Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Feb 22.
Published in final edited form as: Neuroimage. 2021 Nov 29;246:118739. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118739

White matter association tracts underlying language and theory of mind: An investigation of 809 brains from the Human Connectome Project

Leo R Zekelman a,b,1,*, Fan Zhang c,1, Nikos Makris e,f, Jianzhong He c,l, Yuqian Chen c,d, Tengfei Xue c,d, Daniela Liera g, Daniel L Drane h,i,j, Yogesh Rathi c,k, Alexandra J Golby a,c, Lauren J O’Donnell c
PMCID: PMC8862285  NIHMSID: NIHMS1775117  PMID: 34856375

Abstract

Language and theory of mind (ToM) are the cognitive capacities that allow for the successful interpretation and expression of meaning. While functional MRI investigations are able to consistently localize language and ToM to specific cortical regions, diffusion MRI investigations point to an inconsistent and sometimes overlapping set of white matter tracts associated with these two cognitive domains. To further examine the white matter tracts that may underlie these domains, we use a two-tensor tractography method to investigate the white matter microstructure of 809 participants from the Human Connectome Project. 20 association white matter tracts (10 in each hemisphere) are uniquely identified by leveraging a neuroanatomist-curated automated white matter tract atlas. The fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), and number of streamlines (NoS) are measured for each white matter tract. Performance on neuropsychological assessments of semantic memory (NIH Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Test, TPVT) and emotion perception (Penn Emotion Recognition Test, PERT) are used to measure critical subcomponents of the language and ToM networks, respectively. Regression models are constructed to examine how structural measurements of left and right white matter tracts influence performance across these two assessments. We find that semantic memory performance is influenced by the number of streamlines of the left superior longitudinal fasciculus III (SLF-III), and emotion perception performance is influenced by the number of streamlines of the right SLF-III. Additionally, we find that performance on both semantic memory & emotion perception is influenced by the FA of the left arcuate fasciculus (AF). The results point to multiple, overlapping white matter tracts that underlie the cognitive domains of language and ToM. Results are discussed in terms of hemispheric dominance and concordance with prior investigations.

Keywords: White matter tractography, Diffusion MRI, Human Connectome Project, Semantic memory, Emotion perception, Association white matter

1. Introduction

The expansion of the association cortex, supported by long-range white matter fibers within each cerebral hemisphere, has been hypothesized to give rise to the high-level cognition that allows us to process the complexity of our surrounding world (Buckner and Krienen, 2013). Situated within the association cortex are two cognitive capacities, language and theory of mind (ToM), that contribute to successful communication by means of understanding the words, beliefs, and emotional states of others (Braga et al., 2020; DiNicola et al., 2020). Diffusion magnetic resonance imaging (dMRI), which estimates the cellular microstructure through measuring the random diffusion of water molecules in the brain, can be used to noninvasively map the white matter connections (i.e., fiber tracts) (Basser et al., 2000; Basser and Pierpaoli, 1996). Quantitative dMRI measures can also be used to investigate the relationship between the microstructure of the white matter tracts and measures of language and ToM function (Forkel and Catani, 2019; Wang et al., 2018).

Unlike task-based and resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) investigations which are able to consistently localize and differentiate language and ToM networks in the cortex (Braga et al., 2020; Carrington and Bailey, 2009; DiNicola et al., 2020; Dodell-Feder et al., 2011; Fedorenko et al., 2010; Hertrich et al., 2020), dMRI investigations have revealed numerous and sometimes conflicting white matter tracts to be associated with these functions. For example, the right inferior occipital frontal fasciculus (IOFF, also called IFOF) has been linked to improved performance on language assessments in healthy controls (Houston et al., 2019), while studies of glioma patients provide evidence for the role of the left IOFF in language processing (Almairac et al., 2015). Likewise, there have been contradictory findings regarding the relationship between measures of the right uncinate fasciculus (UF) and assessments of facial emotion processing (see Coad et al., 2020, for an investigation of 42 subjects, and Unger et al., 2016, for an investigation of 28 subjects). Reviews that discuss the white matter tracts and their relationship to language or ToM have suggested that the variability of results may be related to the effects of disease, limitations in sample sizes, and differences in dMRI methods and atlas-based approaches (Dick and Tremblay, 2012; Forkel et al., 2020; Gierhan, 2013; Wang et al., 2018; Wang and Olson, 2018). While language and ToM are both social tools that together enable successful communication, the majority of existing dMRI studies have focused their investigations on subcomponents of the language network (Houston et al., 2019) or the ToM network (Unger et al., 2016). Few white matter tract studies and reviews have examined both language and ToM function together (Forkel et al., 2020; Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017). Results from such investigations indicate that the same tracts (e.g. the arcuate fasciculus (AF)) may support both language and ToM networks. Finally, we note that recent lesion analysis studies and cognitive stimulation mapping results also point to more broadly distributed cortical regions and additional white matter tracts involved in supporting these functions (Drane and Pedersen, 2019; Yamada et al., 2021) than reflected by the classical models (i.e., language models that connect Wernicke’s to Broca’s area via the AF Geschwind, 1970 and ToM models that include the right temporal parietal junction as a critical hub Saxe and Wexler, 2005).

To address the limitations and expand upon findings from previous studies, the present study investigates 20 white matter tracts in a large dataset of 809 healthy brains from Human Connectome Project (HCP) (Van Essen et al., 2013) using a machine learning approach to consistently extract the white matter tracts from individual subjects (Zhang et al., 2018b). The long-range intra-hemispheric white matter tract connections examined in this study include the left and right arcuate fasciculus (AF), cingulum bundle (CB), extreme capsule (EmC), inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), inferior occipito-frontal fasciculus (IOFF), middle longitudinal fasciculus (MdLF), superior longitudinal fasciculi I, II, III (SLF-I, SLF-II, SLF-III), and uncinate fasciculus (UF) (Zhang et al., 2018b). Furthering our understanding of how language and ToM are organized in the brain, we investigate how the white matter association tract microstructure in both hemispheres influences performance on the NIH Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Test (TPVT; Gershon et al., 2014 and the Penn Emotion Recognition Test (PERT; Carter et al., 2009). To our knowledge, this is the first study to use a dataset of this size to examine how the microstructure of the white matter tracts relates to cognitive and socio-emotional function. Furthermore, we believe it is the first to investigate the white matter tracts underlying a subcomponent of language and a subcomponent of ToM in the same study, using common methods and healthy adult participants.

The following sections of this paper describe the Human Connectome Project (HCP) dataset, the identification of the white matter association tracts using machine learning (Zhang et al., 2018b), and a description of the multiple linear regression statistical analyses performed. We find the microstructure of multiple white matter tracts in both the left and right hemispheres significantly influences performance on TPVT, a language-related assessment of semantic memory, and PERT, a ToM-related assessment of emotion perception. Our results suggest that multiple white matter tracts support the distributed language and ToM networks. Finally, we conclude with a discussion of the results in terms of their hemispheric dominance and concordance with prior investigations.

2. Methods

Fig. 1 provides an overall conceptual representation of the methodology used to examine how behavior relates to microstructural measures of the white matter tracts. We focused on two types of brain data included in the HCP dataset, namely dMRI data and neuropsychological data. From the dMRI data, we generated whole brain tractography for each participant, and we identified each individual’s unique white matter tracts using a neuroanatomical atlas. Then, we measured microstructural measures from each tract. Linear regression statistical models were created with the microstructural measures as independent variables and the neuropsychological behavioral measures (TPVT and PERT) as dependent variables. Finally, the white matter tracts whose microstructure significantly influenced each behavioral measure were visualized. The following paragraphs of this section describe each of these steps in greater detail.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Schematic of overall methodology. First, dMRI data and neuropsychological assessment scores (TPVT and PERT) were obtained from the HCP. Whole brain tractography was computed using individual dMRI data from 809 participants. Next, using the whole brain tractography, we identified each association tract included in the atlas. From the identified tracts, three microstructural measures (FA, MD and NoS) were computed. These microstructural measures were then used as independent variables in linear regression models, which had either TPVT or PERT as a dependent variable. The linear models were corrected for multiple comparisons using the FDR method. Finally, white matter tracts with microstructural measures that significantly influenced performance on neuropsychological behavioral assessments were visualized. HCP: Human Connectome Project, WMA: White Matter Analysis, FA: fractional anisotropy, MD: mean diffusivity, NoS: number of streamlines.

2.1. Study dataset

This study utilized dMRI data and neuropsychological assessment data from the HCP, a large multimodal dataset composed of 1206 healthy young adults (Van Essen et al., 2013). Written informed consent, including consent to share de-identified data, was obtained from each participant and approved by the Washington University Institutional Review Board. Permission to use this data was obtained from HCP and the protocol was approved by the Mass General Brigham Institutional Review Board.

The dMRI data was collected for a subset of 1065 participants. From the 1065 subjects, 100 participants with dMRI data were excluded from this analysis because they were used to develop the white matter atlas employed in this project (for details see Zhang et al., 2018b). Furthermore, to control for the genetic family structure inherent to this dataset, 154 monozygotic twin siblings were excluded due to their nearly identical genetics. Two subjects without PERT scores were also excluded. The final study dataset thus included 809 participants, 382 male participants and 427 female participants. Ages ranged from 22 years to 36 years, with a mean age of 28.6 years. 710 participants demonstrated a preference of right-handedness, scoring at least 40 on a scale ranging from −100 to 100 denoting exclusively left-handedness to exclusively right-handedness (Oldfield, 1971; Schachter et al., 1987). (See Supplementary Fig. 1 depicting the distribution of participant handedness.)

Scores from two computerized neuropsychological assessments were investigated, TPVT and PERT. TPVT is a measure of single word semantic memory (Gershon et al., 2013) and PERT is a measure of facial emotion perception (Carter et al., 2009). Although TPVT and PERT directly measure very narrow subcomponents of cognition, there is evidence to suggest that these subcomponents relate to the more general and broadly distributed brain networks of language and theory of mind (ToM) (Fedorenko et al., 2010; Hawco et al., 2017). While these studies suggest a relationship between the abilities tested by these assessments and the broader language and ToM cognitive networks in the brain, we note this is an open area of research. (See reviews on the relationship between language and memory (Martin and Slevc, 2012; Van Dyke, 2012) and the overlap between emotion perception and ToM (Mitchell and Phillips, 2015).

To briefly summarize the TPVT assessment, participants heard a word and were presented with four pictures on a screen (Gershon et al., 2014). Using computer adaptive testing, participants were instructed to click on the picture that captured the meaning of the presented word. A maximum of 25 pictures were presented, and responses were scored using item response theory (Gershon et al., 2014). We used the unadjusted TPVT scores that were normed without taking into account effects of age and gender (Slotkin et al., 2012). This allowed us to control for effects of age and sex demongraphics unique to our participant sample. Higher TPVT scores reflect a better vocabulary ability, while lower scores reflect poorer performance. The mean unadjusted TPVT score in the study population was 117.12 points (Fig. 2, left panel).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Histograms of TPVT and PERT. Left: Histogram of TPVT scores, minimum score = 90.69, maximum score = 153.09, mean of scores = 117.12. Right: Histogram of PERT scores, minimum score = 24, maximum score = 40, mean of scores = 35.59

Likewise, to briefly summarize the PERT assessment, participants viewed 40 different faces balanced for gender, age, ethnicity and emotional category (Carter et al., 2009). Participants were asked to identify the emotion elicited by that facial expression. Participants chose from five possible emotional expressions – angry, fearful, happy, sad and neutral (see Carter et al., 2009 for a more detailed explanation of the task). PERT scores were calculated from the number of correct responses out of a maximum score of 40. PERT scores in the study population demonstrated a slight left skew, with 13 participants scoring the maximum possible score of 40 points and a mean score of 35.59 points (Fig. 2, right panel).

2.2. Tractography, white matter tract identification, and diffusion measure extraction

The investigated dMRI data comes from the HCP minimally pre-processed dataset (Glasser et al., 2013). The HCP dMRI data were acquired using three shells (b = 1000, 2000 and 3000 s/mm2 ), TE/TR = 89.5/5520 ms, and an isotropic voxel size of 1.25 mm3 (Van Essen et al., 2013). For each subject, the b = 3000 shell of 90 gradient directions and all b = 0 scans were extracted (for similar methods see also Zhang et al., 2018b). This single shell was chosen to reduce computation time and memory use given our large sample size, while providing the highest angular resolution for tractography (Descoteaux et al., 2007; Ning et al., 2015).

Whole brain tractography was computed by applying a two-tensor Unscented Kalman Filter (UKF) method (Malcolm et al., 2010; Reddy and Rathi, 2016) to each subject’s preprocessed dMRI data, as implemented in the ukftractography package (https://github.com/pnlbwh/ ukftractography). This step produced approximately 700k streamlines per subject on average. A two-tensor model was chosen to account for the many crossing fibers (Farquharson et al., 2013; Vos et al., 2013) found in the architecturally complex white matter association tracts. In this method, the first tensor is associated with the fiber tract that is being traced, while the second tensor captures the fibers that cross through that tract. UKF tractography is highly consistent across ages, health conditions and image acquisitions (Zhang et al., 2018b) and has enabled many studies of tract microstructure (Hamoda et al., 2019; Olszewski et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2018a). Visual and quantitative quality control of the tractography was performed using a quality control tool in the whitematteranalysis (WMA) software package (https://github.com/SlicerDMRI/whitematteranalysis).

For each subject, we identified white matter association tracts using a robust machine learning approach that has been shown to consistently identify white matter tracts across the full human lifespan, across health conditions including brain tumors, and across different image acquisitions (Zhang et al., 2018b). Furthermore, the method has high test-retest reproducibility (Zhang et al., 2019) and has been employed in multiple recent studies (Gong et al., 2018; Irimia et al., 2020; Kochsiek et al., 2021; Levitt et al., 2021). This method is implemented in the WMA package, which uses a well-established fiber clustering pipeline (O’Donnell et al., 2012; O’Donnell and Westin, 2007) in conjunction with an anatomical white matter tract atlas (Zhang et al., 2018b). This neuroanatomist-curated white matter tract atlas was trained using tractography data from a separate subset of 100 HCP participants. The atlas contains 58 unique white matter tracts, including 20 white matter association tracts that connect intrahemispheric cortical regions. We applied the WMA package to parcellate each participant’s whole brain tractography into separate tracts that correspond with the atlas tracts. To achieve this, first a tractography-based registration was performed to align the subject’s tractography data into the atlas space (O’Donnell et al., 2012). Then, a fiber spectral embedding was conducted to compute the similarity of fibers between the subject and the atlas, followed by the assignment of each fiber of the subject to the corresponding atlas tract (O’Donnell and Westin, 2007; Zhang et al., 2018b). This produced a total of 58 identified white matter tracts per subject, including the 20 white matter association tracts under study.

All of the long-range ipsilateral association white matter tracts in the atlas were investigated. This anatomical set of tracts consists of the left and right arcuate fasciculus (AF), cingulum bundle (CB), extreme capsule (EmC), inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), inferior occipito-frontal fasciculus (IOFF), middle longitudinal fasciculus (MdLF), superior longitudinal fasciculi I, II and III (SLF-I, SLF-II, SLF-III), and uncinate fasciculus (UF) (Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006). The long-range association tracts were selected for this investigation because of their well documented relationship to behavior (Schmahmann et al., 2008) and their complete mapping in our atlas (Zhang et al., 2018b). Fig. 3 provides lateral views of these 20 white matter tracts. (See Supplementary Figs. 2 and 4 for additional visualizations of these tracts.) None of the white matter association tracts were excluded from this investigation because there is evidence to suggest that each of the white matter association tracts belonging to either the left or right hemisphere may be involved in subcomponents of language and/or ToM (Almairac et al., 2015; Blom-Smink et al., 2020; Del Tufo et al., 2019; Fujie et al., 2008; Geschwind, 1970; Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Herbet et al., 2014; Herbet et al., 2015; Houston et al., 2019; Luo et al., 2020; Makris and Pandya, 2009; Mandonnet et al., 2007; Nakajima et al., 2018; Pawełczyk et al., 2020; Philippi et al., 2009; Salvan et al., 2017; Saur et al., 2008; Thomas et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2013; Wilson et al., 2011). Therefore, as to not preclude finding a possible microstructural influence on assessments of language and ToM subcomponents, all the long-range association tracts in both hemispheres were investigated.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

The 20 white matter association tracts. Tracts are positioned in front of an HCP population mean T1 mid-sagittal slice. Top panel: Lateral views of the 10 left and 10 right hemisphere tracts. Bottom panel: Each individual left hemisphere tract depicted unaccompanied by other tracts. Tracts visualized do not belong to a single brain, but rather represent the anatomical structure and variability found in the atlas’s 100 subjects. Different colors signify different white matter association tracts. L: left; R: right; AF: arcuate fasciculus; CB: cingulum bundle; EmC: extreme capsule; ILF: inferior longitudinal fasciculus; IOFF: inferior occipital fasciculus; MdLF: middle longitudinal fasciculus; SLF-I,II,III: superior longitudinal fasciculus I, II, III; UF: uncinate fasciculus.

For each individual’s white matter tracts, three white matter tract measurements were calculated: the fractional anisotropy of the first tensor (FA), the mean diffusivity of the first tensor (MD), and the number of streamlines (NoS) within each tract. Mean FA, MD, and NoS were chosen for their common use in diffusion imaging studies, including those of language and ToM (Forkel and Catani, 2019; Wang et al., 2018). FA indexes how far a tensor is from a sphere to quantify the diffusion anisotropy of the water molecules. Very elongated tensors have a high FA value of 1, while spherical tensors have a low FA value of 0 (O’Donnell and Westin, 2011). MD indexes the average magnitude of water diffusion (O’Donnell and Westin, 2011). The NoS is popularly used to detect differences in structural connectivity (Sotiropoulos and Zalesky, 2019). Note, NoS is sometimes termed “number of fibers.” However, the term “fibers” does not refer to the number of axonal projections, but rather the number of reconstructed streamlines (Jones et al., 2013). Additionally, we note that these measures are all sensitive to a variety of tissue properties including the myelination, axonal density, cell membranes, and microtubules (Beaulieu, 2009; Jones et al., 2013). In this sense, each measure reflects multiple aspects of the tract microstructure and not a specific microscopic anatomical property.

2.3. Statistical analyses and white matter tract visualizations

All statistical models were computed using R statistical software (R Core Team, 2020). Multiple linear regression was used to examine the influence of tract measures on assessment scores on TPVT and PERT. For each of the three tract measures (i.e. FA, MD, and NoS), a total of 20 models were created. Half of these models had PERT as the dependent variable and the other half had TPVT as the dependent variable. For each assessment, 10 models were created for each of the 10 bilateral white matter tracts, so that each regression model had two tract measures (left and right tracts) as independent variables. Following the technique of a previous study (Unger et al., 2016), both a left tract and a right tract were included in each model to account for their fairly symmetrical structure across hemispheres (see SupplementaryFig. 3 for plots (Phillips, 2017) depicting each tract’s laterality). Age and sex were included as covariates, as previous studies have demonstrated their association with white matter structure (Charlton et al., 2009; Houston et al., 2019; Ingalhalikar et al., 2014; Lebel et al., 2012; Slater et al., 2019; van Hemmen et al., 2017). For each of the three tract measures investigated, we corrected for the multiple comparisons (i.e., across 20 behavioral regression models). P-values associated with each model and independent variable were adjusted using the false discovery rate (FDR) (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). Standardized beta coefficients were computed for each of the independent variables using the “lm.beta” package in R (Behrendt, 2014). Effect plots were created using the “effects” package in R (Fox and Weisberg, 2018a, 2018b). All brain visualizations were created in 3D Slicer, a platform for medical image informatics (www.slicer.org) via SlicerDMRI, a module to map the white matter connections (http://dmri.slicer.org) (Norton et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2020). White matter tracts whose measures significantly influenced performance on PERT or TPVT are depicted in visualizations. In addition, to assist understanding of the tract results in relation to the cortical regions involved, we aligned the white matter tract atlas to the standard MNI space where a Freesurfer cerebral cortical parcellation is made available (https://identifiers.org/neurovault.image:23262) (Desikan et al., 2006; Fischl et al., 2004). Then, for each tract under study, we provide a visualization of a cortical surface heatmap, where the values indicate the number of steamline endpoints closest to each surface point.

3. Results

Table 1 summarizes the results from the computed multiple regression analyses. Each model was computed with either TPVT or PERT as the dependent variable. The independent variables included in each model are microstructure measures of the left and right white matter tracts, along with age and gender as covariates. In Table 1, we include the models with tract measures that significantly influenced performance after correcting for multiple comparisons. For each model, we present the overall R2 and F values. For the independent variables from each model, we provide the p-value, standardized beta coefficient and t-value. Significant independent variables are noted with asterisks.

Table 1.

Summary of regression models in which the white matter microstructure significantly influenced TPVT or PERT performance. Regression models are ordered by F-statistic. Only the significant (p < 0.05) models after FDR correction are reported. F-statistic degrees of freedom are 4 and 804. 𝛽 denotes the standardized beta coefficients.

