Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2021 Oct 10;61(2):871–884. doi: 10.1007/s00394-021-02688-9

Interactions between dietary patterns and genetic factors in relation to incident dementia among 70-year-olds

Jessica Samuelsson 1,, Jenna Najar 1,2, Ola Wallengren 3, Silke Kern 1,2, Hanna Wetterberg 1, Madeleine Mellqvist Fässberg 1, Henrik Zetterberg 4,5,6,7, Kaj Blennow 4,5, Lauren Lissner 8, Elisabet Rothenberg 9, Ingmar Skoog 1,2,#, Anna Zettergren 1,#
PMCID: PMC8854136  PMID: 34632537

Abstract

Purpose

To investigate potential interactions between dietary patterns and genetic factors modulating risk for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) in relation to incident dementia.

Methods

Data were derived from the population-based Gothenburg H70 Birth Cohort Studies in Sweden, including 602 dementia-free 70-year-olds (examined 1992–93, or 2000–02; 64% women) followed for incident dementia until 2016. Two factors from a reduced rank regression analysis were translated into dietary patterns, one healthy (e.g., vegetables, fruit, and fish) and one western (e.g., red meat, refined cereals, and full-fat dairy products). Genetic risk was determined by APOE ε4 status and non-APOE AD-polygenic risk scores (AD-PRSs). Gene–diet interactions in relation to incident dementia were analysed with Cox regression models. The interaction p value threshold was < 0.1.

Results

There were interactions between the dietary patterns and APOE ε4 status in relation to incident dementia (interaction p value threshold of < 0.1), while no evidence of interactions were found between the dietary patterns and the AD-PRSs. Those with higher adherence to a healthy dietary pattern had a reduced risk of dementia among ε4 non-carriers (HR: 0.77; 95% CI: 0.61; 0.98), but not among ε4 carriers (HR: 0.86; CI: 0.63; 1.18). Those with a higher adherence to the western dietary pattern had an increased risk of dementia among ε4 carriers (HR: 1.37; 95% CI: 1.05; 1.78), while no association was observed among ε4 non-carriers (HR: 0.99; CI: 0.81; 1.21).

Conclusions

The results of this study suggest that there is an interplay between dietary patterns and APOE ε4 status in relation to incident dementia.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s00394-021-02688-9.

Keywords: Dietary pattern, Nutrition, Dementia, Apolipoprotein E genotype, Polygenic risk score

Introduction

Genetic and lifestyle factors influence the risk of developing dementia [1, 2]. Diet is one of the modifiable lifestyle factors thought to affect risk [3, 4], but whether there is an interplay with genetic risk factors is unclear [5, 6]. Several nutrients and foods have been linked with the risk of developing dementia [7]. However, since foods are eaten in combination and contain multiple nutrients that might influence risk, there has been a shift from investigating single nutrients and foods towards investigating the impact of dietary patterns [8]. Healthier Mediterranean-style dietary patterns (MeDi), rich in foods such as wholegrain products, vegetables, pulses, fruits and berries, nuts and seeds, fish and seafood have been associated with reduced risk of dementia [9, 10], while western style dietary patterns with a higher content of foods such as red and processed meat, full-fat dairy products, refined cereal products, sweets and high-sugar drinks have been associated with increased risk [7, 8, 11, 12]. However, people often eat a combination of healthy and unhealthy diets, and the cumulative effects of various nutrients and foods may affect risk differently depending on the combinations [13]. Studying the effect of different dietary patterns could, therefore, be useful to increase the understanding of food and nutrient combinations that may prevent or delay the onset of dementia.

The apolipoprotein E (APOE) gene is the strongest genetic factor modulating risk for AD and dementia [14]. This gene has three common alleles, the protective allele APOE ε2, the neutral allele APOE ε3, and the risk allele APOE ε4 [14]. Through large genome-wide association studies (GWASs), additional AD-risk-modifying genetic variants have been identified [15]. These genetic variants have lower effect sizes and are often combined into polygenic risk scores (PRSs) [16]. Population-based studies have shown that PRSs for AD are associated with AD and all-cause dementia [14, 17, 18], with disease progression [19], and with AD-pathology [17, 2022].

It has been suggested that risk reducing effects from foods may differ depending on an individual’s genetic risk profile [2325]. However, previous studies investigating interactions between dietary patterns and genetic risk factors in relation to cognitive function and dementia are limited (especially when it comes to non-APOE PRSs) and results are inconclusive, showing either no gene-diet interactions or risk reducing effects among either carriers or non-carriers of genetic risk factors [23, 2527]. Results from studies investigating interactions between genetic risk factors (usually APOE) and single foods or nutrients showed similar contradictive results [2831].

The aim of this study was to investigate potential interactions between dietary patterns and genetic factors modulating risk for AD (i.e., APOE ε4 status and non-APOE AD-PRSs) in relation to incident dementia among 70-year-olds.

Materials and methods

Data were derived from the ongoing population-based Gothenburg H70 birth cohort studies that started in 1971 [32], including the Population Study of Women in Gothenburg that started in 1968 [33]. Adults aged 70 years and living in Gothenburg at the time of selection were systematically selected based on birth dates. The Gothenburg H70 birth cohort studies include a wide range of examinations such as genetic, somatic, cognitive, psychiatric, and dietary examinations [34]. This study includes participants born 1922 and 1930, who were examined 1992–93 (born 1922) or 2000–02 (born 1930) and re-examined in 2000, 2005, 2009 and 2015–16. Dementia diagnosis for participants that were lost to follow-up between 2009 and 2015–16 were based on information from the Swedish inpatient registry until 2012 (there were no registry data on dementia diagnosis between 2012 and 2016 available). Information on deaths during follow-up was obtained from the Swedish population registry until 2016. There were 500 (66% response rate) systematically selected 70-year-olds that participated in the 1992–93 examination, and the dietary examination was performed on a subsample of these participants (n = 199). There were 604 (71% response rate) systematically selected 70-year-olds that participated in the 2000–02 examination, and all of them were invited to take part of the dietary examination (n = 554 participated). Out of the 1104 participants with baseline examinations in either 1992–93 (born 1922) or 2000–02 (born 1930), there were a total of 753 (296 men and 457 women) with dietary data. Blood sampling for genetic analyses was performed in 2000–02. Out of the 753 participants with dietary data, there were 616 (393 women and 223 men) participants with genotype data and 615 (392 women and 223 men) with dementia data. The final sample comprised 602 (218 men and 384 women) participants who were free from dementia at baseline examinations, see sample flowchart in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Sample flowchart

Dietary examination procedure

Information on dietary intake was obtained at baseline examinations (1992–93 for those born 1922, and 2000–02 for those born 1930) with the diet history method (DH) [35, 36]. The DH used in this study was a semi-structured face-to-face interview, estimating food intake during the preceding 3 months. Trained registered dietitians performed the interviews at the participants own home or at the clinic. The protocols for the interviews consist of a meal-pattern interview, accompanied by a food list with questions on usual frequencies and portion sizes of foods. Pictures of foods from the Swedish National Food Agency (NFA) were used during the interviews to estimate individual portion sizes. Dietary intake was registered in grams of food items usually consumed per day/week/month in the NFA’s nutrient database (PC-kost) in 2000–02. The DH method has been validated and described in detail previously [3537].

Dietary variables and construction of dietary patterns

Mean daily energy, nutrient and food intake were calculated based on results from the DH interview. Reported food intake was placed into 30 food groups based on similarity of nutritional properties and biological classifications [38] (Supplementary Table 1) and transformed with Box–Cox transformation to normalize the distribution. The Box–Cox transformation is a family of power transform functions that are used to stabilize variance and make a dataset approach normal distribution. Dietary patterns were derived with reduced rank regression analysis (RRR) on the total dietary sample (n = 753). The RRR analysis was performed in SAS version 9.4 to reduce the data into factors representing dietary patterns with the 30 food groups as predictor variables [39]. The SAS code used for the RRR analysis has been described in detail previously [39]. The response variables in the RRR analysis were estimated vitamin E, C, folate, fibre, polyunsaturated fatty acids, saturated fatty acids, and alcohol intake (from the DH interview). These nutrients were selected as response variables based on potential associations with dementia risk. Vitamin E, C, folate, fibre, and polyunsaturated fatty acids have been associated with reduced risk of dementia, whereas saturated fatty acids and higher alcohol intake with increased risk [3, 4046]. The aim of RRR is to explain as much variation of the response variables as possible, by simultaneously reducing the dimensionality of the predictor variables [47, 48]. We chose the RRR approach since it allows us to explore dietary patterns in this population while still considering a priori knowledge about nutrients that have been associated with incident dementia. Data on nutritional supplements were not available.

Two out of five factors from the RRR analysis could be translated into dietary patterns based on factor loading thresholds ≤ − 0.20 and ≥ 0.20 (Table 1). The factor loading thresholds were chosen based on previous studies where dietary patterns have been derived with RRR [4951]. Factor loadings in between − 0.2 and 0.2 (close to 0) can be considered weak and do not explain the variation in the response variables well. The additional three factors were not examined further since they did not add to explaining the variation in the specified response variables (food group factor loadings were in between ≤ − 0.20 and ≥ 0.20 for most food groups). Factor 1 loaded high on foods found in healthier dietary patterns (e.g., MeDi) such as vegetables, pulses, fruits, berries, fibre-rich bread, fish and seafood, and a higher adherence correlated strongest (of the two dietary patterns) with higher intakes of vitamin C, E, folate, fibre, and polyunsaturated fatty acids (Table 2). Factor 1 was, therefore, labelled the “healthy” dietary pattern (Table 1). Factor 2 loaded high on foods found in less healthy western style dietary patterns such as read meat and processed red meat, refined bread, full-fat dairy products and alcohol and low on vegetables, pulses, fruits and berries, and a higher adherence correlated strongest (of the two dietary patterns) with higher intakes of saturated fatty acids, alcohol, and with lower intakes of vitamin C, folate, and fibre (Table 2). Factor 2 was, therefore, labelled the “western” dietary pattern (Table 1).

Table 1.

Food item factor loadings for the “healthy” and the “western” dietary patterns derived by reduced rank regression analysis

Dietary patterns Healthy (factor 1) Western (factor 2)
Variation in responses (%) 33.3 14.9
Variation in predictors (%) 6.5 6.2
Fish and seafood 0.29* 0.11
Meat and processed meat 0.29* 0.35*
Poultry 0.17 − 0.03
Eggs 0.31* 0.18
Potatoes 0.30* 0.20*
Vegetables, pulses, nuts and seeds 0.53* − 0.50*
Fruits and berries 0.42* − 0.57*
Keyholea milk products 0.05 − 0.29*
Non-Keyholea milk products 0.08 0.21*
Cream and crème fraiche 0.16 0.26*
Cheese 0.34* 0.24*
Fast food and savoury bakery 0.13 0.08
Pasta, rice, and food grain 0.15 − 0.13
Bread refined ≤ 5% fibre content 0.10 0.31*
Bread fibre-rich > 5% fibre content 0.39* − 0.13
Cereals 0.27* − 0.15
Sweet bakery 0.22* 0.22*
Desserts 0.25* 0.02
Sweet condiments 0.29* 0.16
Sweets, candy, and chocolate 0.11 0.14
Soups 0.18 − 0.13
Sauces, dressings, and condiment 0.18 0.05
Margarine 0.36* 0.50*
Butter 0.04 0.19
Vegetable oil 0.28* − 0.08
Juice 0.28* − 0.17
Coffee 0.04 0.08
Tea 0.18 − 0.10
Soda 0.13 0.15
Alcoholic beverages 0.28* 0.43*

The content of each food group can be found in supplementary file 1. The dietary patterns were derived with reduced rank regression analysis (RRR) on the total dietary sample (n = 753), producing an individual score for each participant

*Two factors from the RRR were translated into dietary patterns based on factor loading thresholds ≤ − 0.20 and ≥ 0.20

aKeyhole is the Swedish National Food Agency-labelling scheme, which guides healthy food choices. For milk and yogurt to meet the criteria for the Keyhole, fat content must be limited to a maximum of 0.7%, and for flavoured products there is an additional limit for sugars with a maximum of 9%

Table 2.

Correlations between extracted dietary pattern scores and response variables from the reduced rank regression analysis

Healthy dietary pattern
(factor 1)
Western dietary pattern
(factor 2)
Response variables
Vitamin E (mg/day) 0.67 0.13
Vitamin C (mg/day) 0.50 − 0.41
Folate (µg/day) 0.73 − 0.26
Saturated fatty acids (g/day) 0.45 0.60
Polyunsaturated fatty acids (g/day) 0.61 0.36
Fibre (g/day) 0.70 − 0.39
Alcohol (g/day) 0.21 0.40

p values for all correlations were < 0.0001. The dietary patterns were derived with reduced rank regression analysis on the total dietary sample (n = 753), producing an individual score for each participant

Dementia diagnosis

Dementia was diagnosed at the examinations (1992–93, 2000–02, 2005, 2009, 2015–16) following the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third edition, Revised criteria [52], using information from comprehensive neuropsychiatric examinations, a battery of neuropsychological tests and information from close informants, described in detail previously [34]. The same method was used at all examinations for comparability over time. Age of dementia onset was based on information from close informants, the examinations, or the Swedish inpatient register. If no information of age at dementia onset could be obtained, the midpoint between examinations (last examination without dementia to first examination with dementia) was used.

Genotype data

Genotyping was performed with the NeuroChip (Illumina) [53]. QC included the removal of participants due to any of the following: per-sample call rate < 98%, sex mismatch, and excessive heterozygosity [FHET (F coefficient estimate for assessing heterozygosity) outside ± 0.2]. Samples were defined as non-European ancestral outliers, and removed, if their first two principal components (PCs) exceeded six standard deviations from the mean values of the European samples in the 1000 Genome global reference population. Closely related samples were removed based on pairwise PI_HAT (i.e., proportion of genome that are in identity-by-descent; calculated using –genome option in PLINK) ≥ 0.2. Further, markers were excluded due to per-single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) call rate < 98%, minor allele frequency (MAF) < 0.01, and Hardy–Weinberg disequilibrium (p < 1 × 10–6). The Sanger imputation service was used to impute post-QC, using the reference panel of Haplotype Reference Consortium data (HRC1.1) [18, 22]. The variants rs7412 and rs429358 (which define the ε2, ε3, and ε4 alleles) in the APOE gene were also genotyped with the KASPar® PCR SNP genotyping system (LGC Genomics, Hoddesdon, Herts, UK) or by mini sequencing, as previously described in detail [54].

Polygenic risk scores and APOE genotype

AD-PRSs were generated using stage 1 of the most recent AD GWAS including clinically defined AD phenotypes [55]. SNPs were selected using LD clumping. The European ancestry samples from the 1000-genomes project were used as reference panel to remove variants in LD, all variants 250 kb upstream and downstream of the top signal were removed (R2 < 0.001). All variants in the APOE region (chromosome 19, coordinates hg19: 44,412,079 to 46,412,079) were removed. In this study, we created PRSs including variants that surpassed four p value thresholds (p < 5e-8, p < 1e-5, p < 1e-3, p < 1e-1), referred to as 5e-8 AD-PRS (including 15 SNPs), 1e-5 AD-PRS (including 57 SNPs), 1e-3 AD-PRS (including 1333 SNPs), and 1e-1 AD-PRS (including 13 942 SNPs). All PRSs were calculated as the sum of the β-coefficient multiplied with the number (or dosage) of effect alleles of each SNP [18, 22]. The AD-PRSs scores were divided into tertiles and participants were categorised as having either low, middle, or high risk. APOE genotype was divided into ε4 carriers (ε4/ε2, ε4/ε3, or ε4/ ε4) and ε4 non-carriers (ε2/ ε2, ε3/ε3, or ε3 /ε2).

Potential confounders

Information on potential confounders were obtained through semi-structured interviews and health examinations at baseline examinations (1992–93 or 2000–02). Confounders were chosen a priori based on previous literature. Sex, energy intake, birth year, educational level, physical activity level, smoking, body mass index (BMI,), hypertension, diabetes mellitus and serum cholesterol levels were considered potential confounders. Energy intake was measured as kcal/day and BMI as kg/m2. Educational level was dichotomized into compulsory primary education (≤ 6 years for birth cohort 1922, ≤ 7 years for birth cohort 1930) versus more than that. Physical activity level was divided into three groups based on a modified Saltin–Grimby physical activity scale [56]: sedentary lifestyle (sedentary/low physical activity level), moderate physical activity level (low to moderate), or high activity level (moderate to high). Hypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure of ≥ 140 mmHg and/or a diastolic blood pressure of ≥ 90 mmHg (yes/no). Smoking was defined as either current smoker or non-smoker (never smoked or previous smoker). Diabetes mellitus was defined as a diagnosis told by a medical doctor, being on antidiabetic drugs, or having a venous blood glucose value of ≥ 11.1 mmol/L (yes/no). In the analyses including gene data, five principal components (PCs) were added as potential confounders to correct for population stratification (differences in allele frequencies due to genetic ancestry).

Statistical analyses

Chi-square and Mann–Whitney U tests (nominal and ordinal variables) and Student’s independent t tests (continuous variables) were performed to compare characteristics between participants with and without incident dementia.

The interaction variables APOE ε4 (carrier or non-carrier)*healthy dietary pattern (factor score 1) and APOE ε4 (carrier or non-carrier)*western dietary pattern (factor score 2) were calculated. The interaction variables AD-PRS (low, middle or high risk)*dietary pattern score (factor score 1) and AD-PRS (low, middle or high risk)*dietary pattern score (factor score 2) were calculated for all AD-PRSs (5e-8 AD-PRS*healthy dietary pattern, 1e-5 AD-PRS*healthy dietary pattern, 1e-3 AD-PRS*healthy dietary pattern, 1e-1 AD-PRS* healthy dietary pattern, 5e-8 AD-PRS*western dietary pattern, 1e-5 AD-PRS*western dietary pattern, 1e-3 AD-PRS*western dietary pattern and 1e-1 AD-PRS*western dietary pattern).

Cox regression analyses were performed in two models with the healthy and western dietary pattern scores (factor scores 1 and 2) as independent variables and incident dementia as the dependent variable. In model 1, the analyses were adjusted for sex and birth year. In model 2, the analyses were adjusted for sex, birth year, energy intake, BMI, serum cholesterol, diabetes, hypertension, smoking, education, and physical activity level. Further, Cox regression analyses were performed with the interaction variables as independent variables and incident dementia as the dependent variable. All interaction analyses were performed in two models. In model 1, the analyses were adjusted for sex and birth year. In model 2, the analyses were adjusted for sex, birth year, energy intake, BMI, serum cholesterol, diabetes, hypertension, smoking, education, and physical activity level, and 5 PCs to adjust for population stratification. Similar to other studies, we chose a p value threshold of < 0.1 to detect an interaction [26, 28, 29]. To facilitate interpretation of identified interactions and interaction models, effect values for the prediction scores were shown for each genetic group separately.

The time variable in all the Cox regression analyses was calculated as time in years from baseline examination (1992–93 or 2000–02) to either age at dementia diagnosis, age at death or time to end of study (Dec 31, 2016, for those with last examination year 2015–16, and Dec 31, 2012, for those with register data until 2012). Cox model proportional hazard assumptions were verified with Schoenfeld residuals.

The statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS STATISTICS 24. R programming versions 3.6.0 were used to test Cox model proportional hazard assumptions.

Results

Characteristics of the participants who were free from dementia at baseline and had both dietary and genetic data are presented for the total sample and stratified by incident dementia, yes/no in Table 3. During the mean follow-up time of 12.8 years (SD 4.5 years, 7685.5 person-years), 125 participants developed dementia. The mean age of dementia onset was 80.2 years (SD 4.9). There were no significant differences in characteristics between those that developed dementia and those that did not, except for a higher BMI (26 vs. 27) and a higher adherence to the healthy dietary pattern among those that did not develop dementia, and a higher percentage of APOE ε4 carriers among those that developed dementia (26 vs. 40%).

Table 3.

Characteristics of participants with dietary and genetic data

Total
(n = 602)
Incident dementia (n = 125) No incident dementia (n = 477) p valuesa
Mean (SD)
n = cases/total caseb
Mean (SD)
n = cases/total caseb
Mean (SD)
n = cases/total caseb
Age at baseline examination

70.6 (0.3)

602/602

70.6 (0.3)

125/125

70.6 (0.3)

477/477

0.49
5e-8 AD-PRS score

− 0.004 (0.958)

602/602

0.013 (0.909)

125/125

− 0.008 (0.972)

477/477

0.83
1e-5 AD-PRS score

− 0.034 (1.020)

602/602

− 0.012 (1.040)

125/125

− 0.039 (1.015)

477/477

0.79
1e-3 AD-PRS score

− 0.042 (1.009)

602/602

− 0.061 (1.124)

125/125

− 0.036 (0.978)

477/477

0.81
1e-1 AD-PRS score

− 0.0182 (1.008)

602/602

− 0.094 (1.027)

125/125

0.002 (1.003)

477/477

0.35
Healthy dietary pattern (factor score 1)

0.075 (1.253)

602/602

− 0.117 (1.315)

125/125

0.125 (1.233)

477/477

0.05

Western dietary pattern

(factor score 2)

− 0.039 (1.221)

602/602

− 0.050 (1.249)

125/125

− 0.036 (1.214)

477/477

0.91
Energy intake kcal/day

2182 (636)

602/602

2107 (579)

125/125

2202 (649)

477/477

0.14
BMI

26.8 (4.1)

597/602

26.1 (3.9)

124/125

27.0 (4.2)

473/477

0.03
Serum cholesterol mmol/l

5.9 (1.1)

599/602

6.0 (1.0)

125/125

5.9 (1.1)

474/477

0.45
%
n = cases/total caseb
%
n = cases/total caseb
%
n = cases/total caseb
APOE ε4 carriers

29

175/602

40

50/125

26

125/477

0.003
Sex (women)

64

384/602

70

88/125

62

296/477

0.08
Hypertensionc

80

484/602

77

96/125

81

388/477

0.26
Diabetes mellitus

14

81/602

13

16/125

14

65/477

0.81
Physical activity level 0.93
Sedentary/low

10

59/592

11

13/124

10

46/468

Low to moderate

46

270/592

44

55/124

46

215/468

Moderate to high

44

263/592

45

56/124

44

207/468

Educational level > compulsoryd

40

238/597

40

50/124

40

188/473

0.91
Current smoker

16

95/595

13

16/124

17

79/471

0.30

ap values for comparisons between those that developed dementia and those that did not

bTotal number of cases are presented for all characteristics since there are missing data for some of the variables

cHypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure of ≥ 140 mmHg and/or a diastolic blood pressure of ≥ 90 mmHg

dMore than 6 years for birth cohort 1922 and more than 7 years for birth cohort 1930

In the total population, we found no associations between adherence to the dietary patterns and incident dementia in either model 1 or model 2 (Table 4).

Table 4.

Associations between adherence to the dietary patterns and incident dementia in the total sample

Dietary patterns Model 1a p value Model 2a p value
Dementia n = 125/602b Dementia n = 121/573b
HR 95% CI HR 95% CI
Healthy (factor 1) 0.87 0.75; 1.02 0.08 0.83 0.67; 1.03 0.09
Western (factor 2) 1.04 0.89; 1.21 0.62 1.06 0.90; 1.25 0.48

HR and 95% CI estimated using a Cox proportional hazards model with follow-up time (years) as the time scale

aModel 1 is adjusted for sex and birth year. Model 2 is adjusted for sex, birth year, BMI, energy intake, serum cholesterol level, hypertension, diabetes, physical activity level, educational level, and smoking status

bDementia events/total cases. Those with missing data were excluded in model 2 (n = 29 with missing data, n = 5 for BMI, n = 3 for serum cholesterol, n=5 for education, n = 10 for physical activity level and n = 7 for smoking)

Interactions were observed between adherence to the dietary patterns and APOE ε4 status in relation to incident dementia (interaction p value threshold of < 0.1) (Table 5). For the healthy dietary pattern, this interaction was found in model 1, but not in the fully adjusted model 2. For the western dietary pattern, there was an interaction in both model 1 and the fully adjusted model 2 (Table 5).

Table 5.

Interactions between genetic risk factors and dietary patterns in relation to incident dementia

Interaction models Model 1a p value Model 2a p value
Dementia n = 125/602b Dementia n = 121/573b
HR CI HR CI
APOE ε4*healthy dietary pattern 1.29 0.96; 1.74 0.09 1.13 0.82; 1.54 0.47
Healthy dietary pattern 0.79 0.65; 0.95 0.01 0.77 0.61; 0.98 0.03
APOE ε4 status 1.83 1–27; 2.63 0.001 2.0 1.37; 2.91 0.0003
APOE ε4*western dietary pattern 1.31 0.96; 1.79 0.09 1.38 1.01; 1.89 0.05
Western dietary pattern 0.98 0.81; 1.18 0.81 0.99 0.81; 1.21 0.92
APOE ε4 status 1.87 1.31; 2.68 0.001 2.06 1.42; 3.00 0.0001
5e-8 AD-PRS*healthy dietary pattern 0.91 0.77; 1.08 0.29 0.97 0.81; 1.16 0.70
Healthy dietary pattern 1.05 0.72; 1.52 0.81 0.89 0.58; 1.37 0.60
5e-8 AD-PRS 0.98 0.79; 1.22 0.87 0.97 0.77; 1.21 0.77
1e-5 AD-PRS*healthy dietary pattern 0.92 0.78; 1.10 0.36 1.01 0.84; 1.22 0.90
Healthy dietary pattern 1.03 0.70; 1.52 0.88 0.82 0.52; 1.28 0.37
1e-5 AD-PRS 1.09 0.88; 1.36 0.44 1.12 0.89; 1.40 0.35
1e-3 AD-PRS*healthy dietary pattern 1.05 0.88; 1.26 0.59 1.02 0.84; 1.23 0.86
Healthy dietary pattern 0.79 0.54; 1.16 0.23 0.80 0.53; 1.23 0.31
1e-3 AD-PRS 1.05 0.85; 1.30 0.65 1.04 0.84; 1.30 0.70
1e-1 AD-PRS*healthy dietary pattern 0.99 0.82; 1.18 0.88 0.95 0.78; 1.14 0.56
Healthy dietary pattern 0.90 0.61; 1.31 0.57 0.92 0.61; 1.39 0.70
1e-1 AD-PRS 0.94 0.76; 1.17 0.59 0.98 0.78; 1.23 0.88
5e-8 AD-PRS*western dietary pattern 1.10 0.91; 1.32 0.32 1.07 0.89; 1.30 0.47
Western dietary pattern 0.87 0.58; 1.28 0.47 0.93 0.62; 1.39 0.72
5e-8 AD-PRS 1.00 0.81; 1.24 0.99 0.98 0.79; 1.23 0.88
1e-5 AD-PRS*western dietary pattern 1.05 0.87; 1.27 0.62 0.99 0.82; 1.20 0.96
Western dietary pattern 0.95 0.63; 1.42 0.79 1.07 0.71; 1.63 0.74
1e-5 AD-PRS 1.11 0.90; 1.39 0.33 1.12 0.90; 1.41 0.32
1e-3 AD-PRS*western dietary pattern 0.96 0.80; 1.16 0.68 0.98 0.81; 1.18 0.83
Western dietary pattern 1.12 0.76; 1.65 0.57 1.11 0.74; 1.65 0.62
1e-3 AD-PRS 1.05 0.85; 1.30 0.65 1.04 0.83; 1.30 0.72
1e-1 AD-PRS*western dietary pattern 1.06 0.88; 1.28 0.56 1.04 0.85; 1.26 0.71
Western dietary pattern 0.93 0.63; 1.38 0.72 0.99 0.65; 1.50 0.96
1e-1 AD-PRS 0.95 0.77; 1.18 0.65 1.00 0.79; 1.25 0.97

HR and 95% CI estimated using a Cox proportional hazards model with follow-up time (years) as the time scale. APOE ε4 status are divided into carriers/non-carriers. The AD-PRSs are divided into tertiles of low/middle or high risk. Factor score 1 is labelled the “healthy” dietary pattern and factor score 2 is labelled the “western” dietary pattern

aModel 1 is adjusted for sex and birth year. Model 2 is adjusted for sex, birth year, BMI, energy intake, serum cholesterol level, hypertension, diabetes, physical activity level, educational level, smoking, and five principal components (PCs) to correct for population stratification

bDementia events/total cases. Those with missing data were excluded in model 2 (n = 29 with missing data, n = 5 for BMI, n = 3 for serum cholesterol, n = 5 for education, n = 10 for physical activity level and n = 7 for smoking)

APOE ε4 non-carriers with a higher adherence to the healthy dietary pattern had a reduced risk of dementia in model 1 (HR 0.79; 95% CI 0.65–0.95) and in the fully adjusted model 2 (HR 0.77; 95% CI 0.61–0.98), but no such associations were found among APOE ε4 carriers (Table 6). There was an association between the western dietary pattern and an increased risk of dementia among APOE ε4 carriers in the fully adjusted model 2 (HR 1.37; 95% CI 1.05–1.78), but not in model 1 (HR 1.28; 95% CI 0.99–1.66). There was no association between the western dietary pattern and an increased risk of dementia among APOE ε4 non-carriers (Table 6).

Table 6.

Associations between adherence to the dietary patterns and incident dementia among APOE ε4 carriers and APOE ε4 non-carriers

Model 1a p value Model 2a p value
Dementia n = 125/602b Dementia n = 121/573b
HR CI HR CI
Healthy dietary pattern
APOE ε4 non-carriers 0.79 0.65; 0.95 0.01 0.77 0.61; 0.98 0.03
APOE ε4 carriers 1.02 0.80; 1.30 0.89 0.86 0.63; 1.18 0.35
Western dietary pattern
APOE ε4 non-carriers 0.98 0.81; 1.18 0.81 0.99 0.81; 1.21 0.92
APOE ε4 carriers 1.28 0.99; 1.66 0.06 1.37 1.05; 1.78 0.02

HR and 95% CI estimated using a Cox proportional hazards model with follow-up time (years) as the time scale. To facilitate interpretation of identified interactions between dietary patterns*APOE ε4 in relation to incident dementia, separate effect values for the prediction scores are shown for APOE ε4 carriers and APOE ε4 non-carriers

aModel 1 is adjusted for sex and birth year. Model 2 is adjusted for sex, birth year, BMI, energy intake, serum cholesterol level, hypertension, diabetes, physical activity level, educational level, and smoking and five principal components (PCs) to correct for population stratification

bDementia events/total cases. Those with missing data were excluded in model 2 (n = 29 with missing data, n = 5 for BMI, n = 3 for serum cholesterol, n = 5 for education, n = 10 for physical activity level and n = 7 for smoking)

No interactions were found between the AD-PRSs (divided into tertiles of low, middle, or high risk) and the dietary patterns (healthy and western) in relation to incident dementia (Table 5).

Discussion

We found interactions between dietary patterns and APOE ε4 status in relation to incident dementia among 70-year-olds in a population-based sample followed on average for 13 years. Among APOE ε4 non-carriers, we found an association between higher adherence to a healthy dietary pattern and reduced risk of incident dementia. This association was not observed among ε4 carriers. However, there was an association between higher adherence to a less healthy western dietary pattern and an increased risk of dementia among ε4 carriers, but not among ε4 non-carriers. There were no interactions between the AD-PRSs and the dietary patterns in relation to incident dementia.

We could not find other studies that investigated interactions between different dietary patterns and both AD-PRS scores and APOE ε4 status in relation to incident dementia. Some studies have, however, investigated interactions between single genetic variants (usually APOE ε4) and either single foods or nutrients, or dietary patterns, in relation to either dementia or cognitive function [2527, 30, 31, 57]. The PREDIMED-NAVARRA intervention study (mean age 67) found an interaction between CLU and MeDi in relation to cognitive function, but no other gene–MeDi interactions (CR1, PICALM, APOE) were found [25]. A study from the Rush Memory and Aging Project found a marginally statistically significant interaction between the MIND (Mediterranean–DASH Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay) diet and APOE ε4 in relation to AD [23]. Similar to our study, those two studies found higher protective effects of a healthy dietary pattern among those with a favourable genetic predisposition [23, 25]. In our study, we did not find any interactions between the AD-PRSs and the dietary patterns in relation to incident dementia. One previous study has investigated the interplay between AD-PRS and dietary patterns in relation to cognitive function [26]. They found that poorer diet quality interacted with a 12 SNP AD-PRS (including APOE) in relation to lower scores on verbal fluency among African Americans (mean age 57) [26]. However, their results are somewhat hard to compare with ours since we used non-APOE AD-PRSs, and dementia was the outcome. The Three-city cohort study and the Cardiovascular Health Cognition study found interactions between fish consumption and APOE ε4 in relation to dementia among older adults (age > 65 years), and that consumption of fish was associated with reduced risk among ε4 non-carriers, but not among ε4 carriers [27, 57]. The Three-City cohort study also investigated interactions between n-3 fatty acids, a less healthy dietary pattern and APOE ε4 in relation to dementia, but no interactions were found [27]. Another study from the Rush Memory and Aging Project found interactions between n-3 fatty acids, seafood and APOE ε4 in relation to cognitive function among older adults (mean age 84 years), and that consumption of seafood and n-3 fatty acids was associated with slower global cognitive decline among ε4 carriers, but not among ε4 non-carriers [30]. An observational study with pooled participants from the Three-City study and 4 US cohorts (Nurses’ Health study, Women’s health study, Chicago Health and Aging project and Rush Memory and Aging projects) found no interactions between fish consumption and APOE ε4 and 11 other AD-related genes in relation to cognitive decline among older adults (age > 65 years) [31]. Methodological differences between studies in determining genetic risk, cognitive outcome, age, dietary intake, synergistic effects of foods and nutrients, and differences in food choices and consumption levels could be part of the explanation to why results differ between studies [58].

The dietary patterns derived from the RRR analysis in our study resembles dietary patterns previously associated with dementia risk [3, 8, 11]. The healthy dietary pattern loaded high on foods found in dietary patterns that have been associated with reduced risk (i.e., MeDi) [59], and a higher adherence to the healthy pattern correlated positively (strongest of the two dietary patterns) with the potentially risk reducing nutrients included as response variables in the RRR analysis (fibre, folate, vitamin E and C, and polyunsaturated fatty acids) [3, 4144]. The western pattern loaded high on foods found in dietary patterns associated with increased risk of dementia [8], and a higher adherence correlated positively with saturated fat intake, and negatively with vitamin C, folate, and fibre intake. Both dietary patterns in our study correlated with higher alcohol intake, but the correlation was stronger with the western dietary pattern, indicating that alcohol may be a more detrimental factor in the western pattern [46]. Even though the healthy pattern in our study also contained food groups that could be included in a western pattern (red meat, desserts, and sweet bakery), we did find an association between higher adherence and reduced risk of dementia among APOE ε4 non-carriers. A reason for that may be that a healthy diet may attenuate the adverse effects of a western diet [13].

Mechanisms underlying potential APOE-diet interactions in relation to dementia risk may involve altered metabolism of n-3 fatty acids, glucose or ketones, impaired transport of fatty acids and cholesterol, or modification of other risk factors where APOE status is involved [6, 28]. The APOE ε4 allele has been associated with cardiovascular conditions such as hypercholesterolemia, hypertriglyceridemia (lipid metabolism), increased insulin resistance, enhanced response to inflammation (chronic inflammation), and increased atherosclerosis (vascular system) [60], which may provide links between diet and APOE ε4 carriership in relation to dementia risk since several of these cardiovascular conditions also are affected by dietary factors [5, 6, 44, 61, 62]. It might be that the effect of APOE ε4 carriership diminishes the effect of a healthy diet on dementia risk by intervening at specific sites in the pathogenetic process, e.g., related to cholesterol metabolism. It has also been suggested that diets rich in refined carbohydrates could promote dementia and AD through insulin resistance [63], especially among APOE ε4 carriers [64]. A study from the from the French Three-City Study found an interaction between APOE ε4 and high glycemic load (GL) afternoon snacks, and that high afternoon-snack GL was associated with increased dementia and AD risk in APOE-ε4 carriers [64]. However, mechanisms remain elusive and there could be other explanations for an interplay between diet and APOE ε4 status in relation to dementia. Protective effects from diet might not compensate the higher genetic risk associated with APOE ε4 carriership, which could explain why we only found risk reducing effects of a healthy dietary pattern among APOE ε4 non-carriers. However, APOE ε4 carriers could potentially still be vulnerable for detrimental effects of less healthy dietary patterns [65], as our results suggest.

Strengths and limitations

This is one of few studies that have investigated interactions between different dietary patterns and both APOE ε4 status and non-APOE ε4 PRSs in relation to dementia. Strengths of this study are the systematically selected population-based sample, the comprehensive examinations and long follow-up time. The neuropsychiatric examinations were performed by psychiatric nurses and the dementia diagnoses were set in accordance with established diagnostic criteria. The diet history method in this study is validated, and the dietary interviews were performed by registered dietitians, which may reduce misreporting of dietary intake (e.g., recall, over–under reporting), common in dietary examinations [35, 66]. Another strength was the use of the RRR analysis to derive dietary patterns, since it considers both a priori knowledge about dietary factors related to dementia and explores dietary patterns that exist in this population.

The outcome in this study was incident dementia, which may include subtypes of dementia that are not as strongly linked to the genetic risk factors as AD. This could potentially have attenuated the results. It was not possible to perform sub-analyses since there were no data available for subtypes of dementia in the 2015–16 examination. However, in Swedish populations, approximately two-thirds of those with dementia have AD [67, 68]. Moreover, the sample size may be too small to detect a potential interaction between the AD-PRSs and diet in relation to dementia since the effect of the AD-PRSs is not as strong as APOE ε4. We found an interaction between the western dietary pattern and APOE ε4 status in relation to incident dementia in both adjusted models. For the healthy dietary pattern, we found an interaction in the model that was adjusted for sex and birth year, but not in the fully adjusted model. The results should, therefore, be interpreted with caution. Dietary intake may change during life, and since we do not have dietary data before or after the age of 70, we cannot determine potential effects of dietary intake earlier or later in life. Moreover, the analyses were performed on a Swedish population, and the possibility to generalize the results to non-Caucasian populations is limited.

Conclusions

The results of this study suggest that there is an interplay between APOE ε4 status and adherence to dietary patterns in relation to incident dementia. Higher adherence to a healthy dietary pattern was associated with a reduced risk of dementia among ε4 non-carriers, but not among ε4 carriers. A higher adherence to a western dietary pattern was associated with increased risk of dementia among ε4 carriers, but not among ε4 non-carriers. This suggests that diet may play a role and that the effects of risk or risk reducing dietary patterns could differ depending on ε4 carriership. These findings could be of importance for precision nutrition in dementia prevention strategies and for future intervention studies investigating the effect of dietary patterns in relation to dementia incidence.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participants of the Gothenburg H70 birth cohort studies, including the Population Study of Women in Gothenburg.

Abbreviations

APOE

Apolipoprotein E

AD

Alzheimer’s disease

PRS

Polygenic risk score

MeDi

Mediterranean diet

MIND

Mediterranean-DASH Intervention for neurodegenerative delay

GWAS

Genome-wide association studies

SNP

Single-nucleotide polymorphism

RRR

Reduced rank regression

PC

Principal component

Author contributions

JS and AZ: conception and design of the study; JS: drafting and writing the article; IS, HZ, KB, ER, SK, LL, HW, MMF, JN, and AZ: acquisition of data; JS, OW, AZ, and IS: analysis and interpretation of data; IS, HZ, KB, ER, SK, LL, HW, MMF, JN, AZ, and OW: revising the article critically for important intellectual content; AZ: supervision. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

Open access funding provided by University of Gothenburg. SK was financed by grants from the Swedish state under the agreement between the Swedish government and the county councils, the ALF-agreement (ALFGBG-81392, ALF GBG-771071). The Alzheimerfonden (AF-842 471, AF-737641, AF-939825). The Swedish Research Council (2019-02075), Stiftelsen Demensfonden, Stiftelsen Hjalmar Svenssons Forskningsfond, Stiftelsen Wilhelm och Martina Lundgrens vetenskapsfond. HZ is a Wallenberg Scholar supported by grants from the Swedish Research Council (2018-02532), the European Research Council (681712), Swedish State Support for Clinical Research (ALFGBG-720931), the Alzheimer Drug Discovery Foundation (ADDF), USA (201809-2016862), the AD Strategic Fund and the Alzheimer’s Association (ADSF-21-831376-C, ADSF-21-831381-C and ADSF-21-831377-C), the Olav Thon Foundation, the Erling-Persson Family Foundation, Stiftelsen för Gamla Tjänarinnor, Hjärnfonden, Sweden (FO2019-0228), the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No 860197 (MIRIADE), and the UK Dementia Research Institute at UCL. IS was financed by grants from the Swedish state under the agreement between the Swedish government and the county councils, the ALF-agreement (ALF 716681), Stena Foundation, Swedish Research Council (11267, 2005-8460, 2007-7462, 2012-5041, 2015-02830, 2019-01096, 2013-8717, NEAR 2017-00639), Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Wellfare (2004-0145, 2006-0596, 2008-1111, 2010-0870, 2013-1202, 2018-00471, 2001-2646, 2003-0234, 2004-0150, 2006-0020, 2008-1229, 2012-1138, AGECAP 2013-2300, 2013-2496), Konung Gustaf V:s och Drottning Victorias Frimurarestiftelse, Hjärnfonden (FO2014-0207, FO2016-0214, FO2018-0214, FO2019-0163, FO2020-0235), Alzheimerfonden (AF-554461, AF-647651, AF-743701, AF-844671, AF-930868, AF-940139), Eivind och Elsa K:son Sylvans stiftelse, The Alzheimer’s Association Zenith Award (ZEN-01-3151), The Alzheimer's Association Stephanie B. Overstreet Scholars (IIRG-00-2159), The Bank of Sweden Tercentenary Foundation, Stiftelsen Söderström-Königska Sjukhemmet, Stiftelsen för Gamla Tjänarinnor, Handlanden Hjalmar Svenssons Forskningsfond, KB is supported by the Swedish Research Council (2017-00915), the Alzheimer Drug Discovery Foundation (ADDF), USA (RDAPB-201809-2016615), the Swedish Alzheimer Foundation (AF-742881), Hjärnfonden, Sweden (FO2017-0243), the Swedish state under the agreement between the Swedish government and the County Councils, the ALF-agreement (ALFGBG-715986), the European Union Joint Program for Neurodegenerative Disorders (JPND2019-466-236), and the National Institute of Health (NIH), USA (grant 1R01AG068398-01). AZ was supported by the Swedish Alzheimer Foundation (AF-930582, AF-646061, AF-741361, AF-939988), Stiftelsen Demensfonden, Magnus Bergvalls Stiftelse, Stiftelsen för Gamla Tjänarinnor, and Stiftelsen Hjalmar Svenssons Forskningsfond. JS was supported by Stiftelsen Hjalmar Svenssons Forskningsfond.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Code availability

Not applicable.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest. SK has been consultant for Geras Solutions, unrelated to the findings in this manuscript. HZ has served at scientific advisory boards for Eisai, Denali, Roche Diagnostics, Wave, Samumed, Siemens Healthineers, Pinteon Therapeutics, Nervgen, AZTherapies and CogRx, has given lectures in symposia sponsored by Cellectricon, Fujirebio, Alzecure and Biogen, and is a co-founder of Brain Biomarker Solutions in Gothenburg AB (BBS), which is a part of the GU Ventures Incubator Program (outside submitted work). KB has served as a consultant, at advisory boards, or at data monitoring committees for Abcam, Axon, Biogen, JOMDD/Shimadzu. Julius Clinical, Lilly, MagQu, Novartis, Roche Diagnostics, and Siemens Healthineers, and is a co-founder of Brain Biomarker Solutions in Gothenburg AB (BBS), which is a part of the GU Ventures Incubator Program, all outside the submitted work.

Ethical approval

Ethical approval was obtained from the Ethics Committee for Medical Research in Gothenburg, reference numbers 179–92, S22700, S37799, Ö40299 and S06901.

Consent to participate

The tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki were followed, and informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Footnotes

Ingmar Skoog and Anna Zettergren are joint senior authors.

References

  • 1.Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet (London, England). 2020;396:413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lourida I, Hannon E, Littlejohns TJ, Langa KM, Hyppönen E, Kuzma E, et al. Association of lifestyle and genetic risk with incidence of dementia. JAMA. 2019;322:430–437. doi: 10.1001/jama.2019.9879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Scarmeas N, Anastasiou CA, Yannakoulia M. Nutrition and prevention of cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17:1006–1015. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(18)30338-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Cao L, Tan L, Wang HF, Jiang T, Zhu XC, Lu H, et al. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies. Mol Neurobiol. 2016;53:6144–6154. doi: 10.1007/s12035-015-9516-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Barberger-Gateau P, Lambert J-C, Féart C, Pérès K, Ritchie K, Dartigues J-F, et al. From genetics to dietetics: the contribution of epidemiology to understanding Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;33:S457–S463. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-129019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Yassine HN, Finch CE. APOE alleles and diet in brain aging and Alzheimer's disease. Front Aging Neurosci. 2020;12:150. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Solfrizzi V, Custodero C, Lozupone M, Imbimbo BP, Valiani V, Agosti P, et al. Relationships of dietary patterns, foods, and micro- and macronutrients with Alzheimer's disease and late-life cognitive disorders: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;59:815–849. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Samadi M, Moradi S, Moradinazar M, Mostafai R, Pasdar Y. Dietary pattern in relation to the risk of Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review. Neurol Sci. 2019;40:2031–2043. doi: 10.1007/s10072-019-03976-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Lourida I, Soni M, Thompson-Coon J, Purandare N, Lang IA, Ukoumunne OC, et al. Mediterranean diet, cognitive function, and dementia: a systematic review. Epidemiology. 2013;24:479–489. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3182944410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Petersson SD, Philippou E. Mediterranean diet, cognitive function, and dementia: a systematic review of the evidence. Adv Nutr (Bethesda, Md). 2016;7:889–904. doi: 10.3945/an.116.012138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.van de Rest O, Berendsen AA, Haveman-Nies A, de Groot LC. Dietary patterns, cognitive decline, and dementia: a systematic review. Adv Nutr (Bethesda, Md). 2015;6:154–168. doi: 10.3945/an.114.007617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Valls-Pedret C, Sala-Vila A, Serra-Mir M, Corella D, de la Torre R, Martínez-González M, et al. Mediterranean diet and age-related cognitive decline: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med. 2015;175:1094–1103. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2015.1668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Shakersain B, Santoni G, Larsson SC, Faxén-Irving G, Fastbom J, Fratiglioni L, et al. Prudent diet may attenuate the adverse effects of Western diet on cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12:100–109. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.van der Lee SJ, Wolters FJ, Ikram MK, Hofman A, Ikram MA, Amin N, et al. The effect of APOE and other common genetic variants on the onset of Alzheimer's disease and dementia: a community-based cohort study. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17:434–444. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(18)30053-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lambert JC, Ibrahim-Verbaas CA, Harold D, Naj AC, Sims R, Bellenguez C, et al. Meta-analysis of 74,046 individuals identifies 11 new susceptibility loci for Alzheimer's disease. Nat Genet. 2013;45:1452–1458. doi: 10.1038/ng.2802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Chasioti D, Yan J, Nho K, Saykin AJ. Progress in polygenic composite scores in Alzheimer's and other complex diseases. Trends Genet. 2019;35:371–382. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2019.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Licher S, Ahmad S, Karamujić-Čomić H, Voortman T, Leening MJG, Ikram MA, et al. Genetic predisposition, modifiable-risk-factor profile and long-term dementia risk in the general population. Nat Med. 2019;25:1364–1369. doi: 10.1038/s41591-019-0547-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Najar J, van der Lee SJ, Joas E, Wetterberg H, Hardy J, Guerreiro R, et al. Polygenic risk scores for Alzheimer's disease are related to dementia risk in APOE ɛ4 negatives. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2021;13:e12142. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Scelsi MA, Khan RR, Lorenzi M, Christopher L, Greicius MD, Schott JM, et al. Genetic study of multimodal imaging Alzheimer's disease progression score implicates novel loci. Brain. 2018;141:2167–2180. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Escott-Price V, Myers A, Huentelman M, Shoai M, Hardy J. Polygenic risk score analysis of Alzheimer's disease in cases without APOE4 or APOE2 alleles. J Prev Alz Dis. 2019;6:16–9. doi: 10.14283/jpad.2018.46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Altmann A, Scelsi MA, Shoai M, de Silva E, Aksman LM, Cash DM, et al. A comprehensive analysis of methods for assessing polygenic burden on Alzheimer’s disease pathology and risk beyond APOE. Brain Commun. 2020;2:fcz047. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcz047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Skoog I, Kern S, Najar J, Guerreiro R, Bras J, Waern M, et al. A non-APOE polygenic risk score for Alzheimer's disease is associated with CSF neurofilament light in a representative sample of cognitively unimpaired 70-year-olds. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2021 doi: 10.1093/gerona/glab030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Morris MC, Tangney CC, Wang Y, Sacks FM, Bennett DA, Aggarwal NT. MIND diet associated with reduced incidence of Alzheimer's disease. Alz Dementia. 2015;11:1007–1014. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Olsson E, Karlström B, Kilander L, Byberg L, Cederholm T, Sjögren P. Dietary patterns and cognitive dysfunction in a 12-year follow-up study of 70 year old men. J Alz Dis. 2015;43:109–119. doi: 10.3233/jad-140867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Martínez-Lapiscina EH, Galbete C, Corella D, Toledo E, Buil-Cosiales P, Salas-Salvado J, et al. Genotype patterns at CLU, CR1, PICALM and APOE, cognition and Mediterranean diet: the PREDIMED-NAVARRA trial. Genes Nutr. 2014;9:393. doi: 10.1007/s12263-014-0393-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hossain S, Beydoun MA, Kuczmarski MF, Tajuddin S, Evans MK, Zonderman AB. The interplay of diet quality and Alzheimer's disease genetic risk score in relation to cognitive performance among Urban African Americans. Nutrients. 2019;11:2181. doi: 10.3390/nu11092181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Barberger-Gateau P, Raffaitin C, Letenneur L, Berr C, Tzourio C, Dartigues JF, et al. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: the three-city cohort study. Neurology. 2007;69:1921–1930. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000278116.37320.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Barberger-Gateau P, Samieri C, Féart C, Plourde M. Dietary omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and Alzheimer's disease: interaction with apolipoprotein E genotype. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2011;8:479–491. doi: 10.2174/156720511796391926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Samieri C, Lorrain S, Buaud B, Vaysse C, Berr C, Peuchant E, et al. Relationship between diet and plasma long-chain n-3 PUFAs in older people: impact of apolipoprotein E genotype. J Lipid Res. 2013;54:2559–2567. doi: 10.1194/jlr.P036475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.van de Rest O, Wang Y, Barnes LL, Tangney C, Bennett DA, Morris MC. APOE ε4 and the associations of seafood and long-chain omega-3 fatty acids with cognitive decline. Neurology. 2016;86:2063–2070. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000002719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Samieri C, Morris MC, Bennett DA, Berr C, Amouyel P, Dartigues JF, et al. Fish intake, genetic predisposition to Alzheimer disease, and decline in global cognition and memory in 5 cohorts of older persons. Am J Epidemiol. 2018;187:933–940. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Rinder L, Roupe S, Steen B, Svanborg A. Seventy-year-old people in Gothenburg. A population study in an industrialized Swedish city. Acta Med Scand. 1975;198:397–407. doi: 10.1111/j.0954-6820.1975.tb19563.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bengtsson C, Blohmé G, Hallberg L, Hällström T, Isaksson B, Korsan-Bengtsen K, et al. The study of women in Gothenburg 1968–1969—a population study. General design, purpose and sampling results. Acta Med Scand. 1973;193:311–318. doi: 10.1111/j.0954-6820.1973.tb10583.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Rydberg Sterner T, Ahlner F, Blennow K, Dahlin-Ivanoff S, Falk H, Havstam Johansson L, et al. The Gothenburg H70 Birth cohort study 2014–16: design, methods and study population. Eur J Epidemiol. 2019;34:191–209. doi: 10.1007/s10654-018-0459-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Samuelsson J, Rothenberg E, Lissner L, Eiben G, Zettergren A, Skoog I. Time trends in nutrient intake and dietary patterns among five birth cohorts of 70-year-olds examined 1971–2016: results from the Gothenburg H70 birth cohort studies Sweden. Nutr J. 2019;18:66. doi: 10.1186/s12937-019-0493-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Eiben G, Andersson CS, Rothenberg E, Sundh V, Steen B, Lissner L. Secular trends in diet among elderly Swedes–cohort comparisons over three decades. Public Health Nutr. 2004;7:637–644. doi: 10.1079/phn2003576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Rothenberg E, Bosaeus I, Lernfelt B, Landahl S, Steen B, Rothenberg E, Bosaeus I, Lernfelt B, Landahl S, Steen B. Energy intake and expenditure: validation of a diet history by heart rate monitoring, activity diary and doubly labeled water. Eur J Clin Nutr. 1998;52:832–838. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Ax E, Warensjö Lemming E, Becker W, Andersson A, Lindroos AK, Cederholm T, et al. Dietary patterns in Swedish adults; results from a national dietary survey. Br J Nutr. 2016;115:95–104. doi: 10.1017/s0007114515004110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hoffmann K, Schulze MB, Schienkiewitz A, Nöthlings U, Boeing H. Application of a new statistical method to derive dietary patterns in nutritional epidemiology. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159:935–944. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Monacelli F, Acquarone E, Giannotti C, Borghi R, Nencioni A. Vitamin C, aging and Alzheimer's disease. Nutrients. 2017;9:670. doi: 10.3390/nu9070670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Durga J, van Boxtel MP, Schouten EG, Kok FJ, Jolles J, Katan MB, et al. Effect of 3-year folic acid supplementation on cognitive function in older adults in the FACIT trial: a randomised, double blind, controlled trial. Lancet (London, England). 2007;369:208–16. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(07)60109-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Robinson N, Grabowski P, Rehman I. Alzheimer's disease pathogenesis: is there a role for folate? Mech Ageing Dev. 2018;174:86–94. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2017.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Dahl WJ, Stewart ML. Position of the academy of nutrition and dietetics: health implications of dietary fiber. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2015;115:1861–1870. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2015.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Gardener SL, Rainey-Smith SR, Martins RN. Diet and inflammation in Alzheimer's disease and related chronic diseases: a review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;50:301–334. doi: 10.3233/jad-150765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Farina N, Llewellyn D, Isaac M, Tabet N. Vitamin E for Alzheimer's dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2017;4:CD002854. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002854.pub5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Rehm J, Hasan OSM, Black SE, Shield KD, Schwarzinger M. Alcohol use and dementia: a systematic scoping review. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;11:1. doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0453-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Shakya PR, Melaku YA, Page A, Gill TK. Association between dietary patterns and adult depression symptoms based on principal component analysis, reduced-rank regression and partial least-squares. Clin Nutr. 2020;39:2811–2823. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2019.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Melaku YA, Gill TK, Taylor AW, Adams R, Shi Z. A comparison of principal component analysis, partial least-squares and reduced-rank regressions in the identification of dietary patterns associated with bone mass in ageing Australians. Eur J Nutr. 2018;57:1969–1983. doi: 10.1007/s00394-017-1478-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Vermeulen E, Stronks K, Visser M, Brouwer IA, Snijder MB, Mocking RJT, et al. Dietary pattern derived by reduced rank regression and depressive symptoms in a multi-ethnic population: the HELIUS study. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2017;71:987–994. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2017.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Shin D, Lee KW, Kim MH, Kim HJ, An YS, Chung HK. Identifying dietary patterns associated with mild cognitive impairment in older Korean adults using reduced rank regression. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15:100. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15010100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Biesbroek S, van der Brosens Beulens Verschuren van der Schouw ADMCJWWMYT, et al. Identifying cardiovascular risk factor-related dietary patterns with reduced rank regression and random forest in the EPIC-NL cohort. Am J Clin Nutr. 2015;102:146–154. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.092288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental health disorders (DSM-III-R)
  • 53.Blauwendraat C, Faghri F, Pihlstrom L, Geiger JT, Elbaz A, Lesage S, et al. NeuroChip, an updated version of the NeuroX genotyping platform to rapidly screen for variants associated with neurological diseases. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;57:247.e9–247. e13. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Blennow K, Ricksten A, Prince JA, Brookes AJ, Emahazion T, Wasslavik C, et al. No association between the alpha2-macroglobulin (A2M) deletion and Alzheimer's disease, and no change in A2M mRNA, protein, or protein expression. J Neural Transm (Vienna, Austria : 1996). 2000;107:1065–79. doi: 10.1007/s007020070052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Kunkle BW, Grenier-Boley B, Sims R, Bis JC, Damotte V, Naj AC, et al. Genetic meta-analysis of diagnosed Alzheimer's disease identifies new risk loci and implicates Aβ, tau, immunity and lipid processing. Nat Genet. 2019;51:414–430. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0358-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Rödjer L, Jonsdottir IH, Rosengren A, Björck L, Grimby G, Thelle DS, et al. Self-reported leisure time physical activity: a useful assessment tool in everyday health care. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:693. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Huang TL, Zandi PP, Tucker KL, Fitzpatrick AL, Kuller LH, Fried LP, et al. Benefits of fatty fish on dementia risk are stronger for those without APOE epsilon4. Neurology. 2005;65:1409–1414. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000183148.34197.2e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Morris MC. Nutrition and risk of dementia: overview and methodological issues. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2016;1367:31–37. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.van den Brink AC, Brouwer-Brolsma EM, Berendsen AAM, van de Rest O. The Mediterranean, Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH), and Mediterranean-DASH Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay (MIND) diets are associated with less cognitive decline and a lower risk of Alzheimer's disease—a review. Adv Nutr (Bethesda, Md). 2019;10:1040–65. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmz054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Huang Y, Mahley RW. Apolipoprotein E: structure and function in lipid metabolism, neurobiology, and Alzheimer's diseases. Neurobiol Dis. 2014;72:3–12. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2014.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.McGrattan AM, McGuinness B, McKinley MC, Kee F, Passmore P, Woodside JV, et al. Diet and inflammation in cognitive ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Curr Nutr Rep. 2019;8:53–65. doi: 10.1007/s13668-019-0271-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Sato N, Morishita R. The roles of lipid and glucose metabolism in modulation of beta-amyloid, tau, and neurodegeneration in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer disease. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:199. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Arnold SE, Arvanitakis Z, Macauley-Rambach SL, Koenig AM, Wang HY, Ahima RS, et al. Brain insulin resistance in type 2 diabetes and Alzheimer disease: concepts and conundrums. Nat Rev Neurol. 2018;14:168–181. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2017.185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Gentreau M, Chuy V, Féart C, Samieri C, Ritchie K, Raymond M, et al. Refined carbohydrate-rich diet is associated with long-term risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease in apolipoprotein E ε4 allele carriers. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16:1043–1053. doi: 10.1002/alz.12114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kivipelto M, Rovio S, Ngandu T, Kåreholt I, Eskelinen M, Winblad B, et al. Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 magnifies lifestyle risks for dementia: a population-based study. J Cell Mol Med. 2008;12:2762–2771. doi: 10.1111/j.1582-4934.2008.00296.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Hörnell A, Berg C, Forsum E, Larsson C, Sonestedt E, Åkesson A, et al. Perspective: an extension of the STROBE Statement for Observational Studies in Nutritional Epidemiology (STROBE-nut): explanation and elaboration. Adv Nutr (Bethesda, Md). 2017;8:652–78. doi: 10.3945/an.117.015941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Skoog I, Börjesson-Hanson A, Kern S, Johansson L, Falk H, Sigström R, et al. Decreasing prevalence of dementia in 85-year olds examined 22 years apart: the influence of education and stroke. Sci Rep. 2017;7:6136. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-05022-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Najar J, Östling S, Gudmundsson P, Sundh V, Johansson L, Kern S, et al. Cognitive and physical activity and dementia: a 44-year longitudinal population study of women. Neurology. 2019;92:e1322–e1330. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000007021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Not applicable.


Articles from European Journal of Nutrition are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES