Abstract
Listening in noisy or complex sound environments is difficult for individuals with normal hearing and can be a debilitating impairment for those with hearing loss. Extracting meaningful information from a complex acoustic environment requires the ability to accurately encode specific sound features under highly variable listening conditions and segregate distinct sound streams from multiple overlapping sources. The auditory system employs a variety of mechanisms to achieve this auditory scene analysis. First, neurons across levels of the auditory system exhibit compensatory adaptations to their gain and dynamic range in response to prevailing sound stimulus statistics in the environment. These adaptations allow for robust representations of sound features that are to a large degree invariant to the level of background noise. Second, listeners can selectively attend to a desired sound target in an environment with multiple sound sources. This selective auditory attention is another form of sensory gain control, enhancing the representation of an attended sound source while suppressing responses to unattended sounds. This review will examine both “bottom-up” gain alterations in response to changes in environmental sound statistics as well as “top-down” mechanisms that allow for selective extraction of specific sound features in a complex auditory scene. Finally, we will discuss how hearing loss interacts with these gain control mechanisms, and the adaptive and/or maladaptive perceptual consequences of this plasticity.
Keywords: adaptation, gain control, attention, auditory scene analysis, cocktail party problem, hearing loss
Introduction
Auditory scene analysis— the ability to segregate specific sound features from multiple overlapping sources— is essential for extracting meaningful information from a complex sound environment (Bregman, 1990). The classic example of this problem is the cocktail party effect, where a listener can selectively focus on one specific speaker while filtering out a range of other stimuli (Cherry, 1953; Bregman, 2008). While the cocktail party problem represents a particularly challenging situation for the auditory system, as both the target and background sounds are comprised of similar acoustic features, most behaviorally-relevant sounds (such as a person talking) occur against a background of everyday noise (e.g., traffic noise, a loud TV, etc.). Thus, adapting to noisy environments is a fundamental feature of the auditory system important for a range of listening conditions (Willmore et al., 2014; King and Walker, 2020). Understanding how the auditory system adapts to complex sound environments has important clinical implication as well, as individuals with age-related hearing loss or other hearing impairments often have great difficulties listening in noise, even when cochlear amplification is accounted for (Johannesen et al., 2016). How the auditory system solves the problem of auditory scene analysis remains incompletely understood.
Like humans, many animals— such as birds (Hulse et al., 1997), frogs (Endepols et al., 2003), and other mammals (Ma et al., 2010; Chapuis and Chadderton, 2018; Noda and Takahashi, 2019)— are capable of listening to a single sound source in a mixture of sources. Here we will discuss recent evidence from animal and human literature regarding the neurophysiological mechanisms for auditory scene analysis and hearing in complex environments. In particular, we will focus on gain control mechanisms— adjustments to the slope and dynamic range of neural input–output (I/O) relationships— that allow neurons to actively regulate their response sensitivity to the current environmental or behavioral demands (Robinson and McAlpine, 2009; Ferguson and Cardin, 2020). First, we will discuss how the auditory system adapts its response properties to changes in the overall distribution of incoming stimulus features. This bottom-up adaptation to stimulus statistics allows for extraction and invariant representation of key auditory features used to segregate sound sources in complex and continually changing acoustic environments. Next, we will discuss top-down contextual and attentional gain control mechanisms that can highlight behaviorally relevant sound information while selectively filtering distracting sources, even with overlapping acoustic features. Finally, we will examine how the central auditory system adapts to cochlear hearing loss and how this compensatory plasticity can have both adaptive and maladaptive consequences for sound perception and listening in complex auditory environments.
Bottom-Up Adaptation to Sound Statistics
Most natural sounds, including human speech, are characterized by dynamic changes in acoustic energy across spectral and temporal domains (Davenport, 1952; Singh and Theunissen, 2003; Santoro et al., 2014). In order to efficiently analyze an auditory scene and accurately represent the vast range of sounds encountered in the world, auditory neurons must be able to continually adapt their response properties to the prevailing acoustic environment. There is ample evidence that neural representations of sound are sensitive to statistical regularities in the acoustic environment (Winkler et al., 2009). For instance, many neurons across the auditory neuraxis exhibit stimulus-specific adaptation (SSA), in that they become less responsive to frequently occurring or repetitive stimuli but retain their sensitivity to rare stimuli, allowing for an intrinsic capacity to selectively encode unpredictable or novel sounds (Ulanovsky et al., 2003; Nelken, 2014). In addition to adapting to their own stimulus history, auditory neurons can also modify their response properties to match the statistics of the entire distribution of sounds encountered in the environment. Auditory neurons adapt their dynamic range and gain in response to a variety of stimulus statistics (Figure 1), including: mean sound level (Dean et al., 2005; Wen et al., 2009; Barbour, 2011), sound level variance or contrast (Nagel and Doupe, 2006; Rabinowitz et al., 2011; Willmore et al., 2014), interaural sound cues (Dahmen et al., 2010; Stange et al., 2013), and spectral-temporal correlations (Kvale and Schreiner, 2004; Natan et al., 2016; Homma et al., 2020). In this manner, neuronal responses are continuously rescaled to match dynamically changing sound conditions while maintaining overall firing rates across stimuli with different statistics. This adaptation to sound statistics enables auditory neurons to efficiently encode a wide-range of stimulus features under highly variable conditions and may be an effective mechanism for generating relatively invariant sound representations that are robust to the presence of background noise. Below we will discuss evidence for different forms of stimulus statistic adaptation as well as our current understanding of the neurophysiological mechanism and perceptual consequences of these adaptations.
Dynamic Range Adaptation
Natural acoustic scenes are characterized by stimuli that can vary over a wide range of sound levels, roughly 10–12 orders of magnitude (Baccus, 2006; Robinson and McAlpine, 2009). The auditory system maintains a remarkable sensitivity to small differences in sound level over this enormous range of intensities despite the relatively restricted dynamic range of individual auditory neurons, typically 30–50 dB SPL (Figure 1A) (Sachs and Abbas, 1974; Viemeister, 1988). This so-called dynamic range problem is compounded by noisy environments that act to increase the steady-state firing rate of auditory neurons, thereby limiting their dynamic range even further (Figure 1A) (Costalupes et al., 1984; Young and Barta, 1986). One potential solution to the dynamic range problem is to have distinct subsets of auditory neurons with different thresholds and dynamic ranges, such that combining or stitching together individual response functions would allow for representation of intensities across the full range of hearing at the population level (Barbour, 2011). Dynamic range stitching is observed to some degree at the level of the auditory nerve (AN), where fibers can be classified into at least three distinct subsets based on their response threshold and spontaneous firing rates (SR) (Evans, 1972; Sachs and Abbas, 1974; Liberman, 1978). Low-SR fibers have high thresholds and large dynamic ranges, medium-SR fiber have intermediate thresholds and dynamic range, while high-SR fibers have low thresholds and narrow dynamic range. Thus, while individual AN fibers have a restricted dynamic range, their sensitivity is distributed across a range of intensities. Moreover, the high threshold and larger dynamic range of low-SR fibers make them better suited for encoding intensity at higher sound levels and more resistant to background noise, suggesting they may be important for hearing in a noisy environment (Costalupes et al., 1984). Dynamic range stitching is also observed in the central auditory system, with a subset of central auditory neurons exhibiting non-monotonic response functions that respond best to a particular sound level rather than exhibiting a constant increase in firing rate with increasing sound intensity (Suga and Manabe, 1982; Phillips et al., 1994; Sadagopan and Wang, 2008). However, the vast majority of auditory neurons have thresholds and dynamic ranges that are still heavily skewed toward lower sound intensities and it is therefore unlikely that dynamic range stitching can fully account for the maintenance of consistent sound level sensitivity across the entire range of hearing (Sachs and Abbas, 1974; Watkins and Barbour, 2011). It is becoming increasingly clear that a major mechanism for solving the dynamic range problem is that individual auditory neurons can dynamically adapt their threshold and dynamic range to compensate for changes in mean stimulus level, thereby maintaining maximum sensitivity over the most commonly encountered sound levels in the prevailing acoustic environment (Figure 1B) (Dean et al., 2005).
Dynamic range adaptation to mean sound level is observed across multiple levels of the auditory system, most notably at the AN (Wen et al., 2009), inferior colliculus (IC) (Dean et al., 2005, 2008), and auditory cortex (ACx) (Watkins and Barbour, 2008). When sounds are drawn from a distribution with a high probability of loud sounds, such that the mean sound level is high, neurons shift their dynamic range upward, increasing their sensitivity to louder sounds (Figure 1B). Sound-level adaptation is thus compensatory, so that neuronal responses are relatively invariant to changes in background level. The degree of dynamic range adaptation observed in the IC is higher than at the level of the AN, suggesting that adaptation in the auditory midbrain is only partially accounted for by changes occurring in the periphery. This suggest that additional adaptation is occurring within the IC and potentially in the auditory brainstem as well. Indeed, neurons in the ventral cochlear nucleus (VCN) of the brainstem have been shown to adapt their threshold and dynamic range to the presence of background noise (May and Sachs, 1992). However, these studies employed a stationary background noise and experiments using the same dynamic switching stimulus used to measure mean level adaptation in the AN and IC have not been performed at the level of the brainstem. Similar magnitudes of adaptation are seen in subcortical and cortical neurons, suggesting that many features of mean level adaptation in cortex are inherited from lower levels of the auditory system. However, there are unique features of adaptation in the ACx, such as a subset of cortical neurons with non-monotonic rate-level functions that do not undergo adaptive recoding and are thus maladapted to high level sounds but preserve coding accuracy for quiet sounds even in the context of high sound level environments (Watkins and Barbour, 2008).
Contrast Gain Control
The auditory system is not only exposed to a wide range of sound levels, but any acoustic scene may be comprised of a relatively large or restricted subset of intensities across this spectrum. Thus, in addition to adapting to mean sound level, auditory neurons must be able to modulate their response properties to changes in the variance or contrast of sound levels present in the environment (Figure 1C). Contrast-invariant tuning is one of the most well-characterized examples of gain modulation observed across sensory systems (Finn et al., 2007; Olsen and Wilson, 2008; Rabinowitz et al., 2011) and contrast gain control in the auditory system is thought to be an important physiological mechanisms for encoding sound stimuli in noisy background conditions (Willmore et al., 2014). When an auditory neuron is exposed to a wide range of sound intensities, such that the contrast of the input is high, the gain of that neuron is low (Figure 1C). In this manner, the neuron has a broader dynamic range that is relatively insensitive to changes in sound level. When the contrast of the input is low, the gain of the neuron increases, making it more sensitive to small changes in intensity. Thus, like mean level adaptations, contrast gain control is compensatory, allowing neurons to adjust their gain in a manner that allows for representations that are relatively invariant to the level of background noise. Such adaptation allows for sounds that are structurally similar but with different contrast levels to be represented in a similar manner. It should also be noted that auditory neurons adjust their response properties to higher-order stimulus statistics like skewness or kurtosis as well (Kvale and Schreiner, 2004). Studies in ferrets have found that contrast gain control is more complete in the ACx compared to subcortical stations (Rabinowitz et al., 2013). However, more recent work in mice has found similar levels of contrast gain adaptation in the ACx, auditory thalamus (medial geniculate body; MGB), and IC (Lohse et al., 2020). Notably, the authors did find that adaptation time constants become longer at ascending levels of the auditory system, resulting in progressively more stable representations. Thus, there may be progressive changes to contrast gain control along the ascending auditory pathway, similar to that observed with mean level adaptation.
The combined effect of dynamic range adaptation and contrast gain control is to minimize the influence of background noise on auditory feature encoding. Indeed, by the level of the ACx, adaptation to mean level and contrast enables speech sounds to be represented in a way that is robust to the presence of background noise (Rabinowitz et al., 2013; Mesgarani et al., 2014b). However, it is important to note that adaptation to other acoustic features beyond sound intensity is likely important for speech perception and auditory scene analysis as well. Spectral features are a fundamental component of communication signals in mammalian vocalizations (Suga et al., 1983; Kadia and Wang, 2003). Human speech is comprised of several harmonic features and the use of these features can be helpful for identifying a speaker in a complex environment (Ehret and Riecke, 2002). Speech also varies in its temporal profile, including elements of fast temporal modulation and slower changes associated with periodicity of the speech signal, and the temporal structure of human vocalizations plays a crucial role in speech comprehension (Rosen, 1992; Shannon et al., 1995; Hickok and Poeppel, 2007). Frequency-specific adaptations have been observed in the human ACx that depend on the spectral range of acoustic stimuli, suggesting that there are neural adjustments to spectral stimulus statistics of sound stimuli (Herrmann et al., 2014). ACx neurons also display gain adaptations to changes in the temporal properties of sound input, allowing them to maintain their dynamic range across a range of temporal correlations (Natan et al., 2016) and use non-linear sensitivity to temporal and spectral content for adaptation (Figure 1D) (Angeloni and Geffen, 2018). Thus, in addition to sound level adaptations, neuronal adjustments to spectral and temporal sounds properties are also likely important for neural representation of speech in different background conditions (Ding and Simon, 2013; Mesgarani et al., 2014b; Khalighinejad et al., 2019).
Adaptation in Sound Localization
In a natural environment, multiple sound sources often originate from different locations and being able to identify the spatial location of distinct sound sources is a key component to auditory scene analysis. Binaural cues, such as interaural time (ITD) and level (ILD) differences, are essential for localizing sounds in space. The medial superior olive (MSO) and lateral superior olive (LSO) of the auditory brainstem are the initial sites of ITD and ILD processing in the mammalian auditory system, respectively. These brainstem nuclei contain coincidence detecting neurons that encode ITD and ILD differences by comparing the timing of converging inputs from the ipsilateral and contralateral ear with submillisecond precision (Park et al., 2004; Grothe et al., 2010). Because of the degree of precision required for these computations, and the fact that accurate representation of absolute stimulus values may be more important for sound-source localization than for other acoustic features like sound level, traditional models of sound localization have proposed that ITDs and ILDs are encoded via a fixed labeled-line mechanism resulting in a hard-wired place code or map of auditory space (Jeffress, 1948; Grothe and Koch, 2011). However, it has now been shown that sound localization cues and spatial perception are also subject to short-term adaption based on prior stimulus history (Phillips and Hall, 2005; Vigneault-MacLean et al., 2007; Dahmen et al., 2010; Stange et al., 2013). Indeed, coincidence-detector neurons in both the MSO and LSO exhibit dynamic gain adaptations driven by feedback loops in early parts of the binaural pathway that modulate their sensitivity to ITD and ILD differences in response to prior activity levels (Finlayson and Adam, 1997; Magnusson et al., 2008; Park et al., 2008; Stange et al., 2013; Lingner et al., 2018). Adaption to sound source localization is observed in downstream auditory structures as well. By presenting sound sequences in which ILDs rapidly fluctuate according to a Gaussian distribution, it was shown that IC neurons also adapt the dynamic range and gain of their ILD-rate functions to match the mean and variance of the stimulus distribution (Dahmen et al., 2010). Thus, adaptive gain control mechanisms can also modulate the population code for auditory space in a stimulus history dependent manner.
Perceptual Consequences of Stimulus Statistic Adaptation
The above studies demonstrate that the auditory system uses multiple adaptive coding strategies to most efficiently represent and extract features from the sound environment. However, elucidating the perceptual consequences of these adaptations is crucial for determining if and how they facilitate our ability to analyze an auditory scene. Several recent studies have found that perceptual adaptations to stimulus statistics in humans parallel neurophysiological adaptations in animal models using near identical paradigms (Dahmen et al., 2010; Stange et al., 2013; Lohse et al., 2020). For instance, there is a close correspondence between changes to the perceived laterality of a stimulus in humans and adaptations to ILD-rate functions in the IC of ferrets when both are presented with noise sequences with rapidly fluctuating ILDs (Dahmen et al., 2010). Likewise, acuity in an intensity discrimination task is rapidly adjusted with changes to sound contrast in humans and the strength of this perceptual contrast adaptation could be predicted from physiological contrast adaptation observed in mice (Lohse et al., 2020). Chronic in vivo recordings from the ACx of mice trained to detect a target sound in background noise shortly after a change in the background contrast have provided some of the first evidence that cortical gain modulation and sound detection behavior are modulated by contrast in a parallel manner in the same subjects (Angeloni et al., 2021). This study found that ACx activity is necessary for detection of targets in background noise and that inter-subject variability in the magnitude of contrast gain control observed in the ACx predicted behavioral performance. These findings provide evidence that adaptive coding in the ACx has direct implications on perceptual behavior. In contrast, single unit recordings from the IC of macaques performing a masked tone detection task found that, despite observing dynamic range adaptation in the IC of these animals, behavioral detection thresholds were not affected by this neuronal adaptation (Rocchi and Ramachandran, 2018). Likewise, MEG and EEG studies have found evidence for dynamic range adaptation in the ACx of humans but parallel behavioral studies found that perceptual sensitivity to sound level was actually affected in an opposite manner than predicted by dynamic range adaptation, with increased sensitivity to sound intensities in the low probability region of the intensity distribution rather than high (Simpson et al., 2014; Herrmann et al., 2020). Thus, while there is growing evidence that adaption to stimulus statistics does influence perception, more work is needed to determine how different forms of adaptation across levels of the auditory system contribute to sound perception and auditory scene analysis.
Many studies have now shown that auditory neurons adapt their response properties to a range of stimulus statistics and tremendous progress has been made in the neurophysiological characterization of these bottom-up adaptations. However, there are many open questions that remain to be addressed. For instance, while the above studies indicate that adaptive coding is gradually built along the auditory pathway, the relative contributions of different auditory structures remain incompletely understood. More studies utilizing simultaneous recordings from multiple auditory regions are needed to determine how different forms of adaptation emerge along the ascending auditory pathway. Indeed, a recent study using this approach has uncovered a previously underappreciated role for subcortical processing in contrast gain control (Lohse et al., 2020). Second, the underlying cellular and circuit mechanisms driving adaption to sound statistics need to be fully elucidated, as will be discussed in subsequent sections. This knowledge is essential for understanding the biophysical constraints on theses adaptive processes as well as for generating novel strategies for manipulating these processes to better investigate their contribution to auditory scene analysis. Finally, more studies performing neurophysiological recordings from actively behaving animals are necessary to directly assess the impact of bottom-up adaptions on perception (Angeloni et al., 2021). One difficulty in assessing the perceptual consequences of bottom-up adaptations is that, in most cases, measuring behavioral sensitivity to changes in stimulus statistics requires subjects to be engaged in a perceptual decision-making task. Task engagement itself will invoke a multitude of adaptive changes in the auditory system, as will be discussed in the next section.
Top-Down Contributions to Auditory Scene Analysis
Bottom-up adaptations to the prevailing sound statistics enable the auditory system to more efficiently encode target sounds in complex or noisy environments, particularly when the statistics of foreground and background sounds are distinct (Figure 1). However, background sounds that share acoustic features or statistical properties that significantly overlap with the signals of interest, such as is the case for the cocktail party problem, pose unique challenges for auditory scene analysis and additional mechanisms must exist to selectively extract specific sound sources from structurally similar background noise (King and Walker, 2020). Attention is a cognitive process by which organisms filter the most relevant behavioral information from their environment to enhance perception of one particular stimulus over another. Selective attention has been proposed to contribute to auditory scene analysis by acting as a form of sensory gain control, enhancing the representation of an attended sound source while suppressing responses to unattended sounds (Fritz et al., 2007; Kerlin et al., 2010; Zion Golumbic et al., 2013). This process can occur when the stimulus itself directs attention through enhanced salience, referred to as bottom-up or “pop-out” attention (Kayser et al., 2005), or can be endogenously generated through top-down or “task-modulated” processes. The focus of this section will be to discuss these top-down mechanisms and how selective attention contributes to gain modulation, feature selection, and stream separation in the auditory system, which work in concert to improve auditory scene analysis. First, we will discuss the growing body of evidence from animal studies showing that sensory encoding is fundamentally modulated by behavioral state. Then, we will discuss evidence that task-engagement, a proxy for attention in animal models, is associated with receptive field changes that act to maximize encoding of task-relevant information. Finally, we will discuss evidence from human studies showing that selective attention does indeed influence perception and listening performance in complex auditory environments.
Behavioral State and Attentional Modulation of Sensory Processing
Behavioral states have strong influences on neuronal responses associated with sensory processing (Bennett et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2014). Early studies that monitored pupil dilation as a proxy for arousal found that cortical neurons in sensory regions are strongly modulated by dilation onset and that neuronal firing rates correlate with the level of dilation even in the absence of sensory input (Iriki et al., 1996). These results suggest that internal state changes may influence how we process incoming sensory information. Indeed, performance on a tone-in-noise detection task has been shown to be highly state-dependent, with peak behavioral performance being associated with intermediate levels of arousal (McGinley et al., 2015a). These intermediate arousal states coincided with periods of stable hyperpolarization in auditory cortical neurons. Subsequent studies using optogenetic manipulations of inhibitory interneuron populations found that such hyperpolarized states increase encoding specificity by augmenting the threshold for responsivity of excitatory neurons while simultaneously narrowing the frequency tuning properties in principle cell populations (Hamilton et al., 2013; Aizenberg et al., 2015; Phillips and Hasenstaub, 2016). Selective attention also profoundly impacts behavioral sensitivity to sensory stimuli and is in fact operationally defined as an improvement in psychophysical performance for attended versus unattended stimuli (e.g., Carrasco, 2011). Neurophysiological studies have revealed that attentional effects on sensory processing are due at least in part to gain modulation that increases stimulus-evoked response size (Mitchell et al., 2007; Maunsell, 2015). Indeed, tone-evoked responses in the ACx are greater in animals performing a tone-detection task compared to passive listening conditions, indicative of attentional gain modulation (Francis et al., 2018). Taken together, these findings offer insight into how behavioral state changes influence sensory processing irrespective of the sound statistics being conveyed to the sensory system.
While behavioral state and attentional gain increases enhance the magnitude of sensory-evoked responses, it is important to note that a non-selective increase in neuronal activity is not necessarily beneficial to stimulus detection. Rather, attention appears to enhance feature encoding by modulating not only the magnitude of the sensory stimulus but also the spontaneous activity or “noise” of neural responses (Harris and Thiele, 2011). Background noise can include non-stimulus specific activity represented by highly correlated neurons that act to reduce the amount of information that can be encoded for a particular stimulus or through competing distractors in the stimulus field (Zohary et al., 1994; Fries et al., 2001; Moreno-Bote et al., 2014). Selective attention not only increases stimulus-evoked responses, but also reduces the effect of intrinsic background noise, thereby enhancing signal-to-noise ratios for sensory representations and decreasing trial-to-trial variability (Mitchell et al., 2009; Downer et al., 2017; Francis et al., 2018). Selective attention simultaneously reduces variability and noise correlations across populations of cortical neurons in large part by reducing low frequency firing rate correlations to produce a sparse and temporally reliable code (Mitchell et al., 2009; Francis et al., 2018). This appears to be the case for arousal-dependent changes in sound processing as well, as changes in pupil diameter produce bi-modal effects on spontaneous and sensory-evoked activity that improve signal-to-noise ratios of sound-evoked responses (McGinley et al., 2015b). Such findings argue that reduction in spontaneous neural activity is as critical to feature discrimination as gain modulated increases in firing rate. Indeed, attentional control associated with the act of behavioral engagement appears to enhance feature encoding by altering the spontaneous activity of cortical circuits prior to sensory processing. For example, the process of self-directed trial initiation decreases the rate of spontaneous activity in the ACx of rats performing a tone-detection task and optogenetic disruption of cortical activity before tone presentation acts to impair performance (Carcea et al., 2017). Thus, attention has the effect of both increasing responsiveness to a target stimulus while simultaneously reducing the influence of background activity and distracting inputs, which together act to stabilize sensory representations and promote feature tracking in complex environments.
Task-Dependent Modulation of Sound Feature Encoding
The above studies suggest that auditory responses rely not only on the external sounds reaching the ear, but also on the behavioral context and internal state of the subject. While it is clear that attentional modulation of auditory neuron response properties can act to improve signal-to-noise ratios and the reliability of sensory encoding, does selective attention allow subjects to focus on specific sound features in a complex auditory environment? Ideally, a subject in a complex auditory scene could utilize the spectrotemporal content of relevant features to separate attended streams from background unattended streams to better isolate the target. Task engagement has indeed been shown to result in rapid adaptions to auditory neuron response properties in a manner that optimizes encoding of task-specific features. Combined measures of temporal and frequency sensitivity to sound stimuli can be measured by calculating the spectrotemporal receptive fields (STRFs) of cortical neurons. In a series of experiments where ferrets were trained to discriminate a tonal target in the presence of background noise stimuli that were comprised of TORCs (temporally orthogonal ripple combinations), it was demonstrated that the STRFs of ACx neurons dynamically adapt to the stimulus features, enhancing responses to the target frequency while reducing responses to the non-target spectral and temporal features (Figures 2Ai,ii) (Fritz et al., 2003, 2005a). These changes in receptive properties were rapidly and specifically modulated by task-engagement, with the STRFs returning to their original fields shortly after the behavioral task was over. Moreover, STRF changes were highly dependent upon the nature of the task and revealed task-specific signatures based on whether the animal was taxed with spectral or temporal feature discrimination (Fritz et al., 2007). Task reward structure also modulates attention-driven receptive field plasticity, with positive or negative reinforcement for the same target tone resulting in rapid and selective changes in cortical STRFs at the target frequency in equal magnitude but opposite direction (David et al., 2012). Thus, attention reshapes cortical tuning properties in manner that enhances the contrast between task relevant stimulus classes. Encoding of sound spatial location is also highly sensitivity to task demands. In a task where cats were trained to identify changes in sound source origin across trials, spatial selective tuning emerged within seconds of task onset and was mediated via suppression of tuning responsivity in least preferred spatial locations (Figures 2Bi,ii) (Lee and Middlebrooks, 2011). Thus, while spatial tuning is typically broad in auditory cortical neurons, selective attention can rapidly sharpen spatial tuning properties to improve localization of attended sound sources. These findings suggest that auditory selective attention can mediate short-term cortical plasticity to modulate spectrotemporal and spatial sound encoding, thereby improving perceptual performance in a task-specific manner.
While the above studies indicate that top-down attentional signals dynamically reshape receptive fields in the primary ACx in a task-specific manner, an important question that remains is the anatomical locus of these top-down signals. As sound information ascends through the auditory system, neurons preferentially encode more abstract sound entities or categorizations rather than detailed spectrotemporal features (Chechik and Nelken, 2012; Mesgarani et al., 2014a). This abstraction along cortical hierarchies is likely important for building invariant representations of foreground target sounds that are robust to different background sound conditions. Indeed, neurons in non-primary ACx exhibit greater invariance in encoding acoustically distorted communicative signals compared to neurons in primary ACx (Carruthers et al., 2013, 2015). Similarly, dual recordings from primary and secondary ACx in ferrets trained to detect streams of repeated noise samples embedded in a stream of random background samples found that stream-specific gain enhancement was stronger in secondary cortical areas compared to primary ACx (Saderi et al., 2020). Importantly, categorical sound representations in higher-order cortical regions are often behaviorally-gated, adaptively assuming different states or filter properties depending upon the demands of the ongoing task. In ferrets engaged in a tone-discrimination task, belt regions of ACx that typically reflect stimulus properties similar to primary ACx (Figure 2Ci) take on less faithful representation of the stimulus and more abstract properties that reflect components of the motivational properties of the behavioral task (Figure 2Cii) (Atiani et al., 2014). This effect of task engagement is seen to even greater degree in frontal cortical regions, where neurons that rarely responded to sound stimuli during passive listening selectively responded to target sounds during behavior (Figure 2Ciii) (Fritz et al., 2010). These task-specific responses in frontal cortical regions could in principal provide top-down signals that mediate receptive field changes in primary ACx based on task category expectations (Fritz et al., 2007). Consistent with this notion, changes in frontal cortex representations coincide with augmented inter-areal coherence between frontal cortex and regions of primary ACx that were most responsive to target sounds (Fritz et al., 2010). Likewise, pairing electrical stimulation of orbitofrontal cortex with sound stimuli in passively listening animals induced rapid changes in the frequency receptive fields of primary ACx neurons in a manner similar to the effects of task-engagement (Winkowski et al., 2013). These findings suggest that functional interactions between frontal and primary sensory areas can shape the flow of relevant auditory information during active listening and is thus likely to play an important role in auditory scene analysis.
Top-Down Modulation of Stimulus Statistic Adaption
Behavioral task engagement is not only associated with attention driven changes to sound feature encoding but has also been shown to directly influence the degree of bottom-up adaptation to statistical changes in sound. For instance, a recent study revealed that the magnitude of dynamic range adaptation in the IC of macaques was enhanced in animals actively engaged in a tone-in-noise detection task compared to when they were passively listening to the same stimuli (Rocchi and Ramachandran, 2020). Recordings of IC neurons in guinea pigs repeatedly exposed to a switching stimulus that alternates between loud and quiet environment found that auditory midbrain neurons adapt more rapidly with repeated exposure to a loud environment, a phenomenon termed meta-adaptation (Robinson et al., 2016). This meta-adaptation suggests that auditory scene analysis is not only influenced by the statistical properties of sound input but our prior knowledge of the sound environment. Interestingly, cortical inactivation via cryoloop cooling disrupted meta-adaptation in the IC, indicating the top-down nature of this phenomenon. Thus, adaptation to mean sound level is accelerated and more efficient when animals have been previously exposed to an environment or are engaged in an actively listening paradigm. Together, these attentional effects on spectral-temporal receptive properties, spatial tuning, and sound level adaptations are likely to aid in our ability to identify target sound sources in complex listening conditions.
Attentional Contributions to Auditory Scene Analysis in Humans
Animal studies have clearly demonstrated that attentional state and experience can influence auditory response properties and adaption to sound stimulus statistics. Parallel human studies have provided evidence that this attention-driven plasticity is indeed important for auditory scene analysis. In human ACx, selective attention enhances psychophysical performance through increases in neural gain (Kauramäki et al., 2007; Kerlin et al., 2010; Zion Golumbic et al., 2013). Increases in multiplicative gain in ascending pathways as well as enhancement of feature selectivity in secondary auditory cortices associated with “what” and “where” processing pathways appears to occur during auditory scene analysis (Ahveninen et al., 2006). The adoption of stimulus parameters from electrophysiological studies in rodents and ferrets have further revealed complex associations in auditory regions in human studies. For instance, rapid changes in the spectrotemporal response of recorded neurons in human ACx can occur in seconds, mimicking the effects seen in ferrets. Such changes also corresponded with improved perceptual performance in extraction of speech from a degraded stimulus (Holdgraf et al., 2016). In binaural task designs, selective auditory attention enhances the neural representation of relevant sound streams while reducing the neural representation of irrelevant sound streams in distracting environments (Bidet-Caulet et al., 2007). For example, in a study with competing sound streams that were modulated at different frequencies, the auditory steady state response was modulated at the sound stream frequency in the direction of the attended stream within the precentral sulcus (Bharadwaj et al., 2014), a region important for visual spatial attention (Wardak et al., 2006). Imaging studies have further identified a dichotomy of processing where primary regions show enhanced sensitivity to temporal coherence and associate auditory regions show strong activation by sounds with reduced temporal correlations (Zatorre and Belin, 2001; Schönwiesner et al., 2005; Overath et al., 2008).
The level of noise invariance is also highly regulated by the directionality of the attended source in humans, suggesting that hierarchical cortical processing allows for spectrotemporal feature extraction that is strongly spatially modulated (Mesgarani and Chang, 2012; Schneider and Woolley, 2013). High-density EEG has revealed spatial speech stream segregation occurs during selective attention for an attended talker. Importantly, differences in alpha power (8–12 Hz) across hemispheres at parietal sites indicated the direction of auditory attention (Kerlin et al., 2010). Interestingly, analysis of high gamma (75–150 Hz) LFPs in the posterior temporal lobe reveals that reconstruction of the speech spectrograms from neural activity reflect the attended speaker alone despite being presented in the presence of a competing speaker. Importantly, on counterbalanced trials, the reconstructed spectrograms in the same region reflected the change to the new attended speaker, suggesting cortical representation of speech gives rise to the perceptual aspects relevant for the listener’s intended goal (Mesgarani and Chang, 2012). In subsequent studies, Deng et al., found that directed attention cues occurring before the auditory discrimination task promoted supramodal alpha activity ipsilateral to the area of directed attention. Further, this relative ratio of ipsilateral/contralateral alpha activity shifted smoothly across hemispheres as the target source location was moved from the ipsilateral to the contralateral location (Deng et al., 2020). Such findings suggest that an ability to attend to localized sound statistics reflecting a relevant target are an important feature of auditory scene analysis, although not all listeners can do this with the same level of precision. For instance, a recent study in individuals with normal levels of hearing and speech understanding, found that reduced performance for non-speech auditory selective attention accounted for the greatest variation in individual task performance in a cocktail-party listening task (Oberfeld and Klöckner-Nowotny, 2016). This raises the intriguing possibility that listener performance in complex environments is largely a function of attentional capacity. These findings offer insight into the complex interactions between sound feature statistic adaptation and the role of cognitive capacity in attentional gain control during auditory scene analysis.
This section has highlighted our current understanding of behavioral state influences on auditory processing and the evidence for top-down regulation of feature encoding and adaptation in the auditory system. However, several questions remain to be answered. For instance, despite strong evidence for attentional gain control and task-dependent receptive field plasticity in the auditory system, it is still unclear if gain modulation can sufficiently account for the receptive field changes seen with task engagement (Otazu et al., 2009; Lopez Espejo et al., 2019). In addition, while frontal executive control regions are implicated in top-down modulation of auditory feature encoding through studies of coherence, the specific cortical regions involved— and how they are recruited to impart influence in primary sensory regions— remains unclear. Most prominently, little is known about the local and long-range circuit mechanisms and neurotransmitter systems that allow for such dynamic attentional adaptations. In the subsequent section we will discuss potential candidates, including distinct neuronal subtypes which confer specialization to cortical and subcortical circuits during sensory processing.
Mechanisms for Bottom-Up and Top-Down Adaptations
The combination of bottom-up adaptations to the sound statistical environment and top-down modulation of receptive field properties greatly effects sound feature encoding in the auditory system and is likely to impact our ability to listen to target sound sources in a noisy environment. There is also evidence that these bottom-up and top-down adaptations directly interact, as task engagement can modulate how auditory neurons adapt to changes in incoming stimulus statistics. An important question, therefore, is if these bottom-up and top-down adaptations converge on common neuronal mechanisms. Does attention co-opt the circuits that mediate bottom-up gain adaptations, or do top-down and bottom-up gain control rely on independent mechanisms that can interact in complex ways? There are number of cellular and circuit mechanisms that may be used to implement gain adaptations to changes in incoming stimulus statistics and/or selective attention (Figure 3). This section will discuss our current understanding of how these changes are implemented within the auditory system and potential interactions between them.
Synaptic Mechanisms Contributing to Sound Stimulus Adaptations
Efficient information processing in neural circuits is dependent on tightly regulated interactions between excitatory and inhibitory neurons, which may or may not necessarily be balanced depending on conditions or behavioral state (Haider et al., 2006; Shew et al., 2011; Yizhar et al., 2011). Under conditions of tight excitatory-inhibitory balance (E/I balance), as sometimes seen in the ACx (Wehr and Zador, 2003; Zhang et al., 2003), synaptically driven fluctuations in membrane potential (Vm) can multiplicatively regulate tuned neural responses (Chance et al., 2002; Shu et al., 2003). Thus, noisy background synaptic input can exert gain control on neuronal output under different environmental conditions, providing a potential cellular mechanism for gain adaptations to stimulus statistics (Finn et al., 2007). However, in vivo whole cell recordings from ACx neurons during high and low contrast stimulation found that membrane conductance was not significantly modulated by stimulus contrast and contrast-dependent Vm fluctuations could not account for contrast gain control in the ACx (Cooke et al., 2020). Short-term synaptic plasticity is another potential mechanism for gain modulation (Abbott et al., 1997), as short-term synaptic depression at thalamocortical synapses is believed to contribute to contrast gain control in the visual system (Carandini et al., 2002; Banitt et al., 2007). Synaptic depression is observed at synapses across levels of the auditory system (Nelson et al., 2009; Yang and Xu-Friedman, 2009; Blundon et al., 2011), including hair cell-ribbon synapses (Goutman, 2017). While this form of short-term plasticity has been proposed to play a role in SSA (Motanis et al., 2018), its role in stimulus statistic adaptation remains to be determined. Interestingly, STRF models that incorporate both synaptic depression and gain control show that there are additive effects of these properties on the robustness of cortical neuron STRFs to background noise, suggesting that these are complementary processes that may not reflect the same underlying mechanism (Mesgarani et al., 2014b; Pennington and David, 2020).
Modulation of presynaptic synaptic transmission has been shown to play an integral role for adaptive gain control in auditory brainstem circuits that use precise comparison of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs to compute ITDs and ILDs for sound localization (Figures 3Ci,ii) (Finlayson and Caspary, 1989; Park et al., 1996). Interestingly, both excitatory and inhibitory pre-synaptic terminals in the MSO and LSO are dynamically adjusted by GABA via activation of pre-synaptic GABAB receptors that modulate neurotransmitter release (Magnusson et al., 2008; Grothe and Koch, 2011; Stange et al., 2013). In the LSO, GABA is released from principle cells in an activity-dependent manner and bind to pre-synaptic GABAB receptors to mediated gain adaptation on the time scale of seconds (Figure 3Ci) (Magnusson et al., 2008). Retrograde activation of presynaptic GABAB receptors has asymmetric effects on excitatory and inhibitory synaptic terminals in the LSO, suppressing glutamatergic transmission more strongly than glycinergic transmission. The net effect is to decrease excitability of LSO neurons, resulting in a shift in the dynamic range of ILD functions and narrowing the binaural receptive field of LSO neurons so that ipsilateral stimuli are preferentially encoded and perceived as more intense (Magnusson et al., 2008). MSO neurons have also been shown to modulate their sensitivity to ITD through a GABAB feedback mechanism from the superior periolivary nucleus (SPN), which also receives collateral inputs from the MSO (Figure 3Cii). This di-synaptic feedback loop activates pre-synaptic GABAB receptors, causing a slow-acting and long-lasting decrease in MSO neuronal activity in a manner proportional to their prior activity levels. This activity-dependent rate adaptation does not directly alter preferred ITDs in MSO neurons, but results in a form output normalization gain modulation that produces asymmetry in hemispheric population code for sound space (Stange et al., 2013). In this manner, strongly lateralized sound sources induce unequal adaptation preferentially in the contralateral hemisphere, thereby shifting perceived location of a subsequently presented sound source. Parallel psychophysical experiments found that the same paradigm used to evoke GABAB receptor-mediated adaptation in gerbils caused predictable shifts in sound localization percepts in humans (Phillips and Hall, 2005; Stange et al., 2013). Thus, dynamic adjustments to the balance between excitation and inhibition in MSO and LSO neurons via regulation of presynaptic transmitter release are used to modulate sound localization cues and spatial perception.
Cortical Circuit Mechanisms Contributing to Bottom-Up and Top-Down Adaptations
Local inhibitory interneuron networks are prominent regulators of neuronal gain, particularly within cortical circuits (Katzner et al., 2011; Ferguson and Cardin, 2020), and E/I balance is thought to be essential for proper regulation of sensory encoding (Wehr and Zador, 2003; Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011). Indeed, there is evidence that alterations to E/I balance underlie rapid receptive field changes seen in cortical neurons following experimental conditioning or learning (Carcea and Froemke, 2013; Froemke et al., 2013). E/I balance is also highly state-dependent (Haider et al., 2013; Zhou et al., 2014) and top-down modulation of E/I balance may contribute to the observed behavioral state and attentional gain modulation of sensory processing discussed in the previous section (Harris and Thiele, 2011). However, the diversity of inhibitory interneurons subtypes, in terms of physiological properties and computational functions, has complicated our understanding of how local inhibitory networks contribute to sensory gain adaptations. Most recent work has focused on three major GABAergic cell types (Figure 3B): Fast-spiking parvalbumin positive (PV) interneurons that target perisomatic regions of excitatory neurons; low-threshold spiking somatostatin positive (SST) interneurons that target dendrites; and sparse, dendritic targeting vasoactive intestinal peptide (VIP) interneurons that often target other inhibitory interneurons to form disinhibitory circuits (Wood et al., 2017).
PV interneurons act as key mediators of response gain in cortical principal cells (Cardin et al., 2009; Sohal et al., 2009; Atallah et al., 2012; Wilson et al., 2012) and have been broadly implicated in feedforward circuits contributing to frequency tuning, adaptation, and gap encoding in ACx (Moore and Wehr, 2013; Li et al., 2014, 2015; Aizenberg et al., 2015; Natan et al., 2015; Keller et al., 2018). For these reasons, recent work has attempted to elucidate the role of PV neurons in contrast gain control in the ACx (Cooke et al., 2020). Optogenetic manipulation of PV neurons did indeed modulate the overall gain of ACx principal neurons. However, PV-mediated inhibition was minimally involved in gain adaptations to changes in sound variance and PV interneuron activity itself was not modulated by stimulus contrast. Thus, PV neuron activity modulates the gain of auditory cortical responses but not in a contrast-specific manner. Consistent with these findings, both background noise and PV neuron activation alter response gain of ACx principle neurons but the effects of these manipulations are additive, suggesting they involve independent mechanisms (Christensen et al., 2019). It is important to note, however, that similar levels of contrast gain adaptation are observed in the cortex and subcortical structures in mice (Lohse et al., 2020). The fact that local manipulation of PV neurons in the ACx does not influence contrast gain modulation does not preclude a role for inhibitory circuits in subcortical auditory structures. It will be important to use similar approaches to examine the role of inhibition in subcortical auditory areas as well as the role of other inhibitory interneuron cell types in the cortex.
On the other hand, recent work has demonstrated that PV neuron activity in the ACx is strongly modulated by behavioral state, suggesting that this class of interneurons may be involved in top-down attentional gain modulation. In vivo whole-cell recordings in awake mice found that spontaneous and sensory-evoked responses of both excitatory and PV neurons in the ACx are scaled down when animals transitioned from quiescence to active behavior (Zhou et al., 2014). This behavioral-state dependent gain modulation preserved tuning properties of ACx principle neurons but increased signal-to-noise ratios by relatively suppressing spontaneous activity more than evoked activity. PV neurons are also strongly regulated by motor cortical projections that act to suppress ACx activity associated with internally generated acoustic stimuli during locomotion (Nelson et al., 2013; Schneider et al., 2014). While it remains to be determined how attention engages PV neurons in the auditory system, PV neuron activity in the prefrontal cortex is increased with goal-driven attentional processing and PV neuron activity levels correlated with behavioral performance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task, a common rodent attentional task (Kim et al., 2016). Importantly, PV neurons play in integral role in generation of gamma oscillations in the cortex (Cardin et al., 2009; Sohal et al., 2009) and attentional processing is characterized by increases in gamma activity in sensory regions (Fries et al., 2001; Gregoriou et al., 2015; Ni et al., 2016). Gamma activity has been suggested to modulate the gain of incoming sensory input (Tiesinga et al., 2004, 2008; Börgers et al., 2005; Ni et al., 2016), providing a link between PV neuron function and attentional gain control (Tiesinga et al., 2004, 2008; Börgers et al., 2005; Ni et al., 2016). Thus, while PV neurons in the cortex may not be necessary for bottom-up contrast gain control, they are likely to be important mediators of top-down attentional gain modulation.
SST interneurons have been implicated in a variety of forms of auditory cortical adaptations, including SSA (Kato et al., 2015; Natan et al., 2015, 2017) and forward suppression (Phillips et al., 2017). While a role for SST neurons in contrast gain control or other forms of stimulus statistic adaptation remain to be determined, cortical SST neurons do exhibit properties that make them well-suited for these types of computations compared to PV neurons. For instance, while PV neurons are co-tuned for frequency with neighboring excitatory neurons in the ACx (Moore and Wehr, 2013), SST neurons are involved in a form of network-level lateral inhibition in the cortex (Kato et al., 2017). This lateral inhibitory network could provide a substrate for divisive normalization, a canonical computational strategy used throughout sensory systems to implement gain modulation for invariant sensory representation (Olsen and Wilson, 2008; Carandini and Heeger, 2012). SST neurons are also recruited slightly later than PV or excitatory cells, and SST neurons are more tightly tuned with higher intensity thresholds, suggesting they may contribute to feedback modulation of cortical circuits in response to stimulus history (Li et al., 2014, 2015; Kato et al., 2017).
A third important cortical inhibitory cell-type is the VIP expressing interneuron. VIP neurons represent only 1–2% of cortical neurons but can have broad impact on cortical circuit function, as they target other cortical interneurons in superficial layers (Figure 3B) (Pfeffer et al., 2013). VIP neurons also receive strong neuromodulator input and are highly innervated by intracortical projections from outside of primary sensory areas (Zhang et al., 2014). These properties make VIP neurons well-suited to implement top-down modulation of cortical response gain via disinhibition. Consistent with this notion, VIP neuron activity in visual cortex is upregulated during locomotion and optogenetic activation of VIP neurons increases response gain of visual cortical excitatory neurons, mimicking the effect of locomotion (Fu et al., 2014). Optogenetic stimulation of VIP interneurons during a visual contrast detection task improves performance, while activating either SST or PV interneurons reduces the ability of the mouse to detect lower contrasts (Cone et al., 2019). Similar effects are seen for frequency tuning in the ACx, where VIP activation transiently suppresses SST and, to a lesser extent, PV neuron activity, leading to disinhibition in a subset of tone-responsive neurons and an increase in the gain of the corresponding tuning curves (Pi et al., 2013). Moreover, this study demonstrated that VIP neurons were strongly recruited in response to reinforcement signals during a tone discrimination task. Thus, VIP neurons are particularly well-suited to mediate top-down gain modulations via a disinhibitory cortical microcircuit that is engaged under specific behavioral conditions, and may therefore play an important role in attentional modulation of auditory processing.
Cortico-Fugal Circuits Contributing to Bottom-Up and Top-Down Adaptations
Descending projections from the ACx are far more numerous than ascending projections, and these massive yet poorly understood corticofugal projections target virtually every level of the auditory system, including the MGB, IC, cochlear nucleus (CN), superior olivary complex (SOC) and even the cochlea (Figure 3A) (Winer et al., 2002; Xiao and Suga, 2002; Meltzer and Ryugo, 2006; Llano and Sherman, 2009; Jäger and Kössl, 2016). While we are only beginning to understand how these descending projections influence sound perception, there is strong evidence for top-down regulation of subcortical sound processing via corticofugal projections. As with local cortical inhibitory neurons, corticofugal neurons are a heterogeneous set of cells with diverse properties and projection targets. Early studies revealed that stimulating cortico-thalamic (CT) projecting fibers egocentrically enhances tuning to match the origin of the descending cortical region (Yan and Suga, 1996; Zhang et al., 1997). More recent work has identified the complexity of this pathway in serving to balance the competing demands of increasing neuronal sensitivity for rapid signal detection or dampening excitability to enhance fine-tuned feature discrimination (Happel et al., 2014; Guo et al., 2017; Homma et al., 2017). While activation of CT neurons has been shown to decrease cortical response gain via direct activation of local inhibitory interneurons and/or projections to the TRN (Olsen et al., 2012; Bortone et al., 2014), recent evidence indicates that CT neurons can drive both increases and decreases to cortical gain depending on the timing of their activation relative to ascending input (Crandall et al., 2015; Guo et al., 2017). Cortico-collicular (CC) projecting neurons make-up a distinct class of corticofugal neurons with different projection targets and response properties than CT neurons (Winer et al., 2002; Williamson and Polley, 2019). CC projections to the IC enhance SSA and sharpen frequency tuning, biasing the receptive fields of subcortical auditory neurons toward frequently occurring or highly salient stimuli (Zhang et al., 2005; Bajo et al., 2010; Blackwell et al., 2016).
How do descending auditory projections contribute to stimulus statistic adaptation? While cortical silencing has significant effects on neuronal excitability in the MGB and IC, this manipulation does not affect contrast gain control (Lohse et al., 2020) or mean level adaptation (Robinson et al., 2016) in these structures, indicating that subcortical gain and dynamic range adaptations occur independently of top-down cortical feedback. However, cortical inactivation did interfere with meta-adaptation in the IC (Robinson et al., 2016) and several studies have shown that auditory attentional tasks modulate efferent projections back to the cochlea (Giard et al., 1994; Marian et al., 2018). Thus, it has been proposed that corticofugal projections play an important role in providing contextual information to upstream auditory areas that is necessary for interpreting ambiguous signals, such as those encountered in complex or noisy acoustic environments (Asilador and Llano, 2021). Indeed, electrical stimulation of ACx in humans was shown to modulate subcortical auditory pathways and enhance speech recognition under challenging conditions (de Boer and Thornton, 2008; Srinivasan et al., 2012; Shastri et al., 2014). Together, these studies suggest that descending corticofugal projections likely play an important role in top-down modulation of auditory gain in response to changes in behavioral state or context but may not be necessary for adaption to sound statistics observed in subcortical auditory areas.
Within this section we have provided a summary of our current understanding of cellular and circuit mechanisms that contribute to bottom-up and top-down adaptation throughout the auditory system. We have focused on both local synaptic and circuit interactions between excitatory and inhibitory neurons as well long-range connections between auditory regions that play essentials roles in adaptive sound processing. The role of specific interneuron subclasses and their specialized contributions to subcortical and cortical microcircuits remains an active area of interest in sensory processing. In particular, the contribution of specific interneuron classes to bottom-up stimulus statistic adaptation and top-down task-dependent receptive-field plasticity remains to be fully elucidated. While recent work has indicated that PV neurons are unlikely to mediate contrast gain control in the ACx, the role of other interneuron subtypes is less well understood. Recent computational studies have suggested that top-down inhibitory neurons that disinhibit bottom-up cortical circuits, similar to the VIP neuron circuit motif described above, can explain the attentional effects of auditory tuning properties (Chou and Sen, 2021). However, this model remains to be tested experimentally. Furthermore, much less is known about the role of interneurons subcortically as it relates to whether the behavioral state modulations that are actively present in cortex contribute to adaptation and meta-adaptation observed in ascending subcortical structures. Finally, a major challenge for the field going forward is how to best isolate the contributions of these cell classes due to their interconnected nature and the presence of disinhibitory effects that are difficult to disentangle while using traditional methods of manipulation.
Hearing Loss and Hearing in Noisy Environments
One of most prominent and disabling disruptions associated with hearing loss is the inability to hear in noisy environments. Difficulties hearing in noise could be due to a general reduction in audibility and degraded encoding of incoming sound input as a consequence of hearing loss. However, studies have shown that speech perception and hearing in noise difficulties are present even when cochlear amplification is accounted for Peters et al. (1998); Johannesen et al. (2016). Moreover, hearing in noise difficulties often occur even in the absence of overt audiometric threshold shifts (Kraus et al., 2000; Zeng et al., 2005). Hearing loss fundamentally alters the pattern and level of incoming sound, and thus will greatly affect the stimulus statistics the auditory system is exposed to. Indeed, hearing loss is often associated with central auditory gain enhancement in attempts to preserve sound detection levels (Auerbach et al., 2014; Chambers et al., 2016; Salvi et al., 2016). Thus, it is possible that compensatory gain changes with pathological changes to auditory input and adaptations to more physiological changes to sound statistics engage overlapping mechanisms and may interfere with each other. Hearing loss may also interfere with attentional mechanisms important for sound perception. Selective attention performs best when auditory streams can be segregated based on select features. Degradation of spectrotemporal structure impairs adaptation accuracy and reduces the efficiency of anticipated noise. Inaccuracies build at multiple levels, delaying and reducing the efficiency with which attention groups relevant objects. In complex scenes, where background noise statistics and the spectrotemporal features of the target can rapidly fluctuate, hearing impaired listeners have more difficulty forming perceptual objects from their environments (Shinn-Cunningham and Best, 2008). Under these conditions, the benefits of knowing what features to direct attention to are degraded and reduce the capacity for cognitive control to benefit the listener. Thus, hearing loss not only affects bottom-up gain adaptations, but these changes are compounded by reducing the capacity for top-down attentional mechanisms to help contribute to auditory scene analysis. In the preceding sections, we have reviewed the primary bottom-up and top-down adaptations that contribute to auditory scene analysis, as well as the potential mechanisms underlying their generation. In this section we will discuss how our understanding of these adaptive coding strategies in normal hearing can help us understand the often devastating effects of hearing loss on listening in complex acoustic environments.
Hidden Hearing Loss
Sensorineural hearing loss is often associated with overt damage to sensory hair cells, resulting in elevated sound detection thresholds (Figures 4Ai,ii,iii) (Schmiedt, 1984; Cunningham and Tucci, 2017). Permanent threshold shifts in the clinical pure tone audiogram have thus traditionally been a key criterion for diagnosing hearing loss (Simel et al., 2016). However, many individuals with clinically normal audiometric thresholds nonetheless report significant auditory perceptual disruptions, including temporal processing deficits, impaired speech perception, and most prominently, difficulties hearing in noisy environments (Starr et al., 1996; Zeng et al., 2005; Cunningham and Tucci, 2017; Ralli et al., 2019). Recent evidence from animal models has suggested that cochlear neuronal degeneration can occur even without overt hair cell damage or permanent threshold shifts (Kujawa and Liberman, 2009). This so called “hidden hearing loss” (HHL), due to the fact that this dysfunction is not revealed by standard audiometric tests, is estimated to occur in ∼12–15% of individuals (Tremblay et al., 2015; Spankovich et al., 2018). HHL is likely a key contributor to difficulties hearing in noise in the absence of clinically diagnosed hearing loss (Plack et al., 2014; Ralli et al., 2019) and has also been suggested to contribute to auditory perceptual disorders like tinnitus and hyperacusis that are often associated with hearing impairment (Schaette and McAlpine, 2011; Hickox and Liberman, 2014). Below we discuss the consequences of cochlear degeneration on peripheral and central auditory function, with a particular focus on how central adaptations to this form of hearing loss may interfere with our ability to compensate for noisy environments.
Cochlear Synaptopathy
While there are three times as many outer hair cells (OHC) than inner hair cells (IHC), virtually all (∼95%) afferent signals from the cochlea are relayed to the central auditory system via IHCs synapsing on type 1 spiral ganglion neurons (SGNs) (Figure 4Ai) (Spoendlin, 1972). Accumulating evidence suggests that the synapses between IHCs and type I SGNs, whose axons comprise the AN tract, appear to be most vulnerable to noise- or age-related hearing loss. Indeed, animal studies have found that there is a marked reduction of IHC-Type 1 SGN synaptic contacts following exposure to ototoxic drugs, environmental noise, or aging, and this synaptopathy often proceeds overt hair cell damage (Figure 4Aii) (Liberman and Kujawa, 2017; Wu et al., 2019; Kohrman et al., 2020). Remarkably, it has been shown that animals with damage restricted to the IHC-type I SGN complex maintain normal hearing thresholds in quiet despite severely reduced afferent drive to the central auditory system (Figure 4B) (Lobarinas et al., 2013; Chambers et al., 2016). However, these same animals perform much poorer than control animals when challenged with a more difficult task, such as tone detection in background noise, gap-in-noise detection, or a remote masking paradigm (Figure 4C) (Salvi et al., 2016; Lobarinas et al., 2020; Resnik and Polley, 2021). These results indicate that cochlear degeneration could contribute to real-world listening difficulties even in the absence of threshold shifts in the clinical audiogram. It should be noted that AN fiber loss following kainic acid treatment in budgerigars, a common avian model of hearing (Dooling and Popper, 2000), does not result in deficits to tone-in-noise detection (Henry and Abrams, 2021), suggesting there may be species-specific effects of cochlear degeneration and/or central adaptation to hearing loss.
The difference in detection between quiet and noisy conditions following cochlear degeneration may be due in part to peripheral mechanisms. Spared AN fibers maintain normal thresholds and tuning following IHC or SGN degeneration and detecting tones in quiet may only require a small fraction of surviving peripheral afferents (Wang et al., 1997; Kujawa and Liberman, 2009; Salvi et al., 2016). Interestingly, the AN fibers most susceptible to noise-induced synaptopathy are low and medium SR fibers, which have higher thresholds and are thought to be useful for hearing in noisy environments (Wang et al., 1997; Furman et al., 2013). However, substantial recovery of sound detection thresholds is seen even with ototoxic treatments that cause near complete loss (∼95%) of IHC-SGN synapses (Chambers et al., 2016) or selective lesion of IHCs, which are similarly contacted by all subsets of AN fibers (Lobarinas et al., 2013; Salvi et al., 2016). In fact, IHC lesions were actually shown to result in an increased proportion of low SR nerves relative to medium and high, opposite to what is observed with noise-induced synaptopathy (Salvi et al., 2016). Thus, while peripheral mechanisms certainly contribute to perceptual alterations associated with cochlear degeneration, there is growing awareness that adaptations in the central auditory system are essential for fully understanding the perceptual consequences of cochlear hearing impairment.
Central Gain Enhancement Following Hearing Loss
Loss of afferent drive to the central auditory system— be it due to ototoxic drugs, sensorineural hearing loss, or acoustic deprivation— have been shown to result in a compensatory increase in neuronal gain in the central auditory system, a phenomenon termed central gain enhancement (Figure 5A) (Gerken et al., 1984; Syka, 2002; Auerbach et al., 2014; Chambers et al., 2016; Salvi et al., 2016). Gain increases due to sensorineural hearing loss have been observed at every level of the auditory system. AN synapses onto CN bushy cells have been shown to homeostatically adapt their pre-synaptic strength in response to changes in acoustic input (Zhuang et al., 2020), indicating that dynamic gain adaptations are in place at the earliest points of the central auditory system. Indeed, neuronal gain increases in response to hearing loss have been observed in several auditory brainstem nuclei, albeit in a cell-type specific and time-restricted manner (Boettcher and Salvi, 1993; Brozoski et al., 2002; Cai et al., 2009). However, in studies where concurrent recordings from multiple levels of the auditory system were performed, it has been consistently found that gain changes with drug or noise-induced hearing loss are more rapid and more complete in the ACx compared to subcortical structures like in the IC or CN (Figure 5A) (Popelar et al., 1987; Syka et al., 1994; Auerbach et al., 2014; Chambers et al., 2016; Salvi et al., 2016). Thus, like more rapid adaptations to stimulus statistics, sustained gain increases following pathological changes to sound input progressively develop through the ascending auditory system, with the most complete recovery being observed at the level of the ACx (Figure 5A). Central gain enhancement following cochlear degeneration has also been shown to result in enhanced sound-evoked activity in corticofugal projections, suggesting that in addition to changes along the ascending auditory pathway, descending projections are also altered in response to hearing loss (Asokan et al., 2018).
What are the perceptual consequences of central gain enhancement? There is growing evidence that central gain enhancement is associated with restoration of hearing thresholds in quiet (Figure 4B). Parallel behavioral and neurophysiological studies in mice given round window application of the Na+/K+ ATPase pump inhibitor ouabain, which selectively destroys type-I SGNs, or chinchillas treated with the anti-cancer agent carboplatin, which selectively destroys IHCs in these animals, have shown that recovery in tone detection thresholds in quiet (Figure 4B) corresponds with recovery of intensity-response functions in ACx (Figure 5A) (Chambers et al., 2016; Salvi et al., 2016). However, this compensatory plasticity is unable to restore all aspects of auditory processing that are disrupted by sensorineural hearing loss and, in fact, may actively contribute a range of auditory perceptual deficits associated with hearing impairment as well. For instance, while central gain enhancement restores mean firing rates and intensity coding in the ACx, this adaptation cannot compensate for degradation of temporal processing with hearing loss, which depends on specialized subcortical circuits optimized for fast time scales (Chambers et al., 2016). Interestingly, gain increases in the ACx following hearing loss can often overshoot baseline levels, resulting in sound-evoked hyperactivity (Figure 5A). This excessive increase in central gain may contribute to the development of hyperacusis, a sound intolerance disorder often associated with hearing loss (Zeng, 2013; Auerbach et al., 2014; Pienkowski et al., 2014). Indeed, cortical gain increases are associated with maladaptive changes to loudness perception following ototoxic (Auerbach et al., 2018) or noise-induced hearing loss (Figure 4D) (Radziwon et al., 2019). These results highlight the perceptual trade-offs that inevitably arise when sensory systems must adapt their neural representations to changes in the environment.
Recent evidence suggests the intriguing notion that central gain changes that support restoration of sound processing in quiet backgrounds may actively interfere with auditory circuit mechanisms that normally support adaptation to background noise. While thresholds in quiet are remarkably normal in animals with severe cochlear degeneration, performance in hearing damaged animals was much worse when challenged with a tone-in-noise detection task (Figure 4C) (Lobarinas et al., 2013; Salvi et al., 2016; Resnik and Polley, 2021). Examination of mean-level adaptation in the IC of mice given a noise exposure that produces HHL and central gain increases found significant impairment in adaptive coding for loud environments (Bakay et al., 2018). While both dynamic range and gain adaptations were still observed in the IC of noise-exposed animals, threshold shifts were significantly reduced compared to controls and intensity-response functions were less informative about the sound level distribution the animals were exposed to, particularly for loud environments (Figure 5B). This impairment in adaptive coding could contribute to difficulties hearing in noisy environment. Indeed, a follow-up study found that noise-induced HHL in gerbils, which have a hearing range more similar to humans than mice, reduced the ability of IC neurons to discriminate between speech tokens presented in background noise at high sound intensities (75 dB SPL), although discriminability at moderate sound intensities (60 dB SPL) was surprisingly improved (Monaghan et al., 2020). A phenomenological model of cochlear synaptopathy that selectively impairs high threshold, low-SR AN fibers and result in enhanced central gain could reproduce this pattern of improved discrimination at moderate levels but decreased performance at high levels, providing a link between peripheral pathology and central plasticity.
The mechanisms of central gain enhancement remain to be completely elucidated; however, several lines of evidence suggest that a combination of increased excitatory neuronal function and, in particular, decreased inhibitory function contribute to this experience-dependent plasticity (Yang et al., 2011; Auerbach et al., 2014; Resnik and Polley, 2017; Balaram et al., 2019). Recent studies using optogenetic manipulation (Resnik and Polley, 2017) or chronic two-photon calcium imaging of genetically labeled PV inhibitory neuron populations in the ACx (Resnik and Polley, 2021) have demonstrated that central gain changes and perceptual restoration of detection thresholds are correlated with decreased PV-mediated inhibition in the ACx. Intriguingly, these recent studies have indicated that alterations to cortical E/I balance that help restore hearing thresholds in quiet may be actively interfering with hearing in noise. Chronic two photon imaging of putative excitatory and PV inhibitory neuronal populations in the ACx found that cochlear degeneration was associated with distinct forms of plasticity in cortical excitatory and inhibitory neurons, with near complete recovery in sound-evoked responses for cortical excitatory neurons but a persistent decrease in PV neuron activity (Figure 5Ci) (Resnik and Polley, 2021). The combined effect of these changes was an increase in cortical gain that corresponded with recovery of tone detection in quiet, but also an imbalance in spontaneous activity rates between excitatory and PV inhibitory neurons that led to random surges of correlated activity that impaired tone-detection in background noise. Interestingly, combined behavioral and imaging sessions demonstrated that impairments to tone-in-noise detection following cochlear degeneration was not only the result of diminished evoked activity to target sounds but increased sensitivity to background noise, so that signal-to-noise ratios were decreased for foreground sounds (Figure 5Cii). Indeed, perceptual misses in noise were better predicted by levels of neural synchronization during the pre-stimulus period than the size of stimulus-evoked responses (Resnik and Polley, 2021). Thus, diminished PV neuron-mediated inhibition in ACx following hearing loss may be responsible for both adaptive recovery of sound detection in quiet as well as impaired adaption to background noise that disrupts perception in more challenging conditions. This degraded ability to adapt to noisy conditions could reflect impairments in bottom-up contrast gain adaptation, altered top-down modulation of cortical inhibitory circuits that act to reduce spontaneous activity during behavioral engagement, or a combination of the two.
Hearing Loss and Top-Down Cognition
In addition to having sizeable impact on bottom-up sound processing and adaptation, hearing loss is also likely to affect top-down regulation of sound feature encoding. There is a well-characterized relationship between hearing loss and cognitive decline, although the directionality and mechanisms are strongly debated (Lin et al., 2013). It is not clear if cognitive decline or age-related hearing loss precede one another or if any such effects would even be generalizable more broadly across individuals. We will not review this debate here, except to acknowledge that cognitive decline impacts cortical circuits essential for attentional sound processing. As discussed previously, top-down circuits are critical for segregating attended streams in complex environments and reductions in cognitive capacity can alter performance in auditory scene analysis. Cognitive decline is a near universal phenomenon associated with normal aging with decline levels highly correlated with age (Park et al., 2003). Older adults are more influenced by the presence of sensory perceptual conflicts during tasks of focused attention and this coincides with reduced measures of conflict in fronto-parietal ERP markers associated with greater attentional control (Passow et al., 2014). One potential source for this reduction in performance is diminished contextual adaptation of sound level statistics within the listening environment. Herrmann et al. (2018) found that older listeners exposed to a sound distribution with two-levels showed similar neural response magnitudes but reduced capacity for sensory adaptation relative to young listeners. This finding suggests reduced capacity for adaptation to the statistical properties of the context and impaired ability to filter unattended auditory streams. While much more work is needed to elucidate the relationship between hearing loss, cognitive decline, and auditory scene analysis, current evidence suggests that hearing impairments that arise with age are likely the combined effect of disruptions to bottom-up sound processing and top-down auditory attentional regulation.
Conclusion
We live in a world full of sounds. The auditory system employs a variety of adaptive coding strategies (Figures 1, 2) to navigate this cacophonous environment, including: compensatory dynamic range and gain adaptations to incoming stimulus statistics in order to build level and contrast invariant tuning of sound features under different background conditions (Rabinowitz et al., 2013); adaptive spatial tuning for localizing and focusing on specific sound sources to aid in the segregation of auditory streams in the presence of complex sound environment (Reed et al., 2020); and top-down attentional mechanisms that modulate auditory response and receptive field properties to selectively amplify behaviorally relevant sound features (Fritz et al., 2005b). These adaptations are observed throughout the ascending and descending auditory hierarchy to various degrees and can be both rapid, as seen in task-engaged subjects in perceptual decision-making paradigms, as well as sustained, as seen with long-term changes to auditory input associated with hearing loss.
A number of synaptic and circuit mechanisms are used to implement adaptive coding strategies in the auditory system (Figure 3), including: use-dependent changes in synaptic transmission; regulation of E/I balance to modulate response gain and minimize the influence of background noise; and top-down disinhibitory circuit motifs that can selectively modify sound encoding in response to changes in behavioral state. Interestingly, it appears that bottom-up and top-down gain changes are mediated by distinct mechanisms, suggesting the individual contributions of these different forms of adaptation are at a minimum additive or perhaps even work synergistically to enhance performance in challenging auditory scenes. Future work must investigate this possibility further by comparing neurophysiological adaptations to sound statistics in passively listening versus task-engaged animals in combination with in vivo manipulation of putative generators of bottom-up and top-down adaptations. It is also possible that different forms of auditory plasticity can interfere with each other, as may be the case with sensorineural hearing loss.
Listening in noisy environments poses additional challenges for those with hearing loss (Figure 4). This difficulty is due in part to degraded encoding of incoming stimuli, leading to impoverished representation of spectrotemporal sound features and disrupted ability to segregate sound sources based on select features. However, recent evidence suggests that compensatory plasticity mechanisms that help restore rapid signal detection following loss of afferent drive may actively interfere with the auditory system’s ability to adapt to more challenging listening conditions as well. For instance, increased central auditory excitability following hearing loss allows for the amplification of diminished sound-driven input from the periphery, but it may also make the auditory system more sensitive to the influence of background sounds and impair adaptation to noisy environments (Figure 5). A future challenge will be to identify whether central gain enhancement seen with hearing loss reflects bottom-up gain adaptations in response to changes in sound level statistics, reduced top-down modulation of cortical inhibitory circuits that coincide with disruptions in attentional mechanisms, or some interaction between these components.
In summary, the studies reviewed here indicate that the auditory system is highly adaptive, modulating its response properties to best fit the current environmental and/or behavioral goals. These adaptations appear to be crucial for optimal representation of sounds under diverse conditions and for listening in complex auditory environments. Further understanding of the mechanisms mediating bottom-up and top-down adaptations to sound processing, as well as the interaction between them, is crucial for harnessing the auditory system’s vast potential to compensate for difficult listening conditions, particularly following sensorineural hearing loss.
Author Contributions
BA and HG wrote the manuscript together and approved of the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
- Abbott L. F., Varela J. A., Sen K., Nelson S. B. (1997). Synaptic depression and cortical gain control. Science 275 220–224. 10.1126/science.275.5297.221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahveninen J., Jääskeläinen I. P., Raij T., Bonmassar G., Devore S., Hämäläinen M., et al. (2006). Task-modulated “what” and “where” pathways in human auditory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103 14608–14613. 10.1073/pnas.0510480103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aizenberg M., Mwilambwe-Tshilobo L., Briguglio J. J., Natan R. G., Geffen M. N. (2015). Bidirectional regulation of innate and learned behaviors that rely on frequency discrimination by cortical inhibitory neurons. PLoS Biol. 13:e1002308. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Angeloni C. F., Młynarski W., Piasini E., Williams A. M., Wood K. C., Garami L., et al. (2021). Cortical efficient coding dynamics shape behavioral performance. bioRxiv [Preprint] 10.1101/2021.08.11.455845 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Angeloni C., Geffen M. N. (2018). Contextual modulation of sound processing in the auditory cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 49 8–15. 10.1016/j.conb.2017.10.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asilador A., Llano D. A. (2021). Top-down inference in the auditory system: potential roles for corticofugal projections. Front. Neural Circuits 14:89. 10.3389/fncir.2020.615259 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asokan M. M., Williamson R. S., Hancock K. E., Polley D. B. (2018). Sensory overamplification in layer 5 auditory corticofugal projection neurons following cochlear nerve synaptic damage. Nat. Commun. 9:2468. 10.1038/s41467-018-04852-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atallah B. V., Bruns W., Carandini M., Scanziani M. (2012). Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons linearly transform cortical responses to visual stimuli. Neuron 73 159–170. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atiani S., David S. V., Elgueda D., Locastro M., Radtke-Schuller S., Shamma S. A., et al. (2014). Emergent selectivity for task-relevant stimuli in higher-order auditory cortex. Neuron 82 486–499. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auerbach B. D., Radziwon K., Salvi R. (2018). Testing the central gain model: loudness growth correlates with central auditory gain enhancement in a rodent model of hyperacusis. Neuroscience 407 93–107. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.09.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auerbach B. D., Rodrigues P. V., Salvi R. J. (2014). Central gain control in tinnitus and hyperacusis. Front. Neurol. 5:206. 10.3389/fneur.2014.00206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baccus S. A. (2006). From a whisper to a roar: adaptation to the mean and variance of naturalistic sounds. Neuron 51 682–684. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bajo V. M., Nodal F. R., Moore D. R., King A. J. (2010). The descending corticocollicular pathway mediates learning-induced auditory plasticity. Nat. Neurosci. 13 253–260. 10.1038/nn.2466 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakay W. M. H., Anderson L. A., Garcia-Lazaro J. A., McAlpine D., Schaette R. (2018). Hidden hearing loss selectively impairs neural adaptation to loud sound environments. Nat. Commun. 9:4298. 10.1038/s41467-018-06777-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balaram P., Hackett T. A., Polley D. B. (2019). Synergistic transcriptional changes in AMPA and GABAA receptor genes support compensatory plasticity following unilateral hearing loss. Neuroscience 407 108–119. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.08.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banitt Y., Martin K. A. C., Segev I. (2007). A biologically realistic model of contrast invariant orientation tuning by thalamocortical synaptic depression. J. Neurosci. 27 10230–10239. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1640-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbour D. L. (2011). Intensity-invariant coding in the auditory system. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35 2064–2072. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett C., Arroyo S., Hestrin S. (2014). Controlling brain states. Neuron 83 260–261. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bharadwaj H. M., Lee A. K. C., Shinn-Cunningham B. G. (2014). Measuring auditory selective attention using frequency tagging. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:6. 10.3389/fnint.2014.00006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bidet-Caulet A., Fischer C., Besle J., Aguera P.-E., Giard M.-H., Bertrand O. (2007). Effects of selective attention on the electrophysiological representation of concurrent sounds in the human auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 27 9252–9261. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1402-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackwell J. M., Taillefumier T. O., Natan R. G., Carruthers I. M., Magnasco M. O., Geffen M. N. (2016). Stable encoding of sounds over a broad range of statistical parameters in the auditory cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 43 751–764. 10.1111/ejn.13144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blundon J. A., Bayazitov I. T., Zakharenko S. S. (2011). Presynaptic gating of postsynaptically expressed plasticity at mature thalamocortical synapses. J. Neurosci. 31 16012–16025. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3281-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boettcher F. A., Salvi R. J. (1993). Functional changes in the ventral cochlear nucleus following acute acoustic overstimulation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 94 2123–2134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Börgers C., Epstein S., Kopell N. J. (2005). Background gamma rhythmicity and attention in cortical local circuits: a computational study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102 7002–7007. 10.1073/pnas.0502366102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bortone D. S., Olsen S. R., Scanziani M. (2014). Translaminar inhibitory cells recruited by layer 6 corticothalamic neurons suppress visual cortex. Neuron 82 474–485. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bregman A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA: A Bradford Book. [Google Scholar]
- Bregman A. S. (2008). “3.49 - Auditory scene analysis,” in The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference, eds Masland R. H., Albright T. D., Albright T. D., Masland R. H., Dallos P., Oertel D., et al. (New York, NY: Academic Press; ), 861–870. 10.1016/B978-012370880-9.00009-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brozoski T. J., Bauer C. A., Caspary D. M. (2002). Elevated fusiform cell activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of chinchillas with psychophysical evidence of tinnitus. J. Neurosci. 22 2383–2390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cai S., Ma W.-L. D., Young E. D. (2009). Encoding intensity in ventral cochlear nucleus following acoustic trauma: implications for loudness recruitment. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 10 5–22. 10.1007/s10162-008-0142-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carandini M., Heeger D. J. (2012). Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13 51–62. 10.1038/nrn3136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carandini M., Heeger D. J., Senn W. (2002). A synaptic explanation of suppression in visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 22 10053–10065. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-22-10053.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carcea I., Froemke R. C. (2013). “Chapter 3 - Cortical Plasticity, excitatory–inhibitory balance, and sensory perception,” in Progress in Brain Research, eds Michael M. N., Merzenich M., Thomas M. V. V. (Amsterdam: Elsevier; ), 65–90. 10.1016/B978-0-444-63327-9.00003-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carcea I., Insanally M. N., Froemke R. C. (2017). Dynamics of auditory cortical activity during behavioural engagement and auditory perception. Nat. Commun. 8:14412. 10.1038/ncomms14412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin J. A., Carlen M., Meletis K., Knoblich U., Zhang F., Deisseroth K., et al. (2009). Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature 459 663–667. 10.1038/nature08002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrasco M. (2011). Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vision Res. 51 1484–1525. 10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carruthers I. M., Laplagne D. A., Jaegle A., Briguglio J. J., Mwilambwe-Tshilobo L., Natan R. G., et al. (2015). Emergence of invariant representation of vocalizations in the auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 114 2726–2740. 10.1152/jn.00095.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carruthers I. M., Natan R. G., Geffen M. N. (2013). Encoding of ultrasonic vocalizations in the auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 109 1912–1927. 10.1152/jn.00483.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers A. R., Resnik J., Yuan Y., Whitton J. P., Edge A. S., Liberman M. C., et al. (2016). Central gain restores auditory processing following near-complete cochlear denervation. Neuron 89 867–879. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chance F. S., Abbott L. F., Reyes A. D. (2002). Gain modulation from background synaptic input. Neuron 35 773–782. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00820-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapuis G. A., Chadderton P. T. (2018). Using temporal expectation to assess auditory streaming in mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:205. 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chechik G., Nelken I. (2012). Auditory abstraction from spectro-temporal features to coding auditory entities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109 18968–18973. 10.1073/pnas.1111242109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cherry E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 25 975–979. 10.1121/1.1907229 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chou K. F., Sen K. (2021). AIM: a network model of attention in auditory cortex. PLoS Comput. Biol. 17:e1009356. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009356 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen R. K., Lindén H., Nakamura M., Barkat T. R. (2019). White noise background improves tone discrimination by suppressing cortical tuning curves. Cell Rep. 29 2041–2053.e4. 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.10.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cone J. J., Scantlen M. D., Histed M. H., Maunsell J. H. R. (2019). Different inhibitory interneuron cell classes make distinct contributions to visual contrast perception. eNeuro 6:ENEURO.0337-18.2019. 10.1523/ENEURO.0337-18.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooke J. E., Kahn M. C., Mann E. O., King A. J., Schnupp J. W. H., Willmore B. D. B. (2020). Contrast gain control occurs independently of both parvalbumin-positive interneuron activity and shunting inhibition in auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 123 1536–1551. 10.1152/jn.00587.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costalupes J. A., Young E. D., Gibson D. J. (1984). Effects of continuous noise backgrounds on rate response of auditory nerve fibers in cat. J. Neurophysiol. 51 1326–1344. 10.1152/jn.1984.51.6.1326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crandall S. R., Cruikshank S. J., Connors B. W. (2015). A corticothalamic switch: controlling the thalamus with dynamic synapses. Neuron 86 768–782. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham L. L., Tucci D. L. (2017). Hearing loss in adults. N. Engl. J. Med. 377 2465–2473. 10.1056/NEJMra1616601 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahmen J. C., Keating P., Nodal F. R., Schulz A. L., King A. J. (2010). Adaptation to stimulus statistics in the perception and neural representation of auditory space. Neuron 66 937–948. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davenport W. B. (1952). An experimental study of speech-wave probability distributions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 24 390–399. 10.1121/1.1906909 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- David S. V., Fritz J. B., Shamma S. A. (2012). Task reward structure shapes rapid receptive field plasticity in auditory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109 2144–2149. 10.1073/pnas.1117717109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Boer J., Thornton A. R. D. (2008). Neural correlates of perceptual learning in the auditory brainstem: efferent activity predicts and reflects improvement at a speech-in-noise discrimination task. J. Neurosci. 28 4929–4937. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0902-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dean I., Harper N. S., McAlpine D. (2005). Neural population coding of sound level adapts to stimulus statistics. Nat. Neurosci. 8 1684–1689. 10.1038/nn1541 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dean I., Robinson B. L., Harper N. S., McAlpine D. (2008). Rapid neural adaptation to sound level statistics. J. Neurosci. 28 6430–6438. 10.1523/jneurosci.0470-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deng Y., Choi I., Shinn-Cunningham B. (2020). Topographic specificity of alpha power during auditory spatial attention. Neuroimage 207:116360. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ding N., Simon J. Z. (2013). Adaptive temporal encoding leads to a background-insensitive cortical representation of speech. J. Neurosci. 33 5728–5735. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5297-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dooling R. J., Popper A. N. (2000). Comparative Hearing: Birds and Reptiles. New York, NY: Springer, 10.1007/978-1-4612-1182-2_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Downer J. D., Rapone B., Verhein J., O’Connor K. N., Sutter M. L. (2017). Feature-selective attention adaptively shifts noise correlations in primary auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 37 5378–5392. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3169-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehret G., Riecke S. (2002). Mice and humans perceive multiharmonic communication sounds in the same way. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 479–482. 10.1073/pnas.012361999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Endepols H., Feng A. S., Gerhardt H. C., Schul J., Walkowiak W. (2003). Roles of the auditory midbrain and thalamus in selective phonotaxis in female gray treefrogs (Hyla versicolor). Behav. Brain Res. 145 63–77. 10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00098-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans E. F. (1972). The frequency response and other properties of single fibres in the guinea-pig cochlear nerve. J. Physiol. 226 263–287. 10.1113/jphysiol.1972.sp009984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson K. A., Cardin J. A. (2020). Mechanisms underlying gain modulation in the cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 21 80–92. 10.1038/s41583-019-0253-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finlayson P. G., Adam T. J. (1997). Excitatory and inhibitory response adaptation in the superior olive complex affects binaural acoustic processing. Hear. Res. 103 1–18. 10.1016/S0378-5955(96)00158-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finlayson P. G., Caspary D. M. (1989). Synaptic potentials of chinchilla lateral superior olivary neurons. Hear. Res. 38 221–228. 10.1016/0378-5955(89)90067-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finn I. M., Priebe N. J., Ferster D. (2007). The emergence of contrast-invariant orientation tuning in simple cells of cat visual cortex. Neuron 54 137–152. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis N. A., Winkowski D. E., Sheikhattar A., Armengol K., Babadi B., Kanold P. O. (2018). Small networks encode decision-making in primary auditory cortex. Neuron 97 885–897.e6. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries P., Reynolds J. H., Rorie A. E., Desimone R. (2001). Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291 1560–1563. 10.1126/science.1055465 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz J. B., David S. V., Radtke-Schuller S., Yin P., Shamma S. A. (2010). Adaptive, behaviorally gated, persistent encoding of task-relevant auditory information in ferret frontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 13 1011–1019. 10.1038/nn.2598 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz J. B., Elhilali M., Shamma S. A. (2005a). Differential dynamic plasticity of A1 receptive fields during multiple spectral tasks. J. Neurosci. 25 7623–7635. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1318-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz J. B., Elhilali M., David S. V., Shamma S. A. (2007). Does attention play a role in dynamic receptive field adaptation to changing acoustic salience in A1? Hear. Res. 229 186–203. 10.1016/j.heares.2007.01.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz J., Elhilali M., Shamma S. (2005b). Active listening: task-dependent plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex. Hear. Res. 206 159–176. 10.1016/j.heares.2005.01.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz J., Shamma S., Elhilali M., Klein D. (2003). Rapid task-related plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 6 1216–1223. 10.1038/nn1141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Froemke R. C., Carcea I., Barker A. J., Yuan K., Seybold B. A., Martins A. R. O., et al. (2013). Long-term modification of cortical synapses improves sensory perception. Nat. Neurosci. 16 79–88. 10.1038/nn.3274 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fu Y., Tucciarone J. M., Espinosa J. S., Sheng N., Darcy D. P., Nicoll R. A., et al. (2014). A cortical circuit for gain control by behavioral state. Cell 156 1139–1152. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.01.050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furman A. C., Kujawa S. G., Liberman M. C. (2013). Noise-induced cochlear neuropathy is selective for fibers with low spontaneous rates. J. Neurophysiol. 110 577–586. 10.1152/jn.00164.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerken G. M., Saunders S. S., Paul R. E. (1984). Hypersensitivity to electrical-stimulation of auditory nuclei follows hearing-loss in cats. Hear. Res. 13 249–259. 10.1016/0378-5955(84)90078-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giard M.-H., Collet L., Bouchet P., Pernier J. (1994). Auditory selective attention in the human cochlea. Brain Res. 633 353–356. 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91561-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goutman J. D. (2017). Mechanisms of synaptic depression at the hair cell ribbon synapse that support auditory nerve function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114 9719–9724. 10.1073/pnas.1706160114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregoriou G. G., Paneri S., Sapountzis P. (2015). Oscillatory synchrony as a mechanism of attentional processing. Brain Res. 1626 165–182. 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grothe B., Koch U. (2011). Dynamics of binaural processing in the mammalian sound localization pathway – The role of GABAB receptors. Hear. Res. 279 43–50. 10.1016/j.heares.2011.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grothe B., Pecka M., McAlpine D. (2010). Mechanisms of sound localization in mammals. Physiol. Rev. 90 983–1012. 10.1152/physrev.00026.2009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo W., Clause A. R., Barth-Maron A., Polley D. B. (2017). A corticothalamic circuit for dynamic switching between feature detection and discrimination. Neuron 95 180–194.e5. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haider B., Duque A., Hasenstaub A. R., McCormick D. A. (2006). Neocortical network activity in vivo is generated through a dynamic balance of excitation and inhibition. J. Neurosci. 26, 4535–4545. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5297-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haider B., Häusser M., Carandini M. (2013). Inhibition dominates sensory responses in the awake cortex. Nature 493 97–100. 10.1038/nature11665 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton L. S., Sohl-Dickstein J., Huth A. G., Carels V. M., Deisseroth K., Bao S. (2013). Optogenetic activation of an inhibitory network enhances feedforward functional connectivity in auditory cortex. Neuron 80 1066–1076. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Happel M. F. K., Deliano M., Handschuh J., Ohl F. W. (2014). Dopamine-modulated recurrent corticoefferent feedback in primary sensory cortex promotes detection of behaviorally relevant stimuli. J. Neurosci. 34 1234–1247. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1990-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris K. D., Thiele A. (2011). Cortical state and attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12 509–523. 10.1038/nrn3084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry K. S., Abrams K. S. (2021). Normal tone-in-noise sensitivity in trained budgerigars despite substantial auditory-nerve injury: no evidence of hidden hearing loss. J. Neurosci. 41 118–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrmann B., Augereau T., Johnsrude I. S. (2020). Neural responses and perceptual sensitivity to sound depend on sound-level statistics. Sci. Rep. 10:9571. 10.1038/s41598-020-66715-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrmann B., Maess B., Johnsrude I. S. (2018). Aging affects adaptation to sound-level statistics in human auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 38, 1989–1999. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1489-17.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrmann B., Schlichting N., Obleser J. (2014). Dynamic range adaptation to spectral stimulus statistics in human auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 34 327–331. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3974-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hickok G., Poeppel D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8 393–402. 10.1038/nrn2113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hickox A. E., Liberman M. C. (2014). Is noise-induced cochlear neuropathy key to the generation of hyperacusis or tinnitus? J. Neurophysiol. 111 552–564. 10.1152/jn.00184.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holdgraf C. R., de Heer W., Pasley B., Rieger J., Crone N., Lin J. J., et al. (2016). Rapid tuning shifts in human auditory cortex enhance speech intelligibility. Nat. Commun. 7:13654. 10.1038/ncomms13654 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Homma N. Y., Happel M. F. K., Nodal F. R., Ohl F. W., King A. J., Bajo V. M. (2017). A role for auditory corticothalamic feedback in the perception of complex sounds. J. Neurosci. 37 6149–6161. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0397-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Homma N. Y., Hullett P. W., Atencio C. A., Schreiner C. E. (2020). Auditory cortical plasticity dependent on environmental noise statistics. Cell Rep. 30 4445–4458.e5. 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.03.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hulse S. H., MacDougall-Shackleton S. A., Wisniewski A. B. (1997). Auditory scene analysis by songbirds: stream segregation of birdsong by European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). J. Comp. Psychol. 111 3–13. 10.1037/0735-7036.111.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iriki A., Tanaka M., Iwamura Y. (1996). Attention-induced neuronal activity in the monkey somatosensory cortex revealed by pupillometrics. Neurosci. Res. 25 173–181. 10.1016/0168-0102(96)01043-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaacson J. S., Scanziani M. (2011). How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron 72 231–243. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jäger K., Kössl M. (2016). Corticofugal modulation of DPOAEs in gerbils. Hear. Res. 332 61–72. 10.1016/j.heares.2015.11.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeffress L. A. (1948). A place theory of sound localization. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 41 35–39. 10.1037/h0061495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johannesen P. T., Pérez-González P., Kalluri S., Blanco J. L., Lopez-Poveda E. A. (2016). The influence of cochlear mechanical dysfunction, temporal processing deficits, and age on the intelligibility of audible speech in noise for hearing-impaired listeners. Trends Hear. 20:2331216516641055. 10.1177/2331216516641055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kadia S. C., Wang X. (2003). Spectral integration in A1 of awake primates: neurons with single- and multipeaked tuning characteristics. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 1603–1622. 10.1152/jn.00271.2001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kato H. K., Asinof S. K., Isaacson J. S. (2017). Network-level control of frequency tuning in auditory cortex. Neuron 95 412–423.e4. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kato H. K., Gillet S. N., Isaacson J. S. (2015). Flexible sensory representations in auditory cortex driven by behavioral relevance. Neuron 88 1027–1039. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katzner S., Busse L., Carandini M. (2011). GABAA inhibition controls response gain in visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 31 5931–5941. 10.1523/jneurosci.5753-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kauramäki J., Jääskeläinen I. P., Sams M. (2007). Selective attention increases both gain and feature selectivity of the human auditory cortex. PLoS One 2:e909. 10.1371/journal.pone.0000909 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser C., Petkov C. I., Lippert M., Logothetis N. K. (2005). Mechanisms for allocating auditory attention: an auditory saliency map. Curr. Biol. 15, 1943–1947. 10.1016/j.cub.2005.09.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller C. H., Kaylegian K., Wehr M. (2018). Gap encoding by parvalbumin-expressing interneurons in auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 120 105–114. 10.1152/jn.00911.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerlin J. R., Shahin A. J., Miller L. M. (2010). Attentional gain control of ongoing cortical speech representations in a “Cocktail Party.”. J. Neurosci. 30 620–628. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3631-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khalighinejad B., Herrero J. L., Mehta A. D., Mesgarani N. (2019). Adaptation of the human auditory cortex to changing background noise. Nat. Commun. 10:2509. 10.1038/s41467-019-10611-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim H., Ährlund-Richter S., Wang X., Deisseroth K., Carlén M. (2016). Prefrontal parvalbumin neurons in control of attention. Cell 164 208–218. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.11.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King A. J., Walker K. M. (2020). Listening in complex acoustic scenes. Curr. Opin. Physiol. 18 63–72. 10.1016/j.cophys.2020.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohrman D. C., Wan G., Cassinotti L., Corfas G. (2020). Hidden hearing loss: a disorder with multiple etiologies and mechanisms. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 10:a035493. 10.1101/cshperspect.a035493 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraus N., Bradlow A. R., Cheatham M. A., Cunningham J., King C. D., Koch D. B., et al. (2000). Consequences of neural asynchrony: a case of auditory neuropathy. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 1 33–45. 10.1007/s101620010004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kujawa S. G., Liberman M. C. (2009). Adding insult to injury: cochlear nerve degeneration after “Temporary” noise-induced hearing loss. J. Neurosci. 29 14077–14085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kvale M. N., Schreiner C. E. (2004). Short-term adaptation of auditory receptive fields to dynamic stimuli. J. Neurophysiol. 91 604–612. 10.1152/jn.00484.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee A. M., Hoy J. L., Bonci A., Wilbrecht L., Stryker M. P., Niell C. M. (2014). Identification of a brainstem circuit regulating visual cortical state in parallel with locomotion. Neuron 83 455–466. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee C.-C., Middlebrooks J. C. (2011). Auditory cortex spatial sensitivity sharpens during task performance. Nat. Neurosci. 14 108–114. 10.1038/nn.2713 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li L., Ji X., Liang F., Li Y., Xiao Z., Tao H. W., et al. (2014). A feedforward inhibitory circuit mediates lateral refinement of sensory representation in upper layer 2/3 of mouse primary auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 34 13670–13683. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1516-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li L.-Y., Xiong X. R., Ibrahim L. A., Yuan W., Tao H. W., Zhang L. I. (2015). Differential receptive field properties of parvalbumin and somatostatin inhibitory neurons in mouse auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 25 1782–1791. 10.1093/cercor/bht417 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liberman M. C. (1978). Auditory-nerve response from cats raised in a low-noise chamber. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 63 442–455. 10.1121/1.381736 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liberman M. C., Kujawa S. G. (2017). Cochlear synaptopathy in acquired sensorineural hearing loss: Manifestations and mechanisms. Hear. Res. 349 138–147. 10.1016/j.heares.2017.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin F. R., Yaffe K., Xia J., Xue Q.-L., Harris T. B., Purchase-Helzner E., et al. (2013). Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Int. Med. 173 293–299. 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.1868 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lingner A., Pecka M., Leibold C., Grothe B. (2018). A novel concept for dynamic adjustment of auditory space. Sci. Rep. 8:8335. 10.1038/s41598-018-26690-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llano D. A., Sherman S. M. (2009). Differences in intrinsic properties and local network connectivity of identified layer 5 and layer 6 adult mouse auditory corticothalamic neurons support a dual corticothalamic projection hypothesis. Cereb. Cortex 19 2810–2826. 10.1093/cercor/bhp050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobarinas E., Salvi R., Ding D. (2013). Insensitivity of the audiogram to carboplatin induced inner hair cell loss in chinchillas. Hear. Res. 302 113–120. 10.1016/j.heares.2013.03.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobarinas E., Salvi R., Ding D. (2020). Gap detection deficits in chinchillas with selective carboplatin-induced inner hair cell loss. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 21 475–483. 10.1007/s10162-020-00744-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lohse M., Bajo V. M., King A. J., Willmore B. D. B. (2020). Neural circuits underlying auditory contrast gain control and their perceptual implications. Nat. Commun. 11:324. 10.1038/s41467-019-14163-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez Espejo M., Schwartz Z. P., David S. V. (2019). Spectral tuning of adaptation supports coding of sensory context in auditory cortex. PLoS Comput. Biol. 15:e1007430. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007430 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma L., Micheyl C., Yin P., Oxenham A. J., Shamma S. A. (2010). Behavioral measures of auditory streaming in ferrets (Mustela putorius). J. Comp. Psychol. 124 317–330. 10.1037/a0018273 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnusson A. K., Park T. J., Pecka M., Grothe B., Koch U. (2008). Retrograde GABA signaling adjusts sound localization by balancing excitation and inhibition in the brainstem. Neuron 59 125–137. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marian V., Lam T. Q., Hayakawa S., Dhar S. (2018). Top-down cognitive and linguistic influences on the suppression of spontaneous otoacoustic emissions. Front. Neurosci. 12:378. 10.3389/fnins.2018.00378 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maunsell J. H. R. (2015). Neuronal mechanisms of visual attention. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 1 373–391. 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035431 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- May B. J., Sachs M. B. (1992). Dynamic range of neural rate responses in the ventral cochlear nucleus of awake cats. J. Neurophysiol. 68 1589–1602. 10.1152/jn.1992.68.5.1589 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGinley M. J., David S. V., McCormick D. A. (2015a). Cortical membrane potential signature of optimal states for sensory signal detection. Neuron 87 179–192. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGinley M. J., Vinck M., Reimer J., Batista-Brito R., Zagha E., Cadwell C. R., et al. (2015b). Waking state: rapid variations modulate neural and behavioral responses. Neuron 87 1143–1161. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer N. E., Ryugo D. K. (2006). Projections from auditory cortex to cochlear nucleus: a comparative analysis of rat and mouse. Anat. Rec. A. Discov. Mol. Cell Evol. Biol. 288 397–408. 10.1002/ar.a.20300 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesgarani N., Chang E. F. (2012). Selective cortical representation of attended speaker in multi-talker speech perception. Nature 485 233–236. 10.1038/nature11020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesgarani N., David S. V., Fritz J. B., Shamma S. A. (2014b). Mechanisms of noise robust representation of speech in primary auditory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111 6792–6797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesgarani N., Cheung C., Johnson K., Chang E. F. (2014a). Phonetic feature encoding in human superior temporal gyrus. Science 343 1006–1010. 10.1126/science.1245994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell J. F., Sundberg K. A., Reynolds J. H. (2007). Differential attention-dependent response modulation across cell classes in macaque visual area V4. Neuron 55 131–141. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell J. F., Sundberg K. A., Reynolds J. H. (2009). Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in macaque area V4. Neuron 63 879–888. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monaghan J. J. M., Garcia-Lazaro J. A., McAlpine D., Schaette R. (2020). Hidden hearing loss impacts the neural representation of speech in background noise. Curr. Biol. 30 4710–4721.e4. 10.1016/j.cub.2020.09.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore A. K., Wehr M. (2013). Parvalbumin-expressing inhibitory interneurons in auditory cortex are well-tuned for frequency. J. Neurosci. 33 13713–13723. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0663-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreno-Bote R., Beck J., Kanitscheider I., Pitkow X., Latham P., Pouget A. (2014). Information-limiting correlations. Nat. Neurosci. 17 1410–1417. 10.1038/nn.3807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Motanis H., Seay M. J., Buonomano D. V. (2018). Short-term synaptic plasticity as a mechanism for sensory timing. Trends Neurosci. 41 701–711. 10.1016/j.tins.2018.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagel K. I., Doupe A. J. (2006). Temporal processing and adaptation in the songbird auditory forebrain. Neuron 51 845–859. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Natan R. G., Briguglio J. J., Mwilambwe-Tshilobo L., Jones S. I., Aizenberg M., Goldberg E. M., et al. (2015). Complementary control of sensory adaptation by two types of cortical interneurons. Elife 4:e09868. 10.7554/eLife.09868 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Natan R. G., Carruthers I. M., Mwilambwe-Tshilobo L., Geffen M. N. (2016). Gain control in the auditory cortex evoked by changing temporal correlation of sounds. Cereb. Cortex 27, 2385–2402. 10.1093/cercor/bhw083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Natan R. G., Rao W., Geffen M. N. (2017). Cortical interneurons differentially shape frequency tuning following adaptation. Cell Rep. 21 878–890. 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.10.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelken I. (2014). Stimulus-specific adaptation and deviance detection in the auditory system: experiments and models. Biol. Cybern 108 655–663. 10.1007/s00422-014-0585-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson A., Schneider D. M., Takatoh J., Sakurai K., Wang F., Mooney R. (2013). A circuit for motor cortical modulation of auditory cortical activity. J. Neurosci. 33 14342–14353. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2275-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson P. C., Smith Z. M., Young E. D. (2009). Wide-dynamic-range forward suppression in marmoset inferior colliculus neurons is generated centrally and accounts for perceptual masking. J. Neurosci. 29 2553–2562. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5359-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ni J., Wunderle T., Lewis C. M., Desimone R., Diester I., Fries P. (2016). Gamma-rhythmic gain modulation. Neuron 92 240–251. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noda T., Takahashi H. (2019). Behavioral evaluation of auditory stream segregation in rats. Neurosci. Res. 141 52–62. 10.1016/j.neures.2018.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberfeld D., Klöckner-Nowotny F. (2016). Individual differences in selective attention predict speech identification at a cocktail party. Elife 5:e16747. 10.7554/eLife.16747 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsen S. R., Wilson R. I. (2008). Lateral presynaptic inhibition mediates gain control in an olfactory circuit. Nature 452 956–960. 10.1038/nature06864 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsen S. R., Bortone D. S., Adesnik H., Scanziani M. (2012). Gain control by layer six in cortical circuits of vision. Nature 483 47–52. 10.1038/nature10835 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Otazu G. H., Tai L.-H., Yang Y., Zador A. M. (2009). Engaging in an auditory task suppresses responses in auditory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 12 646–654. 10.1038/nn.2306 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overath T., Kumar S., von Kriegstein K., Griffiths T. D. (2008). Encoding of spectral correlation over time in auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 28 13268–13273. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4596-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pakan J. M., Lowe S. C., Dylda E., Keemink S. W., Currie S. P., Coutts C. A., et al. (2016). Behavioral-state modulation of inhibition is context-dependent and cell type specific in mouse visual cortex. eLife 5:e14985. 10.7554/eLife.14985 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park H. L., O’Connell J. E., Thomson R. G. (2003). A systematic review of cognitive decline in the general elderly population. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 18, 1121–1134. 10.1002/gps.1023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park T. J., Brand A., Koch U., Ikebuchi M., Grothe B. (2008). Dynamic changes in level influence spatial coding in the lateral superior olive. Hear. Res. 238 58–67. 10.1016/j.heares.2007.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park T. J., Grothe B., Pollak G. D., Schuller G., Koch U. (1996). Neural delays shape selectivity to interaural intensity differences in the lateral superior olive. J. Neurosci. 16 6554–6566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park T. J., Klug A., Holinstat M., Grothe B. (2004). Interaural level difference processing in the lateral superior olive and the inferior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol. 92 289–301. 10.1152/jn.00961.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passow S., Westerhausen R., Hugdahl K., Wartenburger I., Heekeren H. R., Lindenberger U., et al. (2014). Electrophysiological correlates of adult age differences in attentional control of auditory processing. Cereb. Cortex 24, 249–260. 10.1093/cercor/bhs306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington J. R., David S. V. (2020). Complementary effects of adaptation and gain control on sound encoding in primary auditory cortex. eNeuro 7:ENEURO.205–ENEURO.220. 10.1523/ENEURO.0205-20.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters R. W., Moore B. C., Baer T. (1998). Speech reception thresholds in noise with and without spectral and temporal dips for hearing-impaired and normally hearing people. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 577–587. 10.1121/1.421128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfeffer C. K., Xue M., He M., Huang Z. J., Scanziani M. (2013). Inhibition of inhibition in visual cortex: the logic of connections between molecularly distinct interneurons. Nat. Neurosci. 16 1068–1076. 10.1038/nn.3446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips D. P., Hall S. E. (2005). Psychophysical evidence for adaptation of central auditory processors for interaural differences in time and level. Hear. Res. 202 188–199. 10.1016/j.heares.2004.11.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips D. P., Semple M. N., Calford M. B., Kitzes L. M. (1994). Level-dependent representation of stimulus frequency in cat primary auditory cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 102 210–226. 10.1007/BF00227510 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips E. A. K., Schreiner C. E., Hasenstaub A. R. (2017). Cortical interneurons differentially regulate the effects of acoustic context. Cell Rep. 20 771–778. 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips E. A., Hasenstaub A. R. (2016). Asymmetric effects of activating and inactivating cortical interneurons. Elife 5:e18383. 10.7554/eLife.18383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pi H.-J., Hangya B., Kvitsiani D., Sanders J. I., Huang Z. J., Kepecs A. (2013). Cortical interneurons that specialize in disinhibitory control. Nature 503 521–524. 10.1038/nature12676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pienkowski M., Tyler R. S., Roncancio E. R., Jun H. J., Brozoski T., Dauman N., et al. (2014). A review of hyperacusis and future directions: part II. Measurement, mechanisms, and treatment. Am. J. Audiol. 23 420–436. 10.1044/2014_AJA-13-0037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plack C. J., Barker D., Prendergast G. (2014). Perceptual consequences of “Hidden” hearing loss. Trends Hear. 18:2331216514550621. 10.1177/2331216514550621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popelar J., Syka J., Berndt H. (1987). Effect of noise on auditory evoked-responses in awake guinea-pigs. Hear. Res. 26 239–247. 10.1016/0378-5955(87)90060-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabinowitz N. C., Willmore B. D. B., King A. J., Schnupp J. W. H. (2013). Constructing noise-invariant representations of sound in the auditory pathway. PLoS Biol. 11:e1001710. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001710 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabinowitz N. C., Willmore B. D. B., Schnupp J. W. H., King A. J. (2011). Contrast gain control in auditory cortex. Neuron 70 1178–1191. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radziwon K., Salvi R. (2020). Using auditory reaction time to measure loudness growth in rats. Hear. Res. 395:108026. 10.1016/j.heares.2020.108026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radziwon K., Auerbach B. D., Ding D., Liu X., Chen G. D., Salvi R. (2019). Noise-induced loudness recruitment and hyperacusis: insufficient central gain in auditory cortex and amygdala. Neuroscience 422 212–227. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.09.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ralli M., Greco A., De Vincentiis M., Sheppard A., Cappelli G., Neri I., et al. (2019). Tone-in-noise detection deficits in elderly patients with clinically normal hearing. Am. J. Otolaryngol. 40 1–9. 10.1016/j.amjoto.2018.09.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed D. K., Chait M., Tóth B., Winkler I., Shinn-Cunningham B. (2020). Spatial cues can support auditory figure-ground segregation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 147:3814. 10.1121/10.0001387 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resnik J., Polley D. B. (2017). Fast-spiking GABA circuit dynamics in the auditory cortex predict recovery of sensory processing following peripheral nerve damage. Elife 6:e21452. 10.7554/eLife.21452 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resnik J., Polley D. B. (2021). Cochlear neural degeneration disrupts hearing in background noise by increasing auditory cortex internal noise. Neuron 109 984–996.e4. 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.01.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson B. L., McAlpine D. (2009). Gain control mechanisms in the auditory pathway. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19 402–407. 10.1016/j.conb.2009.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson B. L., Harper N. S., McAlpine D. (2016). Meta-adaptation in the auditory midbrain under cortical influence. Nat. Commun. 7:13442. 10.1038/ncomms13442 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rocchi F., Ramachandran R. (2018). Neuronal adaptation to sound statistics in the inferior colliculus of behaving macaques does not reduce the effectiveness of the masking noise. J. Neurophysiol. 120 2819–2833. 10.1152/jn.00875.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rocchi F., Ramachandran R. (2020). Foreground stimuli and task engagement enhance neuronal adaptation to background noise in the inferior colliculus of macaques. J. Neurophysiol. 124 1315–1326. 10.1152/jn.00153.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosen S. (1992). Temporal information in speech: acoustic, auditory and linguistic aspects. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 336 367–373. 10.1098/rstb.1992.0070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachs M. B., Abbas P. J. (1974). Rate versus level functions for auditory-nerve fibers in cats: tone-burst stimuli. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 56 1835–1847. 10.1121/1.1903521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadagopan S., Wang X. (2008). Level invariant representation of sounds by populations of neurons in primary auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 28 3415–3426. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2743-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saderi D., Buran B. N., David S. V. (2020). Streaming of repeated noise in primary and secondary fields of auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 40 3783–3798. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2105-19.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salvi R., Sun W., Ding D., Chen G. D., Lobarinas E., Wang J., et al. (2016). Inner hair cell loss disrupts hearing and cochlear function leading to sensory deprivation and enhanced central auditory gain. Front. Neurosci. 10:621. 10.3389/fnins.2016.00621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santoro R., Moerel M., Martino F. D., Goebel R., Ugurbil K., Yacoub E., et al. (2014). Encoding of natural sounds at multiple spectral and temporal resolutions in the human auditory cortex. PLoS Comput. Biol. 10:e1003412. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaette R., McAlpine D. (2011). Tinnitus with a normal audiogram: physiological evidence for hidden hearing loss and computational model. J. Neurosci. 31 13452–13457. 10.1523/jneurosci.2156-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmiedt R. A. (1984). Acoustic injury and the physiology of hearing. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 76 1293–1317. 10.1121/1.391446 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider D. M., Woolley S. M. N. (2013). Sparse and background-invariant coding of vocalizations in auditory scenes. Neuron 79 141–152. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.04.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider D. M., Nelson A., Mooney R. (2014). A synaptic and circuit basis for corollary discharge in the auditory cortex. Nature 513 189–194. 10.1038/nature13724 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schönwiesner M., Rübsamen R., von Cramon D. Y. (2005). Hemispheric asymmetry for spectral and temporal processing in the human antero-lateral auditory belt cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22 1521–1528. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04315.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shannon R. V., Zeng F.-G., Kamath V., Wygonski J., Ekelid M. (1995). Speech recognition with primarily temporal cues. Science 270 303–304. 10.1126/science.270.5234.303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shastri U., Mythri H. M., Kumar U. A. (2014). Descending auditory pathway and identification of phonetic contrast by native listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 135 896–905. 10.1121/1.4861350 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shew W. L., Yang H., Yu S., Roy R., Plenz D. (2011). Information capacity and transmission are maximized in balanced cortical networks with neuronal avalanches. J. Neurosci. 31, 55–63. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4637-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinn-Cunningham B. G., Best V. (2008). Selective attention in normal and impaired hearing. Trends Amplif. 12 283–299. 10.1177/1084713808325306 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shu Y., Hasenstaub A., Badoual M., Bal T., McCormick D. A. (2003). Barrages of synaptic activity control the gain and sensitivity of cortical neurons. J. Neurosci. 23 10388–10401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simel D. L., Bagai A., Thavendiranathan P., Detsky A. S. (2016). “Hearing impairment,” in The Rational Clinical Examination: Evidence-Based Clinical Diagnosis, eds Simel D. L., Rennie D. (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Education; ). [Google Scholar]
- Simpson A. J. R., Harper N. S., Reiss J. D., McAlpine D. (2014). Selective adaptation to “Oddball” sounds by the human auditory system. J. Neurosci. 34 1963–1969. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4274-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh N. C., Theunissen F. E. (2003). Modulation spectra of natural sounds and ethological theories of auditory processing. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 114 3394–3411. 10.1121/1.1624067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sohal V. S., Zhang F., Yizhar O., Deisseroth K. (2009). Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature 459 698–702. 10.1038/nature07991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spankovich C., Gonzalez V. B., Su D., Bishop C. E. (2018). Self reported hearing difficulty, tinnitus, and normal audiometric thresholds, the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 1999-2002. Hear. Res. 358 30–36. 10.1016/j.heares.2017.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spoendlin H. (1972). Innervation densities of the cochlea. Acta Otolaryngol. 73 235–248. 10.3109/00016487209138937 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Srinivasan S., Keil A., Stratis K., Woodruff Carr K. L., Smith D. W. (2012). Effects of cross-modal selective attention on the sensory periphery: cochlear sensitivity is altered by selective attention. Neuroscience 223 325–332. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.07.062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stange A., Myoga M. H., Lingner A., Ford M. C., Alexandrova O., Felmy F., et al. (2013). Adaptation in sound localization: from GABAB receptor–mediated synaptic modulation to perception. Nat. Neurosci. 16 1840–1847. 10.1038/nn.3548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starr A., Picton T. W., Sininger Y., Hood L. J., Berlin C., I (1996). Auditory neuropathy. Brain 119(Pt. 3), 741–753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suga N., Manabe T. (1982). Neural basis of amplitude-spectrum representation in auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J. Neurophysiol. 47 225–255. 10.1152/jn.1982.47.2.225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suga N., O’Neill W. E., Kujirai K., Manabe T. (1983). Specificity of combination-sensitive neurons for processing of complex biosonar signals in auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J. Neurophysiol. 49, 1573–1626. 10.1152/jn.1983.49.6.1573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Syka J. (2002). Plastic changes in the central auditory system after hearing loss, restoration of function, and during learning. Physiol. Rev. 82 601–636. 10.1152/physrev.00002.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Syka J., Rybalko N., Popelar J. (1994). Enhancement of the auditory-cortex evoked-responses is awake guinea-pigs after noise exposure. Hear. Res. 78 158–168. 10.1016/0378-5955(94)90021-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiesinga P. H., Fellous J.-M., Salinas E., José J. V., Sejnowski T. J. (2004). Inhibitory synchrony as a mechanism for attentional gain modulation. J. Physiol. Paris 98 296–314. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2005.09.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiesinga P., Fellous J.-M., Sejnowski T. J. (2008). Regulation of spike timing in visual cortical circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9 97–107. 10.1038/nrn2315 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tremblay K. L., Pinto A., Fischer M. E., Klein B. E. K., Klein R., Levy S., et al. (2015). Self-reported hearing difficulties among adults with normal audiograms: the beaver dam offspring study. Ear. Hear. 36 e290–e299. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulanovsky N., Las L., Nelken I. (2003). Processing of low-probability sounds by cortical neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 6 391–398. 10.1038/nn1032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viemeister N. F. (1988). Intensity coding and the dynamic range problem. Hear. Res. 34 267–274. 10.1016/0378-5955(88)90007-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vigneault-MacLean B. K., Hall S. E., Phillips D. P. (2007). The effects of lateralized adaptors on lateral position judgements of tones within and across frequency channels. Hear. Res. 224 93–100. 10.1016/j.heares.2006.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang J., Powers N. L., Hofstetter P., Trautwein P., Ding D., Salvi R. (1997). Effects of selective inner hair cell loss on auditory nerve fiber threshold, tuning and spontaneous and driven discharge rate. Hear. Res. 107 67–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wardak C., Ibos G., Duhamel J.-R., Olivier E. (2006). Contribution of the monkey frontal eye field to covert visual attention. J. Neurosci. 26 4228–4235. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3336-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watkins P. V., Barbour D. L. (2008). Specialized neuronal adaptation for preserving input sensitivity. Nat. Neurosci. 11 1259–1261. 10.1038/nn.2201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watkins P. V., Barbour D. L. (2011). Level-tuned neurons in primary auditory cortex adapt differently to loud versus soft sounds. Cereb. Cortex 21 178–190. 10.1093/cercor/bhq079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wehr M., Zador A. M. (2003). Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature 426 442–446. 10.1038/nature02116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wen B., Wang G. I., Dean I., Delgutte B. (2009). Dynamic range adaptation to sound level statistics in the auditory nerve. J. Neurosci. 29 13797–13808. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5610-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williamson R. S., Polley D. B. (2019). Parallel pathways for sound processing and functional connectivity among layer 5 and 6 auditory corticofugal neurons. Elife 8:e42974. 10.7554/eLife.42974 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willmore B. D., Cooke J. E., King A. J. (2014). Hearing in noisy environments: noise invariance and contrast gain control. J. Physiol. 592 3371–3381. 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.274886 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson N. R., Runyan C. A., Wang F. L., Sur M. (2012). Division and subtraction by distinct cortical inhibitory networks in vivo. Nature 488 343–348. 10.1038/nature11347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winer J. A., Chernock M. L., Larue D. T., Cheung S. W. (2002). Descending projections to the inferior colliculus from the posterior thalamus and the auditory cortex in rat, cat, and monkey. Hear. Res. 168 181–195. 10.1016/s0378-5955(02)00489-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkler I., Denham S. L., Nelken I. (2009). Modeling the auditory scene: predictive regularity representations and perceptual objects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13 532–540. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkowski D. E., Bandyopadhyay S., Shamma S. A., Kanold P. O. (2013). Frontal cortex activation causes rapid plasticity of auditory cortical processing. J. Neurosci. 33 18134–18148. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0180-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood K. C., Blackwell J. M., Geffen M. N. (2017). Cortical inhibitory interneurons control sensory processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 46 200–207. 10.1016/j.conb.2017.08.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu P., Liberman L., Bennett K., de Gruttola V., O’Malley J., Liberman M. (2019). Primary neural degeneration in the human cochlea: evidence for hidden hearing loss in the aging ear. Neuroscience 407 8–20. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.07.053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiao Z., Suga N. (2002). Modulation of cochlear hair cells by the auditory cortex in the mustached bat. Nat. Neurosci. 5 57–63. 10.1038/nn786 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yan J., Suga N. (1996). Corticofugal modulation of time-domain processing of biosonar information in bats. Science 273 1100–1103. 10.1126/science.273.5278.1100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang H., Xu-Friedman M. A. (2009). Impact of synaptic depression on spike timing at the endbulb of Held. J. Neurophysiol. 102 1699–1710. 10.1152/jn.00072.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang S., Weiner B. D., Zhang L. S., Cho S.-J., Bao S. (2011). Homeostatic plasticity drives tinnitus perception in an animal model. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108 14974–14979. 10.1073/pnas.1107998108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yizhar O., Fenno L. E., Prigge M., Schneider F., Davidson T. J., O’Shea D. J., et al. (2011). Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature 477, 171–178. 10.1038/nature10360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young E. D., Barta P. E. (1986). Rate responses of auditory nerve fibers to tones in noise near masked threshold. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 79 426–442. 10.1121/1.393530 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zatorre R. J., Belin P. (2001). Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 11 946–953. 10.1093/cercor/11.10.946 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeng F. G. (2013). An active loudness model suggesting tinnitus as increased central noise and hyperacusis as increased nonlinear gain. Hear. Res. 295 172–179. 10.1016/j.heares.2012.05.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeng F. G., Kong Y. Y., Michalewski H. J., Starr A. (2005). Perceptual consequences of disrupted auditory nerve activity. J. Neurophysiol. 93 3050–3063. 10.1152/jn.00985.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang L. I., Tan A. Y. Y., Schreiner C. E., Merzenich M. M. (2003). Topography and synaptic shaping of direction selectivity in primary auditory cortex. Nature 424 201–205. 10.1038/nature01796 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang S., Xu M., Kamigaki T., Do J. P. H., Chang W.-C., Jenvay S., et al. (2014). Long-range and local circuits for top-down modulation of visual cortex processing. Science 345 660–665. 10.1126/science.1254126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y., Hakes J. J., Bonfield S. P., Yan J. (2005). Corticofugal feedback for auditory midbrain plasticity elicited by tones and electrical stimulation of basal forebrain in mice. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22 871–879. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04276.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y., Suga N., Yan J. (1997). Corticofugal modulation of frequency processing in bat auditory system. Nature 387 900–903. 10.1038/43180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou M., Liang F., Xiong X. R., Li L., Li H., Xiao Z., et al. (2014). Scaling down of balanced excitation and inhibition by active behavioral states in auditory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 17 841–850. 10.1038/nn.3701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhuang X., Wong N. F., Sun W., Xu-Friedman M. A. (2020). Mechanisms and functional consequences of presynaptic homeostatic plasticity at auditory nerve synapses. J. Neurosci. 40 6896–6909. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1175-19.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zion Golumbic E. M., Ding N., Bickel S., Lakatos P., Schevon C. A., McKhann G. M., et al. (2013). Mechanisms underlying selective neuronal tracking of attended speech at a “Cocktail Party.”. Neuron 77 980–991. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zohary E., Shadlen M. N., Newsome W. T. (1994). Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance. Nature 370 140–143. 10.1038/370140a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]