Number of Streamlines
P 𝛽 t R2 F
TPVT 0.07 16.16
 left ILF * 0.025 0.13 2.81
 right ILF 0.057 0.11 2.45
 age *** < 0.001 0.13 3.81
 sex 0.117 0.06 1.57
TPVT 0.06 12.72
 left SLF-II * 0.015 0.13 3.19
 right SLF-II 0.156 0.08 1.93
 age *** < 0.001 0.13 3.84
 sex 0.050 0.07 2.01
TPVT 0.05 10.60
 left UF 0.786 0.03 0.72
 right UF * 0.035 0.13 2.71
 age *** < 0.001 0.14 3.96
 sex 0.074 0.07 1.81
TPVT 0.05 10.05
 left MdLF 0.976 − 0.01 − 0.34
 right MdLF ** 0.009 0.14 3.48
 age *** < 0.001 0.13 3.74
 sex * 0.017 0.11 3.10
TPVT 0.04 9.08
 left AF * 0.014 0.14 3.41
 right AF 0.235 − 0.07 − 1.62
 age *** < 0.001 0.14 3.85
 sex * 0.017 0.11 2.99
TPVT 0.04 9.03
 left IOFF * 0.029 0.12 2.69
 right IOFF 0.780 0.02 0.44
 age *** < 0.001 0.14 3.84
 sex * 0.044 0.08 2.14
TPVT 0.04 9.02
 left SLF-III * 0.025 0.11 2.82
 right SLF-III 0.631 0.03 0.75
 age *** < 0.001 0.14 4.08
 sex * 0.028 0.10 2.62
PERT 0.02 3.93
 left SLF-III 0.953 − 0.02 − 0.43
 right SLF-III ** 0.009 0.13 3.32
 age 0.497 − 0.10 − 2.59
 sex * 0.028 − 0.02 − 0.68
PERT 0.02 3.12
 left UF 0.169 − 0.09 − 1.89
 right UF * 0.025 0.14 2.91
 age 0.492 − 0.09 − 2.28
 sex * 0.039 − 0.03 − 0.73
Fractional Anisotropy
P 𝛽 t R2 F

TPVT 0.07 14.93
 left ILF *** < 0.001 0.21 4.78
 right ILF 0.919 − 0.02 − 0.46
 age *** < 0.001 0.15 4.18
 sex *** < 0.001 0.15 4.18
TPVT 0.05 10.24
 left AF** 0.002 0.16 3.76
 right AF 0.668 − 0.04 − 0.92
 age *** < 0.001 0.14 4.06
 sex *** < 0.001 0.13 3.74
TPVT 0.04 9.07
 left EmC ** 0.004 0.14 3.40
 right EmC 0.508 − 0.07 − 1.78
 age *** < 0.001 0.14 4.09
 sex ** 0.001 0.13 3.63
TPVT 0.04 8.93
 left SLF-II * 0.024 0.12 2.76
 right SLF-II 0.955 0.00 − 0.08
 age *** < 0.001 0.14 4.03
 sex ** 0.002 0.12 3.33
PERT 0.02 4.26
 left AF ** 0.003 0.15 3.50
 right AF 0.668 − 0.05 − 1.27
 age 0.531 − 0.07 − 1.95
 sex 0.061 − 0.03 − 0.76
Mean Diffusivity
P 𝛽 t R2 F

TPVT 0.04 8.97
 left SLF-II * 0.017 − 0.14 − 3.35
 right SLF-II 0.374 0.07 1.60
 age *** < 0.001 0.15 4.30
 sex ** 0.003 0.12 3.30

Significant codes are *** for p < 0.001, ** for p < 0.01, and * for p < 0.05 (after controlling for multiple comparisons). FA: fractional anisotropy, MD: mean diffusivity, NoS: number of streamlines, TPVT: Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Test, PERT: Penn Emotion Recognition Test.

Visualizing the asterisked white matter tracts of Table 1, Fig. 4 depicts the tracts with microstructure that significantly influenced TPVT and PERT performance. The leftmost columns depict the tracts with microstructure that influence TPVT, and the rightmost columns depict tracts with microstructure that influence PERT. Each row in the top and bottom panels displays a different microstructure tract measure (i.e., NoS, FA and MD). In the top panel, white matter tracts are colored according to p-value; light yellow tracts have microstructure that significantly influence TPVT or PERT performance with the lowest p-values, while orange and red tracts have the highest p-values. In the bottom panel, white matter tracts that significantly influence these two assessments are colored according to standardized beta coefficient; light colored tracts have microstructure with the greatest effect, while darker colored tracts have microstructure with the least effect on these assessments.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Visualization of the association white matter tracts with microstructure that significantly influences performance on PERT or TPVT. Top panel: Tracts are colored according to their FDR-corrected p-value. Bottom panel: Tracts are colored according to their standardized beta coefficient. L: left hemisphere, R: right hemisphere, NoS: Number of Streamlines, FA: fractional anisotropy, MD: mean diffusivity; TPVT: Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Test; PERT: Penn Emotion Recognition Test.

We examine the SLF-III more closely because the microstructure of the left SLF-III influenced performance on TPVT, while the microstructure of the right SLF-III influenced performance on PERT. Conversely, the microstructure of the left AF influenced performance on both TPVT and PERT. Fig. 5 depicts effect plots visualizing the partial slope of the tract microstructure measure when the other independent variables (i.e., a measure of the opposite hemisphere tract, age, and sex) are held fixed. Tract microstructure measures examined are the NoS of the SLF-III and FA of the AF.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

SLF-III and AF Effect Plots. Plots depict the effect of the NoS of the SLF-III (top panel) and FA of the AF (bottom panel) in both the left and right hemisphere on TPVT and PERT performance. The shaded region signifies the confidence band for the fitted values computed from the regression model (dark gray: TPVT, light gray: PERT). Tick marks at the bottom of each plot visualize the distribution of FA values and NoS. P-values and beta coefficients are reported for each plot. N.S.: not significant.

To help visualize how the AF and SLF-III relate to the broader language and ToM networks, the cortical regions these tracts connect are depicted in Fig. 6 (see Supplementary Fig. 4 for visualizations of additional tracts). The AF broadly connects the temporal regions (including the superior, middle and inferior temporal cortices) with frontal regions (including caudal middle frontal, rostral middle frontal, pars opercularis, and precentral cortices). The SLF-III forms major connections between parietal regions (including supramarginal and inferior parietal cortices) and prefrontal regions (including the pars opercularis).

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6.

The AF and SLF-III are depicted in relation to the cortical regions they connect (see Supplementary Fig. 4 for additional tracts and their connected regions). The first row depicts the atlas white matter tracts (yellow) superimposed over the mid-sagittal slice of the atlas population mean T2w image from left and right lateral views. The second row depicts a cortical surface heat map of the tract termination points; darker red regions indicate a higher density of termination points, while lighter yellow regions indicate lower density of termination points. For ease of interpretation, the third row depicts the cortical regions colored according to the freesurfer color scheme. Regions with a high density of connections in either the AF or SLF-III are numbered. 1: superior temporal, 2: middle temporal cortex, 3: inferior temporal, 4: pars opercularis, 5: precentral, 6: caudal middle frontal, 7: rostral middle frontal, 8: supramarginal, 9: inferior parietal, L: left, R: right, AF: Arcuate Fasciculus, SLF-III: Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus III.

4. Discussion

This study used dMRI tractography of bilateral association tracts to investigate the relationship between tract microstructure and subcomponents of language and theory of mind (ToM). In a large sample of healthy subjects, we found that multiple dMRI tract measures significantly influence performance on assessments of language and ToM subcomponents (i.e. semantic memory and emotion perception). These results demonstrate a significant relationship between white matter structure and cognitive function, and this work lends support to the concept that multiple white matter tracts contribute to the broad networks of language and ToM.

Although significant, the results indicate that the structural measures investigated only weakly influence cognitive function in healthy adults, as less than 10% of the variability in the cognitive measures can be explained by any one model (the R2 values in Table 1 are less than 0.1). This is in line with a body of recent work that uses neuroimaging measures to examine behavior in the HCP dataset (Sina Mansour et al., 2021; Tian et al., 2020). For example, one recent study used a novel, high-resolution structural connectivity map of the entire brain to predict an overall “cognition” score that was derived using 109 behavioral assessments from the HCP. The resulting explained variance was well under 0.1 (Sina Mansour et al., 2021). Overall, prediction of phenotype data (such as neuropsychological behavioral measures) using neuroimage and connectome information is an open challenge (Finn and Rosenberg, 2021), with novel machine learning methods under investigation for improvement of prediction accuracy (W. Gong et al., 2021).

4.1. Opposing hemispheric dominance

Our results are relevant to the lateralization and hemispheric dominance of language and ToM. fMRI evidence points to language lateralized to the left hemisphere (Bradshaw et al., 2017) and ToM lateralized to the right hemisphere (Saxe and Wexler, 2005). In this sense we expect to find similar results regarding their hemispheric dominance in dMRI studies. However, existing dMRI studies of healthy subjects have provided contradictory results. For example, better performance on language-related assessments has been associated with the white matter tracts in the non-dominant hemisphere (Houston et al., 2019). Similarly, better performance on ToM-related assessments has been associated with the white matter tracts in both hemispheres (Cabinio et al., 2015; Unger et al., 2016). These results have been explained as recruitment of the additional hemisphere resulting in better performance on language and ToM assessments. However, such results are contrasted by evidence gathered from investigations of the white matter tracts in various patient populations. In investigations of patient populations, language deficits are linked to abnormalities of the left hemisphere white matter (Griffis et al., 2017; Luo et al., 2020; Sierpowska et al., 2019) and ToM deficits are found to arise from abnormalities of the right hemisphere white matter (Díez-Cirarda et al., 2015; Meletti et al., 2009; Philippi et al., 2009; Weed et al., 2010). The findings from this present study add support to the understanding that the language network is typically rooted in the left hemisphere, and ToM is situated in the right hemisphere. As depicted in Fig. 4, the majority of the white matter tracts with microstructure that influenced semantic memory performance were in the left hemisphere, while the majority of the white matter tracts with microstructure that influenced emotion perception were in the right hemisphere. Additionally, our results imply that neither of these subcomponents of language and ToM is completely lateralized to a single hemisphere. We found the microstructure of the left AF influenced emotion perception performance, and the microstructure of right MdLF and right UF influenced semantic memory performance. This finding agrees with previous investigations that have found that the language and ToM networks are oppositely lateralized but distributed across both hemispheres in neurotypical individuals (Dufour et al., 2013; Fedorenko et al., 2010; Jouravlev et al., 2020). However, as Supplementary Fig. 1 demonstrates we have included participants with preferences for left and mixed handedness. This population is known to have higher rates of atypical functional laterality compared to the right-handed population (Szaflarski et al., 2002), which may have influenced our findings.

While many of the white matter tracts studied here have been heavily investigated in terms of language and ToM (e.g., AF, SLF-III, SLF-II, IOFF, UF, ILF (Chang et al., 2015; Coad et al., 2020; Dick and Tremblay, 2012; Liégeois et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2018), we will briefly discuss some of the white matter tracts that are less often investigated, including the MdLF and the EmC. The MdLF and EmC are two parallel running white matter tracts that have only recently been delineated using diffusion MRI (Makris, 1999; Makris et al., 2009; Makris and Pandya, 2009). Studies have linked both the MdLF and EmC to aspects of language. More specifically, the MdLF has been associated with semantic deficits and EmC have been found to play a role in language learning (Lopez-Barroso et al., 2011; Luo et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2013). Our results support previous work suggesting that the MdLF and EmC are both involved in language processing, as we found the MdLF and EmC microstructure significantly influenced an important subcomponent of language function, semantic memory. More specifically, we found the NoS of the right MdLF and FA of the left EmC to significantly influence semantic memory performance. It should be specified that the MdLF was traced involving the inferior and superior parietal lobules (IPL and SPL) as well as the superior temporal gyrus and temporal pole. Therefore, one may expect it to be associated with not only language function but also visual spatial processing, which may explain the rightward laterality (Makris et al., 2013; Makris et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2013).

Our results may also shed light on the role of the AF and SLF-III in the broader networks of language and ToM. Many studies have tied the lateralization of the left AF to lateralization of language (Allendorfer et al., 2016; Barrick et al., 2007; Paldino et al., 2016; Sreedharan et al., 2015; Takaya et al., 2015). While our results support the role of the left AF’s involvement in semantic memory, our results additionally indicate that the microstructure of the left AF also influences emotion perception performance (see Fig. 5, bottom panel). While the role of the left AF in the ToM network is less noted in the literature, this study is not the first to observe a relationship between the AF and ToM-related assessments. A study on glioma patients found a relationship between the right AF and the ToM network (Herbet et al., 2014), while a study on developing children found a relationship between both right and left AF and ToM performance (Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017). Our findings and previous research bring into question how the left AF may be involved in right ToM lateralization.

Additionally, our results point to the SLF-III playing a role in the functional lateralization of language and ToM. We found the NoS of the left SLF-III significantly influences semantic memory performance and the NoS of the right SLF-III significantly influences emotion perception performance (see Fig. 5, top panel). That is, the microstructure of the SLF-III in the left hemisphere related to a left-lateralized language network and the microstructure of the SLF-III in the right hemisphere related to a right-lateralized ToM network. Previous work has suggested the interactions mediated by the SLF-III may be a prerequisite for gestural communication that “presages” linguistic communication (Petrides and Pandya, 2002; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006) and has been hypothesized to be involved in articulatory aspects of language (Nikos Makris et al., 2005). In addition, in the monkey, there is evidence that the frontal regions that receive input from the SLF-III contain “mirror neurons” (Rizzolatti et al., 1996, 1999; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006), which may interplay with the ToM network (Schulte-Rüther et al., 2007). More recently, the SLF-III has been considered to contribute to a variety of functions beyond but including semantic processing and mirror neuron related functions (e.g. action observation and ToM) (Parlatini et al., 2017). An interesting direction for future research will be to further investigate the role the SLF-III may play in language and ToM lateralization. Additionally, future studies may investigate the role additional white matter tracts, (e.g., thalamo-cortical projection tracts) play in lateralization and hemispheric dominance of language, ToM, and their subcomponents.

4.2. Relationship of AF and SLF-III to cortical regions

As discussed above, our results may shed light on the role of the AF and SLF-III in the broader networks of language and ToM. Here we discuss the cortical regions that are connected by the AF and SLF-III (as visualized in Fig. 6), with regards to their functional roles in the broader language and ToM networks. First, we note that the regional connections made by the AF and SLF-III in our atlas are in line with previous research describing the AF connecting frontal and temporal regions (Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten, 2008; Makris et al., 2005; Rilling et al., 2008), and the SLF-III connecting parietal and frontal regions (Barbeau et al., 2020; Makris et al., 2005). It is important to note that both the precise cortical terminations of the tracts (Yeh, 2020), and the functional regions associated with language and ToM (Braga et al., 2020; DiNicola et al., 2020; Fedorenko et al., 2010; Ojemann, 1979; Saxe, 2006) vary across individuals.

The left hemisphere regions that are connected by the left AF and left SLF-III are more frequently associated with language-related processes than ToM-related processes. Regions in the left superior, middle and inferior temporal cortices, where many streamlines of the left AF terminate, have been associated with various language and language-related processes including the encoding of speech sounds, thought processes, and comprehension (Binder, 2017; Corina et al., 2010; Dronkers et al., 2004; Turken and Dronkers, 2011; Wensing et al., 2017; Yi et al., 2019). The left frontal regions (and in particular, the left pars opercularis), which receive many terminations from both the left AF and left SLF-III, have also been associated with various language and speech functions (Long et al., 2016; Rogalsky et al., 2008). Finally, the left parietal regions, such as the supramarginal and inferior parietal cortices, which receive terminations from the left SLF-III, have been associated with semantic processing (Gilmore et al., 2021; Numssen et al., 2021). The left frontal and temporal connections of the left AF are less frequently discussed in the ToM literature, although some reports have investigated left frontal regions (Marchetti et al., 2015; Rowe et al., 2001).

The right hemisphere regions that are connected via the right SLF-III are frequently associated with ToM-related processes in the literature. The right parietal regions in which the streamlines from the right SLF-III terminate (sometimes termed the right temporal parietal junction (rTPJ)) are associated with ToM, social cognitive, and emotional processes (Lettieri et al., 2019; Numssen et al., 2021; Schurz et al., 2014). The right inferior frontal regions where the SLF-III streamlines terminate are inconsistently associated with ToM processes, perhaps due to fMRI paradigmatic constraints (Hartwright et al., 2016; Samson et al., 2015; Schurz and Tholen, 2016).

4.3. Microstructural measures and interpretation

The tract microstructure measure that is best able to capture different aspects of cognition is an open area of research. This study investigated 3 measures ( FA, MD and NoS) commonly utilized in diffusion MRI research (Forkel and Catani, 2019). Overall, in this study, NoS was the tract measure that most consistently influenced performance on both TPVT and PERT (Table 1). The NoS of 7 tracts influenced performance on TPVT, and the NoS in 2 tracts influenced performance on PERT. The FA of four tracts (left ILF, left AF, left EmC and left SLF-II) significantly influenced performance on TPVT. The FA of only one tract, the left AF, significantly influenced PERT. MD was the least influential tract measure, with only the MD of the left SLF-II influencing performance on TPVT.

How strongly each of these microstructural measures influence subcomponents of language and ToM (i.e. as measured through TPVT and PERT) may relate to the inherent sensitivity of these microstructural measures. The least influential of the investigated measures, MD, is nearly constant in healthy brain parenchyma (Pierpaoli et al., 1996), though it is somewhat reduced in regions of complex fiber architecture (Vos et al., 2012). FA, which was the second most influential of the investigated measures, is highly sensitive to local differences in fiber architecture (e.g. fiber crossings) (Pierpaoli et al., 1996). Finally, NoS, which was found to be the most influential measure in this study, is an indirect measure of tissue microstructure that is especially sensitive to starting and stopping anisotropy thresholds used to begin and terminate fiber tracking. There is not a direct relationship between NoS and the number of biological axons (Jones et al., 2013), though it is a popular and widely used measure to quantify the connectivity strength (Zhang et al., 2021). In stark contrast to real biological axons, streamlines do not have an associated volume; streamlines are by definition one-dimensional and thus, infinitesimally thin (Smith et al., 2020). NoS must therefore be interpreted with caution (Jones et al., 2013). However, as a measure of the connectivity between cortical regions, the NoS and related measures are highly sensitive to brain changes in health and disease (Fornito et al., 2015; Griffa et al., 2013).

Given the hemispheric lateralization of language and ToM in the human brain, it is also possible that the influence of each of these microstructural measures on subcomponents of language and ToM relates to their degree of lateralization across hemispheres. The lateralization of the white matter fiber tracts has been studied using many different measures (such as NoS, FA and tract volume) (Bain et al., 2019; Catani et al., 2007; Lebel and Beaulieu, 2009; Maffei et al., 2019; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011). To assess sensitivity to brain lateralization in our study, we measured the lateralization of each of the investigated measures (see Supplementary Fig. 3). We found the greatest degree of lateralization in NoS and the least amount of lateralization in MD. Others have found more variable degrees of lateralization in NoS, FA, and volume of the association white matter tracts (Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011).

It is important to note that the white matter measures investigated in this paper are correlated. This is expected, as MD and FA are not mutually mathematically orthogonal measures (Ennis and Kindlmann, 2006), and the NoS is affected by seeding thresholds determined by FA. Despite these well-known correlations, MD and FA are almost always studied together because they provide insight into different properties of the diffusion tensor (the magnitude of diffusivity and the “shape” of the tensor) (O’Donnell and Westin, 2011). In addition, the study of NoS is very popular as it provides a proxy of “connection strength” (Zhang et al., 2021). Interestingly, we found that the correlation between measures varied across tracts and across hemispheres (see Supplementary Fig.5).

4.4. Limitations and future work

It is important to note several limitations of this study. While our study is able to take advantage of an unusually large sample size, we are limited to the neuropsychological measures included in the HCP. Despite the limited sensitivity of PERT (see slight ceiling effect and limited range of scores in Fig. 2), we were able to find 3 tract measures that significantly influenced PERT performance and 12 that influenced TPVT. While it is possible that there are fewer long-range white matter association tracts underlying emotion perception compared to semantic memory, we suspect that with a more sensitive measure (e.g., PERT correct response reaction times), we may find more tracts with microstructure that influence ToM performance. An additional limitation of this study is that we have violated the assumption of independent observations by including dizygotic twins and siblings in our analysis. Future investigations may better take into account family structure by, for example, using multi-level block permutation (Winkler et al., 2015).

Moreover, TPVT and PERT are only two subcomponent measures of highly studied and complicated domains of cognition. The extent to which we can associate TPVT and PERT with high-level naturalistic language and ToM is an open area of investigation (Chen et al., 2019; Oakley et al., 2016). However, multiple neuroimaging studies have used TPVT and PERT-like assessments to understand the broader networks of language and ToM (Chen et al., 2019; Hawco et al., 2017; Jáni and Kašpárek, 2018; Mier et al., 2010; Palmer et al., 2020). In addition, awake intraoperative neurosurgical mapping of language and ToM have utilized similar assessments in the operating room in an effort to preserve the cognition of their patients. Object naming assessments have been utilized to map language since the times of Penfield (Penfield and Roberts, 1959), and more recently facial emotion recognition tasks, such as Reading the Mind in the Eyes, have been used to intraoperatively map ToM (Herbet et al., 2015). In contrast to the majority of tasks used in intraoperative neurosurgical cognitive mapping that require spontaneously generated responses (e.g., confrontation naming tasks), TPVT and PERT only require matching words to their corresponding picture (Drane et al., 2021).

Although we have included participants who demonstrate a preference for left-handedness and ambidexterity, our participant sample predominantly has a preference for right-handedness (see Supplementary Fig. 1). In this sense, our findings most strongly reflect a right-handed population and should be interpreted with caution when applied to left-handed and ambidextrous populations. While there has been documentation of left handers demonstrating reduced functional lateralization in verbal and facial processing tasks compared to right-handed individuals (Johnstone et al., 2021), differences in the white matter are less clearly documented. Some have found increased FA of the white matter when comparing the microstructure of left-handed to right-handed adults (McKay et al., 2017), while other investigators have found no difference in the white matter microstructure between left- and right-handed children (López-Vicente et al., 2021).

We highlight the continued need for large neuroimaging samples and methods to consistently analyze them. Small sample sizes are known to be particularly problematic for neuroimaging and psychology studies because they produce unreliable results (Button et al., 2013; Maxwell et al., 2015). Our study is able to surmount these challenges by employing a machine-learning approach to consistently identify the white matter tracts in hundreds of individual brains. This allows us to investigate how each individual’s unique tract microstructure relates to individual measures of behavior. It is important that future studies investigate the reproducibility of these results using likewise large datasets, especially those with diverse demographics and unrelated participants. In lieu of other comparably large datasets, we have leveraged the current dataset to examine the statistical significance of these results. We implemented a bootstrap subsampling technique to examine the distribution of the p-value statistics calculated for each tract (see Supplementary Fig.6). The results of this bootstrap experiment support the most significant findings from the main experiment; all white matter tracts that had bootstrap p-value confidence intervals below the alpha threshold (𝛼 = 0.05) were also significant in the main experiment shown in Table 1 and visualized in Fig. 4. However, we note that some tracts did not have confidence intervals that fell entirely below the alpha threshold, despite their significance in the main experiment. This suggests additional experiments on the association white matter tracts are warranted to better understand the contribution that tract microstructure may have on TPVT and PERT.

Finally, for simplicity we have investigated only the popular MD, FA, and NoS measures here. In addition to these measures, many more measures can be computed and studied using dMRI data (Jensen et al., 2005; Pasternak et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2012). Future investigations can examine how additional measures (e.g., neurite orientation dispersion and density indices, kurtosis, axial diffusivity, radial diffusivity, tract volume) relate to measures of language and ToM. Future work may also include alternative approaches that perform dimensionality reduction of the number of dMRI measures (Ceschin et al., 2015; Chamberland et al., 2019; Geeraert et al., 2020), especially if more than three measures are of interest.

Finally, we note that this investigation has only focused on healthy subjects. As such, the interpretation of our findings is limited to the tract microstructure that is associated with improved performance and does not reveal the tracts or tract microstructure that may be essential for healthy performance. To further understand how the white matter tracts support cognition, future studies can investigate how the microstructure of these tracts differs in patient populations with known language and ToM deficits, including patients with autism spectrum disorder (Bennett et al., 2013), specific language impairment (Nilsson and de Lopez, 2016), schizophrenia (Gavilán Ibáñez and García-Albea Ristol, 2013), temporal lobe epilepsy (Giovagnoli et al., 2011; Kaestner et al., 2020), and glioma (Antonsson et al., 2018; Nakajima et al., 2018). Additionally, further studies may investigate how the microstructure of these tracts changes over the course of development and evolution. Previous studies have theorized about the intertwined relationship between language and ToM, with suggestive evidence pointing to their developmental and evolutionary influence on each other (Bergelson and Swingley, 2012; de Villiers and de Villiers, 2014; Givón and Malle, 2002; Marchman and Fernald, 2008; Miller, 2006; Ruba and Repacholi, 2019). Our findings in this study support the idea that semantic memory and emotion recognition are influenced by shared and distinct neural resources in healthy young adults.

5. Conclusion

This study maps language and ToM subcomponents in the white matter association tracts using noninvasive measures and an exceptionally large sample size (n = 809). We demonstrate that the white matter microstructure is able to influence performance on assessments of semantic memory and emotion perception, two critical subcomponents of the language and ToM networks. Our results suggest that multiple, overlapping white matter tracts underlie the cognitive domains of language and ToM. As this study demonstrates, cognition is not only situated within the association cortex but is also reflected in the microstructure of the underlying white matter association tracts.

Supplementary Material

1

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge our funding from the Jennifer Oppenheimer Cancer Initiative. Additionally, we acknowledge funding provided by the following National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants: P41EB015902, P41EB015898, P41EB028741, R01MH074794, R01MH119222, R01MH125860. FZ also acknowledges a BWH Radiology Research Pilot Grant Award. NM was a Fellow at the Hanse-Wissenschaftskolleg Institute for Advanced Study, Delmenhorst, Germany while working on this study. JH acknowledges support from the China Scholarship Council (CSC) and National Natural Science Foundation of China provided through grants: 61976190, 61903336. DLD’s efforts are partially funded by the NIH/NINDS R01NS088748.

Footnotes

Declaration of Competing Interest

None.

Credit authorship contribution statement

Leo R. Zekelman: Visualization, Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Fan Zhang: Visualization, Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Nikos Makris: Writing – review & editing. Jianzhong He: Visualization. Yuqian Chen: Visualization. Tengfei Xue: Formal analysis. Daniela Liera: Visualization, Writing – review & editing. Daniel L. Drane: Writing – review & editing. Yogesh Rathi: Writing – review & editing. Alexandra J. Golby: Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Lauren J. O’Donnell: Visualization, Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Data and code availability

All data used in this project is from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) (www.humanconnectome.org). This data is publicly available to researchers who agree to the data use terms (www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult/data-use-terms). All HCP data may be downloaded through the ConnectomeDB (db.humanconnectome.org). Data analysis code is available on (github.com/lzekelma/AWMT_lang_tom).

Supplementary materials

Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118739.

References

  1. Allendorfer JB, Hernando KA, Hossain S, Nenert R, Holland SK, Szaflarski JP, 2016. Arcuate fasciculus asymmetry has a hand in language function but not handedness. Hum. Brain Mapp 37 (9), 3297–3309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Almairac F, Herbet G, Moritz-Gasser S, de Champfleur NM, Duffau H, 2015. The left inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus subserves language semantics: a multilevel lesion study. Brain Struct. Funct 220 (4), 1983–1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Antonsson M, Jakola A, Longoni F, Carstam L, Hartelius L, Thordstein M, Tisell M, 2018. Post-surgical effects on language in patients with presumed low-grade glioma. Acta Neurol. Scand 137 (5), 469–480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bain JS, Yeatman JD, Schurr R, Rokem A, Mezer AA, 2019. Evaluating arcuate fasciculus laterality measurements across dataset and tractography pipelines. Hum. Brain Mapp 40 (13), 3695–3711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barbeau EB, Descoteaux M, Petrides M, 2020. Dissociating the white matter tracts connecting the temporo-parietal cortical region with frontal cortex using diffusion tractography. Sci. Rep 10 (1), 8186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barrick TR, Lawes IN, Mackay CE, Clark CA, 2007. White matter pathway asymmetry underlies functional lateralization. Cereb. Cortex 17 (3), 591–598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Basser PJ, Pajevic S, Pierpaoli C, Duda J, Aldroubi A, 2000. In vivo fiber tractography using DT-MRI data. Magn. Reson. Med. Off. J. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med 44 (4), 625–632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Basser PJ, Pierpaoli C, 1996. Microstructural and physiological features of tissues elucidated by quantitative-diffusion-tensor MRI. J. Magn. Reson. B 111 (3), 209–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Beaulieu C, 2009. The biological basis of diffusion anisotropy. In: Diffusion MRI, pp. 105–126 unknown. [Google Scholar]
  10. Behrendt S (2014). Add standardized regression coefficients to lm-objects [R package lm.beta version 1.5–1] https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lm.beta
  11. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y, 1995. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Stat. Methodol 57 (1), 289–300. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bennett TA, Szatmari P, Bryson S, Duku E, Vaccarella L, Tuff L, 2013. Theory of mind, language and adaptive functioning in ASD: a neuroconstructivist perspective. J. Can. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry J. Acad. Can. Psychiatrie de L’enfant et de L’adolescent 22 (1), 13. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bergelson E, Swingley D, 2012. At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 109 (9), 3253–3258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Binder JR, 2017. Current controversies on Wernicke’s area and its role in language. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports (Vol. 17, Issue 8) doi: 10.1007/s11910-017-0764-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Blom-Smink M, Verly M, Spielmann K, Smits M, Ribbers GM, van de Sandt-Koenderman MWME, 2020. Change in right inferior longitudinal fasciculus integrity is associated with naming recovery in subacute poststroke aphasia. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 34 (9), 784–794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bradshaw AR, Thompson PA, Wilson AC, Bishop DVM, Woodhead ZVJ, 2017. Measuring language lateralisation with different language tasks: a systematic review. PeerJ 5, e3929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Braga RM, DiNicola LM, Becker HC, Buckner RL, 2020. Situating the left-lateralized language network in the broader organization of multiple specialized large-scale distributed networks. J. Neurophysiol 124 (5), 1415–1448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Buckner RL, Krienen FM, 2013. The evolution of distributed association networks in the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 17 (12), 648–665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Button KS, Ioannidis JPA, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ESJ, Munafò MR, 2013. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 14 (5), 365–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cabinio M, Rossetto F, Blasi V, Savazzi F, Castelli I, Massaro D, Valle A, Nemni R, Clerici M, Marchetti A, Baglio F, 2015. Mind-reading ability and structural connectivity changes in aging. Front. Psychol 6, 1808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Carrington SJ, Bailey AJ, 2009. Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature. Hum. Brain Mapp 30 (8), 2313–2335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Carter CS, Barch DM, Gur R, Gur R, Pinkham A, Ochsner K, 2009. CNTRICS final task selection: social cognitive and affective neuroscience-based measures. Schizophr. Bull 35 (1), 153–162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Catani M, Allin MPG, Husain M, Pugliese L, Mesulam MM, Murray RM, Jones DK, 2007. Symmetries in human brain language pathways correlate with verbal recall. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 104 (43), 17163–17168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Catani M, Thiebaut de Schotten M, 2008. A diffusion tensor imaging tractography atlas for virtual in vivo dissections. Cortex 44 (8), 1105–1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ceschin R, Lee VK, Schmithorst V, Panigrahy A, 2015. Regional vulnerability of longitudinal cortical association connectivity: associated with structural network topology alterations in preterm children with cerebral palsy. Neuroimage Clin 9, 322–337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chamberland M, Raven EP, Genc S, Duffy K, Descoteaux M, Parker GD, Tax CMW, Jones DK, 2019. Dimensionality reduction of diffusion MRI measures for improved tractometry of the human brain. Neuroimage 200, 89–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Chang EF, Raygor KP, Berger MS, 2015. Contemporary model of language organization: an overview for neurosurgeons. J. Neurosurg 122 (2), 250–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Charlton RA, Barrick TR, Markus HS, Morris RG, 2009. Theory of mind associations with other cognitive functions and brain imaging in normal aging. Psychol. Aging 24 (2), 338–348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Chen L, Wassermann D, Abrams DA, Kochalka J, Gallardo-Diez G, Menon V, 2019. The visual word form area (VWFA) is part of both language and attention circuitry. Nat. Commun 10 (1), 5601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Coad BM, Postans M, Hodgetts CJ, Muhlert N, Graham KS, Lawrence AD, 2020. Structural connections support emotional connections: uncinate fasciculus microstructure is related to the ability to decode facial emotion expressions. Neuropsychologia 145, 106562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Corina DP, Loudermilk BC, Detwiler L, Martin RF, Brinkley JF, Ojemann G, 2010. Analysis of naming errors during cortical stimulation mapping: implications for models of language representation. Brain Lang 115 (2), 101–112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Del Tufo SN, Earle FS, Cutting LE, 2019. The impact of expressive language development and the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus on listening and reading comprehension. J. Neurodev. Disord 11 (1), 37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Descoteaux M, Angelino E, Fitzgibbons S, Deriche R, 2007. Regularized, fast, and robust analytical Q-ball imaging. Magn. Reson. Med. Off. J. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med 58 (3), 497–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, Albert MS, Killiany RJ, 2006. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31 (3), 968–980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. de Villiers JG, de Villiers PA, 2014. The role of language in theory of mind development. Top Lang. Disord 34 (4), 313. [Google Scholar]
  36. Dick AS, Tremblay P, 2012. Beyond the arcuate fasciculus: consensus and controversy in the connectional anatomy of language. Brain J. Neurol 135 (Pt 12), 3529–3550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Díez-Cirarda M, Ojeda N, Peña J, Cabrera-Zubizarreta A, Gómez-Beldarrain MÁ, Gómez-Esteban JC, Ibarretxe-Bilbao N, 2015. Neuroanatomical correlates of theory of mind deficit in Parkinson’s disease: a multimodal imaging study. PLoS One 10 (11), e0142234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. DiNicola LM, Braga RM, Buckner RL, 2020. Parallel distributed networks dissociate episodic and social functions within the individual. J. Neurophysiol 123 (3), 1144–1179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dodell-Feder D, Koster-Hale J, Bedny M, Saxe R, 2011. fMRI item analysis in a theory of mind task. Neuroimage 55 (2), 705–712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Drane DL, Pedersen NP, 2019. Knowledge of language function and underlying neural networks gained from focal seizures and epilepsy surgery. Brain Lang 189, 20–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Drane DL, Pedersen NP, Sabsevitz DS, Block C, Dickey AS, Alwaki A, Kheder A, 2021. Cognitive and emotional mapping with SEEG. Front. Neurol 12, 407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dronkers NF, Wilkins DP, Van Valin RD Jr, Redfern BB, Jaeger JJ, 2004. Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition 92 (1–2), 145–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Dufour N, Redcay E, Young L, Mavros PL, Moran JM, Triantafyllou C, Gabrieli JDE, Saxe R, 2013. Similar brain activation during false belief tasks in a large sample of adults with and without autism. PLoS One 8 (9), e75468. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ennis DB, Kindlmann G, 2006. Orthogonal tensor invariants and the analysis of diffusion tensor magnetic resonance images. Magn. Reson. Med. Off. J. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med 55 (1), 136–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Farquharson S, Tournier J−D, Calamante F, Fabinyi G, Schneider-Kolsky M, Jackson GD, Connelly A, 2013. White matter fiber tractography: why we need to move beyond DTI. J. Neurosurg 118 (6), 1367–1377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Fedorenko E, Hsieh P−J, Nieto-Castañón A, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Kanwisher N, 2010. New method for fMRI investigations of language: defining ROIs functionally in individual subjects. J. Neurophysiol 104 (2), 1177–1194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Finn ES, Rosenberg MD, 2021. Beyond fingerprinting: choosing predictive connectomes over reliable connectomes. Neuroimage 239, 118254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Fischl B, van der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, Halgren E, Ségonne F, Salat DH, Busa E, Seidman LJ, Goldstein J, Kennedy D, Caviness V, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM, 2004. Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 14 (1), 11–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Forkel SJ, Catani M, 2019. Diffusion imaging methods in language sciences. In: de Zubicaray GI, Schiller Niels O. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Neurolinguistics Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  50. Forkel SJ, Friedrich P, de Schotten MT, & Howells H (2020). White matter variability, cognition, and disorders: a systematic review. In bioRxiv. medRxiv 10.1101/2020.04.22.20075127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Fornito A, Zalesky A, Breakspear M, 2015. The connectomics of brain disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 16 (3), 159–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Fox J, Weisberg S, 2018a. Visualizing fit and lack of fit in complex regression models with predictor effect plots and partial residuals. J. Stat. Softw 87 (9), 1–27 Articles. [Google Scholar]
  53. Fox J, Weisberg S, 2018b. An R Companion to Applied Regression SAGE Publications. [Google Scholar]
  54. Fujie S, Namiki C, Nishi H, Yamada M, Miyata J, Sakata D, Sawamoto N, Fukuyama H, Hayashi T, Murai T, 2008. The role of the uncinate fasciculus in memory and emotional recognition in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord 26 (5), 432–439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gavilán Ibáñez JM, García-Albea Ristol JE, 2013. Theory of mind and language comprehension in schizophrenia. Psicothema 25 (4), 440–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Geeraert BL, Chamberland M, Lebel RM, Lebel C, 2020. Multimodal principal component analysis to identify major features of white matter structure and links to reading. PLoS One 15 (8), e0233244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gershon RC, Cook KF, Mungas D, Manly JJ, Slotkin J, Beaumont JL, Weintraub S, 2014. Language measures of the NIH toolbox cognition battery. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS 20 (6), 642–651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gershon RC, Slotkin J, Manly JJ, Blitz DL, Beaumont JL, Schnipke D, Wallner-Allen K, Golinkoff RM, Gleason JB, Hirsh-Pasek K, Adams MJ, Weintraub S, 2013. IV. NIH toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring language (vocabulary comprehension and reading decoding). Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev 78 (4), 49–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Geschwind N, 1970. The organization of language and the brain. Science https://www.jstor.org/stable/1731540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Gierhan SME, 2013. Connections for auditory language in the human brain. Brain Lang 127 (2), 205–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Gilmore N, Yücel MA, Li X, Boas DA, Kiran S, 2021. Investigating language and domain-general processing in neurotypicals and individuals with aphasia - a functional near-infrared spectroscopy pilot study. Front. Hum. Neurosci 15, 728151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Giovagnoli AR, Franceschetti S, Reati F, Parente A, Maccagnano C, Villani F, Spreafico R, 2011. Theory of mind in frontal and temporal lobe epilepsy: cognitive and neural aspects: theory of mind and epilepsy. Epilepsia 52 (11), 1995–2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Givón T, Malle BF, 2002. The relation between language and theory of mind in development and evolution. Evol. Lang. Pre-Lang 265–284. [Google Scholar]
  64. Glasser MF, Sotiropoulos SN, Wilson JA, Coalson TS, Fischl B, Andersson JL, Xu J, Jbabdi S, Webster M, Polimeni JR, Van Essen DC, Jenkinson M WU-Minn HCP Consortium, 2013. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage 80, 105–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Gong S, Zhang F, Norton I, Essayed WI, Unadkat P, Rigolo L, Pasternak O, Rathi Y, Hou L, Golby AJ, O’Donnell LJ, 2018. Free water modeling of peritumoral edema using multi-fiber tractography: application to tracking the arcuate fasciculus for neurosurgical planning. PLoS One 13 (5), e0197056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Gong W, Bai S, Zheng YQ, Smith SM, & Beckmann CF (2021). Supervised phenotype discovery frommultimodal brain imaging. In bioRxiv (p. 2021.09.03.458926). 10.1101/2021.09.03.458926 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Griffa A, Baumann PS, Thiran J−P, Hagmann P, 2013. Structural connectomics in brain diseases. Neuroimage 80, 515–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Griffis JC, Nenert R, Allendorfer JB, Szaflarski JP, 2017. Damage to white matter bottlenecks contributes to language impairments after left hemispheric stroke. Neuroimage Clin 14, 552–565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Grosse Wiesmann C, Schreiber J, Singer T, Steinbeis N, Friederici AD, 2017. White matter maturation is associated with the emergence of theory of mind in early childhood. Nat. Commun 8, 14692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Hamoda HM, Makhlouf AT, Fitzsimmons J, Rathi Y, Makris N, Mesholam–Gately RI, Wojcik JD, Goldstein J, McCarley RW, Seidman LJ, Kubicki M, Shenton ME, 2019. Abnormalities in thalamo-cortical connections in patients with first-episode schizophrenia: a two-tensor tractography study. Brain Imaging Behav 13 (2), 472–481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Hartwright CE, Hansen PC, Apperly IA, 2016. Current knowledge on the role of the inferior frontal gyrus in theory of mind - a commentary on Schurz and Tholen (2016) [Review of current knowledge on the role of the inferior frontal gyrus in theory of mind - a commentary on Schurz and Tholen (2016)]. Cortex 85, 133–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Hawco C, Kovacevic N, Malhotra AK, Buchanan RW, Viviano JD, Iacoboni M, McIntosh AR, Voineskos AN, 2017. Neural activity while imitating emotional faces is related to both lower and higher-level social cognitive performance. Sci. Rep 7 (1), 1244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Herbet G, Lafargue G, Bonnetblanc F, Moritz-Gasser S, Menjot de Champfleur N, Duffau H, 2014. Inferring a dual-stream model of mentalizing from associative white matter fibres disconnection. Brain J. Neurol 137 (Pt 3), 944–959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Herbet G, Lafargue G, Moritz-Gasser S, Bonnetblanc F, Duffau H, 2015a. Interfering with the neural activity of mirror-related frontal areas impairs mentalistic inferences. Brain Struct. Funct 220 (4), 2159–2169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Herbet G, Lafargue G, Moritz-Gasser S, Menjot de Champfleur N, Costi E, Bonnetblanc F, Duffau H, 2015b. A disconnection account of subjective empathy impairments in diffuse low-grade glioma patients. Neuropsychologia 70, 165–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Hertrich I, Dietrich S, Ackermann H, 2020. The margins of the language network in the brain. Front. Commun 5, 93. [Google Scholar]
  77. Houston J, Allendorfer J, Nenert R, Goodman AM, Szaflarski JP, 2019. White matter language pathways and language performance in healthy adults across ages. Front. Neurosci 13, 1185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Ingalhalikar M, Smith A, Parker D, Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Hakonarson H, Gur RE, Gur RC, Verma R, 2014. Sex differences in the structural connectome of the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 111 (2), 823–828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Irimia A, Fan D, Chaudhari NN, Ngo V, Zhang F, Joshi SH, O’Donnell LJ, 2020. Mapping cerebral connectivity changes after mild traumatic brain injury in older adults using diffusion tensor imaging and Riemannian matching of elastic curves. In: Proceedings of the ISBI [Google Scholar]
  80. Jáni M, Kašpárek T, 2018. Emotion recognition and theory of mind in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 19 (sup3), S86–S96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Jensen JH, Helpern JA, Ramani A, Lu H, Kaczynski K, 2005. Diffusional kurtosis imaging: the quantification of non-Gaussian water diffusion by means of magnetic resonance imaging. Magn. Reson. Med. Off. J. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med 53 (6), 1432–1440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Johnstone LT, Karlsson EM, Carey DP, 2021. Left-handers are less lateralized than right-handers for both left and right hemispheric functions. Cereb. Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Jones DK, Knösche TR, Turner R, 2013. White matter integrity, fiber count, and other fallacies: the do’s and don’ts of diffusion MRI. Neuroimage 73, 239–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Jouravlev O, Kell AJE, Mineroff Z, Haskins AJ, Ayyash D, Kanwisher N, Fedorenko E, 2020. Reduced language lateralization in autism and the broader autism phenotype as assessed with robust individual-subjects analyses. Autism Res 13 (10), 1746–1761. doi: 10.1002/aur.2393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kaestner E, Balachandra AR, Bahrami N, Reyes A, Lalani SJ, Macari AC, Voets NL, Drane DL, Paul BM, Bonilha L, McDonald CR, 2020. The white matter connectome as an individualized biomarker of language impairment in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroimage Clin. 25, 102125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Kochsiek J, O’Donnell LJ, Zhang F, Bonke EM, Sollmann N, Tripodis Y, Wiegand TLT, Kaufmann D, Umminger L, Di Biase MA, Kaufmann E, Schultz V, Alosco ML, Martin BM, Lin AP, Coleman MJ, Rathi Y, Pasternak O, Bouix S, … Koerte IK, 2021. Exposure to repetitive head impacts is associated with corpus callosum microstructure and plasma total Tau in former professional American football players. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging JMRI doi: 10.1002/jmri.27774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Lebel C, Beaulieu C, 2009. Lateralization of the arcuate fasciculus from childhood to adulthood and its relation to cognitive abilities in children. Hum. Brain Mapp 30 (11), 3563–3573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Lebel C, Gee M, Camicioli R, Wieler M, Martin W, Beaulieu C, 2012. Diffusion tensor imaging of white matter tract evolution over the lifespan. Neuroimage 60 (1), 340–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lettieri G, Handjaras G, Ricciardi E, Leo A, Papale P, Betta M, Pietrini P, Cecchetti L, 2019. Emotionotopy in the human right temporo-parietal cortex. Nat. Commun 10 (1), 5568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Levitt JJ, Zhang F, Vangel M, Nestor PG, Rathi Y, Kubicki M, Shenton ME, O’Donnell LJ, 2021. The organization of frontostriatal brain wiring in healthy subjects using a novel diffusion imaging fiber cluster analysis. Cereb. Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Liégeois FJ, Mahony K, Connelly A, Pigdon L, Tournier J−D, Morgan AT, 2013. Pediatric traumatic brain injury: language outcomes and their relationship to the arcuate fasciculus. Brain Lang 127 (3), 388–398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Long MA, Katlowitz KA, Svirsky MA, Clary RC, Byun TM, Majaj N, Oya H, Howard MA 3rd, Greenlee JDW, 2016. Functional segregation of cortical regions underlying speech timing and articulation. Neuron 89 (6), 1187–1193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Lopez-Barroso D, de Diego-Balaguer R, Cunillera T, Camara E, Münte TF, Rodriguez-Fornells A, 2011. Language learning under working memory constraints correlates with microstructural differences in the ventral language pathway. Cereb. Cortex 21 (12), 2742–2750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. López-Vicente M, Lamballais S, Louwen S, Hillegers M, Tiemeier H, Muetzel RL, White T, 2021. White matter microstructure correlates of age, sex, handedness and motor ability in a population-based sample of 3031 school-age children. Neuroimage 227, 117643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Luo C, Makaretz S, Stepanovic M, Papadimitriou G, Quimby M, Palanivelu S, Dickerson BC, Makris N, 2020. Middle longitudinal fascicle is associated with semantic processing deficits in primary progressive aphasia. Neuroimage Clin 25, 102115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Maffei C, Sarubbo S, Jovicich J, 2019. Diffusion-based tractography atlas of the human acoustic radiation. Sci. Rep 9 (1), 4046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Makris N, 1999. Behavioral Neuroscience Boston University, p. 176. [Google Scholar]
  98. Makris N, Kennedy DN, McInerney S, Sorensen AG, Wang R, Caviness VS Jr, Pandya DN, 2005. Segmentation of subcomponents within the superior longitudinal fascicle in humans: a quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study. Cereb. Cortex 15 (6), 854–869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Makris N, Pandya DN, 2009. The extreme capsule in humans and rethinking of the language circuitry. Brain Struct. Funct 213 (3), 343–358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Makris N, Papadimitriou GM, Kaiser JR, Sorg S, Kennedy DN, Pandya DN, 2009. Delineation of the middle longitudinal fascicle in humans: a quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study. Cereb. Cortex 19 (4), 777–785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Makris N, Preti MG, Wassermann D, Rathi Y, Papadimitriou GM, Yergatian C, Dickerson BC, Shenton ME, Kubicki M, 2013. Human middle longitudinal fascicle: segregation and behavioral-clinical implications of two distinct fiber connections linking temporal pole and superior temporal gyrus with the angular gyrus or superior parietal lobule using multi-tensor tractography. Brain Imaging Behav 7 (3), 335–352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Makris N, Zhu A, Papadimitriou GM, Mouradian P, Ng I, Scaccianoce E, Baselli G, Baglio F, Shenton ME, Rathi Y, Dickerson B, Yeterian E, Kubicki M, 2017. Mapping temporo-parietal and temporo-occipital cortico-cortical connections of the human middle longitudinal fascicle in subject-specific, probabilistic, and stereotaxic Talairach spaces. Brain Imaging Behav 11 (5), 1258–1277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Malcolm JG, Shenton ME, Rathi Y, 2010. Filtered multitensor tractography. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 29 (9), 1664–1675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Mandonnet E, Nouet A, Gatignol P, Capelle L, Duffau H, 2007. Does the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus play a role in language? A brain stimulation study. Brain 130 (3), 623–629. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Marchetti A, Baglio F, Costantini I, Dipasquale O, Savazzi F, Nemni R, Sangiuliano Intra F, Tagliabue S, Valle A, Massaro D, Castelli I, 2015. Theory of mind and the whole brain functional connectivity: behavioral and neural evidences with the Amsterdam resting state questionnaire. Front. Psychol 6, 1855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Marchman VA, Fernald A, 2008. Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Dev. Sci 11 (3), F9–F16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Martin R, Slevc LR, 2012. CHAPTER 9 - Memory disorders and impaired language and communication. In: Peach RK, Shapiro LP (Eds.), Cognition and Acquired Language Disorders Mosby, pp. 183–201. [Google Scholar]
  108. Maxwell SE, Lau MY, Howard GS, 2015. Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? Am. Psychol 70 (6), 487–498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. McKay NS, Iwabuchi SJ, Häberling IS, Corballis MC, Kirk IJ, 2017. Atypical white matter microstructure in left-handed individuals. Laterality Asymmetries Body, Brain Cognit 22 (3), 257–267. doi: 10.1080/1357650x.2016.1175469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Meletti S, Benuzzi F, Cantalupo G, Rubboli G, Tassinari CA, Nichelli P, 2009. Facial emotion recognition impairment in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 50 (6), 1547–1559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Mier D, Lis S, Neuthe K, Sauer C, Esslinger C, Gallhofer B, Kirsch P, 2010. The involvement of emotion recognition in affective theory of mind. Psychophysiology 47 (6), 1028–1039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Miller CA, 2006. Developmental relationships between language and theory of mind. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. Am. Speech Lang. Hear. Assoc 15 (2), 142–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Mitchell RLC, Phillips LH, 2015. The overlapping relationship between emotion perception and theory of mind. Neuropsychologia 70, 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Nakajima R, Kinoshita M, Okita H, Yahata T, Matsui M, Nakada M, 2018a. Neural networks mediating high-level mentalizing in patients with right cerebral hemispheric gliomas. Front. Behav. Neurosci 12, 33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Nakajima R, Yordanova YN, Duffau H, Herbet G, 2018b. Neuropsychological evidence for the crucial role of the right arcuate fasciculus in the face-based mentalizing network: a disconnection analysis. Neuropsychologia 115, 179–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Nilsson KK, de Lopez KJ, 2016. Theory of mind in children with specific language impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Dev. 10.1111/cdev.12462?casa_token=bitBCpWG_lsAAAAA:4WNa_fTV1tj_EKGBYSnALh01YsfjT24EJxWTyCQ6JX4aT3-B4JXKbKBRfOU2MHsBmRPnI5p_POvd4vnST. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Ning L, Laun F, Gur Y, DiBella EVR, Deslauriers-Gauthier S, Megherbi T, Ghosh A, Zucchelli M, Menegaz G, Fick R, St-Jean S, Paquette M, Aranda R, Descoteaux M, Deriche R, O’Donnell L, Rathi Y, 2015. Sparse reconstruction challenge for diffusion MRI: validation on a physical phantom to determine which acquisition scheme and analysis method to use? Med. Image Anal 26 (1), 316–331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Norton I, Essayed WI, Zhang F, Pujol S, Yarmarkovich A, Golby AJ, Kindlmann G, Wassermann D, Estepar RSJ, Rathi Y, Pieper S, Kikinis R, Johnson HJ, Westin CF, O’Donnell LJ, 2017. SlicerDMRI: open source diffusion MRI software for brain cancer research. Cancer Res 77 (21), e101–e103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Numssen O, Bzdok D, Hartwigsen G, 2021. Functional specialization within the inferior parietal lobes across cognitive domains. Elife 10. doi: 10.7554/eLife.63591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Oakley BFM, Brewer R, Bird G, Catmur C, 2016. Theory of mind is not theory of emotion: a cautionary note on the reading the mind in the eyes test. J. Abnorm. Psychol 125 (6), 818–823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. O’Donnell LJ, Wells III WM, Golby AJ, Westin C−F, 2012. Unbiased groupwise registration of white matter tractography. In: Ayache N, Delingette H, Golland P, Mori K (Eds.), Proceedings of the Medical Image Computing and Computer-Assisted Intervention – MICCAI 2012 Springer, Berlin Heidelberg, pp. 123–130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. O’Donnell LJ, Westin C−.F., 2007. Automatic tractography segmentation using a high-dimensional white matter atlas. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 26 (11), 1562–1575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. O’Donnell LJ, Westin CF, 2011. An introduction to diffusion tensor image analysis. Neurosurg. Clin. N. Am 22 (2), 185–196 viii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Ojemann GA, 1979. Individual variability in cortical localization of language. J. Neurosurg 50 (2), 164–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Oldfield RC, 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9 (1), 97–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Olszewski AK, Kikinis Z, Gonzalez CS, Coman IL, Makris N, Gong X, Rathi Y, Zhu A, Antshel KM, Fremont W, Kubicki MR, Bouix S, Shenton ME, Kates WR, 2017. The social brain network in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Behav. Brain Funct. BBF 13 (1), 4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Paldino MJ, Hedges K, Golriz F, 2016. The arcuate fasciculus and language development in a cohort of pediatric patients with malformations of cortical development. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol 37 (1), 169–175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Palmer CE, Zhao W, Loughnan R, Fan CC, Thompson WK, Dale AM, Jernigan TL, 2020. Fluid and Crystallised Intelligence are Associated with Distinct Regionalisation Patterns of Cortical Morphology Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory; (p. 2020.02.13.948596) doi: 10.1101/2020.02.13.948596. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  129. Parlatini V, Radua J, Dell’Acqua F, Leslie A, Simmons A, Murphy DG, Catani M, Thiebaut de Schotten M, 2017. Functional segregation and integration within fronto-parietal networks. Neuroimage 146, 367–375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Pasternak O, Sochen N, Gur Y, Intrator N, Assaf Y, 2009. Free water elimination and mapping from diffusion MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. Off. J. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med 62 (3), 717–730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Pawełczyk A, Łojek E, Żurner N, Gawłowska-Sawosz M, Gębski P, Pawełczyk T, 2020. The correlation between white matter integrity and pragmatic language processing in first episode schizophrenia. Brain Imaging Behav doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00314-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Penfield W, & Roberts L (1959). Speech and brain mechanisms https://psycnet.apa.org/fulltext/1959-15026-000.pdf
  133. Petrides M, Pandya DN, 2002. Association pathways of the prefrontal cortex and functional observations. In: Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, 1, pp. 31–50. [Google Scholar]
  134. Philippi CL, Mehta S, Grabowski T, Adolphs R, Rudrauf D, 2009. Damage to association fiber tracts impairs recognition of the facial expression of emotion. J. Neurosci 29 (48), 15089–15099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Phillips N (2017). A companion to the e-book “YaRrr!: the pirate’s guide to R” [R package yarrr version 0.1.5] https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=yarrr
  136. Pierpaoli C, Jezzard P, Basser PJ, Barnett A, Di Chiro G, 1996. Diffusion tensor MR imaging of the human brain. Radiology 201 (3), 637–648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. R Core Team, 2020. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing R Foundation for Statistical Computing; https://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
  138. Reddy CP, Rathi Y, 2016. Joint multi-fiber NODDI parameter estimation and tractography using the unscented information filter. Front. Neurosci 10, 166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Rilling JK, Glasser MF, Preuss TM, Ma X, Zhao T, Hu X, Behrens TEJ, 2008. The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nat. Neurosci 11 (4), 426–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, 1999. Resonance behaviors and mirror neurons. Arch. Ital. Biol 137 (2–3), 85–100. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L, 1996. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res 3 (2), 131–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Rogalsky C, Matchin W, Hickok G, 2008. Broca’s area, sentence comprehension, and working memory: an fMRI Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci 2, 14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Rowe AD, Bullock PR, Polkey CE, Morris RG, 2001. Theory of mind” impairments and their relationship to executive functioning following frontal lobe excisions. Brain J. Neurol 124, 600–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Ruba AL, Repacholi BM, 2019. Do preverbal infants understand discrete facial expressions of emotion? Emot. Rev. J. Int. Soc. Res. Emot, 1754073919871098. [Google Scholar]
  145. Salvan P, Tournier JD, Batalle D, Falconer S, Chew A, Kennea N, Aljabar P, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Arichi T, Edwards AD, Counsell SJ, 2017. Language ability in preterm children is associated with arcuate fasciculi microstructure at term. Hum. Brain Mapp 38 (8), 3836–3847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Samson D, Houthuys S, Humphreys GW, 2015. Self-perspective inhibition deficits cannot be explained by general executive control difficulties. Cortex 70, 189–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Saur D, Kreher BW, Schnell S, Kümmerer D, Kellmeyer P, Vry M−S, Umarova R, Musso M, Glauche V, Abel S, Huber W, Rijntjes M, Hennig J, Weiller C, 2008. Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 105 (46), 18035–18040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Saxe R, 2006. Why and how to study theory of mind with fMRI. Brain Res 1079 (1), 57–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Saxe R, Wexler A, 2005. Making sense of another mind: the role of the right temporo–parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 43 (10), 1391–1399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Schachter SC, Ransil BJ, Geschwind N, 1987. Associations of handedness with hair color and learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia 25 (1B), 269–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN, 2006. Fiber Pathways of the Brain Oxford University Press, USA. [Google Scholar]
  152. Schmahmann JD, Smith EE, Eichler FS, Filley CM, 2008. Cerebral white matter: neuroanatomy, clinical neurology, and neurobehavioral correlates. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci 1142, 266–309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Schulte-Rüther M, Markowitsch HJ, Fink GR, Piefke M, 2007. Mirror neuron and theory of mind mechanisms involved in face-to-face interactions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging approach to empathy. J. Cogn. Neurosci 19 (8), 1354–1372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Schurz M, Radua J, Aichhorn M, Richlan F, Perner J, 2014. Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 42, 9–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Schurz M, Tholen MG, 2016. What brain imaging did (not) tell us about the inferior frontal gyrus in theory of mind - A commentary on Samson et al., (2015) [Review of what brain imaging did (not) tell us about the inferior frontal gyrus in theory of mind - a commentary on Samson et al., (2015)]. Cortex 74, 329–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Sierpowska J, Gabarrós A, Fernández-Coello A, Camins À, Castañer S, Juncadella M, François C, Rodríguez-Fornells A, 2019. White-matter pathways and semantic processing: intrasurgical and lesion-symptom mapping evidence. Neuroimage Clin 22, 101704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Sina Mansour L, Tian Y, Thomas Yeo BT, Cropley V, Zalesky A, 2021. High-resolution connectomic fingerprints: mapping neural identity and behavior. Neuroimage, 117695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Slater DA, Melie-Garcia L, Preisig M, Kherif F, Lutti A, Draganski B, 2019. Evolution of white matter tract microstructure across the life span. Hum. Brain Mapp 40 (7), 2252–2268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Slotkin J, Nowinski C, Hays R, Beaumont J, Griffith J, Magasi S, Gershon R, 2012. NIH Toolbox Scoring and Interpretation Guide Washington (DC): National Institutes of Health, pp. 6–7. [Google Scholar]
  160. Smith R, Raffelt D, Tournier J−D, & Connelly A (2020). Quantitative streamlines tractography: methods and inter-subject normalisation 10.31219/osf.io/c67kn [DOI]
  161. Sotiropoulos SN, Zalesky A, 2019. Building connectomes using diffusion MRI: why, how and but. NMR Biomed 32 (4), e3752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Sreedharan RM, Menon AC, James JS, Kesavadas C, Thomas SV, 2015. Arcuate fasciculus laterality by diffusion tensor imaging correlates with language laterality by functional MRI in preadolescent children. Neuroradiology 57 (3), 291–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Szaflarski JP, Binder JR, Possing ET, McKiernan KA, Ward BD, Hammeke TA, 2002. Language lateralization in left-handed and ambidextrous people: fMRI data. Neurology 59 (2), 238–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Takaya S, Kuperberg GR, Liu H, Greve DN, Makris N, Stufflebeam SM, 2015. Asymmetric projections of the arcuate fasciculus to the temporal cortex underlie lateralized language function in the human brain. Front. Neuroanat 9, 119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Ffytche DH, Bizzi A, Dell’Acqua F, Allin M, Walshe M, Murray R, Williams SC, Murphy DGM, Catani M, 2011. Atlasing location, asymmetry and inter-subject variability of white matter tracts in the human brain with MR diffusion tractography. Neuroimage 54 (1), 49–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Thomas C, Moya L, Avidan G, Humphreys K, Jung KJ, Peterson MA, Behrmann M, 2008. Reduction in white matter connectivity, revealed by diffusion tensor imaging, may account for age-related changes in face perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci 20 (2), 268–284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Tian Y, Margulies DS, Breakspear M, Zalesky A, 2020. Topographic organization of the human subcortex unveiled with functional connectivity gradients. Nat. Neurosci doi: 10.1101/2020.01.13.903542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Turken AU, Dronkers NF, 2011. The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses. Front. Syst. Neurosci 5, 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Unger A, Alm KH, Collins JA, O’Leary JM, Olson IR, 2016. Variation in white matter connectivity predicts the ability to remember faces and discriminate their emotions. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS 22 (2), 180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Van Dyke JA, 2012. CHAPTER 5 - the role of memory in language and communication. In: Peach RK, Shapiro LP (Eds.), Cognition and Acquired Language Disorders Mosby, pp. 94–120. [Google Scholar]
  171. Van Essen DC, Smith SM, Barch DM, Behrens TEJ, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K WU-Minn HCP Consortium, 2013. The WU-Minn human connectome project: an overview. Neuroimage 80, 62–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. van Hemmen J, Saris IMJ, Cohen-Kettenis PT, Veltman DJ, Pouwels PJW, Bakker J, 2017. Sex differences in white matter microstructure in the human brain predominantly reflect differences in sex hormone exposure. Cereb. Cortex 27 (5), 2994–3001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Vos SB, Jones DK, Jeurissen B, Viergever MA, Leemans A, 2012. The influence of complex white matter architecture on the mean diffusivity in diffusion tensor MRI of the human brain. Neuroimage 59 (3), 2208–2216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Vos SB, Viergever MA, Leemans A, 2013. Multi-fiber tractography visualizations for diffusion MRI data. PLoS One 8 (11), e81453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Wang Y, Fernández-Miranda JC, Verstynen T, Pathak S, Schneider W, Yeh FC, 2013. Rethinking the role of the middle longitudinal fascicle in language and auditory pathways. Cereb. Cortex 23 (10), 2347–2356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Wang Y, Metoki A, Alm KH, Olson IR, 2018. White matter pathways and social cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 90, 350–370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Wang Y, Olson IR, 2018. The original social network: white matter and social cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 22 (6), 504–516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Weed E, McGregor W, Feldbaek Nielsen J, Roepstorff A, Frith U, 2010. Theory of mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: what’s the story? Brain Lang 113 (2), 65–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Wensing T, Cieslik EC, Müller VI, Hoffstaedter F, Eickhoff SB, Nickl-Jockschat T, 2017. Neural correlates of formal thought disorder: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp 38 (10), 4946–4965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Wilson SM, Galantucci S, Tartaglia MC, Rising K, Patterson DK, Henry ML, Ogar JM, DeLeon J, Miller BL, Gorno-Tempini ML, 2011. Syntactic processing depends on dorsal language tracts. Neuron 72 (2), 397–403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Winkler AM, Webster MA, Vidaurre D, Nichols TE, Smith SM, 2015. Multi-level block permutation. Neuroimage 123, 253–268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Yamada Y, Inagawa T, Hirabayashi N, Sumiyoshi T, 2021. Emotion recognition deficits in psychiatric disorders as a target of non-invasive neuromodulation: a systematic review. Clin. EEG Neurosci doi: 10.1177/1550059421991688, 155005942199168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Yeh F−C, 2020. Shape analysis of the human association pathways. Neuroimage 223, 117329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Yi HG, Leonard MK, Chang EF, 2019. The encoding of speech sounds in the superior temporal gyrus. Neuron 102 (6), 1096–1110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Zhang F, Daducci A, He Y, Schiavi S, Seguin C, Smith R, Yeh C−H, Zhao T, & O’Donnell LJ (2021). Quantitative mapping of the brain’s structural connectivity using diffusion MRI tractography: a review. In arXiv [q-bio.QM]. arXiv http://arxiv.org/abs/2104.11644 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Zhang F, Noh T, Juvekar P, Frisken SF, Rigolo L, Norton I, Kapur T, Pujol S, Wells W 3rd, Yarmarkovich A, Kindlmann G, Wassermann D, San Jose Estepar R, Rathi Y, Kikinis R, Johnson HJ, Westin C−F, Pieper S, Golby AJ, & O’Donnell LJ, 2020. SlicerDMRI: diffusion MRI and tractography research software for brain cancer surgery planning and visualization. JCO Clin. Cancer Inform 4, 299–309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Zhang F, Savadjiev P, Cai W, Song Y, Rathi Y, Tunç B, Parker D, Kapur T, Schultz RT, Makris N, Verma R, O’Donnell LJ, 2018a. Whole brain white matter connectivity analysis using machine learning: an application to autism. Neuroimage 172, 826–837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Zhang F, Wu Y, Norton I, Rathi Y, Golby AJ, O’Donnell LJ, 2019. Test-retest reproducibility of white matter parcellation using diffusion MRI tractography fiber clustering. Hum. Brain Mapp 40 (10), 3041–3057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Zhang F, Wu Y, Norton I, Rigolo L, Rathi Y, Makris N, O’Donnell LJ, 2018b. An anatomically curated fiber clustering white matter atlas for consistent white matter tract parcellation across the lifespan. Neuroimage 179, 429–447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Zhang H, Schneider T, Wheeler-Kingshott CA, Alexander DC, 2012. NODDI: practical in vivo neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging of the human brain. Neuroimage 61 (4), 1000–1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES