Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Feb 18;17(2):e0263108. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0263108

Goal management training and psychoeducation / mindfulness for treatment of executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: A feasibility pilot trial

Ariane Giguère-Rancourt 1,*, Marika Plourde 1, Eva Racine 1, Marianne Couture 1, Mélanie Langlois 2,3, Nicolas Dupré 2,3, Martine Simard 1
Editor: Walid Kamal Abdelbasset4
PMCID: PMC8856541  PMID: 35180229

Abstract

Introduction

As there is currently no pharmacological treatment for Parkinson’s Disease Mild Cognitive Impairment (PD-MCI) with executive dysfunctions, specific cognitive interventions must be investigated. Most previous studies have tested bottom-up cognitive training programs but have not shown very good results.

Objectives

The aim of this study was to test ease of implementation, differential safety and preliminary efficacy of two top-down (strategy-learning) home-based, individualized, cognitive interventions: Goal Management Training (GMT), adapted for PD-MCI (Adapted-GMT), and a psychoeducation program combined with mindfulness exercises (PSYCH-Mind).

Methods

This was a single-blind block-randomized between-group comparative study. Twelve PD-MCI with mild executive dysfunctions were divided in four blocks and randomly assigned to any of the two interventions. The participants were included if they had PD-MCI diagnosis (no dementia), with stabilized medication. Both groups (Adapted-GMT and PSYCH-mind) received five intervention sessions each lasting 60–90 minutes for five weeks. Measures were collected at baseline, mid-point, one-week, four-week and 12-week follow-ups. Executive functions were assessed with the Dysexecutive questionnaire (DEX) and the Zoo Map Test (ZMT). Quality of life (QoL) and psychiatric symptoms were also evaluated. Repeated measures ANCOVAs (mixed linear analysis) were applied to all outcomes.

Results

There was one drop out, and both interventions were feasible and acceptable. Despite the small sample size limiting statistical power, patients of both groups significantly improved executive functions per the DEX-patient (Time: F(4,36) = 2.96, p = 0.033, CI95%: 10.75–15.23) and DEX-caregiver scores (Time: F(4,36) = 6.02, p = 0.017, CI95%: 9.63–17.23). Both groups significantly made fewer errors between measurement times on the ZMT (Time: F(3,36) = 16.66, p = 0.001, CI95%: 1.07–2.93). However, QoL significantly increased only in PSYCH-Mind patients at four-week follow-up (interaction Time*Group: F(4,36) = 5.31, p = 0.002, CI95%: 15.33–25.61).

Conclusion

Both interventions were easily implemented and proved to be safe. Because both interventions are arguably cost-effective, these pilot findings, although promising, need to be replicated in large samples.

ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier

NCT04636541.

Introduction

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) affects approximately 30% of people with Parkinson’s disease (PD) [1]. Nearly 40% of PD-MCI patients report difficulties with executive functions [2, 3].

Executive functions involve integrative processes supporting goal-directed behaviors [2]. For example, dysexecutive patients experience difficulties to plan and execute complex activities requiring multiple steps, such as preparing meals or maintaining attention to tasks without being distracted. Degeneration of frontal-striatal loops in PD-MCI might explain behavior disorganization [3, 4]. Medications for motor symptoms might improve some cognitive functions but deteriorate others in de novo PD and PD-MCI [58]. Therefore, non-pharmacological approaches, such as cognitive training, shall be considered as possible treatment for cognitive impairment.

Studies assessing the impact of cognitive training on executive functions in PD-MCI are still scarce [9]. Our group recently conducted a systematic review on trials of cognitive interventions administered to PD patients with and without MCI [9]. Only 13 studies met the inclusion criteria of the review. Cognitive training programs described in these studies involved between 10 and 16 sessions, each lasting from 30 to 90 minutes, and were mostly drill-and-practice or bottom-up training with repetition of paper-and-pencil tasks and computer tasks to improve executive functions as well as other cognitive functions. The review reported that in these trials, PD-MCI, compared with controls, only improved performance in 6.9% of cognitive measures after mostly 4 to 6 weeks of cognitive training. However, PD-MCI who received only cognitive training (as opposed of those who received other kinds of cognitive interventions) did not improve on executive functions when compared to controls [9]. The lack of efficacy in these trials to improve executive functions could be explained partly by methodological limitations and partly by the bottom-up training approach chosen so far.

Thus, the trials published to date present methodological limitations [9, 10]. First, cognitive intervention programs were generally not developed to deal with subjective cognitive complaints or objective executive deficits specific to participants; in other words, regardless of the MCI type of a particular patient, all participants received the exact same cognitive training to improve the same cognitive functions. This method does not consider the heterogeneity of MCI in PD [1, 2, 11]. This could induce either a ceiling or a floor effect, depending on the presence of dysexecutive functions at baseline. This problem could be avoided by ensuring that PD-MCI involved in cognitive training of executive functions do in fact present with executive impairment at baseline. This limitation could partly explain the absence of effect or small effect sizes reported in previous studies [9]. Lack of assessment for neuropsychiatric symptoms is another methodological limitation as these symptoms are frequent in PD and could affect feasibility, safety, and efficacy of intervention [12, 13]. Furthermore, important constructs conceived as indicators of generalization, such as quality of life and subjective cognitive complaints made by patients and caregivers, have not been examined in prior research [9, 10]. Finally, one should question the bottom-up approach to improve executive functions adopted in previous trials. A bottom-up training approach makes it more difficult to transfer skills acquired in training session to day-to-day functioning since it focuses on the practice of specific tasks [14]. Most of the time, the efficacy of this kind of training is assessed on the same tests used to perform the training, thus making generalization of training questionable. On the contrary, a strategy-based or top-down cognitive training approach which consists in teaching different cognitive strategies applicable to many daily situations [14], has a greater potential for improving the daily functioning of dysexecutive PD patients. However, strategy-based approaches were rarely tested in PD-MCI patients.

The Goal Management Training (GMT) is a strategy-based or top-down cognitive training program [1517] as opposed to the drill-and practice or bottom-up cognitive training used in prior studies in PD. The GMT, based on Duncan’s theory of goal neglect [15], has been developed to improve executive functions through the management and prioritization of goals in the achievement of daily activities. To teach patients to better manage their daily goals, the GMT program includes, in each of the nine sessions, self-instruction strategies, self-monitoring exercises, cognitive training techniques such as goal definition or task splitting, psychoeducation on cognitive processes, mindfulness and other exercises to practice newly acquired knowledge between sessions. In a systematic review and meta-analysis assessing the effectiveness of GMT [17], the studies included patients with various neurological conditions affecting frontal lobe functioning such as acquired traumatic brain injury, spina bifida, attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder, subjective cognitive complaints, and multiple sclerosis [17]. The GMT has been shown to increase those patients’ awareness of deficits and improve cognitive control in goal-directed behaviors, as well as other executive functions measured by neuropsychological tests and questionnaires [17]. However, the GMT has never been studied in PD-MCI up to date. Although patients who received the GMT so far present with neurological disorders other than PD, they exhibit dysexecutive functions caused by impaired frontal cortex networks, as do PD-MCI patients with executive functions impairment. Thus, GMT might be suitable for PD-MCI patients with executive dysfunction. However, the original GMT was developed in English, was not yet available in French, and is a nine-week program administered in group sessions of 90 to 120-minutes [16]. This program might be too long, tiresome, and difficult to follow for PD patients. In addition, the obligation to leave home to attend group sessions might prove difficult if not impossible for patients with mobility issues as those with PD. All these factors could affect clinical trials feasibility in vulnerable patients with PD-MCI. Therefore, our team first translated in French and adapted the GMT to be administered at home for a PD-MCI patient in a case study [18]. Up to four weeks after the end of Adapted-GMT, the participant had not missed any training sessions, and showed a significant decrease in executive complaints according to the DEX score. Furthermore, the participant did not register any increase in neuropsychiatric symptoms, suggesting that the Adapted-GMT is acceptable and safe. However, this study does not report any data comparing the effects of Adapted-GMT with other treatment and only investigated one patient.

The present pilot study thus aimed at testing feasibility, safety and preliminary efficacy of the Adapted-GMT compared with another top-down treatment less systematically studied in the past. Many clinical guidelines include general recommendations about giving information to PD patients and family [19, 20]. However, few standardized psychoeducation interventions are available, and they do not include information on cognitive decline [2123]. Another less studied treatment in PD, the Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction program includes formal meditative exercises to develop non-judgmental attention to experiences in the present moment [24, 25]. In elderly patients with MCI unrelated to PD, mindfulness interventions have shown positive effects on cognitive functioning, including attention, executive functions, and memory [26, 27]. In PD, previous trials mostly investigated the efficacy of mindfulness to reduce psychological symptoms, but none of them studied its impact on cognition. In addition, a review of the literature on the effect of mindfulness in PD shows that methods and results are inconsistent and that more studies are needed in PD patients [24].

In summary, Adapted-GMT, psychoeducation, and mindfulness are top-down interventions that have never been investigated to improve executive functions in PD. Nonetheless, they are potentially and theoretically interesting options for treatment of executive dysfunctions in patients with PD-MCI, partly because they are less likely to cause adverse effects than the pharmacological interventions. However, before testing the effectiveness of these interventions with large patient samples, one must first verify their feasibility, safety and preliminary efficacy, especially when offered at home, and in the presence of a caregiver. Therefore, the present pilot study shall investigate these aspects, in a small sample of PD-MCI.

Objectives and hypotheses

The main objective of this study was thus to compare feasibility, safety and preliminary efficacy of two interventions: Adapted-GMT versus Psychoeducation combined with Mindfulness exercises, in two groups of PD-MCI patients experiencing executive dysfunctions.

Hypotheses are: 1) Both interventions will prove to be feasible, safe, and acceptable by patients and caregivers; 2) Only Adapted-GMT will improve executive functioning, as measured by neuropsychological tests; 3) Both Adapted-GMT and Psychoeducation- Mindfulness (PSYCH-Mind) will improve subjective executive functioning, as shown with questionnaires; 4) Both interventions will maintain quality of life as measured with the Parkinson Disease Questionnaire-39 items (PDQ-39) and global cognition as measured with the Dementia Rating Scale 2nd edition (DRS-II), compared to baseline (BL).

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from the Parkinson Québec Network through announcements in gatherings and newsletters. Participants were also recruited at the Movement Disorder Clinic of CHU-Hôpital Enfant-Jésus, in Quebec City, as described in previous paper [18]. Flowchart of recruitment is displayed in Fig 1.

Fig 1. Flow diagram of the final sample.

Fig 1

Participants had to meet the following inclusion criteria: 1) the UK Research Brain Bank diagnostic criteria for PD [28]; and 2) the Movement Disorder Society Task Force diagnostic criteria for PD-MCI [1]. Single and multiple-domain MCI were both included, only if executive functions were significantly impaired, with scores between -1 SD and -2 SD on executive function tests, according to age- and education-adjusted norms; 3) MoCA scores between 21 and 27; 4) Anti-Parkinson medication stable at screening since at least two months; and 5) All other medications, including psychotropics, stable for at least three months. Exclusion criteria were: 1) Participants with PD meeting the clinical diagnostic criteria for dementia (PDD) [29]; 2) Patients with concomitant other neurological or psychiatric disorders and 3) patients with PD-MCI unstable on anti-Parkinson medication at screening or stable since less than 2 months on these medications; and 4) PD patients unstable or stable since less than three months on all other medications.

Ethical considerations

This study was approved by the Ethics and Research Committee of CHU de Québec–Université Laval. Before entering into the study, patients and caregivers were fully informed about the project and signed an informed consent. All nominative data were kept confidential by coding of documents. The Ethics and Research Committee of CHU de Québec–Université Laval required the dataset to be kept in encrypted files, with the access protected by a password only known by the doctoral student (A. Giguère-Rancourt) and the laboratory director (Dr. Simard). Data of this study are available from the corresponding author only upon reasonable request.

Research design, randomization, allocation procedure and course of the study

The present research was a single-blind block-randomized between-group comparative study (see Fig 2 for study protocol).

Fig 2. Study protocol.

Fig 2

Clinical trial registration number is NCT04636541. Doctoral dissertation research projects at Laval University, Canada, only require the approval of the Ph.D. Candidate’s advisory committee followed by the approval of the local ethics committee to start the study. The various steps of doctoral training having very tight deadlines, and the project not being funded by a national agency, the research started immediately after the ethical approval. This is the reason why the study protocol was not registered before it started. The authors confirm that all ongoing and related trials for this intervention are registered.

Three randomization blocks, each including four participants, were created during recruitment. Participants of the same block were assessed and received interventions over the same time period. After screening evaluation, participants of each block were randomly assigned to either group A or B, described below. Two participants of each block were thus in Group A (GMT) whereas two other participants were in Group B (PSYCH-Mind). Participants then received baseline evaluation. After the baseline evaluation, participants received five intervention sessions, each lasting 60–90 minutes, for five weeks. Evaluations and interventions took place at participants home, in presence of caregivers. All participants and caregivers were blinded to group allocation. Follow-up assessments were administered by the investigator administering the interventions but were scored by an independent rater blind to previous performances. The same investigator administered all interventions and evaluations for standardization of administration of tests and interventions and to control for clinician characteristics. All questionnaires were administered halfway through the intervention (week 3) and one, four and 12 weeks after the end of the intervention. All questionnaires and tests were rated after the administration and results were entered in the database by independent raters, not informed of the group allocation.

Group A: Goal Management Training (GMT)

GMT modules [16] were adapted for French-speaking patients with PD-MCI. Each session was reduced from nine 90-120-minute sessions (original GMT) to five 60-90-minute sessions, one session per week, to avoid fatigue as much as possible. As for original GMT, participants were given exercises between sessions (mindfulness exercises and metacognitive reflections). All participants reported they practiced the tasks between sessions (between 30 and 150 minutes per week). In original-GMT, some information is repeated several times, but not in Adapted-GMT, to reduce sessions duration and thus avoid fatigue and attrition. Exercises demanding motor dexterity, such as card distribution, were removed because of PD motor impairment. Adapted-GMT included information on PD-MCI and executive dysfunction unlike the original GMT. In addition, Adapted-GMT modules were administered individually with an iPad, as opposed to a power-point group presentation in original-GMT. A workbook was handed to participants, as in previous studies. The original GMT material, including the version translated in French for the present project, is now available for sale on Baycrest Centre website under the name Goal Management Training® Products [30]. Dr. Brian Levine and Baycrest Center are the owners of the copyrights. Prior to the development of the present study, Dr. Levine and Baycrest Centre granted special permission to the authors to translate into French, adapt and use the GMT material for patients with PD-MCI included in this study.

Group B: Psychoeducation with mindfulness (PSYCH-Mind) exercises

Five modules were designed as a discussion with patients and caregivers about various PD symptoms: module I- brain and motor symptoms; module II- autonomic symptoms; module III- psychological symptoms; module IV- brain and cognition; and module V- cognitive impairments in PD. Patients were handed the information book at the beginning of the study. The objective was to improve their understanding of their condition and to discuss other components that could affect cognitive abilities. After the 40-to-60-minute informative part, mindfulness exercises were offered for 20–30 minutes per session. Participants were not invited to practice exercises between sessions, but 3/6 participants reported they did.

The same mindfulness exercises were practiced with all participants of Groups A and B. There were four exercises: mental visualization of ‘eating a grape’ in mindfulness (as an introduction); body scan; long breathing (12 minutes) and short breathing (three minutes).

Evaluation

Feasibility measures

Although some authors offer elements to consider when researchers develop feasibility studies [31, 32], there are currently no recognized standards for such studies. Some authors [31, 32] suggest several key elements that may be studied as feasibility issues depending on the population targeted by a treatment, the treatment itself, and context. Among the proposed focuses by these authors, the present study examined: 1) acceptability, using the following outcomes: satisfaction, intent to continue to use, perceived appropriateness; 2) demand, using the following outcomes: expressed interest or intention to use by patients); 3) implementation, using the following outcomes: degree of execution, success or failure of execution; 4) practicality, with the following outcomes: amount and type of resources needed to implement, i.e. calculation made by analyzing the number of hours worked by the therapist for each participant, including transport, pre-intervention assessment, intervention and correction of questionnaires and protocols, and finally, safety issues on target participants and ability of participants to carry out intervention and activities as measured with attrition rate; 5) limited or preliminary efficacy testing with the following outcomes: intended effects of program on key variables, effect-size estimation, maintenance of changes from initial change. A last element of feasibility that will be considered in the present paper is 6) adaptation, and this final point will be addressed in the discussion of the article by attempting to answer these two questions: 1) How do present GMT results obtained by PD-MCI compare with results obtained previously by participants with other neurological conditions? How do the adapted GMT and PSYCH-Mind results compare with results of previous cognitive training studies in PD-MCI?.

Safety measures

To assess safety and tolerability, attendance, medication change (Levodopa Equivalent Dosage or LED and other medications if appropriate), and fatigue, using a Likert scale scoring from 0 to 10; 0 = none, 10 = extreme fatigue, were recorded at each session (means +/- SD are reported). For the fatigue scale, 0–1 was associated with no fatigue; from 2 to 4 was associated with mild fatigue, 6–7 to moderate fatigue, 8–9 to severe fatigue and 10 was associated with intolerable fatigue.

Screening measures administered before randomization and baseline

Neuropsychological evaluation–Type II [1] was administered at screening to determine MCI subtype (see Table 1).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the final sample, screening evaluation results and descriptive of primary and secondary outcomes at baseline.
Group A Mean (SD) N = 6 Group B Mean (SD) N = 6 p
Age 71.00 (4.03) 70.00 (5.09) 0.77
Sex (n men / 6) 5 / 6 5 / 6 N/A
Education (years) 17.17 (4.32) 16.00 (3.30) 0.84
Occupation (n retired / 6) 4 4 N/A
Time since diagnostic (years) 10.33 (4.41) 5.44 (3.31) 0.006**
H&Y stage 1.92 (0.58) 1.67 (0.75) 0.53
 Stage 1–1.5 (n / 6) 2 3 N/A
 Stage 2–2.5 (n / 6) 3 2 N/A
 Stage 3 (n / 6) 1 1 N/A
LED (mg / day) 728.94 (297.36) 637.32 (436.91) 0.69
Comorbidities
 Sleep problem treated with medication (n / 6) 1 2 N/A
 Hypertension (n / 6) 2 2 N/A
 Type II Diabetes (n / 6) 0 1 N/A
Caregivers
 N 6 5 N/A
 Age 66.33 (4.92) 63.5 (8.21) 0.49
 Sex (n women) 6 5 N/A
Screening Neuropsychological Evaluation Type II
Visuospatial skills
Visual puzzle WAIS-IV (Z score) [33] 0.00 (0.55) 0.22 (1.44) 0.64
BJLO (Z score) [34] 0.50 (0.77) 0.92 (0.58) 0.52
Language
Lexical fluency, T-N-P (Z score) [35] -0.07 (1.19) 0.50 (0.68) 0.34
Category fluency, Animals (Z score) [35] -0.34 (1.17) -0.29 (0.48) 0.92
NAB Screening-Naming (Z score) [36] 0.60 (0.41) -0.05 (1.35) 0.22
Episodic memory
RL / RI 16, learning (Z score) [37] -0.89 (1.02) -0.37 (0.84) 0.52
RL / RI 16, delayed recall (Z score) [37] -0.97 (1.15) -0.86 (1.31) 0.98
BVMT-R, learning (Z score) [38] -0.36 (0.61) -0.25 (1.36) 0.34
BVMT-R, delayed recall (Z score) [38] -0.30 (1.38) -0.32 (1.17) 0.46
Attention / Working memory
D-KEFS—TMT 1 (Z score) [39] -1.38 (1.17) -0.92 (1.40) 0.55
D-KEFS—TMT 2 (Z score) [39] -0.11 (0.91) -0.78 (1.88) 0.44
D-KEFS—TMT 3 (Z score) [39] -0.33 (1.31) -0.05 (1.57) 0.66
Baddeley dual task paradigm (Z score) [40] -1.78 (1.68) -2.14 (1.87) 0.52
Spatial Span (Z score) [41] - 0.17 (0.95) -0.06 (0.8) 0.83
Digit Span, backwards (Z score) [33] -0.06 (0.59) 0.17 (1.29) 0.74
Executive functions
D-KEFS—TMT 4 minus 5 (Z score) [39] -0.83 (1.47) -0.79 (1.54) 0.37
Six Elements task (Z score) [42] -1.66 (0.36) -2.07 (0.38) 0.07
Global cognition
MoCA–Raw score [43] 24.00 (2.19) 23.83 (2.32) 0.89
MoCA–Z Score [43] -1.24 (0.73) -1.17 (0.53) 0.83
Primary outcomes
DEX (raw, self-rated) 21.17 (7.47) 16.33 (5.05) 0.21
DEX-C (raw, caregivers) 22.17 (10.88) 14.8 (11.16) 0.27
ZMT total score (raw) 9.50 (2.74) 9.17 (3.19) 0.84
ZMT (raw errors) 3.60 (4.04) 2.33 (2.34) 0.53
Secondary outcomes
PDQ-39 total (%) 34.5 (10.93) 25.67 (8.59) 0.15
DRS-II total (raw) 137.00 (3.63) 134.5 (6.03) 0.41

Notes. Group A: GMT; Group B: psychoeducation; BJLO: Benton Judgment of Line Orientations; BVMT-R: Brief Visual Memory Test, Revised; DEX: Dysexecutive questionnaire; D-KEFS: Delis and Kaplan Executive Functioning System; DRS-II: Dementia Rating Scale, second edition; LED: Levodopa Equivalent Dose; MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; NAB: Neuropsychological Assessment Battery; PDQ-39: Parkinson Disease Questionnaire, 39 items; RL / RI 16: Rappel Libre / Rappel Indicé 16 items; TMT: Trail Making Test; WAIS-IV: Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale–IV; WMS-III: Weschler Memory Scale–III; ZMT: Zoo Map Test. Group equivalency was tested with T-tests (respective p’s reported in last column).

Efficacy measures administered at baseline, during training and follow-ups

All assessments and interventions were performed by a Senior Ph.D. Candidate under the supervision of a registered neuropsychologist and were conducted at each participant’s home. All sessions were planned beforehand with each participant and occurred at an optimal moment of the day («ON» period, 60–90 minutes after participants took their medication). There were five different times of measure: BL took place just before the start of the intervention; Mid-point occurred after 3 weeks of intervention whereas FU occurred 1 week (FU-1), 4 weeks (FU-2), and 12 weeks (FU-3) after the intervention. The complete procedure is detailed in Fig 2. Independent raters (senior doctoral students), blind to the assignment of participants to groups, rated all protocols and entered data in the database.

In this study, primary efficacy outcomes are 1) subjective complaints of executive functioning, as measured by DEX (patient self-rated) and DEX-C (rated by caregivers); and 2) objective measures of executive functioning, as measured by Zoo Map Test (total raw score and number of errors). The DEX questionnaire and Zoo Map Test are both part of the Behavioral Assessment of Dysexecutive Syndrome battery (BADS) [44].

The DEX includes 20 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (never) to 4 (very often). Higher scores suggest more problems with executive functioning. Both DEX versions were administered to patients (DEX) and caregivers (DEX-C) at BL (Baseline), mid-point and FU-1 (Follow Up-1), FU-2 and FU-3, respectively one, four- and twelve-weeks post-intervention. The DEX has good convergence validity, correlates with BADS scores and has previously been validated in PD patients [44].

The Zoo Map Test (ZMT) measures organization and planning capacities [43]. The ZMT scoring is based on the strategy used by the patient, on a 16-item scale (raw score) transformed into a profile score ranging from 1 (very poor strategy) to 4 (very good strategy). The number of errors was also recorded. ZMT was administered at BL and FU-1, FU-2 and FU-3. It was shown to have good construct validity and good correlation with other executive tests [44, 45].

Secondary outcomes include quality of life as measured by Parkinson Disease Questionnaire–39 items (PDQ-39) and global cognition as measured by Dementia Rating Scale 2nd edition (DRS-II).

Parkinson Disease Questionnaire-39 items (PDQ-39) is a self-rated questionnaire specifically designed for PD patients [46] to assess eight dimensions of quality of life: motor symptoms, activities of daily living (ADL), emotional well-being, stigmatization, emotional support, cognition, communication and body discomfort. Items are rated on a Likert scale from 0 (never) to 4 (always). Higher scores suggest lower quality of life. It was administered at BL, mid-point and FU-1, FU-2 and FU-3. PDQ-39 is widely used in research due to good construct validity, reliability and internal consistency [46].

The Dementia Rating Scale- Second edition (DRS-II) [47] is a brief neuropsychological instrument designed to assess general cognitive functioning and is validated to detect PD-MCI and PDD in French-speaking patients [48]. Because this study was conducted in Canada, the fluency task ‘Names of USA states’ (Initiation / Perseveration subscale) in the alternate version was changed for ‘Boys Names’. A cut-off score of ≤ 140 to detect PD-MCI was shown to have good sensitivity and specificity values (respectively, 0.85 and 0.54) with this translated version of the DRS-II [48]. It includes five subscales: Attention, Initiation/Perseveration, Construction, Concepts and Memory. Raw scores range from 0 to 144 (respective cut-offs for PDD ≤ 132 and PD-MCI ≤ 140) [48]. The original version was administered at BL and FU-2, and the alternate version was administered at FU-1 and FU-3 to avoid practice effect.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics (mean and standard deviation) were calculated for socio-demographic characteristics of participants, all screening scores and BL variables. Then, T-tests for independent samples were conducted to test for statistical differences between groups. Differences between BL data and all other repeated measures were calculated for primary (DEX, DEX-C and ZMT) and secondary outcomes (DRS-II, PDQ-39). All outcomes were analyzed using a repeated measure ANCOVA (linear mixed model), in which the terms Group (Group A or B), Time (1: BL; 2: mid-point; 3: FU-1; 4: FU-2 and 5: FU-3), the interaction Group*Time, and the covariate (disease duration) were considered as fixed effects. The best correlation matrix between participants’ observations taken over time was selected using the Akaike Information Criterion. In all cases, it was a first order autoregressive structure, AR(1), for unequally spaced time points that best fit the data. Effect sizes (η2) were calculated. Interpretation of effect sizes follows Cohen’s [48]: 0.00 = no effect; 0.01 = small effect; 0.06 = intermediate effect. Following a significant effect of any sources of variation, post hoc comparisons (protected LSD) were conducted to determine which treatment combination differs. The normality assumption was verified using the Shapiro-Wilk’s statistic and the homogeneity of variances was investigated visually using the residuals plot. All statistical analyses were performed with SAS software (version 9.4 for Windows) at the α = 0.05 level of significance and were verified by a professional statistician.

Results

Clinical and sociodemographic characteristics of the final sample, as well as screening neuropsychological evaluation scores and BL scores of each group, are shown in Table 1. At BL, groups show some differences. It seems that Group A comprises older and more educated participants, with scores slightly more elevated regarding Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) stages, Levodopa equivalent dosage (LED) and PDQ-39 scores. The performance on lexical and category fluency tests seems inferior in Group A compared to Group B. The non-significant p-values for these results are likely due to a lack of power to detect differences (given the small sample size). The only statistical difference is that Group A suffers from the disease since a significant longer period than Group B. Disease duration was therefore considered as a covariate in statistical analysis. Otherwise, most participants were retired: four in Group A and four in Group B. Other participants were working part-time or full-time. Missing data were not replaced, and all available data were included in analyses.

Feasibility

Acceptability

Overall, it seems that both interventions were considered acceptable by all participants and most caregivers. The only caregiver who was unable to participate in the study had a severe impairment (major chronic illness). Three participants out of 12 (one in Group A and two in Group B) mentioned they would have liked to continue the interventions. Most participants said they liked the intervention and liked the fact that the trainer came at home. At FU-3, two participants in Group A and three in Group B said they were now frequently using some of the exercises learned during the sessions (ex. Mindfulness, STOP! For Group A, etc.). All participants found the intervention appropriate for their condition.

Demand

All participants and screened patients expressed their interest in the intervention. Most of them (n = 11) considered they wanted to develop new tools to better cope with PD-MCI. Caregivers expressed the need to understand cognitive impairment.

Implementation

During the intervention sessions, all participants were able to execute the exercises and reflections appropriately. However, a participant in Group B fell asleep during a mindfulness exercise. Participants of Group A were given approximately 60 minutes of exercises to practice between intervention sessions each week. All but two participants in Group A did the between-session exercises as suggested by the trainer. The drop-out participant did only 30 minutes of practice per week while another participant did more practice than the suggested amount of time (150 minutes per week). Participants of Group B have not been asked to do exercises between sessions, but three participants did the mindfulness exercises between sessions and requested supplemental books or resources.

Practicality

The clinician/trainer needed material to carry out the interventions and evaluations: a computer and an iPad to conduct the interventions and several neuropsychological tests (listed in results) to assess the participants. As mentioned earlier, each participant was given a workbook. Groups A and B had different workbooks. The trainer also needed a mean of transportation. Most participants were living in a 20-km area.

Through the entire study, the trainer worked eight hours for the neuropsychological assessment and six hours in intervention sessions for each participant. In addition, the trainer travels a total of four hours per participant (transportation expenses). Each participant thus required 20 hours of work from the clinician. In Canada, a junior neuropsychologist in a private setting typically charges 130$ CAD per hour for service at home. For one participant: 20 * 130 = 2600$ CAD which translates into 2151,69$ USD.

Safety

One participant in Group A was unable complete the study after mid-point because of the sudden death of a family member. A Group B caregiver was unable to complete any part of the study due to illness. No change in LED occurred in any patients during intervention. However, LED changes occurred in three participants at follow-up: two Group A participants reported a dosage increase (24.5% of LED at FU-2 and 16.6% of LED at FU-3, respectively), whereas a Group B participant reported the introduction of amantadine (12.6% total increase in LED at FU3). There were four missed sessions in Group A and two in Group B. All participants informed the trainer before the session that they were having last minute difficulty and asked her to postpone the missed session. All reported sessions were retaken.

Before each session, Group A participants presented a mean fatigue score of 4.25 /10 (SD = 1.40) which indicates mild-to-moderate fatigue. After each session, they registered a mean fatigue score of 5.29/10 (SD = 1.35) which indicates moderate fatigue. Before each session, Group B participants had a mean fatigue score of 3.34/10 (SD = 1.77) which indicates mild fatigue. After each session, they reported a mean score of 3.97/10 (SD = 1.9; mild fatigue). Overall, participants of Group A expressed slightly more fatigue before and after sessions than participants of Group B. However, the fatigue remained mild to moderate throughout the study as no patient reported severe fatigue.

Preliminary efficacy testing on primary outcomes: Repeated measures ANCOVAs

Table 2 presents all results for primary and secondary outcomes.

Table 2. Results of the repeated measure ANCOVAs for primary and secondary outcomes.

Fixed effect (df1, df2) DEX-p F-values (p-values) Effect size (η2) DEX-C F-values (p-values) Effect size (η2) ZMT (raw) F-values (p-values) Effect size (η2) ZMT (errors) F-values (p-values) Effect size (η2) PDQ-39 F-values (p-values) Effect size (η2) DRS-II F-values (p-values) Effect size (η2)
Group (1, 10) 0.82 (0.388) 0.008 0.36 (0.564) 0.006 0.21 (0.653) 0.006 1.19 (0.316) 0.082 3.08 (0.109) 0.073 1.42 (0.261) 0.04
Time (4, 36) 2.96 (0.033) * 0.087 6.02 (0.017) * 0.047 0.92 (0.443) 0.07 16.66 (0.001) * 0.102 1.77 (0.156) 0.017 0.23 (0.877) 0.003
Time*Group (4, 36) 0.75 (0.567) 0.008 0.483 (0.749) 0.001 0.56 (0.646) 0.026 3.53 (0.064) 0.057 5.31 (0.002) * 0.066 0.44 (0.723) 0.009
Disease duration (1, 36) 1.60 (0.214) 0.007 17.27 (0.004) * 0.0005 1.76 (0.196) 0.003 6.19 (0.03) * 0.066 0.12 (0.734) 0.004 5.48 (0.027) * 0.05
MCID (SEM) 3.054 4.552 1.116 1.315 4.463 2.483

Notes.

* p < 0.05.

Interpretation of effect sizes follows Cohen’s [55]: 0.00–0.003 = no effect; 0.01–0.039 = small effect; 0.06–0.11 = medium effect; 0.11–0.2 = large effect. Notes. DEX-p: Dysexecutive questionnaire for patients; DEX-C: Dysexecutive questionnaire for caregivers; df: degrees of freedom (1: numerator, 2: denominator); DRS-II: Dementia Rating Scale, second edition; MCID: Minimal Clinical Important Difference (for an indication of change); PDQ-39: Parkinson Disease Questionnaire, 39 items; SEM: Standard Error Mean; ZMT: Zoo Map Test.

DEX self-report

Results show a significant difference on the DEX between measurement times, for both groups (F(4,36) = 2.96, p = 0.033) [CI 95%: 10.75–15.23]. The effect size is considered medium (η2 = 0.087). However, the results are not significantly different between the two groups, for all times of measurement (F(1,10) = 0.82, p = 0.388;). The interaction effect (Group*Time) is not significant (F(4,36) = 0.75, p = 0.567). These results mean that the improvement occurring overtime on the DEX are not significantly different between groups. Furthermore, disease duration, as covariable, has no significant effect on the outcomes between groups (F(1,36) = 1.60, p = 0.214). Post-hoc analysis shows that the changes between time periods occurred between BL and FU1; and BL and FU2. Taken together, these results show a decrease in both groups on DEX scores following the interventions, which means that patients report significantly better executive functions after receiving either of the two treatments.

DEX-C

A first analysis shows there is no evidence that DEX-C scores change after either intervention, even when adjusting for disease duration (p > 0.05 for all fixed effects). However, an outlier was identified (Z score > 3 at baseline and all other measurement times). After removing this outlier from the DEX-C data, analyses were reconducted. Without the outlier, results show a significant difference between measurement times on DEX-C scores for both groups (F(4,36) = 6.02, p = 0.017) [CI 95%: 9.63–17.23]. The effect size is considered small (η2 = 0.047). However, groups are not significantly different between the times of measurement (F(1,10) = 0.36, p = 0.564). The interaction effect (Group*Time) is not significant either (F(4,36) = 0.483, p = 0.749), which means that the improvement in DEX-C scores are not significantly different between groups. Disease duration has a significant effect between groups (F(1,36) = 17.27, p = 0.004) which means that patients with the longest disease duration, when rated by caregivers, improve more executive functions than those with shortest disease duration.

ZMT

ZMT total raw scores do not significantly change after either intervention, even when adjusting for disease duration (p > 0.05 for all fixed effects). However, patients of both groups significantly make fewer ZMT errors between measurement times (F(3,36) = 16.66, p = 0.001) [CI 95%: 1.07–2.93]. The effect size is considered medium (η2 = 0.102). Groups are not significantly different at the different times of measurement (F(1,10) = 1.19, p = 0.316). The interaction effect (Group*Time) is not significant (F(4,36) = 3.553, p = 0.06). Thus, both interventions significantly help to reduce the number of errors in ZMT, although there is a tendency for the GMT group to have more linear results than the other group, in this regard. It is interesting that 5/5 (100%) of Group A (GMT) participants improve or maintain their profile scores at FU-1, FU-2 and FU3. On the other hand, 3/6 (50%) of Group B participants improve or maintain their profile scores at FU-1, whereas 4/6 (66.7%) participants improve or maintain their profile score at FU-2 and FU-3.

Preliminary efficacy testing on secondary efficacy outcomes: Repeated measures ANCOVAS

PDQ-39

There is a significant interaction between measurement times and group condition on PDQ-39 (F(4,36) = 5.31, p = 0.002) [CI 95%: 15.33–25.61]. The effect size is considered medium (η2 = 0.066). Disease duration has no significant effect between groups (F(1,36) = 1.60, p = 0.214). Taken together, these results show that PDQ-39 scores improve in Group B, whereas PDQ-39 scores are maintained in Group A (Fig 3E). Post-hoc analysis shows that changes between groups occurred between BL and mid-point. After mid-point, scores were maintained in both groups.

Fig 3. Effects of cognitive interventions on primary and secondary outcomes.

Fig 3

(A) DEX (self-rated), (B) DEX (caregiver, all data included), (C) ZMT raw scores, (D) ZMT number of errors, (E) PDQ-39 and (F) DRS-II scores overtime, presented as mean +/- SD. Group A (GMT) n = 5; Group B (PSYCH-Mind) n = 6.

DRS-II

There is no change in DRS-II scores after either intervention, even when adjusting for disease duration (p >0.05 for Measurement Time, group and interaction Measurement Time*Group). However, disease duration seems to have an effect between groups (F(1,36) = 5.48, p = 0.027), with patients with longest disease duration performing worse on DRS-II than patients with shorter disease duration. The effect size is considered medium (η2 = 0.05).

Discussion

The objective of the present study was to compare feasibility, safety and preliminary efficacy of GMT and PSYCH-Mind to treat executive dysfunction in PD-MCI.

Safety

There was no adverse event related to the administration of interventions. Changes in medication were reported for three participants, two in GMT group and one in PSYCH-Mind group, and that perhaps could explain some results. These changes were reported at FU-2 or FU-3 and ranged from 12 to 24% increases in LED. However, a review on the effects on cognition of anti-Parkinson medications in PD-MCI has found that such changes on short periods did not significantly affect patients’ cognitive functioning [5]. Since positive outcomes were mostly reported on DEX at FU-1, changes in LED can hardly explain the positive results. Fatigue was tolerable, even though Group A overall reported a little more fatigue than Group B. However, the baseline level of fatigue was higher in Group A, which is expected considering that their disease was slightly more advanced than in Group B.

Feasibility

Concerning acceptability, it seems that both interventions were acceptable and appreciated by patients, caregivers, and the clinician trainer. As for the demand, participants did use in their daily life the strategies and information learned and discussed during the intervention sessions. Both interventions and trainer-recommended exercises were well executed and implemented. Regarding practicability, cost versus benefit estimates of the Adapted-GMT and PSYCH-Mind seems reasonable when compared to those of the classical drill-and-practice cognitive training programs. In the present study, one trainer utilized one computer and an iPad in addition to a neuropsychological battery including tests and questionnaires. For 12 participants, most of the costs were thus due to the salary and transportation expenses of the clinician/trainer. In the typical drill-and-practice cognitive training conducted individually or in group, each participant requires a computer, and must learn how to use the computer to properly run the training program. Furthermore, it often requires the assistance of a coach or trainer (implying a salary to pay) to help participants start the program, to monitor assiduity and difficulties of participants. For the cognitive training practiced in group, one must also consider the cost and difficulty of transportation to the meeting place for each participant. Assiduity may possibly be better when interventions are conducted individually at each patient’s home, because it reduces the difficulty and fatigue of transportation often experienced by patients with PD. The results of the present study versus those of previous studies at least partly support this affirmation. Indeed, previous studies reported that 88% of participants completed 75% of the intervention [49], whereas in the present study, all but one participant (11/12 = 91,6%) completed 100% of the sessions. A possible explanation for better assiduity is the duration of each session and of the program. In previous studies, each session was lasting from 30 minutes to 120 minutes and administered over periods from 8 to 24 weeks whereas in the present study, each session lasts from 60 to 90 minutes and the two programs were administered in five weeks. Overall, these results show that both interventions were feasible.

Moreover, the results obtained by PD-MCI after the Adapted-GMT do compare with results obtained previously by participants with other neurological conditions who completed the original GMT. Indeed, in the Stamenova and Levine meta-analysis [17], the effect sizes of improvement after GMT as measured by DEX or Cognitive Failure Questionnaires were respectively small to medium for patients and medium for the caregivers. In the present study, the effect sizes of improvement on both DEX and ZMT-errors were respectively medium for patients and small for the caregivers (DEX-C).

Furthermore, the current results are at least comparable if not better than those obtained in previous studies on cognitive training in PD-MCI. Indeed, the effect sizes registered here are small to medium whereas in the previous PD studies, they are null or rather small [9, 10]. A possible explanation for this better effect size is the strategy-based (top-down) approach used in the present study as opposed to the drill-and-practice (bottom-up) approach used in prior research. This will be further discussed in the next section.

Preliminary efficacy testing

The results on the two DEX versions indicate that both intervention programs have a positive impact overtime on executive complaints by patients and caregivers. This finding is supported by the significant decrease in errors made on the ZMT by participants of Groups A and B, ZMT being an objective measure of organization and planning functions. However, there was no significant improvement in ZMT total raw scores in both groups, although 100% participants who had received Adapted-GMT improved or maintained their profile score (not reaching statistical significance), even at FU2 and FU3, whereas 67% participants of the PSYCH-Mind group, maintained or improved their profile score after 4 and 12 weeks. Both interventions included education on cognitive symptoms and mindfulness exercises. However, only Adapted-GMT comprises reflective exercises and goal fulfilling strategies during and between the intervention sessions. This content primarily distinguishes the Adapted-GMT from the PSYCH-Mind program and could perhaps explain the slightly different improvement profile between the two groups on the ZMT. Altogether, the DEX and ZMT results suggest that both patient groups may have benefited in some way from the interventions. Interestingly, patients with longer disease duration experienced improvement while their DRS-II scores were inferior to those with shorter disease duration, suggesting they benefited from interventions despite greater overall cognitive impairment.

Only Group B improved quality of life (PDQ-39) four weeks after intervention. This could be explained, at least partly, by the format and material of PSYCH-Mind sessions. They were indeed designed as discussions in which there was information sharing with patients and caregivers about a wide range of different PD symptoms, and there were more opportunities to ask questions than in Group A. By contrast, Adapted-GMT focused exclusively on giving information and teaching strategies to remediate executive deficits. It is noteworthy that although the PSYCH-Mind participants were not asked to practice mindfulness exercises between sessions, half of them reported doing so. This is a positive indicator of both acceptability and demand. Neither intervention influenced overall cognitive functioning of the patients (DRS-II).

To our knowledge, this is the first study conducted in PD-MCI patients to investigate the effects of GMT which taps into top-down processes. Previous cognitive training programs specifically aimed at improving executive functions in PD-MCI involved computerized or paper and pencil exercises, mostly ‘drill and practice’ type of training triggering bottom-up processes [9, 10, 49]. Two very recent studies administered traditional drill-and-practice (bottom-up) cognitive training with a particular focus on improving working memory [50], logical executive functions, episodic memory, attention and processing speed [51]. Ophey et al. [50] assessed the effects of a 5-week individualized computerized training targeting working memory on 75 cognitively healthy PD patients. The participants were 64 years old and had 15 years of education. They were thus younger than the present sample but had approximately the same level of education. Three months after the end of the training, there was only a small effect size on the test of verbal working memory [50]. Bernini et al. [51] assessed the effects of a 3-week computerized cognitive training targeting logical executive functions, episodic memory, attention and processing speed. The 57 participants were 74 years old with 8 years of education, which is in the same age group but with an inferior level of education than the present sample. After the end of the intervention, participants recorded improvements in global cognition, attention and processing speed as measured by neuropsychological tests [50]. Importantly, the improvements were not maintained after six months. These recent studies show positive but modest results as measured by ‘laboratory’ neuropsychological tests, as opposed to the self-measure of executive functioning, evaluating daily functioning, used in the present study [5052].

Thus, very few of these studies intended to learn strategies to PD patients triggering top-down processes [10, 5355]. As a result, the previous cognitive training studies generally showed limited improvements on trained tasks, with very little if any transfers in daily activities [9, 10]. The need to transfer the benefit of cognitive training to daily life tasks is supported by the study of Vlagsma et al. [55] who showed that PD patients expressed the same rehabilitation goals, anchored in routine daily tasks than dysexecutive patients with acquired brain injury. Before the present study, only two trials including PD patients utilized cognitive rehabilitation to address the issue of improving cognition in daily functioning. Cognitive rehabilitation is a type of intervention for patients with dementia that focus on individualized goals to improve one or more specific activities of daily activities (ADLs). To do this, the clinical instructor trains the patient in the use of restorative and compensatory cognitive techniques, has him apply these techniques to one or more ADLs to be improved, and has him practice several times a day and a week. As a result, the specific ADL that has been targeted may improve, but the improvement usually does not generalize to other ADLs. In the first study [56], 240 PD patients with any MCI profile were randomly allocated to one of the three training groups: Group A received cognitive training (paper and pencil, and computer) only; Group B received the same cognitive training in addition to transfer (rehabilitation) training whereas Group C also received the same cognitive training in addition to transfer and psychomotor trainings. After six months of training, Group C registered the best improvement on two measures of overall cognitive functioning, the ADAS-Cog and the SCOPA-Cog, as well as on quality of life. More recently, Hindle et al. [54] administered to patients with PDD a goal-oriented cognitive rehabilitation program. The authors reported that after two months of treatment, results of cognitive rehabilitation were better than results of treatment-as-usual and relaxation therapy for the primary outcomes of self-rated goal attainment. After six months, the outcomes of cognitive rehabilitation remained superior to those of treatment-as-usual. These last two trials positively impacted the daily functioning of PD patients. However, the first one was very costly with an implementation in both a rehabilitation centre and at home, and requiring, among other materials, a computer for each patient in both locations. In addition, it involved a physiotherapist for the patient, a coach for the caregivers [55]. The second study administered a less costly program, but that was designed for patients suffering from dementia, which is not appropriate for PD-MCI [54].

Therefore, cognitive training programs for PD-MCI including strategy learning to transfer in various situations and tasks of daily life are still scarce but available data show promising results. The present study is the only one, though, which evaluated the effect of cognitive interventions using both inputs of participants and caregivers, the latter being considered a generalization measure [48]. Indeed, these changes on the DEX can be considered as an effectiveness clinical outcome, given that both patients and caregivers reported significant changes in their daily lives.

Only one previous study conducted in PD patients combined psychoeducation with mindfulness in intervention sessions. This study showed modest but, like our trial, positive results on quality of life. In addition, the authors reported an improvement of depressive symptoms [27]. This was not the case in our study, probably because PD patients were not included if, besides PD-MCI, they met the diagnostic criteria for a psychiatric disorder like major depression. The program of Advocat et al. [27], as well as the standardized psychoeducation program for PD, the Patient Education Program Parkinson (PEPP), do not include education on cognitive symptoms [21]. Studies on the PEPP show that it increases mood and reduces burden among PD patients and caregivers [2123], but it does not provide information on the impact of the PEPP on cognitive functions, contrary to the present PSYCH-Mind program. As for mindfulness administered alone, none of the previous studies included PD-MCI participants, nor did they give information on their cognitive decline [18]. It is noteworthy that sessions combining psychoeducation with mindfulness exercises resulted in a decrease of executive complaints, as well as an increase in quality of life in the present study. This seems to be the first study investigating the combined effect of mindfulness and psychoeducation on cognitive symptoms in PD-MCI.

What could be the possible neurological mechanisms underlying the effects of these top-down interventions? Recent literature might give a possible explanation. A recent study explored the neurological effects of a working memory cognitive training in a patient [57], with PET scan technology. After the training, they showed a significant decrease in the activation of the frontal-parietal circuit during tests requiring working memory, showing that these tests required less and less resources for the patient [57]. This is one of the first studies to show a link between the activity of a neural circuit and a form of cognitive training in PD. In short, it seems that the strategy-learning could have an impact on neural networks involved in cognitive deficits.

According to literature PD-MCI with executive dysfunctions is often linked to dopaminergic decrease in the mesocorticolimbic pathways [14]. In the present study, it is possible to suppose that these pathways might be activated more efficiently with top-down interventions.

Present results show the importance of considering participant’s clinical profile and demographics to personalize treatment. Previous studies included participants aged between 58.8 and 70.9 years old, which is slightly younger than the present sample, with shorter disease duration (between 2 and 8 years), and similar disease stage (H&Y 2–3). However, they provided more training sessions per week (2 to 4, each lasting 30–90 minutes) as well as more sessions for the entire programs (10–16 sessions over 5–12 weeks) and obtained similar or less benefit compared to the present study [9, 10]. Older PD participants whose illness lasts longer may be more easily tired and need more time to recover between training sessions than younger counterparts. Thus, the present participants may have benefited from a reduced number of sessions (5 sessions) delivered over the same amount of time (5 weeks) than previous studies to improve after Adapted-GMT and PSYCH-Mind programs. The spaced training schedule associated with home administration possibly helped to counter the effect of age and fatigue, even if the duration of each session was comparable to that in previous studies.

A last important aspect to consider is the opinion of caregivers included, especially those who share the patient’s everyday routine. The decrease in DEX-C scores registered in both groups was maintained over a three-month period, except for that of a caregiver who was excluded from analysis. This particular Adapted-GMT caregiver observed more important motor deterioration in her husband’ symptoms. As a result, the patient medication had to be adjusted between FU1 and FU2, and there was a significant increase of PDQ-39 and DEX and DEX-C scores, indicating a deterioration of executive functions and quality of life. In other words, the disease condition of this patient worsened over the course of the study. Previous studies on cognitive training in PD participants often do not include questionnaires answered by caregivers about the patient’s cognitive abilities [9, 58]. However, the perception, by the caregiver, of the progress made by the patient, can eventually have an impact on his own feeling of burden.

Although potential lack of statistical power is a major caveat, the present trial does present some strong points. First, interventions were tailored to the needs of participants with executive dysfunctions, especially more important in Group A. The exhaustive neuropsychological evaluation at screening ensured that the intervention matched the participant’s cognitive profile.

Secondly, this is among the first studies to assess effects of two cognitive interventions at home that provided the same time and attention to all participants; one program directly targeted improving executive functions while the other attempted to improve cognition through education and the practice of mindfulness. Results show that both programs with a top-down approach could be effective in improving day-to-day executive functioning in PD-MCI patients. Furthermore, participants from both groups mentioned enjoying the interventions indicating that patient-tailored interventions at home are not only feasible, well tolerated, and safe but can also be enjoyable. Both interventions were easily implemented and administered by a clinician.

Another advantage is that both interventions can be considered more economical than bottom-up computerized cognitive training administered in groups. Indeed, given the price of the material and software needed for computerized training, as well as the difficulties and transportation expenses for the patients, Adapted-GMT and PSYCH-Mind are more economical for the potential benefits of the interventions. Given the small drop-out rates and high adherence, these interventions seem also more advantageous. Future research shall continue to examine which participants benefit the most to a given intervention.

Finally, home sessions seem better for therapeutic alliance, anxiety and treatment compliance [12]. In fact, many participants mentioned they appreciated the therapist coming to their home.

Limitations

The major limitation of this study is obviously the small number of participants, which does not allow drawing general conclusions applicable to all PD-MCI patients. Lack of statistical power might also explain why there was no significant difference between the two groups in ZMT errors and raw scores after the intervention. However, statistical power was sufficient for the principal objectives of this study, which were to assess feasibility and safety as well as preliminary efficacy.

Because of the small sample size, it is not possible to assume group equivalency. The non-significant p-values regarding patients’ characteristics are likely due to a lack of power to detect differences. Indeed, the groups show a few differences that fit with the significant longer disease duration presented by participants of Group A. This finding suggests that progression of the disease is more advanced in Group A. For instance, the fatigue reported in Group A might be explained by the more advanced disease stage. However, it is interesting to underline that the effect of Adapted-GMT is significant in this group on both DEX scores and ZMT errors, despite the fact that participants suffer from the disease since the longest time.

The short duration of intervention, despite being an advantage for vulnerable and frail patients, might also be considered as another possible limitation. A five-week program might not be long enough to have a significant, lasting impact on neuropsychological tests [49], but it is quite typical of feasibility studies [31]. In previous studies assessing the effects of cognitive intervention in PD, most programs lasted between 4 and 12 weeks [9, 10] and often were administered to PD patients without MCI [9, 10]. However, it was important in the present study to offer the shortest possible program, so PD progression or medication changes could be better (at least partly) controlled. On the other hand, GMT studies tend to calculate the number of training hours more than the number of sessions [17]. In the present trial, Adapted-GMT group received from 12 to 20 hours of total training hours (including between sessions exercises), which is average compared to other GMT studies giving approximately a mean of 15 hours of training [17]. Therefore, training time in the Adapted-GMT group seems adequate in the present study.

Another, but important limitation concerns the blinding procedure which could cause an investigator bias. The same clinician was indeed in charge of all interventions and was aware of the allocation of participants in each group. However, that procedure was chosen because it allows to control for the trainer characteristics, such as therapeutic alliance [59].

