministry of Higher Education and Research (2019071611422718Apafis21892). This research was done in compliance with the Animal Research: Reporting of In Vivo Experiments (ARRIVE) guidelines and regulations (https://arriveguidelines.org).
Animals and housing
Piglets (Large White × Piétrain) from 16 litters (15 ± 0.6 piglets born alive, [range: 10–20]; male:female ratio = 1.4) were studied in 2 consecutive cohorts, with 6-week intervals between cohorts and 8 litters per cohort. All piglets were kept with the sows and housed in 10 m2 individual farrowing pens, located within the same maternity unit, from birth until 48 days of age. From 11 days of age, sows and their piglets had free access to a 6.25-m2 outdoor area. Sows were loose and pens were equipped with a heated nest only accessible to the piglets, and had a straw-bedding floor. Fresh straw was added daily. Male piglets were not castrated, teeth were not clipped and tails were kept intact. If needed, cross-fostering was done in the first 3 days of age to standardize litter size. Piglets received daily access to solid feed from 20 days of age onwards, and were weaned at 48 days of age.
Piglets did not receive iron injection because the experiment used piglets from a project that explored whether daily access to soil or peat during the suckling period could be an applicable alternative strategy to the early iron injection to prevent risks of anemia in suckling piglets raised in organic farms (European ERA-Net project). Therefore, at about 4 days of age, litters were randomly allocated to 1 of 2 treatments, with 4 litters per treatment per cohort, thus yielding a total of 8 litters per treatment in total. Piglets received daily access to a small amount of sterilized soil or peat from 4 to 48 days of age. Soil and peat were distributed daily at 9:00 in a small circular feeder located in the nest and not accessible to the sow. Daily rations of soil and peat per pen were 150 g from 4 to 12 days of age, 200 g from 13 to 26 days of age, and 250 g from 27 to 47 days of age.
Measurements
Per litter, 6 experimental piglets (3 males and 3 females) of medium birthweight (1.51 ± 0.03 kg, [0.82–2.32 kg]) were selected for blood sampling and behavioral observations, resulting in 96 experimental piglets in total. Adopted piglets and piglets with health problems (diseases or lameness) were excluded from the selection. If a piglet died between 2 observation days, it was replaced by a piglet from the same sex and litter.
Physiological measures
At 20 and 41 days of age, all piglets were weighed, and blood was collected from the 96 experimental piglets. The sampling procedure started at the same time (~ 09:00 AM) on both days, and blood from the piglets was collected in a random order. At 20 days of age, pigs were maintained on their back on a table by a trained experimenter, and at 41 days of age, pigs were restrained with a wire metal nose snare. The duration of the blood sampling procedure was measured, and took less than 2 min per pig, thus limiting any potential effect of stress on physiological and behavioral measures. Blood was collected into one 5-mL BD Vacutainer EDTA and one heparinized tube by jugular venipuncture and were kept on ice until processing.
Within one hour after sampling, platelets counts in whole blood (109 platelets/L) were measured in EDTA samples with a hematology automated cell counter for pigs (MS9; Melet Schloesing Laboratories, Osny, France). Whole blood EDTA samples were then centrifuged at 200 × g at room temperature for 10 min to obtain platelet-rich plasma (PRP). Samples of 200 µL of extracted PRP were added with 800 µL saline and centrifuged at 4500 × g for 10 min at 4 °C. The supernatants were retrieved, the pellets were resuspended in 200 µL of distilled water and PRP samples were stored at − 80 °C until 5-HT analyses. The 5-HT concentrations were determined using an ultra-performance liquid chromatography (UPLC) apparatus (Waters Acquity Ultra Performance LC, Waters, Milford, MA, USA) coupled to 2 detectors (Acquity Tunable UV detector and Mass SQD detector; Waters, Milford, MA, USA) after derivatization of samples using the AccQ Tag Ultra method (MassTrak AAA; Waters, Milford, MA, USA). Norvaline (Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Quentin Fallavier, France) was used as an internal standard. Concentrations of 5-HT were expressed in μmol/mL of PRP, and inter-assay CV was 11%.
