Skip to main content
Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking logoLink to Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking
. 2022 Feb 10;25(2):87–93. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2021.0188

Social Media Use and Its Link to Physical Health Indicators

David S Lee 1,, Tao Jiang 2, Jennifer Crocker 2, Baldwin M Way 2
PMCID: PMC8864418  PMID: 35021894

Abstract

Social media use has become an integral part of many young adults' daily lives. Although much research has examined how social media use relates to psychological well-being, little is known about how it relates to physical health. To address this knowledge gap, the present research investigated how the amount of social media people use relates to various indices of physical health. Young adults provided a blood sample that was analyzed for C-reactive protein (CRP), a marker of chronic inflammation. They also completed self-report measures of social media use, somatic symptoms, illness-related physician or health center visits, and whether they sought medical care for infection-related illnesses in the last 3 months. Social media use was positively correlated with higher levels of CRP, more somatic symptoms, and more visits to the doctor or health centers for an illness. Although directionally consistent, the correlation with likelihood of seeking medical care for infection-related illnesses was nonsignificant (p = 0.061). All of these results held after controlling for factors such as sociodemographic information and depressive symptoms. Given the prevalence of social media use in daily life, these findings underscore the need for more research examining how social media use relates to physical health.

Keywords: social media use, physical health, somatic symptoms, C-reactive protein, inflammation, social integration

Introduction

Social media use has become an integral part of many people's daily lives. A recent survey indicates that Americans average about 144 minutes per day on social media1; more time than they spend exercising, directly socializing with others, or eating.2 Social media usage is particularly high among Generation Z (i.e., people born in the late 1990s and early 2000s) who spend about 6 hours a day texting, online, and on social media3 and report being online on a “near-constant” basis.4

The past decade has seen an explosion of studies examining the impact of social media use on psychological well-being.5–9 However, little research has examined how social media use is related to physical health. This is surprising given the prevalence of social media in daily lives, and the close link between psychological well-being and physical health.10

Nevertheless, a small number of recent studies suggest a link between social media use and physical health.11–14 For example, Xue and colleagues (2018) found that excessive use of WeChat (the most popular social media platform in China) was associated with lower self-reported health. More recently, Lee and Way (2021) discovered that among older adults with low self-esteem, social media use predicted higher levels of C-reactive protein (CRP) and interleukin (IL)-6—biomarkers of chronic inflammation.

While the above studies provide initial evidence, they have some limitations. First, some studies measured social media use in a single platform (e.g., WeChat) despite evidence that most people use multiple social media platforms.15 Thus, it is unlikely that these studies fully captured the total amount of people's social media use.

Second, prior work has mostly used self-report measures of physical health, which can be vulnerable to demand characteristics and biases. Although a few studies have used biological measures, it is unclear to what extent the elevated levels of biological markers (e.g., cortisol, IL-6) reflect current health status. Thus, our goal was to extend prior work by examining how social media use across several platforms is associated with physical health outcomes measured at multiple levels (i.e., biological, behavioral, self-report).

How might social media use relate to physical health? One potential pathway might be through altering health behaviors. Several studies indicate that high levels of social media use or screen time may reduce users' amount and quality of sleep.16,17 This may be particularly the case for those who use social media excessively—addictive social media or mobile phone use can lead to reduced sleep quality and insomnia.18,19 A substantial body of evidence indicates that lower quality or quantity of sleep is linked to poorer physical health,20–22 including elevated risks for cardiovascular disease,23 hypertension,24 and early mortality.25 This perspective aligns with the displacement hypothesis, which posits that time spent on social media may have detrimental effects by displacing activities that are beneficial to well-being and health—for example, sleep, exercise, or face-to-face interactions.7,26

Second, several scholars contend that hyperconnectivity—the permanent availability of and connectivity to peers, media content, and online services through social media—can increase stress.17,27,28 For example, new communication norms and demands arising from hyperconnectivity (e.g., the need to check or respond to social media posts constantly) can lead to communication overload and higher levels of stress.28–30 Stress can undermine health in many ways, for example, by increasing the number and severity of somatic symptoms,31 the probability of infection and the severity of symptoms following exposure to a cold virus,32 and systemic inflammation.33 Collectively, these perspectives suggest that high levels of social media use may relate to worse physical health.

In this study, we examined whether social media use would be correlated with worse physical health indicators (i.e., higher levels of chronic inflammation, more frequent somatic symptoms, and more visits to health clinics). We tested this hypothesis in a college student sample because it is the age group most actively engaged with social media.34

Our physical health indicators consisted of three measures: (a) CRP, a biological marker of chronic inflammation in the blood; (b) self-report of somatic symptoms; and (c) illness-related physician or health center visits. Chronic inflammation is a potent driver of disease.35 In particular, elevated levels of CRP are associated with increased risk for chronic diseases, including cardiovascular disease, cancers, and depression.36,37 We also assessed somatic symptoms (e.g., headaches, chest or back pains), which are the leading cause of outpatient medical visits and associated with substantial functional impairment, disability, and health care usage. Such reports also predict future chronic illnesses38 and thus serve as a good indicator of physical health.39,40 Finally, as a broader behavioral marker of physical health, we assessed illness-related physician and health center visits.41

Methods

Participants and procedure

Data collection for this study occurred between October, 2018 and February, 2019. Two hundred and fifty-one undergraduate students (123 females; Mage = 19.40, SDage = 2.23; 60.8 percent White, 27.7 percent Asian/Pacific Islander, 5.8 percent African American, 3.5 percent Hispanic/Latin American, 2.2 percent Other) at a large Midwestern university participated in a study of “how people use social media” for course credit.

In the absence of an established literature on social media use and physical health, we estimated effect size from the related literature on social media use and psychological well-being.7,42 A power analysis using G*Power based on a small-to-medium effect size (f2 = 0.09) between social media use and each of our dependent variables indicated that a sample size of 200 provides 90 percent power (α = 0.05) to detect a significant effect; we intentionally oversampled and aimed to recruit 250 participants to account for participants who opt out of providing blood samples. All analyses were conducted after data collection was completed.

Participants were escorted to a laboratory where a trained research assistant collected blood samples through finger sticks to be assayed for CRP. Participants could opt out of providing their blood samples without losing their compensation. Twenty-eight (11 percent) opted out; these participants were excluded whenever we conducted analyses with the CRP variable, but were included for other analyses.a Participants then completed a battery of questionnaires in a separate room. To ensure validity of questionnaire responses, at the end of the study, participants were instructed to report the extent to which they took the study seriously (1 = not at all seriously, 5 = very seriously). Before data collection, we decided to exclude participants who responded “not at all seriously.”

The Institutional Review Board at authors' university approved all research reported in this article. All participants provided informed consent.

Measures

Social media use

Participants indicated how much time they spend on each of four social media platforms (i.e., Snapchat, Instagram, Twitter, and Facebook) on average each day (1 = 10 minutes or less, 2 = 11–30 minutes, 3 = 31–60 minutes, 4 = 1–2 hours, 5 = >2 hours). We measured social media use across these four platforms for two reasons. First, according to the PEW Research Center, they are the most commonly used social media platforms among U.S. adults from ages 18–24.34 Second, emerging work recommends measuring social media use across multiple platforms because people use them for different purposes and in different amounts.43 Thus, we averaged the four items to create a composite social media use variable (α = 0.57, mean [M] = 2.37, standard deviation [SD] = 0.85).b

C-reactive protein

CRP, our biological indicator of physical health, was assayed from dried blood spots with a protocol slightly modified from prior research.44 In brief, the participant's finger was swabbed with alcohol and then pricked with an 18-gauge needle (Owen Mumford Unistick 3). Blood drops were collected on a Whatman 903 Protein Saver Card and left to dry for 24 hours at room temperature. Samples were then punched using a 3 mm Biopsy Punch (Henry Schein) and stored in microcentrifuge tubes at −80°C until assay.

For assay, a single 3 mm punch was thawed and 200 μL of buffer (phosphate-buffered saline with 0.1 percent Tween 20) was added followed by overnight (∼16 hours) incubation at 4°C while shaking at 60 rpm. The following morning, eluate was diluted 1:10 and CRP was assayed according to the manufacturer's instructions using the Meso Scale Delivery Vplex Plus Kits (K151STG). The assay coefficient of variation (CV) was 2.05 percent and interassay CV was 4.94 percent (M = 1.73, SD = 4.25).

Somatic symptoms

For our subjective health indicator, participants indicated how frequently they experienced somatic symptoms (e.g., chest pain, headaches) in the past month by completing the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-15; α = 0.90, M = 2.03, SD = 0.72).39 Because over half of our sample comprised male participants, we dropped one item that measured menstrual cramps from analyses. Including this item in the analyses did not alter the results. Because values for PHQ-15 were highly skewed, they were log-transformed to achieve a normal distribution.

Health care usage

Finally, for our behavioral health indicator, participants reported how many times they visited the health center or doctor's office for an illness in the last 3 months (Medical visits; M = 0.79, SD = 1.53, range = 0–15) and whether they sought medical care for any sort of cold, flu, or infection in the last 3 months (0 = no, 1 = yes [n = 80]). Because values for the health center visits were highly skewed, they were log-transformed.

Covariates

Based on prior work,12,45,46 we controlled for extraneous factors associated with inflammation. Sociodemographic covariates were age, gender, household income, and highest level of education completed by father and mother (1 = some high school, 5 = graduate school; M = 3.60, SD = 1.02). Health behavior covariates included body mass index (BMI; M = 23.46, SD = 4.80), cigarette smoking (i.e., number of cigarettes smoked per day on average; 1 = none, 2 = 1 to 10, 3 = 11–20, 4 = 21–30, 5 = 31 or more; M = 1.10, SD = 0.31), alcohol consumption frequency (1 = 4 or more times a week, 2 = 2–3 times a week, 3 = 2–4 times a month, 4 = monthly, 5 = never; M = 3.43, SD = 1.19), and frequency of aerobic exercise (1 = once a week, 7 = 7 times a week; M = 3.08, SD = 1.73).

We also controlled for depressive symptoms using the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (α = 0.93, M = 1.05, SD = 0.69)47 and birth control pill use (0 = no, 1 = yes [n = 58]), as they can influence inflammation levels.45,48

Results

First, individuals with CRP values over 10 μg/mL (n = 5; < 2 percent) were excluded because these values may indicate the presence of an acute infection.49 Then, CRP was log-transformed to achieve a normal distribution. Second, two participants who indicated at the end of the study that they “did not take the study seriously at all” were excluded. Including all excluded participants in the analyses did not substantively change any results. Table 1 presents zero-order correlations among all key variables.

Table 1.

Means, Standard Deviations, and Zero-Order Correlations for Main Variables

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
1. SNS use 2.37 0.85                      
2. CRP (log) −0.38 0.68 0.21**                    
3. PHQ (log) 0.28 0.15 0.24*** 0.09                  
4. Med visits (log) 0.18 0.24 0.21** 0.09 0.35***                
5. Med care 0.15* 0.05 0.18** 0.51***              
6. Depres 2.29 0.69 0.12 −0.08 0.50*** 0.19** −0.05            
7. Gender 0.20** 0.08 0.26*** 0.21** 0.20** 0.10          
8. Age 19.40 2.22 −0.02 0.08 0.01 −0.10 −0.11 0.03 −0.08        
9. Edu (M) 3.52 1.12 −0.11 0.09 −0.05 0.04 0.06 −0.19*** −0.05 −0.16*      
10. Edu (F) 3.67 1.20 −0.15* 0.06 −0.08 0.03 0.06 −0.18** −0.10 −0.19** 0.55***    
11. Income 5.80 2.72 −0.20** −0.04 −0.20** 0.05 −0.07 −0.21** −0.09 −0.16* 0.31*** 0.43***  
12. BMI 23.46 4.80 0.04 0.22** 0.01 −0.12 −0.06 0.10 −0.14* 0.16* −0.07 −0.10 −0.26***

Notes: n = 249 except when correlated with CRP (n = 219).

p ≤ 0.10. *p ≤ 0.05. **p ≤ 0.01. ***p ≤ 0.001 (two-tailed).

BMI, body mass index; CRP, C-reactive protein; depress, depressive symptoms; Edu (M), highest degree obtained by mother; Edu (F), highest degree obtained by father; Income, family annual income; M, mean; PHQ, Patient Health Questionnaire; SD, standard deviation; SNS use, social media use.

Is social media use associated with CRP?

Following prior work, we conducted a series of multiple regression analyses with social media use as a predictor of CRP.12,46 The models sequentially controlled for the following covariates: (a) sociodemographic factors, (b) health behaviors, (c) depressive symptoms, and (d) birth control pill use. Consistent with our hypothesis, social media use was associated with elevated levels of CRP in Model 1 (β = 0.20, p = 0.007), Model 2 (β = 0.17, p = 0.023), Model 3 (β = 0.19, p = 0.011), and Model 4 (β = 0.17, p = 0.019). The results of these analyses are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2.

Coefficients from Linear Regression Models Predicting C-Reactive Protein

Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
Predictor β p 95 percent CI Predictor β p 95 percent CI Predictor β p 95 percent CI Predictor β p 95 percent CI
Sex 0.07 0.32 −0.07–0.21 Sex 0.11 0.12 −0.03–0.25 Sex 0.12 0.10 −0.02–0.26 Sex −0.05 0.57 −0.21 to 0.11
Age 0.10 0.15 −0.04–0.24 Age 0.07 0.30 −0.07–0.21 Age 0.07 0.32 −0.07–0.20 Age 0.10 0.15 −0.03 to 0.23
Edu (M) 0.09 0.28 −0.07–0.25 Edu (M) 0.07 39 −0.11–0.23 Edu (M) 0.06 0.48 −0.11–0.22 Edu (M) 0.06 0.44 −0.09 to 0.21
Edu (F) 0.09 0.31 −0.08–0.26 Edu (F) 0.06 0.47 −0.09–0.23 Edu (F) 0.06 0.52 −0.10–0.21 Edu (F) 0.03 0.71 −0.13 to 0.19
Income −0.06 0.48 −0.21–0.10 Income 0.01 0.87 −0.15–0.18 Income −0.01 0.97 −0.17–0.16 Income 0.03 0.73 −0.13 to 0.19
SNS use 0.20 0.007 0.06–0.34 BMI 0.23 0.001 0.09–0.37 BMI 0.25 0.001 0.10–0.38 BMI 0.25 <0.001 0.11 to 0.38
        Smoking 0.06 0.44 −0.09–0.20 Smoking 0.06 0.38 −0.08–0.20 Smoking 0.09 0.20 −0.05 to 0.23
        Alcohol −0.06 0.43 −0.21–0.09 Alcohol −0.04 0.59 −0.19–0.11 Alcohol 0.01 0.98 −0.15 to 0.15
        Exercise −0.07 0.32 −0.20–0.07 Exercise −0.09 0.19 −0.23–0.05 Exercise −0.11 0.11 −0.24 to 0.03
        SNS use 0.17 0.023 0.02–0.32 Depres −0.14 0.06 −0.28–0.01 Depres −0.18 0.014 −0.31 to −0.04
                SNS use 0.19 0.011 0.04–0.35 BirthCon 0.31 <0.001 0.14 to 0.46
                        SNS use 0.17 0.019 0.03 to 0.32
R 2   0.07 (0.04)     0.13 (0.10)     0.14 (0.11)     0.20 (0.18)

Notes: Sex was coded with 1 (male) and 2 (female). BirthCon was coded with 0 (not currently taking birth control medication) and 1 (currently taking birth control medication). R2 values reflect those with SNS use in the models. R2 values in parentheses reflect those without SNS use in the models.

Alcohol, frequency of alcohol consumption; BirthCon, consumption of birth control medication; CI, confidence interval; Exercise, no. of times engaged in aerobic exercise per week; Smoking, no. of cigarettes smoked per day.

Is social media use associated with somatic symptoms?

Next, we conducted multiple regression analyses to examine the link between social media use and self-reports of somatic symptoms. Controlling for sociodemographic factors, as predicted, social media use was associated with more frequent somatic symptoms experienced in the past month (β = 0.18, p = 0.006, 95 percent confidence interval [CI] = 0.05–0.30). Adjusting for depressive symptoms did not substantively change the results (p = 0.01).

Is social media use associated with health care usage?

Consistent with our hypothesis, multiple regression analyses indicated that social media use was positively associated with more visits to the health center or doctor's office for an illness in the past 3 months (β = 0.19, p = 0.005, 95 percent CI = 0.06–0.32). Controlling for depressive symptoms did not alter our results (p = 0.007). In addition, a logistic regression analysis revealed a nonsignificant, but directionally consistent link between social media use and seeking medical care for any sort of cold, flu, or infection in the last 3 months (Wald coefficient = 3.50, odds ratio = 1.38, 95 percent CI = 0.99–1.77, p = 0.061). Controlling for depressive symptoms did not substantively change the results (p = 0.054).

Discussion

The current research examined whether social media use is associated with various physical health indicators among college students. Social media use was correlated with higher levels of CRP—a biomarker of chronic inflammation that is associated with chronic illnesses such as cardiovascular diseases and cancers. Social media use was also related to experiencing more frequent somatic symptoms, and to behavioral health indices such as more visits to the doctor or health centers for an illness. The pattern of results remained the same even after adjusting for various factors, such as gender and depressive symptoms.

Our findings make several novel contributions. To our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate the association of social media use across several platforms with CRP, a chronic inflammatory and health marker, in a college sample. Importantly, the use of a biological marker as a key health indicator is a strength of this study given that prior studies on social media use have primarily relied on self-report well-being measures, which can be vulnerable to demand characteristics. Furthermore, by measuring college students' social media use across several platforms (vs. one particular platform), our study captured social media usage in a more ecologically valid fashion43: By showing how this overall social media use variable was related to multiple health indicators, this study integrates and extends the nascent research on social media and physical health.

Broadly, our findings highlight the potential role of social media use in the context of social relationships and physical health research.50,51 Although people can engage in “nonsocial” activities on social media (e.g., reading the news), much of what they do on social media involves efforts to initiate, maintain, and develop relationships with others. For example, similar to the traditional conceptualization of social integration,52,53 people use social media platforms to have intimate conversations and exchange social support,54 to participate in groups and organizations (e.g., Facebook groups), and to cultivate diverse types of relationships.

Thus, an interesting question is why social media use was not associated with better physical health in this study, especially given the salubrious health effects typically seen with traditional measures of social integration and interaction (e.g., Social Network Index).53 Given the changing nature of social interactions and communication norms, it would be a timely and important endeavor to understand how social media use may contribute to social integration, which would have implications for research on social relationships and health.

In addition to the possibility that high social media usage leads to stress or displacement of health-promoting activities, problematic social media use (e.g., social networking site (SNS) addiction, social comparison) may trigger psychological processes or change in lifestyles that can undermine health.55–57 For instance, SNS addiction (e.g., preoccupation with social media, excessive use) is associated with lower well-being and depression,14,58 which can predict worse physical health.59 Although it is unclear how much our participants engaged in problematic social media use in this study, future studies may directly assess social media addiction and examine its relation to physical health (e.g., Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale).55

Caveats and limitations

This study has some limitations. First, the cross-sectional design of this study limits our ability to make causal or temporal inferences about the relation between social media use and physical health. For example, we cannot rule out the possibility that people with undermined health may use social media more (e.g., to seek health information or distraction from their dysphoria). Thus, future research should consider using longitudinal or experimental designs to establish causal and temporal effects.

Second, the effect sizes found in this study are small (0.17 < βs < 0.20), although comparable to those typically found in studies on social media use and psychological well-being (−0.05 < rs < −0.15). Thus, it would be important to consider whether these effect sizes have clinical or practical significance.

Finally, this study documented an aggregate association between overall amount of social media use and physical health. Although focusing on the amount of social media use—the most commonly studied variable—allowed us to connect to extant literature, this broad metric does not provide any insight into how people use social media. Given that people use social media for a variety of reasons, and that the ways in which they use social media can also influence their well-being,60,61 future research should examine how the types of social media use may relate to health.

Conclusion

The present study found that social media use is associated with multiple indicators of physical health. Given the prevalence of social media in daily lives and the importance of social relationships to physical health, we call for additional research to examine the relation between social media use and physical health by utilizing diverse methodologies.

Notes

  • a. Missing data analyses indicated that participants who opted in vs. out of the dried blood spotting procedure did not differ in terms of their gender, social media use, or any dependent variables.

  • b. While an average score of 2 on this measure can roughly be interpreted as spending about 44 minutes to 2 hours on social media daily, the nonlinear scale used in this measure warrants a cautious interpretation.

Supplementary Material

Supplemental data
Supp_Data.docx (14.9KB, docx)

Author Disclosure Statement

No competing financial interests exist.

Funding Information

The current research was supported in part by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (award no. UL1TR001070).

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Data

References

  • 1. Statista. Daily time spent on social networking by internet users worldwide from 2012 to 2019. https://www.statista.com/statistics/433871/daily-social-media-usage-worldwide/ (accessed Jun. 2021).
  • 2. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2020) American time use survey. https://www.bls.gov/news.release/atus.nr0.htm (accessed Aug. 2020).
  • 3. Twenge JM, Martin GN, Spitzberg BH. Trends in U.S. adolescents' media use, 1976–2016: the rise of digital media, the decline of TV, and the (near) demise of print. Psychology of Popular Media Culture 2018; 8:329–345. [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Anderson M, Jiang J. (2018) Teens, social media & technology 2018. http://assets.pewresearch.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/14/2018/05/31102617/PI_2018.05.31_TeensTech_FINAL.pdf (accessed Aug. 2020).
  • 5. Coyne SM, Rogers AA, Zurcher JD, et al. Does time spent using social media impact mental health?: An eight year longitudinal study. Computers in Human Behavior 2020; 104:106160. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Kross E, Verduyn P, Demiralp E, et al. Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PLoS One 2013; 8:e69841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Liu D, Baumeister RF, Yang CC, et al. Digital communication media use and psychological well-being: a meta-analysis. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 2019; 24:259–273. [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Orben A, Dienlin T, Przybylski AK. Social media's enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2019; 116:10226–10228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Tromholt M. The Facebook experiment: quitting Facebook leads to higher levels of well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 2016; 19:661–666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Hernandez R, Bassett SM, Boughton SW, et al. Psychological well-being and physical health: associations, mechanisms, and future directions. Emotion Review 2018; 10:18–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Afifi TD, Zamanzadeh N, Harrison K, et al. WIRED: the impact of media and technology use on stress (cortisol) and inflammation (interleukin IL-6) in fast paced families. Computers in Human Behavior 2018; 81:265–273. [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Lee DS, Way BM. Social media use and systemic inflammation: the moderating role of self-esteem. Brain, Behavior, & Immunity-Health 2021; 16:100300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Nikbin D, Iranmanesh M, Foroughi B. Personality traits, psychological well-being, Facebook addiction, health and performance: testing their relationships. Behaviour & Information Technology 2021; 40:706–722. [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Xue Y, Dong Y, Luo M, et al. Investigating the impact of mobile SNS addiction on individual's self-rated health. Internet Research 2018; 28:278–292. [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Smith A, Anderson M. (2018). Social media use in 2018. Pew Research Center. www.pewinternet.org/2018/03/01/social-media-use-in-2018 (accessed Aug. 2020).
  • 16. Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: a review. Sleep Medicine 2010; 11:735–742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Thomée S, Härenstam A, Hagberg M. Mobile phone use and stress, sleep disturbances, and symptoms of depression among young adults-a prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health 2011; 11:1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Jenaro C, Flores N, Gómez-Vela, M, et al. Problematic internet and cell-phone use: psychological, behavioral, and health correlates. Addiction Research & Theory 2007; 15:309–320. [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Woods HC, Scott H. # Sleepyteens: social media use in adolescence is associated with poor sleep quality, anxiety, depression and low self-esteem. Journal of Adolescence 2016; 51:41–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Dew MA, Hoch CC, Buysse DJ, et al. Healthy older adults' sleep predicts all-cause mortality at 4 to 19 years of follow-up. Psychosomatic Medicine 2003; 65:63–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Irwin MR, Wang M, Campomayor CO, et al. Sleep deprivation and activation of morning levels of cellular and genomic markers of inflammation. Archives of Internal Medicine 2006; 166:1756–1762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Guo X, Zheng L, Wang J, et al. Epidemiological evidence for the link between sleep duration and high blood pressure: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine 2013; 14:324–332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Buxton OM, Marcelli E. Short and long sleep are positively associated with obesity, diabetes, hypertension, and cardiovascular disease among adults in the United States. Social Science & Medicine 2010; 71:1027–1036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Vgontzas AN, Liao D, Bixler EO, et al. Insomnia with objective short sleep duration is associated with a high risk for hypertension. Sleep 2009; 32:491–497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Kripke DF, Garfinkel L, Wingard DL, et al. Mortality associated with sleep duration and insomnia. Archives of General Psychiatry 2002; 59:131–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Kushlev K, Leitao MR. The effects of smartphones on well-being: theoretical integration and research agenda. Current Opinion in Psychology 2020; 36:77–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Freytag A, Knop-Huelss K, Meier A, et al. Permanently online—always stressed out? The effects of permanent connectedness on stress experiences. Human Communication Research 2021; 47:132–165. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Reinecke L, Aufenanger S, Beutel ME, et al. Digital stress over the life span: the effects of communication load and internet multitasking on perceived stress and psychological health impairments in a German probability sample. Media Psychology 2017; 20:90–115. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Kushlev K, Dunn EW. Checking email less frequently reduces stress. Computers in Human Behavior 2015; 43:220–228. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Misra S, Stokols D. Psychological and health outcomes of perceived information overload. Environment and Behavior 2012; 44:737–759. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Beutel ME, Wiltink J, Kerahrodi JG, et al. Somatic symptom load in men and women from middle to high age in the Gutenberg Health Study-association with psychosocial and somatic factors. Scientific Reports 2019; 9:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Cohen S, Tyrrell DA, Smith AP. Psychological stress and susceptibility to the common cold. New England Journal of Medicine 1991; 325:606–612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Hughes A, Kumari M, McMunn A, et al. Unemployment and inflammatory markers in England, Wales and Scotland, 1998–2012: meta-analysis of results from 12 studies. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 2017; 64:91–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Perrin A, Anderson M. (2018). Share of US adults using social media, including Facebook, is mostly unchanged since 2018. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2019/04/10/share-of-u-s-adults-using-social-media-including-facebook-is-mostly-unchanged-since-2018/ (accessed Aug. 2020).
  • 35. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Gouin JP, Hantsoo L. Close relationships, inflammation, and health. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 2010; 35:33–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Emerging Risk Factors Collaboration. C-reactive protein concentration and risk of coronary heart disease, stroke, and mortality: an individual participant meta-analysis. The Lancet 2010; 375:132–140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Mac Giollabhui N, Ng TH, Ellman LM, et al. The longitudinal associations of inflammatory biomarkers and depression revisited: systematic review, meta-analysis, and meta-regression. Molecular Psychiatry 2021; 26:3302–3314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Leger KA, Charles ST, Ayanian JZ, et al. The association of daily physical symptoms with future health. Social Science & Medicine 2015; 143:241–248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Kroenke K. Patients presenting with somatic complaints: epidemiology, psychiatric co-morbidity and management. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 2003; 12:34–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Sha MC, Callahan CM, Counsell SR, et al. Physical symptoms as a predictor of health care use and mortality among older adults. American Journal of Medicine 2005; 118:301–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Campbell R, Pennebaker JW. The secret life of pronouns: flexibility in writing style and physical health. Psychological Science 2003; 14:60–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Appel M, Marker C, Gnambs T. Are social media ruining our lives? A review of meta-analytic evidence. Review of General Psychology 2020; 24:60–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Bayer JB, Triệu P, Ellison NB. Social media elements, ecologies, and effects. Annual Review of Psychology 2020; 71:471–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. McDade TW, Burhop J, Dohnal J. High-sensitivity enzyme immunoassay for C-reactive protein in dried blood spots. Clinical Chemistry 2004; 50:652–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Horn SR, Long MM, Nelson BW, et al. Replication and reproducibility issues in the relationship between C-reactive protein and depression: a systematic review and focused meta-analysis. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 2018; 73:85–114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Lee DS, Way BM. Perceived social support and chronic inflammation: the moderating role of self-esteem. Health Psychology 2019; 38:563–566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement 1977; 1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 48. O'Connor MF, Bower JE, Cho HJ, et al. To assess, to control, to exclude: effects of biobehavioral factors on circulating inflammatory markers. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 2009; 23:887–897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Pearson TA, Mensah GA, Alexander RW, et al. Markers of inflammation and cardiovascular disease: application to clinical and public health practice: a statement for healthcare professionals from the centers for disease control and prevention and the American Heart Association. Circulation 2003; 107:499–511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Cohen S. Social relationships and health. American Psychologist 2004; 59:676–684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Uchino BN. Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine 2006; 29:377–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Thoits PA. Multiple identities and psychological well-being: a reformulation and test of the social isolation hypothesis. American Sociological Review 1983; 48:174–187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, et al. Social ties and susceptibility to the common cold. JAMA 1997; 277:1940–1944. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Park J, Lee DS, Shablack H, et al. When perceptions defy reality: the role of actual and perceived Facebook social support in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 2016; 200:37–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Andreassen CS, Billieux J, Griffiths MD, et al. The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: a large-scale cross-sectional study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 2016; 30:252–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Kuss DJ, Griffiths, MD. Online social networking and addiction—a review of the psychological literature. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 2011; 8:3528–3552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Panda A, Jain NK. Compulsive smartphone usage and users' ill-being among young Indians: does personality matter? Telematics and Informatics 2018; 35:1355–1372. [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Lin LY, Sidani JE, Shensa A, et al. Association between social media use and depression among US young adults. Depression and Anxiety 2016; 33:323–331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Ohrnberger J, Fichera E, Sutton M. The relationship between physical and mental health: a mediation analysis. Social Science & Medicine 2017; 195:42–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Verduyn P, Lee DS, Park J, et al. Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: experimental and longitudinal evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 2015; 144:480–488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Clark JL, Algoe SB, Green MC. Social network sites and well-being: the role of social connection. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2018; 27:32–37. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental data
Supp_Data.docx (14.9KB, docx)

Articles from Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking are provided here courtesy of Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.

RESOURCES