At last, a performance bias could have affected the way participants acted through the study. For instance, this bias may have prompted them to exercise more often between sessions to please the research team.

Conclusion

Although cognitive symptoms in PD are frequent and have adverse consequences for patients and caregivers, available data on cognitive interventions are still scarce, and accessibility to tailored cognitive treatments is restrained. Both interventions were acceptable, feasible, easily implemented and showed satisfactory preliminary efficacy. According to the present results, interventions could be suggested to patients aged between 50 and 75 years old, with mild to moderate motor symptoms and moderate executive complaints. It seems also that patients with a longer disease duration can complete the Adapted-GMT sessions without too much fatigue. In the future, the effects of these two top-down programs should be assessed in larger samples of PD-MCI with executive dysfunctions and presenting with other clinical characteristics.

Supporting information

S1 Checklist. CONSORT 2010 checklist of information to include when reporting a randomised trial*.

(DOCX)

S1 File

(DOCX)

S2 File

(ZIP)

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Dr Brian Levine and Baycrest Centre for granting permission to use the GMT material, and Dr Leslie Fellows and her team at McGill University for their contribution to the GMT translation in French. Finally, the authors wish to thank Réseau Parkinson Québec as well as patients and families who accepted to participate in this study.

Data Availability

Data availability: The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, AGR, upon reasonable request. You can also contact Laval University data research manager at the following email address: gdr@ulaval.ca Data can be accessed at the following page (please send an access request): Giguère-Rancourt, Ariane, 2020, "Cognitive Training in Parkinson’s disease", https://doi.org/10.5683/SP2/0LMBRX, Scholars Portal Dataverse, V1, UNF:6:RAQT1DXYblrlo+gpYplHyA== [fileUNF].

Funding Statement

Funding details: AGR was supported by a doctoral training award from Laval University’s Fonds Facultaire d’Enseignement et de Recherche (FFER). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Litvan I, Goldman JG, Tröster AI, Schmand BA, Weintraub D, Petersen RC, et al. Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Movement Disorder Society Task Force guidelines. Mov Disord. 2012; 27(3): 349–356. doi: 10.1002/mds.24893 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Dirnberger G, & Jahanshahi M. Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: a review. J of Neuropsych. 2013; 7(2); 193–224. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kalbe E, Rehberg SP, Heber I, Kronenbuerger M, Schulz JB, Storch A, et al. Subtypes of mild cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson’s disease: evidence from the LANDSCAPE study. J of Neurol, Neurosurg, and Psych. 2016; 87(10): 1099–1105. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2016-313838 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Chahine LM, Weintraub D, Hawkins KA, Siderowf A, Eberly S, Oakes D, et al. Cognition in individuals at risk for Parkinson’s: Parkinson associated risk syndrome (PARS) study findings. Mov Disord. 2016: 31(1); 86–94. doi: 10.1002/mds.26373 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Roy MA, Doiron M, Talon-Croteau J, Dupré N, & Simard M. Effects of Antiparkinsonians on Cognition in Parkinson’s Disease: A Systematic Review. Can J of Neurol Sci. 2018: 45; 375–404. doi: 10.1017/cjn.2018.21 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Connolly BS & Lang AE. Pharmacological treatment of Parkinson’s Disease A Review. JAMA. 2014; 311(16): 1670–1683. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.3654 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Pagano G, Rengo G, Pasqualetti G, Femminella GD, Monzani F, Ferrara N, et al. Cholinesterase inhibitors for Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J of Neurol, Neurosurg & Psych. 2015; 86(7): 767–773. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-308764 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rolinski M, Fox C, Maidment I, & McShane R. Cholinesterase inhibitors for dementia with Lewy bodies. Parkinson’s disease dementia and cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Cochrane database reviews. 2012; 14(3). doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD006504.pub2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Couture M, Giguère-Rancourt A, & Simard M. The Impact of Cognitive Intervention on Cognitive Symptoms in Idiopathic Parkinson’s Disease: A Systematic Review. Aging, Neuropsych and Cog. 2019; 26(5): 637–659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lawrence BJ, Gasson N, Bucks RS, Troeung L, & Loftus AM. Cognitive Training and Noninvasive Brain Stimulation for Cognition in Parkinson’s Disease: A Meta-analysis. Neurorehab and Neural Rep. 2017; 31(7): 597–608. doi: 10.1177/1545968317712468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Berg D., Borghammer P., Fereshtehnejad S. M., Heinzel S., Horsager J., Schaeffer E., et al. Prodromal Parkinson disease subtypes—key to understanding heterogeneity. Nature Rev Neurol. 2021, 17 (6): 349–361. doi: 10.1038/s41582-021-00486-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Goldman JG, Vernaleo BA, Camicioli R, Dahodwala N, Dobkin RD, Ellis T, et al. Cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: a report from a multidisciplinary symposium on unmet needs and future directions to maintain cognitive health. NPJ Parkinson’s Disease. 2018; 4(1): 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41531-018-0055-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Poewe W, Seppi K, Tanner CM, Halliday GM, Brundin P, Volkmann J, et al. Parkinson disease. Nature Rev Dis Prim. 2017; 3: 17013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mowszowski L, Lampit A, Walton CC, & Naismith SL. Strategy-Based Cognitive Training for Improving Executive Functions in Older Adults: a Systematic Review. Neuropsych rev. 2016; 26(3), 252–270. doi: 10.1007/s11065-016-9329-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Duncan J. Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage. Cogn Neuropsych. 1986; 3(3): 271–290. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Robertson IH. Goal Management Training: A clinical manual. PsyConsult. 1996; Cambridge, U.K.
  • 17.Stamenova V & Levine B. Effectiveness of goal management training® in improving executive functions: A meta-analysis. Neuropsych rehab. 2019; 1–31. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2018.1438294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Giguère-Rancourt A, Plourde M, Doiron M, Langlois M, Dupré N, & Simard M. Goal management training® home-based approach for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: a multiple baseline case report. Neurocase. 2019; 24(5–6): 276–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Rogers G, Davies D, Pink J, & Cooper P. Parkinson’s disease: summary of updated NICE guidance. BMJ. 2017; 358: j1951. doi: 10.1136/bmj.j1951 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Grimes D, Gordon J, Snelgrove B, Lim-Carter I, Fon E, Martin W, et al. Canadian guidelines on Parkinson’s disease. Can J of neurolog sci. 2012; 39(4 Suppl 4): S1–S30. doi: 10.1017/s031716710001516x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hellqvist C, Berterö C, Dizdar N, Sund-Levander M, & Hagell P. Self-management education for persons with Parkinson’s disease and their care partners: A quasi-experimental case-control study in clinical practice. Parkinson’s Dis. 2020. doi: 10.1155/2020/6920943 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Navarta-Sánchez MV, Ursua ME, Fernández MR, Ambrosio L, Medina M, deCerio SD, et al. Implementation of a multidisciplinary psychoeducational intervention for Parkinson’s disease patients and carers in the community: study protocol. BMC fam pract. 2018; 19(1): 45. doi: 10.1186/s12875-018-0730-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.A’Campo LEI, Wekking EM, Spliethoff-Kamminga NGA, Le Cessie S, & Roos RAC. Caregiver education in Parkinson’s disease: formative evaluation of a standardized program in seven European countries. Qual of Life Res. 2010; 19(1): 55–64. doi: 10.1007/s11136-009-9559-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.McLean G, Lawrence M, Simpson R, & Mercer SW. Mindfulness-based stress reduction in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review. BMC neurol. 2017; 17(1), 92. doi: 10.1186/s12883-017-0876-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.van der Heide A, Meinders MJ, Speckens AE, Peerbolte TF, Bloem BR, & Helmich, RC. Stress and mindfulness in Parkinson’s Disease: clinical effects and potential underlying mechanisms. Mov Disord. 2021; 36(1): 64–70. doi: 10.1002/mds.28345 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gard T, Hölzel BK, & Lazar SW. The potential effects of meditation on age-related cognitive decline: a systematic review. Ann of the NY Acad of Sci. 2014; 1307: 89. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12348 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Advocat J, Enticott J, Vandenberg B, Hassed C, Hester J, & Russell G. The effects of a mindfulness-based lifestyle program for adults with Parkinson’s disease: a mixed method, wait list controlled randomised control study. BMC neurol. 2016; 16(1), 166. doi: 10.1186/s12883-016-0685-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, & Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinicopathological study of 100 cases. J of Neurol, Neurosurg, and Psych. 1992; 55(3): 181–184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Emre M, Aarsland D, Brown R, Burn DJ, Duyckaerts C, Mizuno Y, et al. Clinical diagnostic criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Mov disord. 2007; 22(12): 1689–1707. doi: 10.1002/mds.21507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.GMT products: https://shop.baycrest.org/collections/the-goal-management-training-program
  • 31.Bowen DJ, Kreuter M, Spring B, Cofta-Woerpel L, Linnan L, Weiner D, et al. How we design feasibility studies. American journal of preventive medicine. 2009; 36(5), 452–457. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.02.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Pearson N., Naylor PJ., Ashe M.C. et al. Guidance for conducting feasibility and pilot studies for implementation trials. Pilot Feasibility Stud 6, 167 (2020). doi: 10.1186/s40814-020-00634-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition. TX: Pearson. 2008; San Antonio, Texas.
  • 34.Benton AL. Contributions to neuropsychological assessment: clinical manual. Oxford University Press. 1994; USA. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.St-Hilaire A, Hudon C, Vallet GT, Bherer L, Lussier M, Gagnon JF., et al. Normative Data for Phonemic and Semantic Verbal Fluency Test in the Adult French-Quebec Population and Validation Study in Alzheimer’s Disease and Depression. Clin Neuropsychol. 2016; 30(7): 1126–1150. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2016.1195014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Stern RA & White T. Neuropsychological Assessment Battery. WPS. 2003.
  • 37.Dion M, Potvin O, Belleville S, Ferland G, Renaud M, Bherer L, et al. Normative Data for the Rappel libre/Rappel indicé à 16 items (16-item Free and Cued Recall) in the Elderly Quebec-French Population. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015; 28: 1–19. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2014.915058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Benedict RHB, Schretlen D, Groninger L, Dobraski M, & Shpritz B. Revision of the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test: Studies of normal performance, reliability, and validity. Psych Assess. 1996; 8(2): 145–153. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Delis D, Kaplan E, & Kramer J. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. The Psychological Corporation. 2001; San Antonio, Texas. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sala SD, Foley JA, Beschin N, Allerhand M, & Logie RH. Assessing Dual-Task Performance Using a Paper-and-Pencil Test: Normative Data. Arch of Clin Neuropsych: Official J of Nat Acad of Neuropsychol. 2010; 25(5): 410–419. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kessels R, van Zandvoort M, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, & de Haan EH. The Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Standardization and Normative Data. 2000; 252–258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wilson BA, Alderman N, Burgess PW, Emslie H, & Evans JJ. The behavioural assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome (BADS). Thames Valley Company. 1996; Bury St Edmunds.
  • 43.Kandiah N, Zhang A, Cenina AR, Au WL, Nadkarni N, & Tan LC. Montreal Cognitive Assessment for the screening and prediction of cognitive decline in early Parkinson’s disease. Parkinson & Rel Disord. 2014; 20(11): 1145–1148. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Vlagsma TT, Koerts J, Tucha O, Dijkstra HT, Duits AA, van Laar T, et al. Objective versus subjective measures of executive functions: predictors of participation and quality of life in Parkinson disease? Arch of Phys Med and Rehab. 2017; 98(11): 2181–2187. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2017.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wilson BA, Evans JJ, Emslie H, Alderman N, & Burgess P. The development of an ecologically valid test for assessing patients with dysexecutive syndrome. Neuropsych Rehab. 1998; 8(3): 213–228. doi: 10.1080/713755570 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Peto V, Jenkinson C, & Fitzpatrick R. PDQ-39: a review of the development, validation and application of a Parkinson’s disease quality of life questionnaire and its associated measures. J of Neurol. 1998; 245: S10–4. doi: 10.1007/pl00007730 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Mattis S. Dementia Rating Scale-2. Psychological Assessment Ressources Inc. 2001; Lutz, Florida.
  • 48.Matteau E, Dupré N, Langlois M, Provencher P, & Simard M. Clinical validity of the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale-2 in Parkinson disease with MCI and dementia. J of Geriat Psych and Neurol. 2012; 25(2): 100–106. doi: 10.1177/0891988712445086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Orgeta V, McDonald KR, Poliakoff E, Hindle JV, Clare L, & Leroi I. Cognitive training interventions for dementia and mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Cochrane Datab of System Rev. 2020; (2). doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011961.pub2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ophey A, Giehl K, Rehberg S, Eggers C, Reker P, van Eimeren T, et al. Effects of working memory training in patients with Parkinson’s disease without cognitive impairment: A randomized controlled trial. Parkinsonism & Rel disord. 2020; 72: 13–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bernini S, Panzarasa S, Barbieri M, Sinforiani E, Quaglini S, Tassorelli C, et al. A double-blind randomized controlled trial of the efficacy of cognitive training delivered using two different methods in mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: preliminary report of benefits associated with the use of a computerized tool. Aging Clin & Experim Res. 2020; 1–9. doi: 10.1007/s40520-020-01665-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sanchez-Luengos I, Balboa-Bandeira Y, Lucas-Jiménez O, Ojeda N, Peña J, & Ibarretxe-Bilbao N. Effectiveness of Cognitive Rehabilitation in Parkinson’s Disease: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J of Personal Med. 2021; 11(5): 429. doi: 10.3390/jpm11050429 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Clare L, Teale JC, Toms G, Kudlicka A, Evans I, Abrahams S., et al. Cognitive rehabilitation, self-management, psychotherapeutic and caregiver support interventions in progressive neurodegenerative conditions: a scoping review. Neuro Rehab. 2018; 43(4): 443–471. doi: 10.3233/NRE-172353 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Hindle JV, Watermeyer TJ, Roberts J, Brand A, Hoare Z, Martyr A, et al. Goal-orientated cognitive rehabilitation for dementias associated with Parkinson’s disease—A pilot randomised controlled trial. Internat J of geriat psych. 2018; 33(5), 718–728. doi: 10.1002/gps.4845 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Vlagsma TT, Koerts J, Fasotti L, Tucha O, van Larr T, Dijkstra H, et al. Parkinson’ patients executive complains: why is cognitive rehabilitation not common practice? Neuropsych Rehab. 2016; 26(2): 216–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Reuter I, Mehnert S, Sammer G, Oechsner M, & Engelhardt M. Efficacy of a multimodal cognitive rehabilitation including psychomotor and endurance training in Parkinson’s disease. J of Aging Res. 2012; 235765. doi: 10.1155/2012/235765 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Walton L, Domellöf ME, Boraxbekk CJ, Domellöf E, Rönnqvist L, Bäckström D, et al. (2021). The Effects of Working Memory Updating Training in Parkinson’s Disease: A Feasibility and Single-Subject Study on Cognition, Movement and Functional Brain Response. Frontiers in psychology, vol. 11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cohen J. Statistical Power for the behavioral sciences (2nd edition). Lawrence Elbaum Associates. 1988; USA. 579 pages.
  • 59.Constantino MJ, Boswell JF, Coyne AE, Kraus DR, & Castonguay LG. Who works for whom and why? Integrating therapist effects analysis into psychotherapy outcome and process research. In LG Castonguay & CE Hill (Editors.) How and why are some therapists better than others? Understanding therapist effects. American Psychological Association. 2017; 55–68.

Decision Letter 0

Walid Kamal Abdelbasset

5 May 2021

PONE-D-21-08484

Goal Management Training and Psychoeducation for Treatment of Executive Dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: A Feasibility Pilot Trial.

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Giguère-Rancourt,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jun 19 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Walid Kamal Abdelbasset, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for submitting your clinical trial to PLOS ONE and for providing the name of the registry and the registration number. The information in the registry entry suggests that your trial was registered after patient recruitment began. PLOS ONE strongly encourages authors to register all trials before recruiting the first participant in a study.

As per the journal’s editorial policy, please include in the Methods section of your paper:

1) your reasons for your delay in registering this study (after enrolment of participants started);

2) confirmation that all related trials are registered by stating: “The authors confirm that all ongoing and related trials for this drug/intervention are registered”.

3. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

4. Please include a copy of Table 1 which you refer to in your text on page 7.

5. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you for giving me the opportunity to review this article.

Abstract:

1. Mention the acronym of abbreviations when it is used for first time. (GMT, PDQ, DEX, ZMT etc..)

2. Include the study design, randomization and allocation procedure.

3. Mention the treatment for the both groups.

4. Results part should be more informative including CI 95% with p scores.

5. Make the conclusion more precise and avoid abbreviations.

Introduction

6. Please change the reference pattern as per author guidelines.

7. Please avoid subtitles.

8. How come this pilot study is differing from (McLean et al., 2017 and Stamenova & Levine, 2019 studies?

9. The introduction part fails to show the research gap and prove the novelty with recent references.

10. Add the clinical significance of this article over the participants and researchers.

Methods

11. Include the clinical trial registration number.

12. Include the study design, randomization and allocation procedure in detail.

13. Mention the selection criteria in detail (exclusion).

14. Include the intervention procedures in detail for study repetition.

15. Whether the interventions are performed at clinic? If performed at clinic means, how they are monitored?

16. Include the reliability and validity of outcome measures with references.

17. What is the need of doing sample size calculation as it is a feasibility pilot trial.

18. Mention about the blinding procedure.

Results

19. Include the information about the test used for analyzing homogeneity and its interpretation.

20. Include the reports with CI 95% with p scores.

21. Mention the effect size of primary and secondary variables and its MCID scores.

Discussion

22. Summarize the discussion part.

23. The discussion part should discuss the relation between the outcome variables and PD patients with latest references.

24. Make the conclusion more precise and avoid abbreviations.

25. Future recommendations and clinical significance of the study is missing.

Reviewer #2: In this study, the authors examined feasibility and preliminary efficacy of two home-based cognitive interventions in patients with Parkinson’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Six patients experiencing executive dysfunction were randomized to each intervention, which consisted of weekly 60-90 minute sessions for five weeks. Measures were collected at baseline, mid-point, one-week, four week and 12-week follow-ups. Executive functions were assessed by the DEX questionnaire and Zoo Map Test. Secondary outcomes were quality of life and presence of psychiatric symptoms.

On the Consort checklist, the authors have entered that information on the registration of the trial, and the availability of the trial protocol, are “N/A.” This information (as follows) is provided in a letter to the editor, but should also be included in the manuscript:

“In the compressed file uploaded, you will find the originial protocol that was accepted by the Ethical Committee of CHU de Québec - Université Laval, as well as the last confirmation letter sent by the ethical committee. As you will see, the original documents are in French. In order to provide an english translation, we attached the Clinicaltrial.gov registration that explains the ethically approved protocol in English. The clinicaltrial.gov number of the trial is: NCT04636541.”

The study has a number of strengths. The authors have done an excellent job of justifying and describing the tests used in the study, and of explaining the test modifications and reasons for these in the cohort. Obtaining neuropsychological measurements at baseline is also a strength of the study. The analyses are clearly described and the paper has been nicely and clearly written. I do have some comments and questions, however.

Methods

• Were the same mindfulness exercises used for both groups?

• Did authors adjust for time spent doing mindfulness exercises between sessions in the GMT group?

• In the psychoeducation group? Did these differ between the two groups?

• Are there different versions of the ZMT or are patients repeating the same test/questions each time they are evaluated?

• Power analysis: What “effect size” in the study did the authors base the power calculation on? Change in DEX scores? Change in ZMT? Feasibility (if so, please define how this was measured in terms of sample size calculation)? Was the study powered for feasibility or efficacy? The authors should clarify. Also, please explain what a “medium” effect size means in terms of the quantitative measurement scores/changes (or whatever the primary measure was on which the power was calculated).

Results

• The authors should not assume that the two groups are comparable at baseline. In terms of LED and some of the Screening Neuropsychological Evaluation tests (Lexical fluency, NAB, e.g.), results appear quite different, and the non-significant p-values are likely due to a lack of power to detect differences (given the very small sample size). This should be acknowledged in the results in addition to being discussed as a limitation.

• The authors state in the Methods that non-parametric tests were used to compare groups. Data in Table 1 should therefore be summarized with non-parametric measures (median, IQR). If variables are actually normally distributed, they should be summarized with means and standard deviations and compared using parametric tests, which are more powerful when data are normally distributed.

Results

• In presenting the results, are the authors equating safety with feasibility? Given that “the aim of this pilot study was to test ease of implementation, differential safety and preliminary efficacy,” a section that addresses feasibility should be presented in the results section, probably prior to the sections on efficacy (for which the study was not really powered, I don’t think?).

• Results for DEX (self-report) are given “regardless of group.” Do the authors mean that all twelve patients were analyzed together? This should be more clearly stated, and analyses over the entire group should be distinguished from analyses between groups throughout the Results section.

• Post-hoc analyses should not be done if the omnibus test is not significant. I think it is fine for the authors to give these data descriptively, without p-values attached/interpreted.

• Again, given the very small sample size, an absence of difference between groups has not been shown, as this could be a power issue. The study has not been powered to detect interactions, which would be difficult to see with such a small sample size. Adjusting for covariates also has little chance of resulting in significance. The authors would do well to be more descriptive in presenting results. Although I appreciate the fact that the authors have used repeated measures analyses, they should acknowledge that given the small sample size, these would be unlikely to have significant results. Conclusions should not be based on not finding significant differences between groups.

• DEX-C: post-hoc analyses are not really appropriate here and should be excluded.

Discussion

• Discussion of feasibility results should be given precedence over “preliminary efficacy” in the discussion section. The authors, in discussing feasibility, should address the obvious fact that while at-home, highly individualized programs are more likely to be successful than not, they are also expensive and difficult to implement on a large scale. They are expensive in terms of the time of those administering the tests/services and conducting these in the home also requires scheduling effort and additional traveling time. This seems like the biggest drawback to this type of intervention and should definitely be discussed. Would insurance pay for this type of intervention? Because of the progressive nature of PD, it would have to be ongoing, and not a one-time intervention, which is also a concern in terms of cost.

• “Patients with longer disease duration registered improvement while their DRS-II scores were inferior to those with shorter disease duration, suggesting they benefited from interventions despite a more global cognitive impairment.” This is very likely due to regression to the mean.

• The assumption that patients if the psychoeducation/mindfulness group were motivated by their intervention to practice mindfulness between sessions is just an assumption. Involvement in a trial is also well-known to influence the behavior of the “control” group to adopt “intervention” practices.

• “…this sample size is typical for this kind of phase 1 study in the field of cognitive intervention.” Please omit this statement. I do not feel that doing something because “everyone else does it” is a valid justification. The reasons the authors have given sufficiently address this point.

• Again, the labor-intensive/expensive nature of the intervention should be addressed as a limitation.

Overall, this study has been nicely done and was a pleasure to review. I do feel that the authors should be more conservative in their conclusions, however, since the study was not powered for anything other than preliminary efficacy. It nonetheless offers important information on an intervention that has promise and in an area where it is widely needed. But more emphasis needs to be placed on feasibility of implementation than on efficacy, since the study is clearly underpowered to detect differences between the two groups, and a finding of “no difference” doesn’t mean there is no difference.

Reviewer #3: thanks alot for effort in this paper

i have minor issues want to be clear

Introduction

need editing and rewrite

Abstract:

Methods section is poorly framed. It has to be re-written.

Demographic profile of patients is not mentioned clear

Discussion:

needs to be as per well defined objectives

Describe sources of potential bias and imprecision

It has to be framed in such a way that readers are able to have good understanding of the current evidences and rationale of the paper

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Gopal Nambi

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Feb 18;17(2):e0263108. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0263108.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


2 Nov 2021

Editorial comments

1. Thank you for updating your Data Availability statement: "Data availability: The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, AGR, upon reasonable request.

Giguère-Rancourt, Ariane, 2020, "Cognitive Training in Parkinson's disease", https://can01.safelinks.protection.outlook.com/?url=https%3A%2F%2Fdoi.org%2F10.5683%2FSP2%2F0LMBRX&data=04%7C01%7Cariane.giguere-rancourt.1%40ulaval.ca%7C76e25e3f2895440eafdc08d98df6630e%7C56778bd56a3f4bd3a26593163e4d5bfe%7C1%7C0%7C637696911624743123%7CUnknown%7CTWFpbGZsb3d8eyJWIjoiMC4wLjAwMDAiLCJQIjoiV2luMzIiLCJBTiI6Ik1haWwiLCJXVCI6Mn0%3D%7C1000&sdata=1xMLTa1Y1kFvUt2%2Fsk9y%2B%2Box0UKjZEFTfnVSHimn3nU%3D&reserved=0, Scholars Portal Dataverse, V1, UNF:6:RAQT1DXYblrlo+gpYplHyA== [fileUNF]"

Please note that PLOS ONE's data policy requires a non-author institutional contact for data access in the interest of maintaining long-term data accessibility. At this time, please provide a non-author point of contact that is able to receive queries regarding data access.

RESPONSE: We added an institutional point of contact e-mail address for the database: gdr@ulaval.ca (the address of the research data manager at Laval university).

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.pdf

Decision Letter 1

Walid Kamal Abdelbasset

29 Nov 2021

PONE-D-21-08484R1Goal Management Training and Psychoeducation / Mindfulness for Treatment of Executive Dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: A Feasibility Pilot Trial.PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Giguère-Rancourt,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 13 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Walid Kamal Abdelbasset, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #3: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Reviewer comments

Thank you for giving me this opportunity to review this article.

Abstract:

1. Summarize the abstract (follow the abstract guidelines).

2. Include the study duration and eligibility criteria of study participants.

3. Mention the reports with 95%CI (Upper – lower limit) for all the variables.

4. The conclusion should be more concise and drawn on the basis of study reports.

Manuscript

1. Summarize the introductory part.

2. How come this trial is differing from reference number 9? – please justify.

3. Please describe about Psych – mind treatment and its benefits.

4. The authors fail to find and report the research gap in this session.

5. Include the clinical significance of this trial over clinicians, patients and researchers.

6. Present the manuscript as per CONSORT guidelines.

7. Include the study setting and study duration.

8. Include the reliability and validity of all the outcome measures used in the study.

9. Include the detail description of intervention and control group.

10. Include the method of sample size calculation with suitable reference.

11. Mention about the demographic details of the participants in the results section.

12. In the results section, please discuss about the treatment compliance rate, adverse effects and the number of dropouts.

13. Mention the reports with 95%CI (Upper – lower limit) for all the variables.

14. Report the effect size and MCID values of all the primary and secondary variables.

15. Summarize the discussion part and include the mechanism of interventions on different variables with recent references.

16. The conclusion should be more concise and drawn on the basis of study reports.

Reviewer #3: thanks alot for your response

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Dr.Gopal Nambi

Reviewer #3: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Feb 18;17(2):e0263108. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0263108.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


21 Dec 2021

Referee(s)' Comments to Author:

Reviewer #1: Reviewer comments

Thank you for giving me this opportunity to review this article.

Abstract:

1. Summarize the abstract (follow the abstract guidelines). RESPONSE: the abstract was summarized in 300 words.

2. Include the study duration and eligibility criteria of study participants. RESPONSE: Added. Please see the method section of the abstract.

3. Mention the reports with 95%CI (Upper – lower limit) for all the variables. RESPONSE: Added.

4. The conclusion should be more concise and drawn on the basis of study reports. RESPONSE: The conclusion was summarized.

Manuscript

1. Summarize the introductory part. RESPONSE: The introduction was briefly summarized.

2. How come this trial is differing from reference number 9? – please justify. RESPONSE: Couture et al. (2019) is a systematic review of all cognitive intervention RCTs for PD patients, whereas the present study is a feasibility pilot trial assessing the effects of two interventions that were never tested before with PD-MCI patients.

3. Please describe about Psych – mind treatment and its benefits. RESPONSE: Please see the description provided at page 8-9.

4. The authors fail to find and report the research gap in this session. RESPONSE: Please see the last paragraph of the introduction, page 6, which tries to explain the research gap.

5. Include the clinical significance of this trial over clinicians, patients and researchers. RESPONSE: Please see the end of introduction at pages 5 and 6.

6. Present the manuscript as per CONSORT guidelines. RESPONSE: You will find at the end of the present letter the CONSORT Table.

7. Include the study setting and study duration. RESPONSE: Study setting, and duration is described at page 8 and in figure 2.

8. Include the reliability and validity of all the outcome measures used in the study. RESPONSE: Please see page 14-15 for reliability and validity of outcome measures.

9. Include the detail description of intervention and control group. RESPONSE: There was no control group, since this is a single blind randomized between group comparative study. Both groups are described in detail in the method section, please see page 8 and 9.

10. Include the method of sample size calculation with suitable reference. RESPONSE: The power analysis was withdrawn at the first round of revision, please see your comment 17 of the first letter: « What is the need of doing sample size calculation as it is a feasibility pilot trial? ». The power calculation was therefore deleted.

11. Mention about the demographic details of the participants in the results section. RESPONSE: Please see table 1 at page 11-12 and the first paragraph of the result section, page 15.

12. In the results section, please discuss about the treatment compliance rate, adverse effects and the number of dropouts. RESPONSE: Please see the feasibility results provided at pages 15, 16 and 17, as well as the discussion, page 21.

13. Mention the reports with 95%CI (Upper – lower limit) for all the variables. RESPONSE: Added in the text, please see the result section, pages 19-20.

14. Report the effect size and MCID values of all the primary and secondary variables. RESPONSE: Added in Table 2.

15. Summarize the discussion part and include the mechanism of interventions on different variables with recent references. RESPONSE: We basically restructured and re-written the entire text in the

Introduction and Discussion sections and discussed potential mechanism of interventions (please see page 26), as well as discussion with recent references (pages 23-24).

16. The conclusion should be more concise and drawn on the basis of study reports. RESPONSE: Please see page 29 for the revised conclusion.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers dec 2021.pdf

Decision Letter 2

Walid Kamal Abdelbasset

13 Jan 2022

Goal Management Training and Psychoeducation / Mindfulness for Treatment of Executive Dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: A Feasibility Pilot Trial.

PONE-D-21-08484R2

Dear Dr. Giguère-Rancourt,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Walid Kamal Abdelbasset, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #3: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: No

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Dear authors,

Really appreciate your efforts for satisfactorily addressed my comments.

Really appreciate your efforts for satisfactorily addressed my comments.

Really appreciate your efforts for satisfactorily addressed my comments.

Reviewer #3: thanks alot for your respose

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Gopal Nambi

Reviewer #3: No

Acceptance letter

Walid Kamal Abdelbasset

9 Feb 2022

PONE-D-21-08484R2

Goal Management Training and Psychoeducation / Mindfulness for Treatment of Executive Dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: A Feasibility Pilot Trial.

Dear Dr. Giguère-Rancourt:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Walid Kamal Abdelbasset

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Checklist. CONSORT 2010 checklist of information to include when reporting a randomised trial*.

    (DOCX)

    S1 File

    (DOCX)

    S2 File

    (ZIP)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers dec 2021.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    Data availability: The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, AGR, upon reasonable request. You can also contact Laval University data research manager at the following email address: gdr@ulaval.ca Data can be accessed at the following page (please send an access request): Giguère-Rancourt, Ariane, 2020, "Cognitive Training in Parkinson’s disease", https://doi.org/10.5683/SP2/0LMBRX, Scholars Portal Dataverse, V1, UNF:6:RAQT1DXYblrlo+gpYplHyA== [fileUNF].


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES