Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Emotion. 2021 Sep 30;22(2):244–257. doi: 10.1037/emo0000996

Individual Variation in White Matter Microstructure is Related to Better Recovery from Negative Stimuli

Walker S Pedersen 1, Douglas C Dean III 1, Nagesh Adluru 1, Lauren K Gresham 1, Seungbeum D Lee 1, Michael P Kelly 1, Jeanette A Mumford 1, Richard J Davidson 1, Stacey M Schaefer 1
PMCID: PMC8881294  NIHMSID: NIHMS1689153  PMID: 34591511

Abstract

The uncinate fasciculus is a white matter tract that may facilitate emotion regulation by carrying connections from the prefrontal cortex to regions of the temporal lobe, including the amygdala. Depression and anxiety are associated with reduced uncinate fasciculus fractional anisotropy (FA) – a diffusion tensor imaging measure related to white matter integrity. In the current study, we tested whether FA in the uncinate fasciculus is associated with individual differences in emotional recovery measured with corrugator supercilii electromyography in response to negative, neutral, and positive images in 108 participants from the Midlife in the US (MIDUS; http://midus.wisc.edu) Refresher study. Corrugator activity is linearly associated with changes in affect, and differentiated negative, neutral, and positive emotional responses. Higher uncinate fasciculus FA was associated with lower corrugator activity 4–8 seconds after negative image offset, indicative of better recovery from negative provocation. In an exploratory analysis, we found a similar association for the inferior fronto-occipital, inferior longitudinal and superior longitudinal fasciculi. These results suggest that the microstructural features of the uncinate fasciculus, and these other association white matter fibers, may support emotion regulatory processes with greater white matter integrity facilitating healthier affective functioning.


The uncinate fasciculus is a white matter fiber bundle carrying projections from the orbitofrontal cortex to the anterior temporal lobe (Bracht et al., 2009; Petrides & Pandya, 2007), and is hypothesized to facilitate top-down emotion regulation by carrying projections from the orbitofrontal cortex to the amygdala (d’Arbeloff et al., 2018; Zuurbier et al., 2013). The amygdala contributes to affective processing and plays a central role in detecting and responding to threatening and negative stimuli (Davis & Whalen, 2001; Fox & Shackman, 2017). Inhibition of the amygdala via both the orbitofrontal cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is thought to mediate emotion regulation of responses to threatening or negatively-valenced stimuli (Delgado et al., 2008; Diekhof et al., 2011; Roy et al., 2012; Shiba et al., 2017). The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex may regulate amygdala activity, in part, through connections with the orbitofrontal cortex, which in turn projects to the amygdala via uncinate fasciculus fibers (d’Arbeloff et al., 2018; Delgado et al., 2008).

Past studies have investigated how individual differences in affective functioning are related to the microstructure of the uncinate fasciculus using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) measures such as fractional anisotropy (FA). FA is the degree to which diffusion is directionally dependent, with higher FA indicating greater diffusion anisotropy, and is highly sensitive to the underlying tissue microstructure (Alexander et al., 2011; Chanraud et al., 2010). Depressed patients have lower uncinate fasciculus FA (Bhatia et al., 2018; Bracht et al., 2015; de Kwaasteniet et al., 2013; A. Zhang et al., 2012). Low uncinate fasciculus FA has been related to trait anxiety (Kim et al., 2016; Kim & Whalen, 2009) and anxiety disorders (Hettema et al., 2012; Liao et al., 2014; Phan et al., 2009). However, Modi et al. (2013) found a positive correlation between trait anxiety and uncinate fasciculus FA, while Montag et al. (2012) found a similar association for males, but not females. Similarly, in a sample containing young, middle-aged, and older adults, Clewett et al. (2014) found a positive relationship between trait anxiety and amygdala-vPFC FA (using an ROI that included the uncinate fasciculus). Higher uncinate fasciculus FA has been associated with greater self-reported use of the emotion regulation strategy of cognitive reappraisal, particularly in women (Zuurbier et al., 2013). Higher uncinate fasciculus FA is also related to decreased amygdala activation to emotional faces in children and adolescents (Swartz et al., 2014), and greater amygdala habituation to fearful faces (Hein et al., 2018). In addition, individuals with high uncinate fasciculus FA exhibit a stronger negative relationship between self-reported cognitive reappraisal and symptoms of depression and anxiety (d’Arbeloff et al., 2018).

While past studies have investigated how uncinate fasciculus FA relates to anxiety and depression (Bhatia et al., 2018; Bracht et al., 2015; de Kwaasteniet et al., 2013; Greenberg et al., 2021; Hettema et al., 2012; Liao et al., 2014; Phan et al., 2009; A. Zhang et al., 2012) and amygdala reactivity (Swartz et al., 2014), whether uncinate fasciculus FA is related to the behavioral response to emotional provocation is not clear. Examining this relationship may provide insights into how the microstructure of the uncinate fasciculus is related to basic affective functioning and how alterations in this microstructure may confer risk for mood and anxiety disorders. To examine how the microstructure of the uncinate fasciculus relates to emotional responses to affective stimuli, we investigated whether individual differences in uncinate fasciculus FA were associated with the response to affective images, as measured by electromyography of the corrugator supercilii, the muscle involved in brow furrowing. Corrugator activity increases in response to negatively valenced stimuli, and decreases to positively valenced stimuli, and this modulation correlates reliably with participants’ valence ratings of the stimuli (Cacioppo et al., 1986; Larsen et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2009; Tan et al., 2012). We also tested whether uncinate fasciculus microstructure correlated with activity of the zygomaticus major, the muscle involved in smiling. Zygomaticus activity increases in response to positively valenced stimuli, but is not sensitive to negative stimuli (Larsen et al., 2003; Tan et al., 2012). This allowed us to use the zygomaticus as a control region, to determine whether electromyographic activity to negative stimuli was specific to the corrugator or more widespread, which could indicate an effect due to an artifact or a process that is not specific to negative affect.

As the uncinate fasciculus may be involved in emotion regulation (d’Arbeloff et al., 2018; Delgado et al., 2008), we expected that the microstructure of this pathway would be associated with recovery processes following stimulus offset. Emotional recovery – how long the emotional response persists following the offset of an emotional provocation – is an important constituent of an individual’s affective style. For example, less amygdala recovery is associated with inhibited temperament (Blackford et al., 2009), anxiety (Lau et al., 2012), depression, (Siegle et al., 2002) and neuroticism (Schuyler et al., 2014), and emotion regulation can decrease the duration of the amygdala response (Waugh et al., 2016). Individual differences in the time-course of corrugator activity are also associated with an individual’s affective style (for review see Schaefer et al., 2018). Greater corrugator activity during the recovery from negative provocation suggests emotion regulation difficulties, because corrugator activity is sensitive to voluntary emotion regulation (Lee et al., 2009). In the absence of emotion regulation instructions, better corrugator recovery may index implicit emotion regulation – emotion regulation processes that operate automatically and without conscious effort (Mauss et al., 2007). Greater corrugator activity after negative picture offset, indicative of poorer recovery, has been associated with lower levels of conscientiousness and self-control (Javaras et al., 2012). In addition, persistent marital stress is related to higher corrugator activity following positive images (indicative of blunted positive emotional responses), with corrugator activity mediating the relationship between marital stress and depression symptoms (Lapate et al., 2014).

We predicted that greater FA of the uncinate fasciculus would be associated with reduced corrugator activity following negative image offset, indicating better recovery from emotional provocation. Based on past evidence that uncinate fasciculus FA is related to the regulation of negative emotions (d’Arbeloff et al., 2018; Hein et al., 2018), we expected relations between uncinate FA and corrugator and zygomaticus activity in response to positive images to be negligible. We also explored whether uncinate fasciculus FA was related to self-reported trait anxiety and negative affect, anxiety and depression symptomatology, and typical use of emotion regulation strategies.

While we chose the uncinate fasciculus as an a priori ROI, at the request of a reviewer, we also explored relationships between FA in each of the major fiber bundles and both the corrugator response to emotional images and affect-related self-report. There is little research linking these tracts to either affective responses or to the function of the amygdala, as most of this work has focused on the uncinate fasciculus (Hein et al., 2018; Swartz et al., 2014). However, studies investigating which tracts exhibit microstructural changes in anxiety disorders and anxiety-related traits are heterogeneous, with many major fiber bundles implicated, but often inconsistently across studies: Reduced FA in the uncinate fasciculus, cingulum bundle, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, and superior longitudinal fasciculus has been implicated in generalized anxiety disorder (Adluru et al., 2017; Hettema et al., 2012; W. Wang et al., 2016). However, while some past studies implicate reduced uncinate fasciculus FA in trait anxiety (Kim et al., 2016; Kim & Whalen, 2009), others have found a positive relationship between trait anxiety and FA in the uncinate fasciculus and fornix (Modi et al., 2013), as well as in the cingulum bundle, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, the inferior longitudinal fasciculus and the superior longitudinal fasciculus in males, but not females (Montag et al., 2012). Meanwhile, Westlye et al. (2011) reported that harm avoidance – a trait related to anxiety – was associated with reduced FA in widespread white matter regions, extending into virtually all major tracts. Several studies have reported reduced FA in obsessive compulsive disorder, including the uncinate fasciculus, the cingulum bundle, the inferior occipital fasciculus, the inferior longitudinal fasciculus, and superior longitudinal fasciculus (Koch et al., 2014; Peng et al., 2012; Piras et al., 2013). In a meta-analysis, Jenkins et al. (2016) found that compared to healthy controls, participants with mood and anxiety disorders had reduced FA in the uncinate fasciculus, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, forceps minor, and superior longitudinal fasciculus. Thus, given the heterogenous nature of these past results, we explored relationships between the corrugator response to negative images and FA in each of the major fiber bundles, including the cingulum bundle, the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, the inferior frontal fasciculus, the superior longitudinal fasciculus, the corticospinal tract, the forceps major, the forceps minor, and the fornix.

Method

Participants

Participants were enrolled in the Midlife in the United States study (MIDUS; http://midus.wisc.edu), a national longitudinal study of health and well-being across the lifespan. Data came from the MIDUS refresher sample, a group of participants enrolled in the MIDUS study beginning in 2011 to refresh the original MIDUS cohort. Random digit dialing was the primary method of recruitment for the MIDUS refresher cohort, although a sample of primarily African American participants recruited in Milwaukee, WI, by door-to-door solicitation was also included. Data for the current analysis came from the MIDUS Refresher Neuroscience Project, which included a psychophysiology session and an MRI session completed on consecutive days. While we previously reported that higher resting-state connectivity between the central amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis is related to greater corrugator reactivity to affective images using the MIDUS refresher dataset (Pedersen et al., 2020), relationships between structural connectivity (i.e. diffusion weighted imaging data) and the response to affective stimuli remain untested in this sample.

Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) data were collected for 118 participants. Two participants were excluded from analysis because they were missing psychophysiology data, and 8 more were excluded due to excessive noise in their corrugator electromyography data. As a result, data from 108 participants (59 female, 49 male) with a mean age of 48.6 (range: 26–76) years were used in our primary analyses. Twelve participants had excessive noise in their zygomaticus electromyography data. Because of this, analyses involving zygomaticus data include data from 104 participants. When participants were asked their main racial origins, 70 endorsed White, 31 endorsed Black or African American, 2 endorsed Native American or Alaskan Native, and 1 endorsed Asian and 4 endorsed “other”. One participant was missing data for trait anxiety, emotion regulation strategy use and negative affect, with an additional subject missing data for emotion regulation strategy use only, and another missing data for symptoms of anxiety and depression. Additional demographic information can be found in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographics and Psychiatric Disorders. This table includes participants used in our primary analysis (i.e. those whose corrugator and DTI data passed quality assurance, N=108).

Sex
 Female 54.6%
 Male 45.4%
Race
 White 64.8%
 Black or African American 28.7%
 Native American or Alaskan 1.9%
Native
 Asian 0.9%
 Other 3.7%
Education
 High school or less 22.2%
 Some college, no degree 21.3%
 Graduated college 38%
 Advanced degree 18.5%
Marital Status
 Married 56.5%
 Never Married 22.2%
 Divorced or Windowed 21.3%
Depression (past 12 months) 13.9%
Anxiety disorder (past 12 months) 3.7%

Image Acquisition

MRI scanning was performed on a 3-Tesla MR750 General Electric scanner (Waukesha, WI) using an 8-channel head coil. Diffusion weighted images were collected using a Stejskal-Tanner (Stejskal & Tanner, 1965) diffusion prepared single-shot echo-planar imaging pulse sequence. Sixty-five axial slices of 2 mm thickness were acquired with 6 diffusion-encoding directions at b= 400 s/mm2 and 70 diffusion encoding directions at b=1200 s/mm2, in addition to 4 baseline images (b=0 s/mm2). Images were acquired with an in-plane matrix of 128 mm × 128 mm, resulting in a 2 mm × 2 mm resolution (FOV=256 mm, TR=7000 ms, TE=68.7 ms, partial Fourier [62.5%], ASSET [SENSE] × 2).

Diffusion Weighted Image Processing and Tractography

The software package MRtrix3 ( v. 3.0; Tournier et al., 2019) was used to denoise the data (Veraart et al., 2016), and correct Gibbs ringing artifacts (Kellner et al., 2016). The FMRIB Software Library (FSL v. 5.0.11) ‘eddy’ tool was used to correct for eddy current-induced distortions and subject movements (Andersson & Sotiropoulos, 2016), and to skull-strip the data (Smith, 2002). Weighted least squares was used to fit the tensors at each voxel using the Diffusion Imaging In Python package (v. 0.14.0; Garyfallidis et al., 2014). The Advanced Normalization Tools software package (v. 2.2.0; Avants, Tustison, & Song, 2009) was used to create a study specific FA template, and to normalize each participant’s images to this template. Tensors from individual subjects were reoriented to preserve the principal orientations (which is important for tractography) when spatially transforming to the template, and a population-averaged tensor template was generated using the reoriented tensor maps using the Diffusion Tensor Imaging ToolKit (v. 2.3.3; Zhang, Yushkevich, Rueckert, & Gee, 2007).

ROIs from the Illinois Institute of Technology (IIT) Human Brain Atlas (v. 4.1; S. Zhang & Arfanakis, 2018) were warped to our study-specific template (Avants et al., 2009). ROIs from the IIT atlas warped to our study-specific template can be seen in Supplementary Figure 1. The alignment between each participant’s normalized FA map and the normalized atlas was visually inspected to ensure accuracy. For each participant FA values were extracted for each tract in the IIT atlas. A white matter mask with voxels whose FA values was greater than 0.2 was created for each participant. Mean FA values within these masks were extracted for each subject to provide a measure of total white matter FA. We also extracted mean diffusivity (MD), radial diffusivity (RD), and axial diffusivity (AD) from these maps for additional analyses presented in the Supplementary Table 1.

Self-Report Measures

Participants completed self-report questionnaires including the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory – Trait Form (STAI-X2; Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983), the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule – General (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), the Mood and Anxiety Symptoms Questionnaire (MASQ; Watson & Clark, 1991), and the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003). Trait anxiety scores used in analyses were mean scores for the STAI-X2, while mean scores for the negative affect subscale of the PANAS-General were used as trait negative affect scores. Sums for the Anxious Arousal and General Distress – Anxiety subscales of the MASQ were computed and averaged to obtain an estimate of anxiety symptoms. Similarly sums for the Loss of Interest and General Distress – Depression subscales of the MASQ were computed and averaged to produce an estimate of depression symptoms. Mean scores from the ERQ expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal subscales were used as measures of the two emotion regulation styles. Scores were excluded from analysis for a given subject when fewer than 50% of items were completed. One participant was missing data for the STAI-X2, ERQ and the PANAS, with an additional subject missing data for the ERQ, and another missing data on the MASQ. Descriptive statistics for self-reported measures can be found in Table 2.

Table 2.

Descriptive statistics for self-report scales. Trait anxiety scores used in analyses were mean scores for the STAI-X2, while mean scores for the negative affect subscale of the PANAS-General were used as trait negative affect scores. Sums for the Anxious Arousal and General Distress – Anxiety subscales of the MASQ were computed and averaged to obtain an estimate of anxiety symptoms. Similarly sums for the Loss of Interest and General Distress – Depression subscales of the MASQ computed and averaged to produce an estimate of depression symptoms. Mean scores from the ERQ expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal subscales were used as measures of the two emotion regulation styles.

Scale Minimum Maximum Mean SD
Trait Negative Affect 1 3.5 1.485 .486
Trait Anxiety 1.05 3.15 1.722 .478
Suppression 1 7 3.507 1.278
Reappraisal 2.33 7 5.24 .956
Anxiety Symptoms 14 34.5 18.834 4.008
 MASQ General Distress - Anxious Symptoms 11 43 16.629 4.991
 MASQ Anxious Arousal 17 36 21.038 4.076
Depression Symptoms 10 32 15.232 4.251
 MASQ General Distress – Depressive Symptoms 12 37 18.411 5.603
 MASQ Loss of Interest 8 27 12.053 3.279

Data Sharing

EMG and self-report data can be downloaded through the MIDUS-Colectica Portal (https://midus.colectica.org) or the Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research data archive (visit https://doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR36901 for data from the MASQ questionnaire, and https://doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR37094 for all other self-report and EMG data). Diffusion-weighted data (and other raw imaging data collected in MIDUS) are available by contacting SMS.

Experimental Design and Statistical Analysis

Facial electromyography task.

Participants viewed 30 negative, 30 neutral, and 30 positive images from the International Affective Picture System (Lang et al., 2008). Trials consisted of a 1 s fixation period followed by an image displayed for 4 s and were separated by a random 14–18 s intertrial interval. For the first .5 seconds of image presentation, images were surrounded by a yellow or purple border. To promote attention to the images, participants were asked to report the color of the border with a button press. Ag-AgCl Touchproof shielded electrodes were used to measure activity from the corrugator supercilii and zygomaticus major muscles.

BIOPAC hardware amplified raw electromyography signals, which were sampled at 1000 Hz using Acknowledge software. A 60 Hz notch filter was applied to the corrugator data. Artifacts were then removed, following visual inspection. Spectral power density (μV2/Hz) in the 30–200 Hz frequency band was estimated for 1 s epochs (extracted through Hanning windows with 50% overlap) of artifact-free data using a Fast Fourier Transform. The data were then log-transformed and used to create 12 baseline-corrected, one-second epochs for each valence condition, using the 1 second fixation period preceding image presentation as the baseline. Data were z-scored within subject and averaged to create estimates for three 4-second time windows for each valence condition, including an early time window representing activity during image presentation, a middle time window (0–4 seconds after image offset) and a late time window (4–8 seconds after image offset). Further information concerning the collection and processing of this data can be found in the Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research data archive (doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR37094) or see Van Reekum et al. (2011).

Statistical analysis.

Effects Tested.

Statistical analysis was performed in R v. 3.6 (Team, 2018). Linear regression was used to test whether greater FA values in each ROI were associated with altered corrugator recovery following negative images across two epochs: early recovery (0–4 s after image offset), and late recovery (4–8 s after image offset).

As a point of comparison, we also tested whether FA in each of the major fiber bundles was related to corrugator reactivity, i.e., corrugator activity during the 4 second image presentation. In the case of a significant relationship, the correlation for the corrugator response to neutral and positive images, as well as the zygomaticus responses to negative, neutral, and positive images for the same epoch was tested. Next relationships for FA values in each tract with trait anxiety and negative affect, as well as typical use of the emotion regulation strategies of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression were tested.

Both standardized and unstandardized beta-coefficients are reported, along with semi-partial correlation (sr) for regressions involving more than one independent variable. All regressions were checked for outliers using a Cook’s D cutoff of .1. Except where noted, removing outliers had no impact on whether an effect was significant, and all data points are used in the reported analyses.

Whether relationships between FA values in each tract and a given dependent variable varied for the left and right hemisphere was tested using difference tests for dependent correlations with the paired.r function in the R package, “psych” (v. 1.8.12; Revelle, 2018). None of our dependent variables exhibited a significantly different correlation with FA values coming from the left and right hemisphere for any of tracts investigated for any reported effects after correcting for multiple comparisons (all p-values > .06). Because of this, mean FA values averaged across hemisphere were used in all analyses.

Covariates.

Starting with middle age, aging is associated with a decline in white matter microstructure (Kochunov et al., 2012; Yeatman et al., 2014). Age is also often accompanied by changes in emotion functioning, with older adults reporting less negative emotion and stable levels of positive emotion (Carstensen et al., 2000; Charles et al., 2001; Charles & Carstensen, 2010; Schneider, 2018). Furthermore, Cotter et al. (2020) found that older adults with declining FA report worsening mood over time, while those with stable FA report improved mood. Therefore, age could cause a spurious relationship between FA and corrugator recovery by acting as a confounding variable. However, controlling for age may be an overly conservative standard, as age could also drive variability in the white matter microstructure in a given tract, which could in turn affect corrugator activity. Separate regressions were first run with an interaction term for FA for a given ROI with age and sex, respectively. When no significant interactions were found, the interaction term was dropped and demographic variables were instead included as additional covariates to test whether effects remained while controlling for age, sex, and race, as well as global FA. If an effect remains significant after including global FA, it suggests that the effect was specific to the tract in question, and not driven by global changes in white matter microstructure such as those expected to accompany aging (Kochunov et al., 2012; Yeatman et al., 2014).

Correction for Multiple Comparison.

Uncinate Fasciculus.

Regressions testing relationships between uncinate fasciculus and corrugator activity were Bonferroni-corrected for two comparisons, representing the time windows of interest (early and late recovery). For consistency, this same correction was applied to regressions relating uncinate fasciculus FA and the corrugator reactivity time-window, which were included as a point of comparison. Regressions between self-reports of depression and anxiety related traits or symptoms were controlled for four comparisons (trait anxiety, trait negative affect, anxiety symptoms, depression symptoms), while analyses involving self-reported dispositional use of emotion regulation strategies were controlled for two comparisons (expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal). Reported p-values have been adjusted accordingly using Bonferroni correction. This correction scheme was applied for main effects, as well as effects involving interactions with sex and age. For tables reporting diffusion measures other than FA, the same correction scheme was applied within each diffusion measure.

Exploratory Tracts.

Regressions testing relationships between the exploratory tracts and corrugator activity were corrected across the 9 tracts for regressions involving the reactivity time window and across the 9 tracts and 2 time points (i.e. 18 comparisons) for regressions including the 2 recovery time windows. This correction was applied using the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure for false discovery rate (FDR). FDR correction was also applied across the 9 tracts and 4 depression and anxiety self-report measures (36 comparisons), as well as the 9 tracts and the 2 emotion regulation self-report measures (18 comparisons). This correction scheme was applied for main effects, as well as effects involving interactions with hemisphere, sex and age. For tables reporting diffusion measures other than FA, the same correction scheme was applied within each diffusion measure.

Results

Manipulation Check

Consistent with expectations, greater corrugator activity was observed in response to negative vs. neutral images for the reactivity, t(107)=8.312, 98.3% CI[.52, .94], p<.001, early recovery, t(107)=6.055, 98.3% CI[.36, .85], p<.001, and late recovery time windows, t(107)=2.495, 98.3% CI[.01, .48], p=.042. Greater corrugator activity was also observed in response to negative vs. positive (reactivity: t(107)=11.904, 98.3% CI[.91, 1.37], p<.001; early recovery: t(107)=9.826, 98.3% CI[.7, 1.16], p<.001; late recovery: t(107)=4.758, 98.3% CI[.24, .74], p<.001), and neutral vs. positive images (reactivity: t(107)=5.035, 98.3% CI[.21, .6], p<.001; early recovery: t(107)=3.517, 98.3% CI[.1, .54], p=.002; late recovery: t(107)=2.644, 98.3% CI[.02, .48], p=.028) for all time windows (Figure 1). These results suggest that corrugator activity indexed affective reactions as expected, with higher activity for negative than neutral images, and lower activity for positive than neutral images.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Time-course for baseline-corrected z-scored corrugator activity by image valence condition. For each time-window (reactivity, early recovery, and late recovery), mean corrugator activity was larger for negative than neutral and larger for neutral than positive.

Zygomaticus activity was significantly larger on positive vs. neutral image trials for the reactivity, t(103)=4.919, 98.3% CI[.26, .77], p<.001, early recovery, t(103)=3.338, 98.3% CI[.1, .63], p=.004, but not late recovery time-windows, t(103)=1.419, 98.3% CI[−.12, .45], p=.477. Zygomaticus activity was also higher for positive vs. negative image trials for the reactivity, t(103)=5.732, 98.3% CI[.34, .84], p<.001, and early recovery, t(103)=3.852, 98.3% CI[.17, .73], p<.001, but not late recovery time-windows, t(103)=1.2, 98.3% CI[−.14, .41], p=.69. There was no difference for negative vs. neutral image trials for any time-window (all p-values > 1). These results align with past findings that zygomaticus activity is sensitive to positive affect, in comparison to both negative and neutral, but insensitive to negative vs. neutral affect (Tan et al., 2012).

FA and Electromyography

Uncinate Fasciculus

Mean uncinate fasciculus FA did not significantly interact with age (all p-values > .39) for any reported effects using electromyographic measures as outcome variables. Mean uncinate fasciculus FA was not related to the corrugator response during negative images, B=4.76, β=0.13, t(106)=1.3, p=0.392, or during early recovery from negative images, B=−4.03, β=−0.12, t(106)=−1.2, p=0.467. However, greater uncinate fasciculus FA was associated with lower late recovery corrugator activity following negative images, B=−14.72, β=−0.35, t(106)=−3.89, p<0.001 (Figure 2), and this effect remained when controlling for overall white matter FA, B=−18.57, β=−0.45, sr=−0.3, t(105)=−3.35, p=0.002, as well as when controlling for race, age and sex, B=−11.19, β=−0.27, sr=−0.23, t(100)=−2.55, p=.024. In contrast, we did not detect a relationship between uncinate fasciculus FA and late recovery corrugator activity following neutral, B=−3.58, β=−0.1, t(106)=−1.05, p=.596, or positive images, B=−6.11, β=−0.16, t(106)=−1.67, p=0.197, or late zygomaticus activity to negative, B=3.73, β=0.1, t(102)=1.02, p=.623, or positive images, B=−2.91, β=−0.08, t(102)=−0.83, p=.814. We did find an interaction between late zygomaticus activity following neutral images and sex, B=20.44, β=.23, t(100)=2.34, p=.043, with a positive relationship between late recovery zygomaticus activity and uncinate fasciculus FA in men, B=19.23, β=.39, t(44)=2.82, p=.014, but not women, B=−1.21, β=−.03, t(56)=−.22, p=1. Although, this interaction was no longer significant after the removal of one outlying observation (cook’s D = .39), B=19.77, β=.22, t(99)=2.26, p=.052. No other effects reported in this section interacted with sex (all p-values > .15). Table 3 presents results for relationships between the corrugator response and additional DTI measures, including mean diffusivity (MD), radial diffusivity (RD), and axial diffusivity (AD).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Partial plots depicting relationship between fractional anisotropy (FA) and mean baseline-corrected z-scored corrugator activity in response to negative images for reactivity (left), and late recovery (right) time windows while controlling for global FA. Late corrugator activity was significantly related to FA in the uncinate, inferior longitudinal, inferior fronto-occipital, and superior longitudinal fasciculi. Mean FA was not related to corrugator reactivity for any tract.

Table 3.

Effect of mean diffusion (MD), radial diffusion (RD), and axial diffusion (AD) on late recovery corrugator activity following negative images by ROI, while controlling for global white matter for a given diffusion measure. Significant p-values are in bold (see Statistical Analysis for correction for multiple comparison scheme).

Tracts MD RD AD

β t p β t p β t p
Uncinate Fasciculus .29 2.19 .062 .46 3.23 .003 −.03 −.31 1
Cingulum (Cingulate Portion) −.03 −.19 .942 .15 .81 .579 −.18 −1.67 .672
Cingulum (Hippocampal Portion) .15 1.04 .603 .2 1.5 .275 −.07 −.6 .887
Corticospinal Tract <.01 <.01 .997 .15 .99 .486 −.17 −1.36 .672
Forceps Major .45 3.14 .025 .45 3.23 .01 .03 .28 .887
Forceps Minor .29 2.17 .115 .32 2.38 .058 .03 .27 .887
Fornix .24 2.36 .09 .24 2.38 .058 .15 1.55 .672
Inferior Fronto-Occipital Fasciculus .48 3.06 .025 .63 3.79 .005 .07 .47 .887
Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus .45 2.68 .051 .6 3.59 .005 −.04 −.3 .887
Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus .34 1.68 .288 .48 2.43 .058 −.14 −.84 .756

Exploratory Tracts

FA did not significantly interact with age (all p-values>.14) or sex (all p-values>.25) for any of the reported effects with EMG measures as outcome variables for any of the exploratory tracts. There were no significant relationships between corrugator activity during negative image presentation and mean FA in any of the exploratory tracts (all p-values>.18).

There was a significant zero-order relation between early recovery corrugator activity and FA in the forceps major, B=−6.89, β=−0.23, t(106)=−2.48, p=0.044, but this relationship was no longer significant when controlling for either age, sex and race, B=−6.31, β=−0.21, sr=−0.2, t(100)=−2.13, p=.106, or global white matter FA, B=−8.29, β=−0.28, sr=−0.21, t(105)=−2.2, p=0.136. There were no other significant relationships between FA and corrugator activity during negative images in the early recovery time window (p>.08).

Table 4 contains results for relationships between FA in each of the exploratory tracts and late recovery corrugator activity following negative images with no covariates, while controlling for age, sex and race, and while controlling for global FA. There was a zero-order relation between corrugator activity following negative images in the late recovery time window and FA in the hippocampal portion of the cingulum bundle, corticospinal tract, forceps major, forceps minor, inferior-frontal occipital fasciculus, inferior longitudinal fasciculus and superior longitudinal fasciculus, but not the cingulate portion of the cingulum bundle or fornix. For the corticospinal tract, this effect was no longer significant after removing one outlier (Cook’s D=.13), B=−11.26, β=, t(105)=−2.27, p=.057. Despite these significant zero-order relationships, only inferior longitudinal fasciculus FA was significantly related to late recovery corrugator activity following negative images when controlling for age, sex and race. In addition, mean FA in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus and superior longitudinal fasciculus was significantly related to late recovery corrugator activity following negative images when controlling for global FA. In contrast, there were no significant relationships between inferior longitudinal fasciculus, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus or superior longitudinal fasciculus FA and late recovery corrugator activity following positive (inferior longitudinal fasciculus: B=−4.72, β=−0.12, t(106)=−1.19, p=0.423; inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus: B=−5.16, β=−0.12, t(106)=−1.28, p=0.423; superior longitudinal fasciculus: B=−5.45, β=−0.12, t(106)=−1.25, p=0.423) or neutral images (inferior longitudinal fasciculus: B=−3.26, β=−0.09, t(106)=−0.89, p=0.506; inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus: B=−4.15, β=−0.11, t(106)=−1.11, p=0.407; superior longitudinal fasciculus: B=−5.61, β=−0.13, t(106)=−1.39, p=0.272). Similarly, FA in these tracts was not significantly associated with late recovery zygomaticus activity following negative (inferior longitudinal fasciculus: B=4.55, β=0.11, t(102)=1.16, p=0.415; inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus: B=4.82, β=0.12, t(102)=1.21, p=0.415; superior longitudinal fasciculus: B=7.02, β=0.16, t(102)=1.64, p=.415), positive (inferior longitudinal fasciculus: B=−2.71, β=−0.07, t(102)=−0.72, p=0.91; inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus: B=−1.54, β=−0.04, t(102)=−0.4, p=0.91; superior longitudinal fasciculus: B=−2.7, β=−0.06, t(102)=−0.65, p=0.91) or neutral images (inferior longitudinal fasciculus: B=8.77, β=0.18, t(102)=1.86, p=.183; inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus: B=8.13, β=0.16, t(102)=1.69, p=.184; superior longitudinal fasciculus: B=11.05, β=0.21, t(102)=2.14, p=.183).

Table 4.

Effect of mean fractional anisotropy (FA) on late recovery corrugator activity following negative images by ROI. While all tracts except for the cingulate portion of the cingulum and fornix exhibited a zero-order relation between mean FA and late corrugator activity following negative images, only the uncinate, fronto-occipital, inferior longitudinal, and superior longitudinal fasciculi exhibited this relationship when controlling for global white matter FA. Significant p-values are in bold (see Statistical Analysis for correction for multiple comparison scheme).

Tract Zero-Order Controlling Age, Sex and Race Controlling Global FA
β t p β t p β t p
Uncinate Fasciculus −.35 −3.89 <.001 −.27 −2.55 .024 −.45 −3.35 .002
Cingulum (Cingulate Portion) −.21 −2.18 .063 −.15 −1.52 .257 −.13 −.92 .499
Cingulum (Hippocampal Portion) −.23 −2.4 .047 −.18 −1.89 .139 −.17 −1.58 .291
Corticospinal Tract −.22 −2.33 .049 −.23 −2.44 .06 −.16 −1.37 .346
Forceps Major −.27 −2.94 .018 −.24 −2.45 .06 −.26 −2.03 .16
Forceps Minor −.25 −2.67 .032 −.14 −1.22 .366 −.23 −1.64 .291
Fornix −.19 −1.97 .088 −.1 −.94 .526 −.13 −1.24 .394
Inferior Fronto- Occipital Fasciculus −.35 −3.85 .002 −.28 −2.66 .06 −.62 −3.76 .002
Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus −.37 −4.07 .002 −.32 −3.19 .034 −.56 −3.85 .002
Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus −.32 −3.53 .004 −.26 −2.45 .06 −.51 −3.13 .014

Self-Reports

Uninate Fasciculus

There were no significant interactions between uncinate fasciculus FA and either age (all p-values>.76) or sex (all p-values>.52) on any of the self-reported measures. There was no significant relationship between uncinate fasciculus FA and self-reported anxiety symptoms, B=−25.77, β=−0.13, t(105)=−1.35, p=.724, depression symptoms, B=−29.9, β=−0.14, t(105)=−1.48, p=.572, trait anxiety, B=−1.79, β=−0.08, t(105)=−0.78, p=1, or trait negative affect, B=0.49, β=0.02, t(105)=0.21, p=1. We also did not detect any associations between uncinate fasciculus FA and the dispositional use of the emotion regulation strategies of expressive suppression, B=7.7, β=0.12, t(104)=1.26, p=.424, or cognitive reappraisal, B=−1.01, β=−0.02, t(104)=−0.22, p=1. Supplementary Table 1 contains results for other diffusion measures, including MD, RD, and AD.

Exploratory Tracts

There were no significant interactions between FA and either age (all p-values>.98) or sex (all p-values>.75) for any of the self-reported measures in any of the exploratory tracts. There were no significant associations between FA and self-reported anxiety symptoms (all p-values>.72), depression symptoms (all p-values>.72), trait anxiety (all p-values>.73), trait negative affect (all p-values>.72), expressive suppression (all p-values>.61) or cognitive reappraisal (all p-values>.61) in any of the exploratory tracts.

Discussion

Our hypothesis that the white matter microstructure of the uncinate fasciculus would be related to better recovery from negative provocation was supported. Higher FA in this tract was related to lower corrugator activity 4–8 s following negative image offset while controlling for global FA, suggesting that these effects were not driven by global differences in FA. In a sample of children and adolescent participants, Swartz et al. (2014) found that uncinate fasciculus FA is associated with less amygdala activation to emotional faces, suggesting higher FA within the uncinate fasciculus may indicate white matter microstructure that facilitates neural communication underlying regulation of the amygdala via signals from the prefrontal cortex. Thus, our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the uncinate fasciculus facilitates top-down regulation signals from the prefrontal cortex to the amygdala, resulting in greater recovery following affective provocation. While past research has demonstrated that greater FA in the uncinate fasciculus is associated with more amygdala habituation to fearful faces (Hein et al., 2018), to our knowledge this is the first study to demonstrate that uncinate fasciculus FA is associated with individual differences in the time-course of the affective response, specifically mean recovery 4–8 s after offset, to individual negative stimuli.

In an exploratory analysis, we found that higher FA in the inferior fronto-occipital, inferior longitudinal and superior longitudinal fasciculi was also related to reduced corrugator activity in the late recovery time window when controlling for global FA. This effect was also significant in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus while controlling for sex, race, and age. In the inferior fronto-occipital and superior longitudinal fasciculi this effect was at trend level when controlling for sex, race, and age. These findings suggest that these tracts may participate in the emotional response to negative images and support the disengagement of emotional processing following the offset of emotional stimuli.

Both the inferior longitudinal and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculi are thought to support the modulation of visual processing for emotionally salient stimuli (Latini, 2015; Pessoa & Adolphs, 2010; Rudrauf et al., 2008). The inferior longitudinal fasciculus originates in the dorsolateral occipital cortex, cuneus, fusiform gyrus and lingual gyrus and projects to anterior temporal cortex (Latini et al., 2017). This tract is thought to support the transmission of information from early visual cortices to the amygdala (Latini, 2015). This connection enables the amygdala to rapidly detect affective stimuli and modulate visual processing through projections that run back to the visual cortex (Pessoa & Adolphs, 2010; Rudrauf et al., 2008), which also likely run through the inferior longitudinal fasciculus. This is supported by studies finding that damage to the inferior longitudinal fasciculus is related to impaired recognition of emotional facial expressions (Crespi et al., 2014; Genova et al., 2015; Philippi et al., 2009). As such, our finding that low FA in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus leads to sustained responses to negative stimuli may be tied to its role in modulating the processing of affective stimuli, although further research is needed to elucidate the exact mechanism for this effect.

The inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus originates in the visual cortex, including the lingual gyrus, posterior fusiform, cuneus and polar occipital cortex, and travels through the temporal stem and external/extreme capsules terminating in the inferior frontal gyrus, fronto-orbital region and frontal pole (Forkel et al., 2014). This pathway may enable the orbitofrontal cortex to extract affective salience from early visual areas and to modulate visual processing via projections back to the occipital lobe (Pessoa & Adolphs, 2010; Rudrauf et al., 2008). While the orbitofrontal cortex has been implicated in the attentional bias toward negatively-valenced stimuli (Hartikainen et al., 2012; Murphy & Bachevalier, 2020), it is also involved in emotion regulation (Dixon et al., 2017; Goldin et al., 2008). Hooker and Knight (2006) argue that the lateral orbitofrontal cortex inhibits the processing of task-irrelevant affective stimuli by down-regulating their processing in sensory regions, including the visual cortex (Bishop, 2008; Vuilleumier et al., 2001). This suggests that connections facilitated by the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus may be important for emotion regulation, including disengaging attention from negatively valenced stimuli. If so, high white matter integrity of the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus may contribute to greater emotion regulation following negatively valenced stimuli by allowing more efficient disengagement of attention from them.

The superior longitudinal fasciculus connects the frontal lobe with the parietal lobe and temporoparietal junction region (Kamali et al., 2014; Makris et al., 2005; X. Wang et al., 2016). This tract is thought to play an important role in language, the regulation of motor behavior, as well as visuospatial attention and working memory (Koshiyama et al., 2020; Nakajima et al., 2020). Connections between the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices may play a role in preventing salient distractors from entering working memory (Jacob & Nieder, 2014; Lanssens et al., 2020; Minamoto et al., 2012) and disengaging attention from threat-related stimuli (De Raedt et al., 2010; Mulckhuyse et al., 2017). In addition, both the prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal regions are involved in the emotion regulation strategy of cognitive reappraisal (Buhle et al., 2014; Morawetz et al., 2017). It is thought that dorsolateral prefrontal cortex connections with posterior parietal cortex support cognitive reappraisal by directing attention and working memory to select a reappraisal strategy that will achieve a given regulatory goal (Silvers & Guassi Moreira, 2019). As such, high integrity in the superior longitudinal fasciculus may result in lower corrugator activity following emotional stimuli by facilitating connections between regions important for disengaging from negatively valenced stimuli (De Raedt et al., 2010; Mulckhuyse et al., 2017), and emotion regulation (Silvers & Guassi Moreira, 2019).

Our results demonstrate that the neural mechanisms related to variation in reactivity to and recovery from emotional provocation are distinct. Affective processes are dynamic, and individual differences in how these processes unfold over time constitute important aspects of affective style. Sustained responses to negative affective provocation are related to mood dysphoria (Taubitz et al., 2013), trait hostility (Fredrickson et al., 2000), low conscientiousness (Javaras et al., 2012), and lower well-being (Schaefer et al., 2013) and may indicate reduced capacity for emotion regulation (Lee et al., 2009; Schaefer et al., 2018). Further research is needed to investigate how variation in uncinate, inferior longitudinal, inferior fronto-occipital and superior longitudinal fasciculi white matter microstructure is related to personality and other aspects of affective style.

White matter microstructure was not significantly related to symptoms of anxiety and depression, trait anxiety, trait negative affect, or dispositional use of the emotion regulation strategies of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression for the tracts investigated in our community sample. This suggests that the time-course of participant’s corrugator responses to images indexed a portion of their affective profile that was not accounted for in these self-report measures. Past research has demonstrated that corrugator activity 4–8 s after picture offset also corresponds to other affect-related individual differences, such as marital stress, symptoms of depression (Lapate et al., 2014), and conscientiousness (Javaras et al., 2012). Our results highlight the importance of measuring individual differences in affect in multiple ways, as differing methods may assess different constructs, and are susceptible to different sources of error variance (Mauss & Robinson, 2009). However, it remains unclear why past studies have found relationships between the uncinate fasciculus white matter microstructure and measures of depression and anxiety (d’Arbeloff et al., 2018; Hettema et al., 2012; Liao et al., 2014; Phan et al., 2009), while we did not. Many of these past studies recruited groups of participants with and without anxiety or mood disorders, while our sample was intended to be representative of the U.S. population (Ryff et al., 2015). As a result, our sample may have limited range of anxiety and depression symptoms, with fewer participants reporting clinically significant levels. Schalet et al. (2014) suggested that a score of 25 or higher on the General Distress – Anxiety subscale of the MASQ may represent clinically-relevant anxiety symptomatology. In our sample, 5.6% of participants met this criterion. Breddmeier et al. (2010) suggested a cut-off of 23 or higher on the loss of interest subscale of the MASQ when screening for possible current major depressive episode. Two of our subjects (1.9%) reached this cut-off. While our results suggest a relationship between uncinate fasciculus FA and affective functioning in a relatively psychologically healthy sample, associations between self-reported anxiety and depression with white matter microstructure in this tract may be more likely to emerge when the sample includes a large proportion of participants with clinical levels of anxiety or mood disorders.

A major limitation to the current study is the correlational nature of our findings. While past studies demonstrating an association between uncinate fasciculus FA and both emotion regulation (Zuurbier et al., 2013) and amygdala reactivity (Swartz et al., 2014) suggest plausible mechanisms whereby the white matter microstructure of the uncinate fasciculus may affect emotional recovery, the current analysis cannot establish causality. Similarly, while past work on mood and anxiety disorders suggests that the inferior longitudinal, inferior fronto-occipital and superior longitudinal fasciculi may each play a role in affective processing (Jenkins et al., 2016), the particular mechanism underlying a relationship between the white matter microstructure of these tracts and emotional recovery requires further study. Additionally, given that results for the inferior longitudinal fasciculus, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus and superior longitudinal fasciculus were the result of post-hoc analyses employing false discovery rate correction, a relatively liberal method, these findings should be interpreted cautiously. Future research should employ longitudinal designs and/or manipulations aimed at altering affective functioning – such as emotion regulation training – to better understand the relationship between the microstructure of these tracts and emotional recovery. We hope to do so in future waves of the MIDUS project.

Our results implicate the uncinate fasciculus in the recovery of emotional responses to negative provocation. This finding is consistent with work suggesting that this tract carries fibers involved in regulating affective processing (d’Arbeloff et al., 2018; Delgado et al., 2008; Zuurbier et al., 2013), and suggests that this tract may participate in implicit emotion regulation processes. White matter microstructure of the uncinate fasciculus may be an important neural correlate of individual differences in affective functioning. Our results also suggest that the inferior longitudinal, inferior fronto-occipital and superior longitudinal fasciculi support emotional recovery following negative stimuli. This finding suggests a potential mechanism for the involvement of these tracts in mood and anxiety disorders (Jenkins et al., 2016). While we found no evidence for a relationship between the microstructure of any of these tracts and self-reported anxiety and depression symptomatology in the current sample, future work should continue to investigate the role that these tracts may play in emotional processes contributing to vulnerability to psychopathology in addition to well-being.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Figure and Table

Funding

The MIDUS Refresher study was funded by the National Institute on Aging (P01-AG020166, U19-AG051426). The MIDUS Neuroscience Project data collection was supported in part by a core grant to the Waisman Center from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (U54 HD090256). WSP was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (T32MH018931) and the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (1F32AT010101–01). DCD was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (4 R00 MH110596 03). NA was supported in part by grants from the following: The BRAIN Initiative (R01-EB022883), the Waisman Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center (U54-HD090256), the Center for Predictive and Computational Phenotyping (CPCP; U54-AI117924), and the Alzheimer’s Disease Connectome Project (ADCP; UF1-AG051216, R56-AG037639, R01-AG037639, and RF1-AG059312).

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest Statement

R.J.D. serves on the board of directors for the non-profit organization Healthy Minds Innovations. The other authors report no perceived or real conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Adluru N, Luo Z, Van Hulle CA, Schoen AJ, Davidson RJ, Alexander AL, & Goldsmith HH (2017). Anxiety-related experience-dependent white matter structural differences in adolescence: A monozygotic twin difference approach. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–11. 10.1038/s41598-017-08107-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexander AL, Hurley SA, Samsonov AA, Adluru N, Hosseinbor AP, Mossahebi P, Tromp DPM, Zakszewski E, & Field AS (2011). Characterization of Cerebral White Matter Properties Using Quantitative Magnetic Resonance Imaging Stains. Brain Connectivity, 1(6), 423–446. 10.1089/brain.2011.0071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersson JLR, & Sotiropoulos SN (2016). An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage, 125, 1063–1078. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Avants BB, Tustison N, & Song G. (2009). Advanced normalization tools (ANTS). Insight J, 2(Journal Article), 1–35. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bhatia KD, Henderson LA, Hsu E, & Yim M. (2018). Reduced integrity of the uncinate fasciculus and cingulum in depression: A stem-by-stem analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 235, 220–228. 10.1016/j.jad.2018.04.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bishop SJ (2008). Neural mechanisms underlying selective attention to threat. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1129(1), 141–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blackford JU, Avery SN, Shelton RC, & Zald DH (2009). Amygdala temporal dynamics: Temperamental differences in the timing of amygdala response to familiar and novel faces. BMC Neuroscience, 10(1), 145. 10.1186/1471-2202-10-145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bracht T, Linden D, & Keedwell P. (2015). A review of white matter microstructure alterations of pathways of the reward circuit in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 187, 45–53. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.06.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bracht T, Tüscher O, Schnell S, Kreher B, Rüsch N, Glauche V, Lieb K, Ebert D, Il’yasov KA, Hennig J, Weiller C, van Elst LT, & Saur D. (2009). Extraction of prefronto-amygdalar pathways by combining probability maps. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 174(3), 217–222. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, Weber J, & Ochsner KN (2014). Cognitive Reappraisal of Emotion: A Meta-Analysis of Human Neuroimaging Studies. Cerebral Cortex, 24(11), 2981–2990. 10.1093/cercor/bht154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cacioppo JT, Petty RE, Losch ME, & Kim HS (1986). Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and intensity of affective reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 260–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carstensen LL, Pasupathi M, Mayr U, & Nesselroade JR (2000). Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 644–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chanraud S, Zahr N, Sullivan EV, & Pfefferbaum A. (2010). MR Diffusion Tensor Imaging: A Window into White Matter Integrity of the Working Brain. Neuropsychology Review, 20(2), 209–225. 10.1007/s11065-010-9129-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Charles ST, & Carstensen LL (2010). Social and Emotional Aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 61(1), 383–409. 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100448 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Charles ST, Reynolds CA, & Gatz M. (2001). Age-related differences and change in positive and negative affect over 23 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 136–151. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Clewett D, Bachman S, & Mather M. (2014). Age-related reduced prefrontal-amygdala structural connectivity is associated with lower trait anxiety. Neuropsychology, 28(4), 631–642. 10.1037/neu0000060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cotter DL, Walters SM, Fonseca C, Wolf A, Cobigo Y, Fox EC, You MY, Altendahl M, Djukic N, Staffaroni AM, Elahi FM, Kramer JH, & Casaletto KB (2020). Aging and Positive Mood: Longitudinal Neurobiological and Cognitive Correlates. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28(9), 946–956. 10.1016/j.jagp.2020.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Crespi C, Cerami C, Dodich A, Canessa N, Arpone M, Iannaccone S, Corbo M, Lunetta C, Scola E, Falini A, & Cappa SF (2014). Microstructural white matter correlates of emotion recognition impairment in Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis. Cortex, 53, 1–8. 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. d’Arbeloff TC, Kim MJ, Knodt AR, Radtke SR, Brigidi BD, & Hariri AR (2018). Microstructural integrity of a pathway connecting the prefrontal cortex and amygdala moderates the association between cognitive reappraisal and negative emotions. Emotion, 18(6), 912–915. 10.1037/emo0000447 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Davis M, & Whalen PJ (2001). The amygdala: Vigilance and emotion. Molecular Psychiatry, 6(1), 13–34. 10.1038/sj.mp.4000812 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. de Kwaasteniet B, Ruhe E, Caan M, Rive M, Olabarriaga S, Groefsema M, Heesink L, van Wingen G, & Denys D. (2013). Relation Between Structural and Functional Connectivity in Major Depressive Disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 74(1), 40–47. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. De Raedt R, Leyman L, Baeken C, Van Schuerbeek P, Luypaert R, Vanderhasselt M-A, & Dannlowski U. (2010). Neurocognitive effects of HF-rTMS over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on the attentional processing of emotional information in healthy women: An event-related fMRI study. Biological Psychology, 85(3), 487–495. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Delgado MR, Nearing KI, LeDoux JE, & Phelps EA (2008). Neural Circuitry Underlying the Regulation of Conditioned Fear and Its Relation to Extinction. Neuron, 59(5), 829–838. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Diekhof EK, Geier K, Falkai P, & Gruber O. (2011). Fear is only as deep as the mind allows: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on the regulation of negative affect. NeuroImage, 58(1), 275–285. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dixon ML, Thiruchselvam R, Todd R, & Christoff K. (2017). Emotion and the prefrontal cortex: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143(10), 1033–1081. 10.1037/bul0000096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Forkel SJ, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Kawadler JM, Dell’Acqua F, Danek A, & Catani M. (2014). The anatomy of fronto-occipital connections from early blunt dissections to contemporary tractography. Cortex, 56, 73–84. 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fox AS, & Shackman AJ (2017). The central extended amygdala in fear and anxiety: Closing the gap between mechanistic and neuroimaging research. Neuroscience Letters. 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.11.056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fredrickson BL, Maynard KE, Helms MJ, Haney TL, Siegler IC, & Barefoot JC (2000). Hostility predicts magnitude and duration of blood pressure response to anger. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 23(3), 229–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Garyfallidis E, Brett M, Amirbekian B, Rokem A, van der Walt S, Descoteaux M, Nimmo-Smith I, & Dipy Contributors. (2014). Dipy, a library for the analysis of diffusion MRI data. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 8. 10.3389/fninf.2014.00008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Genova HM, Rajagopalan V, Chiaravalloti N, Binder A, Deluca J, & Lengenfelder J. (2015). Facial affect recognition linked to damage in specific white matter tracts in traumatic brain injury. Social Neuroscience, 10(1), 27–34. 10.1080/17470919.2014.959618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Goldin PR, McRae K, Ramel W, & Gross JJ (2008). The Neural Bases of Emotion Regulation: Reappraisal and Suppression of Negative Emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63(6), 577–586. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Greenberg T, Bertocci MA, Versace A, Lima Santos JP, Chase HW, Siffler R, Aslam HA, Graur S, Bebko G, Lockovich JC, & Phillips ML (2021). Depression and anxiety mediate the relationship between frontotemporal white matter integrity and quality of life in distressed young adults. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 132, 55–59. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.10.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gross JJ, & John OP (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348–362. 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hartikainen KM, Ogawa KH, & Knight RT (2012). Orbitofrontal cortex biases attention to emotional events. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34(6), 588–597. 10.1080/13803395.2012.666231 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hein TC, Mattson WI, Dotterer HL, Mitchell C, Lopez-Duran N, Thomason ME, Peltier SJ, Welsh RC, Hyde LW, & Monk CS (2018). Amygdala habituation and uncinate fasciculus connectivity in adolescence: A multi-modal approach. NeuroImage, 183, 617–626. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hettema JM, Kettenmann B, Ahluwalia V, McCarthy C, Kates WR, Schmitt JE, Silberg JL, Neale MC, Kendler KS, & Fatouros P. (2012). A Pilot Multimodal Twin Imaging Study of Generalized Anxiety Disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 29(3), 202–209. 10.1002/da.20901 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hooker CI, & Knight RT (2006). The role of lateral orbitofrontal cortex in the inhibitory control of emotion. In Zald D. & Rauch S. (Eds.), The Orbitofrontal Cortex (pp. 307–324). Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198565741.003.0012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jacob SN, & Nieder A. (2014). Complementary Roles for Primate Frontal and Parietal Cortex in Guarding Working Memory from Distractor Stimuli. Neuron, 83(1), 226–237. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Javaras KN, Schaefer SM, van Reekum CM, Lapate RC, Greischar LL, Bachhuber DR, Love GD, Ryff CD, & Davidson RJ (2012). Conscientiousness Predicts Greater Recovery from Negative Emotion. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 12(5), 875–881. 10.1037/a0028105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jenkins LM, Barba A, Campbell M, Lamar M, Shankman SA, Leow AD, Ajilore O, & Langenecker SA (2016). Shared white matter alterations across emotional disorders: A voxel-based meta-analysis of fractional anisotropy. NeuroImage: Clinical, 12, 1022–1034. 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kamali A, Flanders AE, Brody J, Hunter JV, & Hasan KM (2014). Tracing superior longitudinal fasciculus connectivity in the human brain using high resolution diffusion tensor tractography. Brain Structure and Function, 219(1), 269–281. 10.1007/s00429-012-0498-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kellner E, Dhital B, Kiselev VG, & Reisert M. (2016). Gibbs-ringing artifact removal based on local subvoxel-shifts. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 76(5), 1574–1581. 10.1002/mrm.26054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kim MJ, Brown AC, Mattek AM, Chavez SJ, Taylor JM, Palmer AL, Wu Y-C, & Whalen PJ (2016). The Inverse Relationship between the Microstructural Variability of Amygdala-Prefrontal Pathways and Trait Anxiety Is Moderated by Sex. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 10. 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kim MJ, & Whalen PJ (2009). The Structural Integrity of an Amygdala-Prefrontal Pathway Predicts Trait Anxiety. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(37), 11614–11618. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2335-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Koch K, Reeß TJ, Rus OG, Zimmer C, & Zaudig M. (2014). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD): A review. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 54, 26–35. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kochunov P, Williamson DE, Lancaster J, Fox P, Cornell J, Blangero J, & Glahn DC (2012). Fractional anisotropy of water diffusion in cerebral white matter across the lifespan. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(1), 9–20. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.01.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Koshiyama D, Fukunaga M, Okada N, Morita K, Nemoto K, Yamashita F, Yamamori H, Yasuda Y, Matsumoto J, Fujimoto M, Kudo N, Azechi H, Watanabe Y, Kasai K, & Hashimoto R. (2020). Association between the superior longitudinal fasciculus and perceptual organization and working memory: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuroscience Letters, 738, 135349. 10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, & Cuthbert BN (2008). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. In Technical Report A-8. University of Florida, Gainesville, FL. [Google Scholar]
  49. Lanssens A, Pizzamiglio G, Mantini D, & Gillebert CR (2020). Role of the dorsal attention network in distracter suppression based on features. Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(1–2), 37–46. 10.1080/17588928.2019.1683525 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lapate RC, van Reekum CM, Schaefer SM, Greischar LL, Norris CJ, Bachhuber DRW, Ryff CD, & Davidson RJ (2014). Prolonged Marital Stress is Associated with Short-Lived Responses to Positive Stimuli. Psychophysiology, 51(6), 499–509. 10.1111/psyp.12203 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Larsen JT, Norris CJ, & Cacioppo JT (2003). Effects of positive and negative affect on electromyographic activity over zygomaticus major and corrugator supercilii. Psychophysiology, 40(5), 776–785. 10.1111/1469-8986.00078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Latini F. (2015). New insights in the limbic modulation of visual inputs: The role of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus and the Li-Am bundle. Neurosurgical Review, 38(1), 179–190. 10.1007/s10143-014-0583-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Latini F, Mårtensson J, Larsson E-M, Fredrikson M, Åhs F, Hjortberg M, Aldskogius H, & Ryttlefors M. (2017). Segmentation of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus in the human brain: A white matter dissection and diffusion tensor tractography study. Brain Research, 1675, 102–115. 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lau JYF, Guyer AE, Tone EB, Jenness J, Parrish JM, Pine DS, & Nelson EE (2012). Neural responses to peer rejection in anxious adolescents: Contributions from the amygdala-hippocampal complex. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 36(1), 36–44. 10.1177/0165025411406854 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lee H, Shackman AJ, Jackson DC, & Davidson RJ (2009). Test-retest reliability of voluntary emotion regulation. Psychophysiology, 46(4), 874–879. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00830.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Liao M, Yang F, Zhang Y, He Z, Su L, & Li L. (2014). White matter abnormalities in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder: A diffusion tensor imaging study. BMC Psychiatry, 14(1), 41. 10.1186/1471-244X-14-41 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Makris N, Kennedy DN, McInerney S, Sorensen AG, Wang R, Caviness VS, & Pandya DN (2005). Segmentation of Subcomponents within the Superior Longitudinal Fascicle in Humans: A Quantitative, In Vivo, DT-MRI Study. Cerebral Cortex, 15(6), 854–869. 10.1093/cercor/bhh186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Mauss IB, Bunge SA, & Gross JJ (2007). Automatic Emotion Regulation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1(1), 146–167. 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00005.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Mauss IB, & Robinson MD (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition & Emotion, 23(2), 209–237. 10.1080/02699930802204677 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Minamoto T, Osaka M, Engle RW, & Osaka N. (2012). Incidental encoding of goal irrelevant information is associated with insufficient engagement of the dorsal frontal cortex and the inferior parietal cortex. Brain Research, 1429, 82–97. 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.10.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Modi S, Trivedi R, Singh K, Kumar P, Rathore RKS, Tripathi RP, & Khushu S. (2013). Individual differences in trait anxiety are associated with white matter tract integrity in fornix and uncinate fasciculus: Preliminary evidence from a DTI based tractography study. Behavioural Brain Research, 238, 188–192. 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Montag C, Reuter M, Weber B, Markett S, & Schoene-Bake J-C (2012). Individual differences in trait anxiety are associated with white matter tract integrity in the left temporal lobe in healthy males but not females. Neuroscience, 217, 77–83. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.05.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Morawetz C, Bode S, Derntl B, & Heekeren HR (2017). The effect of strategies, goals and stimulus material on the neural mechanisms of emotion regulation: A meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 72, 111–128. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Mulckhuyse M, Engelmann JB, Schutter DJLG, & Roelofs K. (2017). Right posterior parietal cortex is involved in disengaging from threat: A 1-Hz rTMS study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(11), 1814–1822. 10.1093/scan/nsx111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Murphy LE, & Bachevalier J. (2020). Damage to Orbitofrontal Areas 12 and 13, but Not Area 14, Results in Blunted Attention and Arousal to Socioemotional Stimuli in Rhesus Macaques. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 14. 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Nakajima R, Kinoshita M, Shinohara H, & Nakada M. (2020). The superior longitudinal fascicle: Reconsidering the fronto-parietal neural network based on anatomy and function. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 14(6), 2817–2830. 10.1007/s11682-019-00187-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Pedersen WS, Schaefer SM, Gresham LK, Lee SD, Kelly MP, Mumford JA, Oler JA, & Davidson RJ (2020). Higher resting-state BNST-CeA connectivity is associated with greater corrugator supercilii reactivity to negatively valenced images. NeuroImage, 207, 116428. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116428 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Peng Z, Lui SSY, Cheung EFC, Jin Z, Miao G, Jing J, & Chan RCK (2012). Brain structural abnormalities in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Converging evidence from white matter and grey matter. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 5(4), 290–296. 10.1016/j.ajp.2012.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pessoa L, & Adolphs R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 11(11), 773–783. 10.1038/nrn2920 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Petrides M, & Pandya DN (2007). Efferent Association Pathways from the Rostral Prefrontal Cortex in the Macaque Monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(43), 11573–11586. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2419-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Phan KL, Orlichenko A, Boyd E, Angstadt M, Coccaro EF, Liberzon I, & Arfanakis K. (2009). Preliminary Evidence of White Matter Abnormality in the Uncinate Fasciculus in Generalized Social Anxiety Disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66(7), 691–694. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Philippi CL, Mehta S, Grabowski T, Adolphs R, & Rudrauf D. (2009). Damage to Association Fiber Tracts Impairs Recognition of the Facial Expression of Emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(48), 15089–15099. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0796-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Piras F, Piras F, Caltagirone C, & Spalletta G. (2013). Brain circuitries of obsessive compulsive disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10, Part 2), 2856–2877. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Revelle W. (2018). psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research (1.8.12) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych
  75. Roy M, Shohamy D, & Wager TD (2012). Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical systems and the generation of affective meaning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 147–156. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rudrauf D, David O, Lachaux J-P, Kovach CK, Martinerie J, Renault B, & Damasio A. (2008). Rapid Interactions between the Ventral Visual Stream and Emotion-Related Structures Rely on a Two-Pathway Architecture. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(11), 2793–2803. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3476-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Ryff CD, Almeida D, Ayanian J, Binkley N, Carr DS, Coe CL, Davidson RJ, Grzywacz J, Karlamangla A, Krueger RF, Lachman M, Love GD, Mailick M, Mroczek D, Radler B, Seeman T, Sloan R, Thomas D, Weinstein M, & Williams D. (2015). Midlife in the United States National Study of Health and Well-Being Field Report For the MIDUS Refresher Telephone Recruitment Interview and Self-Administered Questionnaire (P9904 / P9905). http://www.icpsr.umich.edu/cgi-bin/file?comp=none&study=36532&ds=0&file_id=1245474&path=ICPSR
  78. Schaefer SM, Boylan JM, Reekum CM van Lapate RC, Norris CJ, Ryff CD, & Davidson RJ (2013). Purpose in Life Predicts Better Emotional Recovery from Negative Stimuli. PLOS ONE, 8(11), e80329. 10.1371/journal.pone.0080329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Schaefer SM, van Reekum CM, Lapate RC, Heller AS, Grupe DW, & Davidson RJ (2018). The Temporal Dynamics of Emotional Responding: Implications for Well-Being and Health From the MIDUS Neuroscience Project. In Ryff CD & Krueger RF (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Integrative Health Science (pp. 354–366). Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190676384.013.27 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Schalet BD, Cook KF, Choi SW, & Cella D. (2014). Establishing a common metric for self-reported anxiety: Linking the MASQ, PANAS, and GAD-7 to PROMIS Anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(1), 88–96. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.11.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Schneider S. (2018). Extracting Response Style Bias From Measures of Positive and Negative Affect in Aging Research. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 73(1), 64–74. 10.1093/geronb/gbw103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Schuyler BS, Kral TRA, Jacquart J, Burghy CA, Weng HY, Perlman DM, Bachhuber DRW, Rosenkranz MA, MacCoon DG, van Reekum CM, Lutz A, & Davidson RJ (2014). Temporal dynamics of emotional responding: Amygdala recovery predicts emotional traits. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(2), 176–181. 10.1093/scan/nss131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Shiba Y, Oikonomidis L, Sawiak S, Fryer TD, Hong YT, Cockcroft G, Santangelo AM, & Roberts AC (2017). Converging Prefronto-Insula-Amygdala Pathways in Negative Emotion Regulation in Marmoset Monkeys. Biological Psychiatry, 82(12), 895–903. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.06.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Siegle GJ, Steinhauer SR, Thase ME, Stenger VA, & Carter CS (2002). Can’t shake that feeling: Event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals. Biological Psychiatry, 51(9), 693–707. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01314-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Silvers JA, & Guassi Moreira JF (2019). Capacity and tendency: A neuroscientific framework for the study of emotion regulation. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 35–39. 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.09.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Smith SM (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain Mapping, 17(3), 143–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, & Jacobs GA (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press. [Google Scholar]
  88. Stejskal EO, & Tanner JE (1965). Spin Diffusion Measurements: Spin Echoes in the Presence of a Time-Dependent Field Gradient. The Journal of Chemical Physics, 42(1), 288–292. 10.1063/1.1695690 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  89. Swartz JR, Carrasco M, Wiggins JL, Thomason ME, & Monk CS (2014). Age-related changes in the structure and function of prefrontal cortex–amygdala circuitry in children and adolescents: A multi-modal imaging approach. NeuroImage, 86, 212–220. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Tan J-W, Walter S, Scheck A, Hrabal D, Hoffmann H, Kessler H, & Traue HC (2012). Repeatability of facial electromyography (EMG) activity over corrugator supercilii and zygomaticus major on differentiating various emotions. Journal of Ambient Intelligence and Humanized Computing, 3(1), 3–10. 10.1007/s12652-011-0084-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Taubitz LE, Robinson JS, & Larson CL (2013). Modulation of the startle reflex across time by unpleasant pictures distinguishes dysphoric from non-dysphoric women. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 87(2), 124–129. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Team, R. C. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  93. Tournier J-D, Smith RE, Raffelt DA, Tabbara R, Dhollander T, Pietsch M, Christiaens D, Jeurissen B, Yeh C-H, & Connelly A. (2019). MRtrix3: A fast, flexible and open software framework for medical image processing and visualisation. BioRxiv, 551739. 10.1101/551739 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Van Reekum CM, Schaefer SM, Lapate RC, Norris CJ, Greischar LL, & Davidson RJ (2011). Aging is associated with positive responding to neutral information but reduced recovery from negative information. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 6(2), 177–185. 10.1093/scan/nsq031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Veraart J, Novikov DS, Christiaens D, Ades-aron B, Sijbers J, & Fieremans E. (2016). Denoising of diffusion MRI using random matrix theory. NeuroImage, 142, 394–406. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, & Dolan RJ (2001). Effects of Attention and Emotion on Face Processing in the Human Brain: An Event-Related fMRI Study. Neuron, 30(3), 829–841. 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00328-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Wang W, Qian S, Liu K, Li B, Li M, Xin K, & Sun G. (2016). Reduced white matter integrity and its correlation with clinical symptom in first-episode, treatment-naive generalized anxiety disorder. Behavioural Brain Research, 314, 159–164. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.08.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Wang X, Pathak S, Stefaneanu L, Yeh F-C, Li S, & Fernandez-Miranda JC (2016). Subcomponents and connectivity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus in the human brain. Brain Structure and Function, 221(4), 2075–2092. 10.1007/s00429-015-1028-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Watson D, & Clark LA (1991). The mood and anxiety symptom questionnaire (MASQ). Unpublished Manuscript, University of Iowa, Iowa City. [Google Scholar]
  100. Watson D, Clark LA, & Tellegen A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Waugh CE, Zarolia P, Mauss IB, Lumian DS, Ford BQ, Davis TS, Ciesielski BG, Sams KV, & McRae K. (2016). Emotion regulation changes the duration of the BOLD response to emotional stimuli. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(10), 1550–1559. 10.1093/scan/nsw067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Westlye LT, Bjørnebekk A, Grydeland H, Fjell AM, & Walhovd KB (2011). Linking an Anxiety-Related Personality Trait to Brain White Matter Microstructure: Diffusion Tensor Imaging and Harm Avoidance. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(4), 369–377. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.24 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Yeatman JD, Wandell BA, & Mezer AA (2014). Lifespan maturation and degeneration of human brain white matter. Nature Communications, 5(1). 10.1038/ncomms5932 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Zhang A, Leow A, Ajilore O, Lamar M, Yang S, Joseph J, Medina J, Zhan L, & Kumar A. (2012). Quantitative tract-specific measures of uncinate and cingulum in major depression using diffusion tensor imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(4), 959–967. 10.1038/npp.2011.279 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Zhang H, Yushkevich PA, Rueckert D, & Gee JC (2007). Unbiased White Matter Atlas Construction Using Diffusion Tensor Images. In Ayache N, Ourselin S, & Maeder A. (Eds.), Medical Image Computing and Computer-Assisted Intervention – MICCAI 2007 (pp. 211–218). Springer Berlin Heidelberg. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Zhang S, & Arfanakis K. (2018). Evaluation of standardized and study-specific diffusion tensor imaging templates of the adult human brain: Template characteristics, spatial normalization accuracy, and detection of small inter-group FA differences. NeuroImage, 172, 40–50. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Zuurbier LA, Nikolova YS, Åhs F, & Hariri AR (2013). Uncinate fasciculus fractional anisotropy correlates with typical use of reappraisal in women but not men. Emotion, 13(3), 385–390. 10.1037/a0031163 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Figure and Table

RESOURCES