Heparinized blood samples were centrifuged at 1800 × g for 10 min at 4 °C. Plasma was collected and stored at − 20 °C until analyses of inflammatory (haptoglobin) and oxidative stress markers (hydrogen peroxide and plasma antioxidant capacity). Haptoglobin (Tridelta Development Ltd, Maynooth, Ireland), and hydrogen peroxides generated by the peroxidation of lipids, proteins or nucleic acids (diacron Reactive Oxygen Metabolites , dROM kit, H&D srl, Parma, Italy) were assayed using commercial kits. The ferric reducing ability of plasma (FRAP) was assayed using a home-made assay, as previously described58. All measurements were performed in duplicates using a multianalyzer apparatus (Konelab 20i, ThermoFisher Scientific, Courtaboeuf, France). The minimum concentration detectable for haptoglobin was 0.033 mg/mL, and the intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation (CV) were 7 and 24%, respectively. Concentrations of dROM were expressed in CARRU (Carratelli Unit, 1 CARRU = 0.08 mg H2O2/100 mL of sample) and intra- and inter-assay CV were 6 and 8%, respectively. FRAP was expressed as molar Trolox equivalents/L.
Behavioral measures
Immediately after blood sampling, the experimental piglets were individually marked with a symbol sprayed on their back, and the next day, video were continuously recorded for the analysis of social behavior from 06:30 to 19:30 at 21 and 42 days of age. Social behaviors and non-social behavioral activities were scored during four 2-h sessions of observation (8:00–10:00, 11:00–13:00, 14:00–16:00, and 17:00–19:00) on both days.
Social behaviors were scored using the all-occurrence behavioral sampling method. A total of 5 categories of social behaviors were defined (Supplemental Table S1): social nosing behaviors (nosing snout, head or body), agonistic behaviors (head and shoulder knocks, aggressive bites), other social behaviors (mounting and nudging), avoidance behaviors (turning head or moving away from an interaction initiated by a pen mate, refusing play invite), and social play (play invite, mutual play-fight, chasing, climbing or pushing49). For social nosing, agonistic and other social behaviors, both the social interactions given and received by focal animals were scored, resulting in 8 behavioral categories in total.
Non-social behavioral activities were scored using a 5-min scan sampling during the four 2-h sessions of observation on both days, resulting in 96 samples per pig per day. A total of 4 categories of activities were scored: standing/kneeling active (exploring, playing, walking or running), lying/sitting active or inactive (exploring, resting), ingestive behaviors (drinking, feeding), suckling or massaging the udder. Supplemental Table S2 shows the detailed behavioral repertoire.
Statistical analyses
Data analysis was conducted using the statistical R version 4.0.059 with the packages ‘lme4’ version 1.1-26, ‘FactoMineR’ version 2.3, ‘factorextra’ version 1.0.7, and ‘ggplot2’ version 3.3.1.
Descriptive analysis of behavioral data
Of the 96 experimental pigs from 16 litters, 4 litters were excluded (2 due to high mortality between 21 and 42 days of age, 1 due to piglets being weaned before 42 days of age, and 1 because the video recordings failed), thus resulting in a sample of 12 litters (21 days: 11 ± 0.5 piglets per litter; 42 days: 10 ± 0.6 piglets per litter). Furthermore, 6 piglets (4 at 21 days and 3 at 42 days of age) could not be identified on the video recordings (marking was erased) and were thus excluded from the observations, resulting in a final sample of 68 piglets at 21 days and 69 piglets at 42 days of age.
Behavioral data were analyzed with linear mixed models including the fixed effects of day, sex and treatment, and the random effect of pen nested within batch. Interactions between fixed effects were not significant and were, therefore, not included in the final model. Model residuals were visually inspected for normality and homoscedasticity, and if the residuals did not meet the assumptions for normal distribution and equality of variances, response variables were transformed (square-root transformation for count data and arcsin transformation for proportion data). Data are presented as means ± SEM.
Identification of early social styles
A global principal component analysis (PCA) was done to analyze the correlational structure between the 8 social behavioral classes at 21 and 42 days of age. Only the 60 animals (29 females and 31 males) that were observed at both 21 and 42 days of age were included in the PCA. The behavioral items included as active variables in the PCA were the count of agonistic behaviors given and received, the count of social nosing behaviors given and received, the count of other social behaviors given and received, the count of social play episodes and avoidance behaviors. The total number of social interactions was added as a supplementary quantitative variable, and sex, day and treatment were added as qualitative supplementary variables. All active variables were subjected to a linear model with pen and batch as fixed effect to obtain residuals used for the PCA. The criteria for extracting principal components were an eigenvalue > 1 and a minimum cumulative variance of 50%. The extracted principal components were described with the variable residuals with loadings > 0.50 or < ‒0.50. A hierarchical clustering on principal components (HCPC) was performed on the principal components extracted from the PCA to identify clustered groups of pigs based on social behaviors. The Euclidean distance was used between individuals and the Ward’s criterion was applied as clustering method60.
Characterization of clusters with non-social individual parameters
Statistical tests were computed using the HCPC function of the ‘FactorMineR’ package to identify the principal components and variables used in the multivariate analyses that characterized the clusters. Principal components and the supplementary quantitative variable (total number of social interactions) characterizing the clusters were identified using v-tests – a v-test greater than 1.96 or less than − 1.96 corresponds to a p value less than 0.05, and the sign of the v-test indicates if the mean of the cluster is lower or greater than the overall mean of all clusters60. Qualitative variables (sex, day, treatment) characterizing the clusters were identified using, first, a chi-square test (χ2) and, then, hypergeometric tests to determine which clusters were characterized by each category of the qualitative variables60,61. Because clusters were not significantly associated with treatment in these tests (χ2, p > 010), treatment effects were not discussed further.
The association between clusters and the quantitative variables that were not used for the PCA and HCPC (non-social behavioral traits, production traits and blood parameters) was assessed with one-way ANOVA using mixed linear models including the fixed effects of cluster, sex, treatment and their two-by-two interactions, and the random effects of day and pen nested within batch. Treatment (daily access to soil or peat) or its interaction with cluster or sex had no effect on any variables, and was thus excluded from the final model. Model residuals were visually inspected for normality and homoscedasticity, and if these assumptions were not met, models were performed on transformed variables (square-root transformation for count data, and arcsin square-root transformation for proportion data). Data are presented as LS-means ± S.E.M., unless stated otherwise.
Stability of social-related traits
A series of correlation tests were conducted to assess the constancy of social-related parameters (social behaviors given, blood levels of 5-HT, and individual positions on PCA axes) over time. Raw variables (social behaviors and 5-HT levels) were first subjected to a linear model with pen and batch as fixed effects to obtain residuals, as previously described by Ursinus et al.38. Because model residuals were normally distributed, parametric Pearson’s correlation tests were conducted on the variable residuals. Both p values and adjusted p values corrected for multiple comparisons using the Holm method are shown. The stability of individual coordinates on each of the 3 extracted principal components was assessed with non-parametric Spearman’s correlation tests.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
The authors thank R. Comte, D. Grivault, M. Oiry and C. Robert for their technical assistance in the laboratory, with a special thanks to C. Mustière and F. Thomas for developing the method for 5-HT analysis. The authors gratefully acknowledge the personnel from the ‘Porganic’ experimental organic station, led by S. Ferchaud, for taking excellent care of the animals and for their help in field measures, with a special thanks to T. Terrasson for managing video material and recordings.
Author contributions
C.C., E.M., A.P. and C.T. designed the study, C.C. collected and analyzed the behavioral data; R.R. and C.C. performed the statistical analyses; C.C. drafted and revised the manuscript; A.P., E.M., R.R. and C.T. reviewed the manuscript.
Funding
This study was part of the POWER project (Power to strengthen welfare and resilience in organic pig production) supported by the ERA-NET CORE (Coordination of European Translational Research in Organic Food and Farming Systems) Organic Cofund. The study received additional internal funding from the department of Animal Physiology and Livestock of the National Research Institute for Agriculture, Food and Environment (INRAE).
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-06354-w.
References
- 1.Graves HB. Behavior and ecology of wild and feral swine (Sus Scrofa) J. Anim. Sci. 1984;58:482–492. doi: 10.2527/jas1984.582482x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Keeling LJ, Gonyou HW. Social behaviour in farm animals. CABI Publishing; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ewbank R. Social hierarchy in suckling and fattening pigs—review. Livest. Prod. Sci. 1976;3:363–372. doi: 10.1016/0301-6226(76)90070-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Rault JL. Friends with benefits: social support and its relevance for farm animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012;136:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.10.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Rault JL. Be kind to others: prosocial behaviours and their implications for animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019;210:113–123. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.10.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Boissy A, et al. Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol. Behav. 2007;92:375–397. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Beattie VE, O'Connell NE, Moss BW. Influence of environmental enrichment on the behaviour, performance and meat quality of domestic pigs. Livest. Prod. Sci. 2000;65:71–79. doi: 10.1016/s0301-6226(99)00179-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Oostindjer M, van den Brand H, Kemp B, Bolhuis JE. Effects of environmental enrichment and loose housing of lactating sows on piglet behaviour before and after weaning. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011;134:31–41. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Camerlink I, Proßegger C, Kubala D, Galunder K, Rault J-L. Keeping littermates together instead of social mixing benefits pig social behaviour and growth post-weaning. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021;235:105230. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105230. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Newberry RC, Woodgush DGM. Social relationships of piglets in a seminatural environment. Anim. Behav. 1986;34:1311–1318. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(86)80202-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Camerlink I, Turner SP. The pig's nose and its role in dominance relationships and harmful behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013;145:84–91. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.02.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Clouard, C. & Bolhuis, E., Jantina. Olfactory behaviour in farm animals. In Olfaction in animal behaviour and welfare (ed Nielsen Birte, L.) (CABI, 2017).
- 13.Camerlink I, Turner SP, Ursinus WW, Reimert I, Bolhuis JE. Aggression and affiliation during social conflict in pigs. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e113502. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Uvnas-Moberg K. Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998;23:819–835. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00056-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Newberry RC, Woodgush DGM. Development of some behavior patterns in piglets under semi-natural conditions. Anim. Prod. 1988;46:103–109. doi: 10.1017/s0003356100003160. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Chaloupkova H, Illmann G, Bartos L, Spinka M. The effect of pre-weaning housing on the play and agonistic behaviour of domestic pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007;103:25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.04.020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rauw WM. A note on the consistency of a behavioral play marker in piglets. J. Anim. Sci. Biotechnol. 2013 doi: 10.1186/2049-1891-4-33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Horback K. Nosing around: play in pigs. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2014;1:186–196. doi: 10.12966/abc.05.08.2014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dobao MT, Rodriganez J, Silio L. Choice of companions in social play in piglets. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1985;13:259–266. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(85)90049-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Mendl M, Zanella AJ, Broom DM. Physiological and reproductive correlates of behavioral strategies in female domestic pigs. Anim. Behav. 1992;44:1107–1121. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80323-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Mendl M, Deag JM. How useful are the concepts of alternative strategy and coping strategy in applied studies of social behavior. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1995;44:119–137. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(95)00609-v. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Finkemeier MA, Langbein J, Puppe B. Personality research in mammalian farm animals: Concepts, measures, and relationship to welfare. Front. Vet. Sci. 2018 doi: 10.3389/fvets.2018.00131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Koolhaas JM, Van Reenen CG. Animal behavior and well-being symposium: Interaction between coping style/personality, stress, and welfare: Relevance for domestic farm animals. J. Anim. Sci. 2016;94:2284–2296. doi: 10.2527/jas.2015-0125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lecorps B, Kappel S, Weary DM, von Keyserlingk MAG. Dairy calves' personality traits predict social proximity and response to an emotional challenge. Sci. Rep. 2018 doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-34281-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mulleder C, Palme R, Menke C, Waiblinger S. Individual differences in behaviour and in adrenocortical activity in beef-suckler cows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2003;84:167–183. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2003.08.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Miranda-de la Lama GC, Sepulveda WS, Montaldo HH, Maria GA, Galindo F. Social strategies associated with identity profiles in dairy goats. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011;134:48–55. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.06.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Pascual-Alonso M, et al. Identity profiles based on social strategies, morphology, physiology, and cognitive abilities in goats. J. Vet. Behav.-Clin. Appl. Res. 2013;8:458–465. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2013.05.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Miranda-de la Lama GC, et al. Social personality in sheep: can social strategies predict individual differences in cognitive abilities, morphology features, and reproductive success? J. Vet. Behav.-Clin. Appl. Res. 2019;31:82–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2019.03.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hayne SM, Gonyou HW. Effects of regrouping on the individual behavioural characteristics of pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2003;82:267–278. doi: 10.1016/s0168-1591(03)00084-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Samarakone TS, Gonyou HW. Domestic pigs alter their social strategy in response to social group size. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009;121:8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.08.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kristensen HH, Jones RB, Schofield C, White RP, Wathes CM. The use of olfactory and other cues for social recognition by juvenile pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2001;72:321–333. doi: 10.1016/s0168-1591(00)00209-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Munsterhjelm C, et al. Sick and grumpy: changes in social behaviour after a controlled immune stimulation in group-housed gilts. Physiol. Behav. 2019;198:76–83. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Chen HH, et al. Serum acute phase proteins and swine health status. Can. J. Vet. Res. 2003;67:283–290. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Young SN, Moskowitz DS. Serotonin and affiliative behavior. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005;28:367–368. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05390066. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Insel TR, Winslow JT. Serotonin and neuropeptides in affiliative behaviors. Biol. Psychiatry. 1998;44:207–219. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00094-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Leon M, et al. Assessment of serotonin in serum, plasma, and platelets of aggressive dogs. J. Vet. Behav.-Clin. Appl. Res. 2012;7:348–352. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2012.01.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Poletto R, Meisel RL, Richert BT, Cheng HW, Marchant-Forde JN. Behavior and peripheral amine concentrations in relation to ractopamine feeding, sex, and social rank of finishing pigs. J. Anim. Sci. 2010;88:1184–1194. doi: 10.2527/jas.2008-1576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Ursinus WW, Van Reenen CG, Reimert I, Bolhuis JE. Tail biting in pigs: blood serotonin and fearfulness as pieces of the puzzle? PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e107040. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sato S, Tarumizu K, Hatae K. The influence of social factors on allogrooming in cows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1993;38:235–244. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(93)90022-h. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Temple D, Manteca X, Velarde A, Dalmau A. Assessment of animal welfare through behavioural parameters in Iberian pigs in intensive and extensive conditions. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011;131:29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.01.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Clouard C, Stokvis L, Bolhuis JE, van Hees HMJ. Short communication: insoluble fibres in supplemental pre-weaning diets affect behaviour of suckling piglets. Animal. 2018;12:329–333. doi: 10.1017/s1751731117001501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cox LN, Cooper JJ. Observations on the pre- and post-weaning behaviour of piglets reared in commercial indoor and outdoor environments. Anim. Sci. 2001;72:75–86. doi: 10.1017/s1357729800055570. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Newberry RC, Woodgush DGM, Hall JW. Playful behavior of piglets. Behav. Processes. 1988;17:205–216. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(88)90004-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Bolhuis JE, Schouten WGP, Schrama JW, Wiegant VM. Behavioural development of pigs with different coping characteristics in barren and substrate-enriched housing conditions. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005;93:213–228. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.01.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Rydhmer L, et al. Aggressive and sexual behaviour of growing and finishing pigs reared in groups, without castration. Acta Agric. Scand. Sect. A-Anim. Sci. 2006;56:109–119. doi: 10.1080/09064700601079527. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mesarec N, Skok J, Škorjanc D, Prevolnik Povše M. Group dynamics in a spontaneously established group of newly weaned piglets. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021;238:105317. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105317. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cameron EZ, Linklater WL, Stafford KJ, Minot EO. Maternal investment results in better foal condition through increased play behaviour in horses. Anim. Behav. 2008;76:1511–1518. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Sachs BD, Harris VS. Sex differences and developmental changes in selected juvenile activities (play) of domestic lambs. Anim. Behav. 1978;26:678–684. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(78)90134-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Weller JE, Camerlink I, Turner SP, Farish M, Arnott G. Socialisation and its effect on play behaviour and aggression in the domestic pig (Sus scrofa) Sci. Rep. 2019 doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-40980-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Brown SM, Klaffenbock M, Nevison IM, Lawrence AB. Evidence for litter differences in play behaviour in pre-weaned pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015;172:17–25. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2015.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Meaney MJ. The sexual differentiation of social play. Trends Neurosci. 1988;11:54–58. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(88)90164-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Clouard C, et al. Maternal fish oil supplementation affects the social behavior, brain fatty acid profile, and sickness response of piglets. J. Nutr. 2015;145:2176–2184. doi: 10.3945/jn.115.214650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Murata H, Shimada N, Yoshioka M. Current research on acute phase proteins in veterinary diagnosis: an overview. Vet. J. 2004;168:28–40. doi: 10.1016/s1090-0233(03)00119-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Salamano G, et al. Changes in haptoglobin, C-reactive protein and pig-MAP during a housing period following long distance transport in swine. Vet. J. 2008;177:110–115. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Pineiro M, et al. Characterisation of the pig acute phase protein response to road transport. Vet. J. 2007;173:669–674. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2006.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Forkman B, Furuhaug IL, Jensen P. Personality, coping patterns and aggression in piglets. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1995;45:31–42. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(95)00601-n. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Rius MM, et al. Selection of putative indicators of positive emotions triggered by object and social play in mini-pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018;202:13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.02.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Sierżant K, Perruchot MH, Merlot E, Le Floc'h N, Gondret F. Tissue-specific responses of antioxidant pathways to poor hygiene conditions in growing pigs divergently selected for feed efficiency. BMC Vet. Res. 2019;15:341. doi: 10.1186/s12917-019-2107-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Team, R. C. R: A language and environment for statistical computing <https://www.r-project.org/> (2020).
- 60.Husson, F., Josse, J. & Pagès, J. Principal component methods - hierarchical clustering - partitional clustering: why would we need to choose for visualizing data? Techn. Rep. (2010).
- 61.Lê S, Josse J, Husson F. FactoMineR: A R package for multivariate analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 2008;25(1):1–18. doi: 10.18637/jss.v025.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.