Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: AIDS Behav. 2021 Apr 24;25(10):3398–3412. doi: 10.1007/s10461-021-03268-y

Inconsistent sexual behavior reporting among youth affected by perinatal HIV exposure in the United States

Krystal Cantos 1, Molly F Franke 2, Katherine Tassiopoulos 1, Paige L Williams 1,3, Anna-Barbara Moscicki 4, George R Seage III 1,*; Pediatric HIV/AIDS Cohort Study
PMCID: PMC8858594  NIHMSID: NIHMS1775313  PMID: 33893876

Abstract

We examined reporting agreement of oral, vaginal, and anal sex in adolescents and young adults living with perinatally-acquired HIV and those perinatally HIV-exposed and uninfected in the Pediatric HIV/AIDS Cohort Study Adolescent Master Protocol (AMP) and AMP Up studies. Agreement between fixed constructs (e.g., age at first sex) and prevalence of logical inconsistencies (e.g., reclaimed virginity status) over time were assessed. Internal consistency was also examined using an attention check question and questions regarding condom use in the prior three months. Those who reported having anal sex in adolescence had a higher proportion of inconsistent responses compared to vaginal and oral sex measures. At their most recent survey, 84% of young adults correctly answered an attention check question and 74% agreed within the survey on condom use in the prior three months. In bivariate analyses, HIV status was not associated with responding inconsistently. Increased time between surveys, male sex, and younger age at first survey were associated with multiple measures of inconsistency over time, while lower cognitive scores, having less than a high school diploma, and negatively answering post-survey acceptability questions were associated with incorrectly answering an attention check question.

INTRODUCTION

Stakeholders, including policymakers, program directors, and researchers, use data on the prevalence of sexual behavior to monitor trends, identify target populations, set program priorities and advocate for support. One inherent limitation of these data is that sexual behavior history is almost always measured through self-report, which can be subject to many forms of bias. For instance, a question’s intent may be unclear and misunderstood, or the question may be subject to social-desirability bias leading respondents to answer in a manner they believe will be viewed favorably by others, even if the survey is anonymous. Failure to reliably assess self-reported sexual behaviors can lead to significant errors in research on sexual health as well as policy creation and implementation.

Among adolescents, inconsistencies in self-reported sexual behaviors are well documented (1, 2). Adolescents may purposely underreport or overreport some behaviors if they believe engaging in these behaviors is socially undesirable or desirable, respectively. Cognitive demands associated with recalling past behaviors and poor comprehension of survey questions, inattentiveness, and lack of motivation may also threaten reliability of self-report.

To assess the validity of self-report, researchers have used the presence of biomarkers of vaginal exposure to semen such as the presence of sperm, prostate-specific antigen, or Y chromosome in vaginal fluids (35). These measures are relevant for evaluation of very recent condomless sexual intercourse in women only and may not be relevant in most epidemiological studies where long-term exposure and variety of exposures are of interest (6). Some have used corroboration of partner reports of sexual behavior (7), but partner report may be influenced by the nature of the relationship between partners and jeopardizes confidentiality. Recording sexual behaviors in daily diaries (8) shortens recall time, but it may alter the behavior itself and is burdensome, which may lead to high attrition rates and non-compliance.

Test–retest reliability, which assesses the stability of responses to questionnaires over time, is a pre-requisite for validity. Findings from several studies on self-reported sexual behavior test-retest reliability among adolescents in the United States have varied. For dichotomous items, kappa reliability estimates have ranged anywhere from 0.40 to 0.90 (911). Continuous outcome measures, particularly the frequency of sexual intercourse, have performed poorly (9, 12). Adolescents also retract earlier-reported behaviors, for instance, initially reporting sexual activity and subsequently reporting never having engaged in the behavior. This is a logical inconsistency given the respondent was incorrect in at least one of the two surveys (13). An analysis of two waves of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health found that 11% of adolescents reclaimed virginity status (i.e., by reporting prior sex in one survey and no prior sex in the second survey), while only 22% reported the same month and year of sexual initiation (14). Better consistency has been demonstrated when timing of sexual initiation is reported as age in years, though results from these studies in adolescents have found varying levels of consistency in self-reported age at first sexual encounter, suggesting that recall may vary across populations and survey design (1517).

Several studies have identified factors associated with inconsistent reporting, including survey design, interview modality, and setting, as well as participant characteristics such as age, sex, race and ethnicity, living in a single-parent family, HIV status, knowledge about sexually transmitted infections and perception of peer norms (2, 16, 18, 19). Research has shown that adolescents who take a virginity pledge or become born-again Christians are more likely to retract earlier reports of sex (20). Studies generally have indicated that high-frequency sexual behaviors are reported less consistently than low-frequency sexual behaviors, because respondents who engage in a behavior frequently are less likely to remember specific instances (19). However, when adolescents report infrequent behaviors, they often retract those behaviors later on (21). For instance, if a person experienced anal sex only once a decade earlier, they may not consider themselves to be someone who has anal sex, rendering their report an indicator of whether they currently have anal sex as opposed to actual anal sex history.

Attention check questions, which have become an increasingly popular albeit somewhat debated method, are utilized in survey research as a means to filter out careless, or even mischievous, responses (2225). Attention check questions are often referred to as trap questions, red herrings, validation questions, verification ratings, and instructed response or instructional manipulation questions. The proportion of people who incorrectly answer an attention check can vary widely across survey groups, setting, placement within a survey, and complexity of question, but it can help identify respondents more likely to have item nonresponse, inconsistent answers, speeding, and implausible statements throughout a survey (26, 27).

Since self-report of sexual behavior may be subject to social desirability bias, which may differ by age, sex, and cultural norms, it is important to evaluate reliability in the context of research in adolescents affected by HIV. More than half of states in the US have HIV-specific criminal laws, including laws targeting non-disclosure to sexual partners. Many states criminalize behaviors, such as spitting, that pose even a low or negligible risk for HIV transmission (28). Participant knowledge of such laws, or feelings of stigma related to their diagnosis, may lead to greater inconsistency in sexual reporting compared to the wider US adolescent population.

Among youth living with perinatally-acquired HIV (PHIV) and youth living with perinatal HIV exposure but uninfected (PHEU) in the United States, we examined reporting agreement of oral, vaginal and anal sex over time. We also considered associations of inconsistent reports with several demographic, health and psychosocial measures. Understanding factors associated with inconsistent responses may help identify areas for improvement of questionnaires in this population and inform researchers and policymakers of the precision of sexual behavior prevalence estimates.

METHODS

Study participants

The Pediatric HIV/AIDS Cohort Study (PHACS) Adolescent Master Protocol (AMP) is an ongoing prospective cohort study following youth living with PHIV and PHEU. Eligibility criteria for youth with PHIV included perinatal HIV infection, age 7 to <16 years at enrollment, and engagement in medical care with available antiretroviral treatment (ART) history. Enrollment took place from March 2007 through October 2009 at 15 sites in the United States, including Puerto Rico. Youth who were PHEU, in a similar age range, and from the same clinical sites were enrolled for comparison. An audio computer-assisted self-interview (ACASI) was administered annually in the clinic to participants 10 years of age or older with parental consent to collect information on substance use and sexual behaviors. Questions related to sexual behavior included prompts to ensure that youths understood words or concepts that may have been unfamiliar to them.

Once participants living with PHIV and PHEU in the AMP study become 18 years of age, follow-up is continued through enrollment into the AMP Up study (29), in which clinic-based visits during adolescence are transitioned to a hybrid of online and in-person data collection. Enrollment for AMP Up began in May 2014 and is ongoing. Young adults living with PHIV, who were not enrolled in AMP, but with available medical record documentation since early childhood are also eligible for enrollment. At the entry visit, clinical site staff introduce participants to the online surveys, and participants complete the surveys in the clinic. For follow-up surveys completed outside the clinic, participants can access online surveys either through links provided to them from clinical site staff or through a participant website. Participants in AMP are referred to as adolescents and in AMP Up as young adults. AMP and AMP Up were approved by the institutional review boards of each site and the Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health. Written informed consent is obtained from all participants or from guardians for those unable to consent themselves.

Sexual reporting agreement and prevalence of logical inconsistencies

Prevalence of logical inconsistencies (e.g., reclaimed virginity status) and agreement between fixed constructs (e.g., age at first sexual encounter) over time were assessed across two different intervals. The first within-adolescence analysis assesses agreement over a shorter period, where all surveys were administered via ACASI in AMP. In contrast, the adolescence to adulthood analysis examines agreement and inconsistencies over a longer period. ACASIs were administered at first report (to youth in AMP) and online surveys with no audio component were administered at second report (to young adults in AMP Up).

Participants in AMP were asked every year about whether they ever had vaginal sex with opposite sex partners, as well as oral or anal sex with opposite- or same-sex partners (see Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 for full list of questions analyzed). All participants who reported ever having oral, vaginal, or anal sex and had at least one subsequent survey response were included in the analysis for that particular item. Participants were also asked, “in your lifetime, how many partners have you had vaginal sex with,” and we considered responses logically inconsistent if fewer partners were enumerated in a latter survey when compared to a participant’s first response. Participants were asked, “in your lifetime, when you had vaginal sex, how often did you/your partner wear a condom,” and response options were: always, sometimes, or never. If a participant’s response changed from always to never, never to always, or sometimes to either always or never in the subsequent survey, it was considered logically inconsistent.

Any young adult participant who answered the attention check question was eligible for inclusion in this agreement measure, including those who did not answer sexual behavior questions. The attention check is a question that instructs the respondent to “please select ‘usually’ from the options below,” and if the participant does not select ‘usually,’ their response is considered incorrect (see Supplementary Table S4).

For agreement measures, two random surveys in which the question was answered were selected for comparison using simple random sampling. Participants were asked to input an integer value for their own age and partner age at their first vaginal sexual encounter, as well as their own age at first anal sexual experience. We considered responses to be in agreement if the latter report of age was within (+/−) one year of the first reported age. Participants were also asked about condom use at their first sexual encounter. We reported the simple percent agreement, Cohen’s kappa coefficient (30), and prevalence-adjusted bias-adjusted kappa (PABAK). We calculated PABAK to better account for the overestimation of kappa when bias is high and underestimation of kappa when prevalence is either very high or low for a particular response option (31, 32).

In adolescents’ surveys, condomless vaginal sex was determined based on two questions: the first asked if a condom was used at first vaginal sex encounter, and the second asked of those who reported condom use if condomless sex occurred even for a moment. Surveys administered to young adults, conversely, only included the first question. Thus, two separate measures were examined in the adolescence to adulthood analysis. Kappa values were calculated comparing the young adult condom use question to the first condom use question in adolescence only and to the combined questions including the follow-up question assessing only partial condom use.

We assessed within-survey agreement of condom use in the prior three months in online surveys administered to young adults. As opposed to other measures, any sexually active young adult who completed at least one survey was included in this agreement measure, and only the most recent survey was used for an individual. Participants were asked within the same survey about condom use in the previous three months, to which they could respond that they “always,” “sometimes,” or “never” used a condom. Additionally, two separate questions requested a time interval for most recent sexual encounter with a condom and without a condom. From these two questions we could determine if they had always, sometimes, or never used a condom in the three months prior to survey completion and compare their within-survey agreement using the Fleiss-Cohen weighted kappa. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals (95% CI) of kappa and PABAK were calculated using asymptotic variance.

Associations between factors and inconsistent reporting

Data collection

We examined the association of the following factors with inconsistent reporting. Except where indicated, these factors were measured during AMP follow-up.

Sociodemographic data

Information was obtained on age, sex, race, ethnicity, and caregiver education. The location of a participant’s primary study site was used to ascertain whether the participant lived in the South as defined by US Census Region. A 4-point scale question from Monitoring the Future, an ongoing study of the behaviors, attitudes, and values of Americans from adolescence through adulthood (33), was used to determine the importance of religion to the participant and was dichotomized to contrast those who feel religion is very important in their lives compared to less or non-important.

Survey administration

The time it took participants to complete a survey and survey administration dates were obtained from ACASI in AMP and from the Datstat Illume software (34) for young adults in AMP Up.

Cognitive functioning and academic achievement

Standardized Wechsler and NIH Toolbox neuropsychological examinations (3538) were performed and are explained in greater detail in Supplementary Table S5. We examined the following: working memory scores, which have been implicated in inattention, hyperactivity, and mind-wandering when impaired (3942); processing speed, which is also linked to inattention as well as anxiety and a lack of motivation to complete the task at hand when impaired (43); word reading and numerical operations scores assessing phonological awareness, decoding skills, and the ability to identify and write numbers; and crystallized cognition scores, which in contrast to working memory and processing speed, represent an accumulated store of verbal knowledge and skills, and are thus more heavily influenced by education and cultural exposure. These measures were taken from the examination closest to the date of survey completion, as neuropsychological exams were only performed every two to three years to reduce participant burden.

General and mental health

Participants and their caregivers were interviewed separately by a psychologist using the Behavior Assessment System for Children- 2nd Edition (BASC-2) (44), a series of scales assessing their perceptions of the participant’s emotional and behavioral health. We included the caregiver-reported Behavioral Symptoms Index (BSI) and participant-reported Emotional Symptoms Index (ESI). BSI scales measure hyperactivity, aggression, depression, attention problems, atypicality and withdrawal. ESI scales reflect a composite of social stress, anxiety, depression, sense of inadequacy, self-esteem and self-reliance. These measures were also taken from the examination closest to the date of survey completion in AMP. Self-reported substance use was collected through the same surveys as sexual behavior history.

Psychological instrument validity indices

We also examined BASC-2 validity indices for the participant-reported emotional symptoms index, including the F, L, V, consistency, and response pattern indices. Validity indices alert researchers to the presence of questionable data. The F index is a measure of the participant’s tendency to be excessively negative about their behaviors or emotions. The L index measures the participant’s tendency to give an extremely positive picture of themselves. The V index rates a respondent’s proclivity to agree with nonsensical or highly implausible statements (such as “I drink 50 glasses of milk every day”). The consistency index flags cases in which the participant has given different responses to items that usually are answered similarly. The response pattern index detects response patterning in which a participant repeats or cycles through response options.

Post-survey questions

Optional post-survey acceptability questions were included as part of the AMP Up online surveys. Six items regarding the acceptability of the survey (e.g., “the survey instructions were explained clearly”) included response options: strongly agree, agree, disagree, and strongly disagree. An additional statement asked participants to rate the difficulty of the survey overall on a 4-point scale from very easy to very hard, and another question ascertained the location in which the survey was completed.

Statistical Methods

Bivariate associations between inconsistent reporting and these factors were examined using logistic regression. For within-adolescence analyses, we ran separate models for two outcomes of interest: inconsistently reported age at first vaginal sex (> +/− 1 year) and reclaimed vaginal sex virginity. For adolescence to adulthood analyses we ran models for inconsistently reported age at first vaginal sex (> +/− 1 year) and reclaimed anal sex virginity. For two items measured in the young adult online survey only, we also examined associations with prior three-month within-survey inconsistency of condom use reporting and an incorrect attention check response.

RESULTS

The within-adolescence analysis assessed agreement over an average interval of 1.5 years (range: 0.2–7.6 years) among 277 participants. The mean age at first and second report was 15.9 and 17.4 years, respectively. The adolescence to adulthood analysis, assessing agreement and inconsistencies over an average interval of 3.1 years (range: 0.5–9.9 years), consisted of 250 participants with a mean age at first and second report of 17.0 and 20.1 years, respectively.

Demographic characteristics of participants are described in Table I for both the within-adolescence and adolescence to adulthood analyses. About half of participants were male, two-thirds were Black, a quarter were Hispanic, and two-thirds were living with PHIV. About one-third of participants were from the Northeast and another third from the South. At first report, most were between 14 and 19 years of age, but for adolescence to adulthood analyses, those at retest were all over the age of 18, with about half over 20 years old.

Table I.

Demographic characteristics of 277 eligible participants for within-adolescence analysis and 250 eligible participants for adolescence to adulthood analysis

Variable Within-adolescence Adolescence to adulthood
Percent (N = 277) Percent (N = 250)

Sex at birth
 Male 49.8 47.2
 Female 50.2 52.8
Race
 Black or African American 67.9 66.4
 White 24.9 25.6
 Other/ Multi / Unknown 7.2 8.0
Ethnicity
 Hispanic or Latino 24.9 28.8
Perinatal HIV Status
 PHIVa 62.8 67.2
 PHEUb 37.2 32.8
US Region
 Northeast 34.7 39.2
 Midwest 15.2 11.2
 South 32.5 31.2
 West 11.9 11.2
 Puerto Rico 5.8 7.2
Age, years At test, % (N=1819)c At retest, % (N=1819)c At test, % (N=1742)d At retest, % (N=1742)d
10–13 12.1 4.2 7.3 n/a
14–16 64.3 30.2 37.3 n/a
17–19 22.5 56.7 50.1 50.6
20–23 1.1 8.9 5.3 45.2
24+ 0.0 0.0 0.0 4.1
Average time between surveys 1.5 years 3.1 years
a

PHIV: Living with perinatally-acquired HIV

b

PHEU: Exposed to HIV perinatally, but uninfected

c

N = Total number of agreement measures assessed among 277 participants

d

N = Total number of agreement measures assessed among 250 participants

Logical inconsistencies

Agreement in ever and lifetime measures are shown in Table II. Within adolescence, of those who reported oral, vaginal, and anal sex with an opposite sex partner at first report, 14% (32/228), 12% (28/229), and 48% (51/107), later reclaimed oral, vaginal, and anal sex virginity at second report, respectively. Fifteen percent (31/211), 6% (12/217), and 54% (53/99) reclaimed oral, vaginal, and anal sex virginity as a young adult, respectively. Of those who reported same-sex oral sex at first report, 34% (21/62) and 33% (19/58) reclaimed oral sex virginity at second report in adolescence and adulthood, respectively, and 50% (5/10) and 13% (1/8) reclaimed anal sex virginity at second report in adolescence and adulthood, respectively.

Table II.

Proportion of logically inconsistent response in ever and lifetime measures across two surveys

Within-adolescence Adolescence to adulthood

Ever had sex measures Percent reclaiming virginity between first and second survey (proportion) Mean time between surveys, in years (IQR)a Percent reclaiming virginity between first and second survey (proportion) Mean time between surveys, in years (IQR)

Opposite sex partner
 Oral sex 14 (32/228) 1.1 (0.9, 1.3) 15 (31/211) 3.1 (1.5, 4.4)
 Vaginal sex 12 (28/229) 1.2 (0.9, 1.3) 6 (12/217) 2.9 (1.4, 4.1)
 Anal sex 48 (51/107) 1.2 (0.9, 1.3) 54 (53/98) 3.6 (2.1, 4.9)
Same sex partner
 Oral sex 34 (21/62) 1.1 (1.0, 1.3) 33 (19/58) 2.9 (1.2, 4.3)
 Anal sex (males only) 50 (5/10) 1.1 (1.0, 1.1) 13 (1/8) 4.3 (3.0, 5.3)

Within-adolescence Adolescence to adulthood
Lifetime sex measures Percent inconsistent between first and second survey (proportion) Mean time between surveys, in years (IQR) Percent inconsistent between first and second survey (proportion) Mean time between surveys, in years (IQR)

Vaginal sex
 Lifetime condom use 15 (26/176) 1.8 (1.0, 2.4) 19 (37/190) 2.9 (1.3, 4.0)
 Lifetime number of sex partners 22 (40/178) 1.7 (1.0, 2.3) 22 (39/176) 3.0 (1.4, 4.5)
Anal sex
 Lifetime condom use 32 (20/63) 1.6 (0.9, 2.3) 37 (20/54) 3.8 (2.2, 5.1)
 Lifetime number of sex partners 22 (14/63) 1.7 (0.9, 2.2) 25 (13/51) 3.8 (2.1, 5.4)
a

IQR: Interquartile range

For lifetime measures, 15% (26/176) and 19% (37/190) were inconsistent in reporting lifetime vaginal sex condom use and 22% (40/178) and 22% (39/176) were inconsistent in reporting lifetime number of vaginal sex partners in adolescence and adulthood, respectively. Thirty-two percent (20/63) and 37% (20/54) were inconsistent in reporting lifetime anal sex condom use and 22% (14/63) and 25% (13/51) were inconsistent in reporting lifetime number of anal sex partners in adolescence and adulthood. Additionally, 16% (88/559) of participants answered the attention check incorrectly at their most recent survey as a young adult. Twenty-four percent (21/88) of those who answered the attention check incorrectly selected “rather not answer” instead of “usually.” Excluding these responses, 12% (67/538) answered incorrectly.

Agreement in fixed constructs

Within adolescence, 78% agreed on their own age at first vaginal sexual encounter (+/− one year), 70% agreed on their first partner’s age, and 65% agreed on their own age at first anal sexual encounter (Table III). Across adolescence and young adulthood, 68% agreed on their own age at first vaginal sexual experience (+/− one year) and 67% agreed on their first partner’s age, while only 47% agreed on their own age at first anal sexual experience. For the within-adolescence analysis, reported age was 0.4 to 0.9 years greater on average at second report, with a 1.7 year mean interval between surveys. For the adolescence to adulthood analysis, reported age at second report was about three quarters of a year greater on average for age and partner age at first vaginal sexual encounter and almost 2 years greater for age at first anal sexual encounter compared to first report. There was an average of 3 years between surveys for the vaginal sex measures and 3.5 years between surveys for age at first anal sexual encounter.

Table III.

Participant and partner age at first sex as reported at first and second survey

Response at first survey Response at second survey Percent agreementb Mean time between surveys, in years (IQR)


Mean, in years IQRa Mean, in years IQR

Within-adolescence
 Age at first vaginal sex (N=212) 14.0 (13.0, 15.0) 14.4 (14.0, 16.0) 78 1.8 (1.0, 2.3)
 Partner age at first vaginal sex (N=214) 14.8 (13,0, 17.0) 15.7 (15.0, 17.0) 70 1.7 (1.0, 2.2)
 Age at first anal sex (N=63) 13.9 (13.0, 16.0) 14.8 (13.0, 17.0) 65 1.7 (1.0, 2.2)
Adolescence to adulthood
 Age at first vaginal sex (N=208) 14.6 (14.0, 16.0) 15.3 (14.0, 16.0) 68 3.0 (1.4, 4.3)
 Partner age at first vaginal sex (N=209) 15.7 (15.0, 17.0) 16.5 (15.0, 18.0) 67 3.0 (1.4, 4.0)
 Age at first anal sex (N=53) 15.2 (14.0, 17.0) 17.1 (16.0, 19.0) 47 3.5 (1.6, 4.9)
a

IQR: Interquartile range

b

Responses in agreement if age provided at second survey is within +/− 1 year of age provided at first survey

Condom use during a participant’s first vaginal sexual experience (yes/no) had fair agreement in both the within-adolescence analysis (kappa = 0.45, 95% CI: [0.33, 0.57]; PABAK = 0.46, 95% CI: [0.34, 0.58]; 73% [156/214] in agreement) and the adolescence to adulthood analysis (kappa = 0.43, 95% CI: [0.28, 0.57]; PABAK = 0.59, 95% CI: [0.48, 0.70]; 79% (166/209) in agreement). When we define partially condomless sex as condomless in the first survey as determined by the follow-up question regarding condomless sex even for a moment, adolescence to adulthood agreement drops (kappa = 0.39, 95% CI: [0.28, 0.51]; PABAK = 0.45, 95% CI: [0.32, 0.57]; 72% (151/209) in agreement) as reports of condomless sex increase in the first survey.

Within-survey agreement of condom use in the previous three months showed moderate agreement (weighted kappa = 0.72, 95% CI: [0.67, 0.78]; 74% [324/439] in agreement). While 74% agreed within the survey on condom use in the prior three months, 18% (79/439) reported more condom use, and only 8% (36/439) reported less condom use, when responding generally about always, sometimes, or never using a condom compared to selecting time intervals for the last time they had sex with and without a condom.

Associations between factors and inconsistent reporting

Table IV shows the estimated odds ratios (OR) and 95% CIs for factors associated with inconsistent reporting over time. In bivariate analyses, increasing time between survey administrations was associated with inconsistent reporting of age at first vaginal sexual encounter in both within-adolescence and adolescence to adulthood analyses and reclaimed anal sex virginity status in the latter analysis. Older age at first survey is associated with decreased odds of incorrect reporting of age at first sexual encounter or reclaimed virginity status across all inconsistency measures. Male sex was positively associated with inconsistent reporting of age at first vaginal sexual encounter as well as reclaimed vaginal sex virginity within adolescence, while Black race was associated with increased inconsistent reporting of age within adolescence only. Living in the South was associated with inconsistent reporting of age at first vaginal sexual encounter. No health, cognitive functioning, or academic achievement measures were associated with inconsistent reporting over time. Increased L and V indices, which measure a participant’s tendency to give an extremely positive picture of their life and to agree with highly implausible statements, respectively, were both associated with increased odds of reclaiming virginity status within adolescence. An increased V index was also associated with inconsistent reporting of age across adolescence to adulthood.

Table IV.

Bivariate factor associations with inconsistent reporting over time

Variablea Within-Adolescence Adolescence to Adulthood
Reclaimed vaginal virginity status Inconsistent age of first vaginal sex Reclaimed anal virginity status Inconsistent age of first vaginal sex
Nb ORc 95% CId N OR 95% CI N OR 95% CI N OR 95% CI
Administration

Interval between surveys (years) 229 0.68 (0.28, 1.67) 212 1.93 (1.43, 2.60) 98 1.32 (1.05, 1.66) 208 1.21 (1.03, 1.42)
Time to complete first survey (min) 229 0.95 (0.90, 1.01) 212 0.98 (0.95, 1.01) 98 1.00 (0.99, 1.01) 208 1.00 (0.99, 1.02)

Sociodemographic

Age at first survey (years) 229 0.70 (0.56, 0.88) 212 0.73 (0.60, 0.88) 98 0.72 (0.59, 0.89) 208 0.81 (0.70, 0.95)
Age at second survey (years) 229 0.71 (0.58, 0.88) 212 0.98 (0.80, 1.20) 98 0.83 (0.66, 1.05) 208 0.97 (0.82, 1.15)
Male 229 2.50 (1.08, 5.80) 212 2.46 (1.26, 4.80) 98 0.91 (0.41, 2.01) 208 2.34 (1.29, 4.24)
Black race 219 2.43 (0.81, 7.32) 202 3.25 (1.30, 8.17) 95 2.16 (0.90, 5.17) 199 1.88 (0.93, 3.82)
Caregiver high school graduate 229 0.73 (0.30, 1.81) 212 0.90 (0.44, 1.82) 98 1.03 (0.44, 2.39) 208 1.32 (0.71, 2.44)
Live in the South 229 1.01 (0.44, 2.37) 212 1.86 (0.96, 3.61) 98 3.56 (1.34, 9.46) 208 2.48 (1.20, 4.04)
Religion very important 212 1.29 (0.45, 3.69) 198 1.01 (0.41, 2.53) 89 1.53 (0.53, 4.39) 195 1.48 (0.67, 3.29)

Health

Living with HIV 229 1.05 (0.46, 2.39) 212 1.74 (0.86, 3.55) 98 1.70 (0.71, 4.07) 208 1.45 (0.76, 2.76)
Behavioral symptom index (BASC) 205 1.02 (0.98, 1.05) 186 1.00 (0.97, 1.03) 78 1.00 (0.96, 1.04) 171 1.01 (0.99, 1.04)
Emotional symptom index (BASC) 226 0.98 (0.94, 1.02) 208 0.97 (0.94, 1.01) 95 1.00 (0.96, 1.05) 204 1.01 (0.98, 1.04)
Ever marijuana use at first survey 227 0.91 (0.41, 2.02) 210 1.03 (0.53, 1.98) 98 0.57 (0.25, 1.29) 208 1.01 (0.56, 1.82)
Ever marijuana use at second survey 229 0.57 (0.26, 1.26) 212 0.93 (0.47, 1.83) 97 1.43 (0.58, 3.57) 204 0.91 (0.47, 1.76)

BASC validity indices e

F index 226 1.02 (0.74, 1.40) 208 0.76 (0.51, 1.14) 95 1.29 (0.85, 1.97) 204 1.26 (0.99, 1.60)
L index 225 1.14 (1.00, 1.29) 208 1.02 (0.91, 1.14) 95 1.03 (0.89, 1.18) 204 1.04 (0.94, 1.16)
V index 225 1.73 (1.01, 2.96) 208 0.63 (0.24, 1.68) 95 2.28 (0.94, 5.49) 204 1.98 (1.08, 3.63)
Consistency index 217 1.01 (0.93, 1.11) 204 1.03 (0.95, 1.11) 94 1.04 (0.95, 1.13) 203 1.06 (0.99, 1.13)
Response pattern index 217 1.01 (0.98, 1.04) 204 0.99 (0.97, 1.01) 94 1.02 (1.00, 1.05) 203 1.00 (0.98, 1.02)

Cognitive functioning and academic achievement

Processing speed 224 1.01 (0.98, 1.03) 208 1.00 (0.97, 1.02) 94 1.02 (0.99, 1.05) 203 0.99 (0.97, 1.01)
Working memory 225 1.01 (0.98, 1.04) 209 1.00 (0.98, 1.02) 95 1.02 (0.99, 1.05) 204 0.99 (0.97, 1.01)
WIATf math score 199 1.01 (0.98, 1.03) 185 1.00 (0.98, 1.01) 81 1.01 (0.99, 1.04) 181 0.99 (0.98, 1.01)
WIAT word score 199 1.00 (0.97, 1.02) 185 0.98 (0.96, 1.00) 81 1.01 (0.99, 1.04) 181 0.99 (0.97, 1.01)
a

At time closest to first survey unless otherwise stated

b

Numbers differ due to refusals and missing observations

c

OR: odds ratio

d

CI: confidence interval

e

BASC: Behavior Assessment System for Children- 2nd Edition; F index: tendency to be excessively negative; L index: tendency to give an extremely positive picture of self; V index: proclivity to agree with nonsensical or highly implausible statements; Consistency index: participant provides different responses to items that usually are answered similarly; Response Pattern index: participant repeats or cycles through response options.

f

WIAT: Wechsler Individual Achievement Test, Second Edition, Abbreviated 33 is a standardized measure of academic achievement

Estimated ORs and 95% CIs for factors associated with inconsistent reporting within one young adult survey are shown in Table V. The time it took to complete the survey, clinic versus home-based survey completion, responses to post-survey acceptability questions, cognitive functioning, and sociodemographic or health factors were not associated with inconsistent reporting of condom use in the previous three months within the same survey. Not responding to the attention check correctly was the only factor associated with increased odds of inconsistent reporting of condom use within the same survey (OR=3.19, 95% CI:[1.52, 6.69]).

Table V.

Bivariate factor associations with inconsistent reporting within young adult survey

Within-survey condom use inconsistency, N=440 Within-survey incorrect attention check, N=559
Variable Median (IQR)a or N (%)b ORc 95% CId Median (IQR) or N (%) OR 95% CI

Sociodemographic
Age (years) 23 (21, 27) 1.00 (0.95, 1.05) 23 (21, 27) 0.98 (0.93, 1.04)
Male 155 (35) 1.03 (0.66, 1.60) 214 (38) 1.21 (0.76, 1.91)
Black race 284 (68) 1.30 (0.81, 2.10) 380 (71) 0.88 (0.53, 1.45)
High school graduate 354 (82) 0.80 (0.47, 1.36) 465 (85) 0.40 (0.23, 0.70)
Live in the South 134 (30) 1.24 (0.79, 1.96) 160 (29) 0.92 (0.56, 1.54)
Health
Living with HIV 360 (82) 0.79 (0.46, 1.35) 466 (83) 0.88 (0.49, 1.59)
Ever marijuana use 330 (77) 1.33 (0.78, 2.30) 387 (72) 0.54 (0.33, 0.90)
Cognitive functioning
Processing speed 103 (90, 117) 1.00 (0.99, 1.01) 101 (88, 115) 0.99 (0.97, 1.00)
Working memory 105 (97, 113) 1.00 (0.98, 1.01) 105 (94, 113) 0.96 (0.95, 0.98)
Crystallized cognition 100 (94, 106) 0.99 (0.97, 1.01) 100 (93, 108) 0.95 (0.93, 0.98)
Survey administration and post survey questions
Time to complete survey (minutes) 40 (31, 55) 1.00 (1.00, 1.00) 39 (30, 55) 1.00 (1.00, 1.00)
Survey completion location
 Clinic 300 (90) REF 500 (90) REF
 Home or elsewhere 39 (10) 0.59 (0.25, 1.39) 53 (10) 0.69 (0.29, 1.67)
The survey instructions were explained clearly
 Strongly agree 203 (49) REF 275 (50) REF
 Agree 180(43) 1.21 (0.77, 1.91) 228 (42) 1.97 (1.19, 3.28)
 Disagree 5 (1) 0.77 (0.08, 7.01) 12 (2) 8.49 (2.57, 28.0)
 Strongly disagree 30 (7) 1.53 (0.67, 3.49) 31 (6) 2.47 (0.98, 3.28)
The survey questions were easy to understand
 Strongly agree 197 (47) REF 262 (48) REF
 Agree 180 (43) 0.99 (0.62, 1.56) 239 (44) 1.89 (1.13, 3.14)
 Disagree 10 (2) 1.86 (0.51, 6.85) 14 (3) 8.36 (2.73, 25.6)
 Strongly disagree 28 (7) 1.32 (0.56, 3.01) 29 (5) 2.18 (0.82, 5.81)
The survey questions were easy to answer
 Strongly agree 189 (46) REF 252 (47) REF
 Agree 188 (45) 1.01 (0.64, 1.60) 241 (45) 1.47 (0.88, 2.45)
 Disagree 10 (2) 1.23 (0.31, 4.92) 17 (3) 6.58 (2.36, 18.4)
 Strongly disagree 27 (7) 1.43 (0.60, 3.39) 29 (5) 1.93 (0.73, 5.12)
The online survey format was user friendly
 Strongly agree 197 (48) REF 266 (49) REF
 Agree 179 (44) 1.08 (0.68, 1.71) 232 (43) 1.93 (1.16, 3.23)
 Disagree 8 (2) 0.98 (0.19, 5.01) 13 (2) 8.51 (2.57, 28.2)
 Strongly disagree 27 (7) 1.47 (0.62, 3.48) 30 (6) 2.13 (0.80, 5.64)
I’m fine with how long it took to complete the survey
 Strongly agree 183 (44) REF 247 (45) REF
 Agree 188 (45) 1.02 (0.64, 1.62) 237 (44) 1.57 (0.94, 2.63)
 Disagree 15 (4) 0.43 (0.09, 1.99) 29 (5) 2.39 (0.94, 6.09)
 Strongly disagree 29 (7) 1.72 (0.76, 3.90) 31 (6) 2.19 (0.87, 5.54)
The $75 gift card is fair compensation for taking part
 Strongly agree 189 (46) REF 257 (47) REF
 Agree 168 (41) 1.02 (0.64, 1.63) 220 (41) 1.57 (0.94, 2.62)
 Disagree 22 (5) 0.82 (0.29, 2.33) 27 (5) 2.55 (1.00, 6.53)
 Strongly disagree 32 (8) 1.46 (0.66, 3.23) 39 (7) 1.88 (0.79, 4.46)
Overall, completing this set of surveys was
 Very easy 257 (59) REF 329 (60) REF
 Easy 156 (36) 1.37 (0.88, 2.15) 195 (36) 1.29 (0.79, 2.10)
 Hard 14 (3) 2.46 (0.82, 7.38) 18 (3) 3.16 (1.13, 8.83)
 Very hard 5 (1) -- -- 7 (1) 2.52 (0.48, 13.4)
Incorrect attention check response (n=342) 32 (9) 3.19 (1.52, 6.69) -- -- --
a

IQR: Interquartile range

b

N = number of participants in a given category

c

OR: odds ratio

d

CI: confidence interval

Being a high school graduate and all cognitive scores, including higher working memory, processing speed, and crystallized cognition scores, were associated with decreased odds of responding incorrectly to the attention check. Compared to strongly agreeing positively with optional post-survey acceptability responses, agreeing or disagreeing were associated with increased odds of an incorrect attention check, with the exception of feelings about how long it took to complete the survey. Compared to strongly agreeing with the statement that survey instructions were explained clearly, agreeing (OR=1.97, 95% CI:[1.19, 3.28]) or disagreeing (OR=8.49, 95% CI:[2.57, 28.0]) were associated with increased odds of an incorrect attention check response. No other factors were associated with an incorrect attention check response.

DISCUSSION

We found that participants who reported having sex in adolescence had a higher proportion of inconsistent responses across surveys for anal sex compared to vaginal and oral sex measures and for same-sex partners compared to opposite-sex partners. This is the first study to our knowledge to examine reclaimed oral and anal sex virginity with both opposite- and same-sex partners. About half of those who reported anal sex in adolescence later reported not having had the sexual experience. This may support findings that retraction of earlier-reported sexual encounters is most common among adolescents for experimental behaviors initially reported as infrequent, given reports related to anal sex and same-sex anal or oral sex were much less frequent than oral and vaginal sex (13, 21). This could also be related to perceived stigma attached to these misreported behaviors (45). The proportion reclaiming vaginal sex virginity was almost identical to proportions reported in other adolescent populations (14, 20). For lifetime vaginal sex condom use and partner counts, anywhere from 14 to 22% of those who responded provided answers at the second survey that were logically inconsistent with their responses at the first survey, congruous with the results of a study using similar measures (46). For anal sex, these inconsistency percentages were slightly higher at 21 to 39 percent. Comparing these proportions between the within-adolescence and adolescence to adulthood analyses, inconsistencies did not appear to differ dramatically despite slight differences in wording of questions and administration format, age differences, and almost twice the time interval between surveys for the adolescence to adulthood analysis.

Agreement related to first sexual encounters was reasonable, with most adolescents and young adults consistently reporting the same age and condom use at two surveys, similar to analyses in other adolescent and young adult populations (1517). For the within-adolescence analysis, reported age was on average about half to three-quarters of a year greater at second report, while in the adolescence to adulthood analysis, average reported age at second report was three quarters of a year to two years greater for age at first sexual encounter. Agreement on condom use during an adolescent’s first sexual experience was also fair. A follow-up question about whether sex was condomless even for a short time increased reports of condomless sex, corroborating previous research in youth living with PHIV (47). Condoms may be applied only for a portion of sex, or condoms may break or slip off during intercourse. Asking only if a condom was used is likely to underestimate the actual prevalence of condomless sex.

There were also two measures of reliability examined within the same survey among young adults. Inquiring about the last time a participant had sex with and without a condom compared to asking how often they used a condom (i.e., always, sometimes, never) produced a higher number of ‘inconsistent’ and ‘never’ condom use reports within a three-month recall period. Under the assumption that these behaviors are underreported (4, 6), this finding supports use of the former method to ascertain condom use in a given period. In this instance, episodic recall may reduce social desirability compared to general frequency recall because the participant is asked to dig into their memory and recall specific events, avoiding translation of a vague impression of their own condom use to fit a category (48). Additionally, 16% of participants selected an imperfect attention check response at their most recent survey as a young adult, a proportion similar to other studies that have included such a question (23, 26).

Consistent with other studies, several sociodemographic characteristics were associated with inconsistent reporting. Older age at first survey was associated with decreased odds of inconsistent report across all consistency measures, and living in the South and male sex were associated with multiple measures of inconsistency. There are a number of reasons younger age at first report may be associated with inconsistent reporting. Though surveys among participants under the age of 18 included prompts to define different terms, including body parts, it is possible that the prompts were not used or were insufficient for some young participants to understand sexual behavior questions fully. Immaturity, less care for research goals, and stronger compulsion to respond in a socially desirable manner may impact the responses of younger participants. As a means to attain adult status, some adolescents may exaggerate their sexual involvement. It is likely, therefore, that socially desirable answers change with age and developmental stage (13).

The association between living in the South and inconsistent reports may capture regional differences in beliefs about sexual norms. For instance, prior research has shown that a respondent’s belief that fewer peers were engaging in sex and the belief that more peers will wait until marriage to have sex were associated with inconsistent reports (18). Among all southern states in this cohort, sex education is required to frame sexual activity as appropriate only within marriage, while only one non-southern state requires such framing (49). This regional finding warrants further study.

HIV status was not associated with inconsistent reporting, counter to the hypothesis that stigma associated with HIV status leads to greater inconsistency in reporting. Additionally, behavioral and emotional symptoms were not associated with inconsistent reporting. Two BASC validity indices were associated with reclaimed virginity status in adolescence. The L index, which can indicate assertion of a falsely positive picture of a participant’s life, was associated with increased odds of reclaiming vaginal sex virginity. This suggests that participants who reclaim virginity may be more likely to present themselves positively and therefore may alter their responses based on perceived social desirability. Additionally, the V index, which quantifies a participant’s penchant to agree with highly implausible statements, was associated with increased odds of inconsistent report of age at first vaginal sexual encounter across adolescence to adulthood and reclaiming vaginal sex virginity within adolescence, indicating reports of sex may be incorrect and are correlated with other inaccurate reports. Given participants who reclaim virginity may be more likely to select nonsensical and implausible options, this could point to intentional misreporting, though it may also indicate poor comprehension or lack of attention in completing surveys.

While sociodemographic variables were associated with inconsistent reporting over time, they were not important for within-survey inconsistency as a young adult. In examining inconsistency over time, identity and beliefs about sensitive reporting, peer norms, and social desirability may be more crucial to explaining why responses change over time. Conversely, the social and demographic variables that are correlated with identity and beliefs may matter less when examining inconsistency at one point in time. Beliefs about sex and what is socially desirable are likely to remain constant and instead, cognitive abilities or general attentiveness or feelings toward completing a survey may be better markers of inconsistency in this case. Though numerous cognitive and academic achievement scores, post-survey acceptability questions and higher education were associated with an incorrect attention check, incorrectly responding to the attention check was the only factor associated with increased odds of inconsistent reporting of condom use within the same survey, suggesting the measure does indicate unreliable reporting. Additionally, reports of ever using marijuana appeared to be associated with decreased odds of an incorrect attention check response. This association may exist if those who are more likely to report honestly about drug use are also more likely to answer an attention check correctly. Sensitivity analyses restricted to those who correctly answer an attention check could allow examination of the possible effects of unreliable reporting on effect estimates.

Although this study provides a wealth of information regarding self-reported sexual behaviors, there are several limitations. First, it remains unclear whether the initial report of sexual activity, the subsequent report of virginity, or both are false. We can only speculate that measures of reclaimed virginity are capturing only some of those adolescents who are reporting incorrectly (50). Nonetheless, it is important to identify the extent to which adolescents report inconsistently as an indicator of validity of the self-reports. Second, most comparisons required a participant to report at two time periods, and it may be the case that those who remain in the study for more than one year feel that research is more important and are more truthful in their responses. Additionally, measures such as age at first vaginal sex are examined only among those who consistently reported having ever had vaginal sex at both time points. Finally, neuropsychological measures were not always taken at the same time as survey administration since most occurred every three years. Therefore, these measures may not reflect a participant’s cognitive abilities at the moment they completed the survey and may attenuate an association between cognitive scores and inconsistent report.

Even when carefully worded, sexual behavior questions can be interpreted in ways that differ from a researcher’s intent. Our use of detailed descriptions of what was meant by terms such as “vaginal sex” (i.e., “by vaginal sex I mean that a boy had his penis in your vagina”), in addition to culturally normative terms to describe specific sexual behaviors, presumably helped to provide clarity in these surveys.

An additional strategy to reduce socially desirable responding would be to reframe questions so that they ask “how many times” rather than “if” a participant engaged in a particular sexual behavior, thus placing the burden of denial on the participant.

Despite the limitations, our study provides prevalence estimates of logically inconsistent sexual behavior reports and agreement measures on report of first sexual encounters among adolescents and young adults affected by HIV. We focused on a wide range of sexual behaviors not often examined, including vaginal, anal, and oral sex as well as same-sex oral and anal sex. The associations found between inconsistent reports and several demographic, health, and psychosocial measures suggest distinct mechanisms behind inconsistent reporting across surveys compared to within a given survey.

We offer several recommendations based on our findings that may reduce inconsistencies in self-reported sexual behavior. Inclusion of a follow-up question about whether sex was condomless even for a short time may produce greater reports of condomless sex, and asking about the last time a participant had sex with and without a condom, compared to asking how often they used a condom in a given time period, may produce higher inconsistent and never condom use reports. Attention check questions provide a measure for inattentiveness and/or mischievous responses that is easy to implement; we recommend placing the question near the end of the survey to avoid concerns that exposure to the question may affect responses. Although asking “how many times” rather than “if” a participant engaged in a particular sexual behavior may avoid underreporting of sensitive sexual behaviors, this advice may only apply after a certain age because social desirability bias at younger ages may favor overreporting. Further research should examine reporting reliability of anal sex and sex with same-sex partners in adolescents and young adults because participants reported about these encounters less consistently. Finally, participants should be reminded how study data will be used and that they are making an important contribution to improve the lives of other young people affected by HIV.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary material

References

  • 1.DiClemente RJ. Validity of self-reported sexual behavior among adolescents: where do we go from here? AIDS Behav. 2016;20 Suppl 1:S215–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Brener ND, Billy JOG, Grady WR. Assessment of factors affecting the validity of self-reported health-risk behavior among adolescents: evidence from the scientific literature. J Adolesc Health. 2003;33(6):436–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Aho J, Koushik A, Diakité SL, Loua KM, Nguyen VK, Rashed S. Biological validation of self-reported condom use among sex workers in Guinea. AIDS Behav. 2010;14(6):1287–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gallo M, Behets F, Steiner M et al. Prostate-specific antigen to ascertain reliability of self-reported coital exposure to semen. Sex Transm Dis. 2006;33(8):476–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ghanem GK, Melendez HJ, McNeil-Solis AC et al. Condom use and vaginal Y-chromosome detection: the specificity of a potential biomarker. Sex Transm Dis. 2007;34(8):620–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Catania JA, Gibson DR, Chitwood DD, Coates TJ. Methodological problems in AIDS behavioral research: influences on measurement error and participation bias in studies of sexual behavior. Psychol Bull. 1990;108(3):339–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Seage GR III, Mayer KH, Horsburgh CR Jr., Cai B, Lamb GA. Corroboration of sexual histories among male homosexual couples. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;135(1):79–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Stalgaitis C, Glick SN. The use of web-based diaries in sexual risk behaviour research: a systematic review. Sex Transm Infect. 2014;90(5):374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Vanable PA, Carey MP, Brown JL et al. Test-retest reliability of self-reported HIV/STD-related measures among African-American adolescents in four U.S. cities. J Adolesc Health. 2009;44(3):214–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Brener ND, Kann L, McManus T, Kinchen SA, Sundberg EC, Ross JG. Reliability of the 1999 Youth Risk Behavior Survey questionnaire. J Adolesc Health. 2002;31(4):336–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Rosenbaum JE. Truth or consequences: the intertemporal consistency of adolescent self-report on the Youth Risk Behavior Survey. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;169(11):1388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sieving R, Hellerstedt W, McNeely C, Fee R, Snyder J, Resnick M. Reliability of self-reported contraceptive use and sexual behaviors among adolescent girls. J Sex Res. 2005;42(2):159–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Alexander CS, Somerfield MR, Ensminger ME, Johnson KE, Kim YJ. Consistency of adolescents’ self-report of sexual behavior in a longitudinal study. J Youth Adolesc. 1993;22:455–71. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Upchurch DM, Lillard L, Aneshensel CS, Li N. Inconsistencies in reporting the occurrence and timing of first intercourse among adolescents. J Sex Res. 2002;39(3):197–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Goldberg SK, Haydon AA, Herring AH, Halpern CT. Longitudinal consistency in self-reported age of first vaginal intercourse among young adults. J Sex Res. 2014;51(1):97–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lauritsen JL, Swicegood CG. The consistency of self-reported initiation of sexual activity. Fam Plann Perspect. 1997;29(5):215–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Liang W, Chikritzhs T. Errors in recall of age at first sex. PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e72947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Brown J, Sales J, DiClemente R et al. Predicting discordance between self-reports of sexual behavior and incident sexually transmitted infections with African American female adolescents: results from a 4-city study. AIDS Behav. 2012;16(6):1491–500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.McFarlane M, St. Lawrence JS. Adolescents’ recall of sexual behavior: consistency of self-report and effect of variations in recall duration. J Adolesc Health. 1999;25(3):199–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Rosenbaum JE. Reborn a virgin: adolescents’ retracting of virginity pledges and sexual histories. Am J Public Health. 2006;96(6):1098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mensch BS, Kandel DB. Underreporting of substance use in a national longitudinal youth cohort: individual and interviewer effects. Public Opin Q. 1988;52(1):100–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Oppenheimer DM, Meyvis T, Davidenko N. Instructional manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2009;45(4):867–872. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Paas LJ, Dolnicar S, Karlsson L. Instructional Manipulation Checks: A longitudinal analysis with implications for MTurk. Int J Res Mark. 2018;35(2):258–269. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kung FYH, Kwok N, Brown DJ. Are Attention Check Questions a Threat to Scale Validity? Appl Psychol. 2018;67(2):264–283. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fish J, Russell S. Have Mischievous Responders Misidentified Sexual Minority Youth Disparities in the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health? Arch Sex Behav. 2018;47(4):1053–1067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Liu M, Wronski L. Trap questions in online surveys: Results from three web survey experiments. Int J Res Mark. 2018;60(1):32–49. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Gummer T, Roßmann J, Silber H. Using Instructed Response Items as Attention Checks in Web Surveys: Properties and Implementation. Sociol Methods Res. 2021;50(1):238–264. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.The Center for HIV Law and Policy. HIV criminalization in the United States: a sourcebook on state and federal HIV criminal law and practice. 2019. Available at: http://www.hivlawandpolicy.org/sourcebook. Accessed March 2020.
  • 29.Tassiopoulos K, Patel K, Alperen J et al. Following young people with perinatal HIV infection from adolescence into adulthood: the protocol for PHACS AMP Up, a prospective cohort study. BMJ open. 2016;6(6):e011396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cohen J. A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ Psychol Meas. 1960;20(1):37–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Byrt T, Bishop J, Carlin JB. Bias, prevalence and kappa. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46(5):423–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Sim J, Wright CC. The kappa statistic in reliability studies: use, interpretation, and sample size requirements. Phys Ther. 2005;85(3):257–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wallace JM, Forman TA, Caldwell CH, Willis DS. Religion and U.S. secondary school students: current patterns, recent trends, and sociodemographic correlates. Youth Soc. 2003;35(1):98–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.DatStat. DatStat. http://www.datstat.com. Accessed Jan 2020.
  • 35.Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children—fourth edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler adult intelligence scale–fourth edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Psychological Corporation. Wechsler individual achievement test second edition examiner’s manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gershon RC, Wagster MV, Hendrie HC, Fox NA, Cook KF, Nowinski CJ. NIH Toolbox for assessment of neurological and behavioral function. Neurology Mar 2013;80(11 Supplement 3):S2–S6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Martinussen R, Hayden J, Hogg-Johnson S, Tannock R. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44(4):377–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Schoechlin C, Engel RR. Neuropsychological performance in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of empirical data. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;20(6):727–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kofler MJ, Rapport MD, Bolden J, Sarver DE, Raiker JS. ADHD and working memory: the impact of central executive deficits and exceeding storage/rehearsal capacity on observed inattentive behavior. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2010;38(2):149–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ju YJ, Lien YW. Who is prone to wander and when? Examining an integrative effect of working memory capacity and mindfulness trait on mind wandering under different task loads. Conscious Cogn. 2018;63:1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Cook NE, Braaten EB, Surman CBH. Clinical and functional correlates of processing speed in pediatric Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Neuropsychol. 2018;24(5):598–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Reynolds C, Kamphaus C. Behavior Assessment System for Children 2. Circle Pines, Minnesota: American Guidance Service; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fendrich M, Vaughn CM. Diminished lifetime substance use over time: an inquiry into differential underreporting. Public Opin Q. 1994;58(1):96–123. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bell ML, van Roode T, Dickson NP, Jiang ZJ, Paul C. Consistency and reliability of self-reported lifetime number of heterosexual partners by gender and age in a cohort study. Sex Transm Dis. 2010;37(7):425–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Dolezal C, Warne P, Santamaria EK, Elkington KS, Benavides JM, Mellins CA. Asking only “did you use a condom?” underestimates the prevalence of unprotected sex among perinatally HIV infected and perinatally exposed but uninfected youth. J Sex Res. 2014:1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wright JD, Marsden PV. Handbook of Survey Research. 2nd ed. Bingley, UK: Emerald; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Guttmacher Institute. Sex and HIV Education. State laws and policies, as of June 1, 2019. Available at: https://www.guttmacher.org/state-policy/explore/sex-and-hiv-education. Accessed June 2019.
  • 50.Rodgers JL, Billy JOG, Udry JR. The rescission of behaviors: Inconsistent responses in adolescent sexuality data. Soc Sci Res. 1982;11(3):280–96. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material

RESOURCES