Abstract
Objective:
To determine if feeding problems are an indicator of developmental delay.
Study design:
In this prospective longitudinal cohort study, mothers of 3,597 children (49% female, 35% multiples) reported on their children’s feeding problems and developmental delays (using the Ages and Stages Questionnaire, ASQ) when children were 18, 24, and 30 months of age. Average scores of feeding problems were computed at each age, as well as a categorical score indicating persistently high feeding problems ≥90th percentile across time. The Battelle Developmental Inventory (BDI-2) assessed development in 5 domains for a subset of children at 4 years.
Results:
In adjusted analyses, feeding problems (per point increase) were increasingly associated with six ASQ domains from 18 months (Odds Ratios (ORs) ranged from 1.30 to 1.98) to 24 months (ORs = 2.07 – 2.69) to 30 months (ORs = 3.90 – 5.64). Compared with children who never experienced feeding problems, children who experienced high feeding problems at one or two time points were more than twice as likely to have a delay on all ASQ domains (ORs = 2.10 – 2.50), and children who experienced high feeding problems at all three time points were four or more times as likely to have a delay on all ASQ domains (ORs = 3.94 – 5.05). Children with one-point higher feeding problems at 30 months scored 3-4 points lower on all BDI-2 domains at 4 years.
Conclusions:
Frequent feeding problems, especially those that persist into the third year, could be used to identify children at risk for developmental delay for more targeted screening.
Children with severe developmental disabilities have high rates of feeding disorders1 that result in insufficient weight gain or growth. Feeding problems (eg, crying during meals, pushing food away, and gagging)2 are also common in young children without disabilities.3-6 Few population-based studies of associations between feeding problems and developmental delays are available, and feeding problems are often operationalized in a limited way (e.g., only picky eating). Feeding problems may relate to developmental delays because they are indicative of underlying neurological problems,7 they contribute to undernutrition and poor physical growth which impact development,8, 9 or they relate to emotional temperament,10, 11 which may contribute to social and behavioral delays.12
Feeding is a complex activity that requires coordinating oral-motor, neurologic (e.g., hunger cues), gastrointestinal, cognitive (e.g., sensation and perception), and social cues and feedback.7 Feeding problems can be organic or nonorganic (e.g., behavioral/social).13, 14 Feeding is a major parental concern,15 with nutrition/feeding discussed extensively at pediatric visits.16 Feeding problems can be stressful for parents,3, 17 and may reinforce coercive interaction patterns between parents and children.10, 18 The severity of feeding problems may be an early indicator of developmental delays and may indicate failure of one or more of its integrated systems (e.g., neurodevelopment, behavioral regulation, cognition).1
One longitudinal study noted stability in feeding problems from 5-20 months and 4.5 years, and that feeding problems at 5 months, and persistent problems over time, were associated with more problems in adaptive and social behavior at 4.5 years.19 In another prospective study, persistent feeding problems (refusing food and not eating well at 1.5, 3, and 6 years) were predictive of pervasive developmental problems at age 7 years.20 Additional cross-sectional studies also suggest a link between early childhood feeding disorders and developmental disabilities.21-23
The current study investigated associations between the frequency of feeding problems and developmental delays at three time points from 18 to 30 months. We explored feeding problems in the second and third years because this is when solid food feeding habits are established,24 and studies have demonstrated stability in feeding problems across early childhood.19, 22 In a subgroup, we explored associations between feeding problems at 30 months and performance on a standardized developmental test at 4 years. We hypothesized that feeding problems and developmental delays would be associated at all time points with stronger associations later in development and for high-level feeding problems that persist across the three time points.
Method
Participants
The Upstate KIDS Study is a population-based birth cohort designed to evaluate the impact of infertility treatment on child health.25 Infants conceived by infertility treatment were oversampled, but no association between treatment and early development was observed.26 A total of 5,034 mothers and 6,171 children were recruited into the study when children were 4 months old. Main analyses were restricted to 3,597 children with feeding behaviors and developmental delay data for at least one timepoint from 18 to 30 months of age, including 2,338 singletons and 1,259 multiples.
A subgroup of children was invited to participate in a developmental assessment at age 4 years including children who had a probable developmental delay on the Ages and Stages Questionnaire27 (ASQ) at age 30-36 months or the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers28 (MCHAT) at 18-24 months; children referred to the New York State Early Intervention Program for evaluation; twins and higher order multiples (regardless of their screening status); and a random selection of singleton children who passed both the ASQ and MCHAT. Of the 2,154 children invited, 601 (27.9%) participated and feeding problem data were available from 516 children, 408 (79%) of whom passed the ASQ and MCHAT, and 108 (21%) of whom failed one or both.
Human subjects research approval was obtained from participating institutions (NYSDOH IRB #07-097; UAlbany #08-179), and informed consent was obtained.
Procedures
Feeding Problems.
Mothers responded to questions, derived from previous research,8, 9 about how often their children engaged in behaviors indicating feeding problems (0=never, 1=rarely, 2=sometimes, 3=often) at 18, 24, and 30 months. Items focused on observable behaviors at mealtimes indicating distress, food refusal, and mechanical feeding difficulties. Additional items were asked at 24 and 30 months that reflect behaviors more common at older ages (Table I (available at www.jpeds.com) shows specific items). At each age, items were averaged to create scales indicating the severity of feeding problems (range = 0-3). Internal consistency (α) reliability ranged from .81 to .86 across age, and feeding problem scales were correlated across age, rs ranged from .37 to .45, ps < .001. A revised version of this scale has been validated in healthy and undernourished 6- to 24-month-old children in six countries.29
Table 1.
How often does your child do the following when given food? | Subscale | 18 months |
24 months |
30 months |
---|---|---|---|---|
Push food/spoon away | R | X | X | X |
Turns head away repeatedly | R | X | X | X |
Closes mouth when offered food | R | X | X | X |
Can’t chew solid foods | M | X | X | X |
Gags on food | M | X | X | X |
Holds food in mouth | M | X | X | X |
Spits food out | M | X | X | X |
Throws food | M | X | X | X |
Cries/screams during meals | M | X | X | X |
Refuses to eat a specific meal | R | X | X | |
Refuses to eat a specific type of food | R | X | X | |
Spills food | M | X |
Note. R= Item loaded on food refusal subscale. M = Item loaded on the mechanical/distress subscale. X = item was administered at this age. See Wright, Gurney 29 for a revised and validated version of this questionnaire.
In addition to the continuous feeding problems scale score, we coded persistent feeding problems as 0=never, 0 time points, 1=sometime, 1-2 time points, or 2=always, all 3 time points for scoring ≥ 90th percentile on the feeding problems score from 18-30 months. The 90th percentile cutoffs were 1.66 at 18 months, 1.64 at 24 months, and 1.75 at 30 months (on a 0-3 scale).
Developmental Delay.
Mothers completed the ASQ, third edition27 at 18, 24, and 30 months. The ASQ is a validated screening instrument designed to detect developmental impairments in five domains: fine motor, gross motor, communication, personal-social functioning, and problem-solving skills.30, 31 ASQ items were scored as “yes” (10 points), “sometimes” (5 points), and “not yet” (0 points). A probable developmental delay on a given domain of the ASQ is defined by a score that is two or more standard deviations below the United States national average for the age group. In addition to domain scores, a total ASQ delay score was computed as having a probable delay on any domain of the ASQ. Probable delay on the ASQ was somewhat stable across time, phi (ϕ)=.40–.55, ps<.001 for ASQ total, and ϕ=.33–.60, ps<.001 for subdomains.
At the 4-year clinic visit, a subset of children were administered the Battelle Developmental Inventory, Second Edition32 (BDI-2). The BDI-2 is a standardized (M=100, SD=15) assessment that is used to measure developmental skills. The BDI-2 provides a total score as well as 5 subdomain scores: motor, communication, cognitive, personal/social, and adaptive. The BDI-2 has demonstrated reliability, internal consistency, and content and criterion validity.33
Congenital Malformations.
Participants were linked to the New York State Department of Health Congenital Malformations Registry. Malformations include permanent conditions like chromosomal and central nervous system abnormalities as well as correctable issues (e.g., cleft lip/palate, cataract, and hernia). Overall, 244 children (6.7%) had one or more congenital malformations, which is slightly higher than the percentage across New York in 2008 (5.0%),34 probably explained by the oversampling of multiples, who have a higher rates of malformations than singletons.35
Statistical Analyses
All statistical analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4. To be eligible for inclusion in the analysis, participants had at least one measurement of feeding problems and ASQ from 18 to 30 months. Missing data was handled with multiple imputation using fully conditional regression estimation for continuously distributed variables and discriminant function estimation for binary variables. To account for differential missingness (detailed in the Appendix [available at www.jpeds.com]), all independent and dependent variables and covariates were included in the imputation model. Fifty datasets were imputed and statistics aggregated over the different datasets using Rubin’s rules.36
To assess associations between the feeding problems scale and developmental delay on the ASQ across age, generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) with restricted maximum likelihood estimation and the logit link were performed. Because multiple births were included in the sample, a random effect of family with unstructured covariance was included to account for potential within-family variance. In addition, child age was modeled with a random intercept with an auto-regressive covariance structure to account for correlations over time. An interaction between feeding problems and age group was modeled to capture age-varying associations between feeding problems and developmental delay, and if significant, analyses were stratified by age group. Following unadjusted analyses, models were adjusted for child sex, gestational age, sleep problems, and history of GERD, and maternal age, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, infertility treatment, parity, plurality, and depressive symptoms (see justification and measurement of covariates in the Appendix).
Next, persistence of high feeding problems (1 sometime and 2 always ≥ 90th percentile versus 0 never) from 18-30 months was modeled in relation to ASQ at 30 months using the same measures previously described.
Four sensitivity analyses were conducted. First, children with registered congenital malformations were removed and analyses re-run.37 This analysis was conducted to ensure that a small number of children with abnormalities that often impact feeding and development were not skewing the results.38 Second and third, multiples and children born preterm (< 37 weeks gestation) were sequentially removed from the sample and analyses re-run. The study design oversampled twins, who may be at higher risk of feeding and developmental problems,39 which could skew the results. Preterm children are also likely to be at risk for both feeding problems and developmental delay. Finding consistency in the results for only full-term children would support the generalizability of the findings. Fourth, exploratory principal components analyses with promax rotation were conducted to determine whether the feeding items separated into subscales. Two components were extracted at each time point representing food refusal and mechanical and distress problems (Table 1). Given the theoretical distinction between mechanical and distress items, we explored mechanical/distress as a single scale, and also separated these items into two scales. Analyses were explored with these subscales to determine if one or another type of feeding problem was more strongly associated with developmental delays.
In subgroup analyses of the Battelle, the analyses of the continuous feeding scale were repeated (substituting the logit link for an identity link), using the 30-month feeding problems scale as a predictor of developmental functioning on the Battelle total and subdomains.
Results
Table 2 displays the descriptive statistics. On average, children experienced feeding problems between rarely and sometimes. Most children (77%) did not experience high levels of feeding problems any time between 18 and 30 months, but 21% experienced high feeding problems at one or two time points, and 2% experienced persistently high feeding problems at all three time points. A small percentage of children has an ASQ delay at any time (8-11% had a delay in one or more domains of the ASQ, and 3-7% had a delay in an individual domain).
Table 2.
n | M (SD) or % | range | |
---|---|---|---|
Feeding Problems Scale | |||
18 months | 2795 | 1.14 (0.46) | 0.11-2.89 |
24 months | 2462 | 1.00 (0.49) | 0.09-2.82 |
30 months | 2475 | 1.26 (0.39) | 0.08-3.00 |
Persistent Feeding Problems 1 | 3597 | ||
Never ≥ 90th percentile | 77 | ||
Sometime ≥ 90th percentile | 21 | ||
Always ≥ 90th percentile | 2 | ||
Developmental Delays | |||
ASQ Total | |||
18 months | 2573 | 8 | |
24 months | 2180 | 11 | |
30 months | 1957 | 10 | |
ASQ Fine Motor | |||
18 months | 2623 | 3 | |
24 months | 2221 | 4 | |
30 months | 2012 | 3 | |
ASQ Gross Motor | |||
18 months | 2626 | 3 | |
24 months | 2229 | 4 | |
30 months | 2030 | 5 | |
ASQ Communication | |||
18 months | 2616 | 3 | |
24 months | 2234 | 7 | |
30 months | 2011 | 5 | |
ASQ Personal-Social | |||
18 months | 2626 | 3 | |
24 months | 2236 | 5 | |
30 months | 2029 | 3 | |
ASQ Problem Solving | |||
18 months | 2605 | 3 | |
24 months | 2214 | 3 | |
30 months | 2006 | 4 | |
4-year Development | |||
Battelle Total | 515 | 113.31(18.91) | 46-145 |
Motor | 516 | 107.79(17.97) | 56-144 |
Communication | 515 | 111.81(19.31) | 55-145 |
Cognitive | 514 | 109.43(18.67) | 55-143 |
Personal-Social | 516 | 115.15(16.13) | 55-145 |
Adaptive | 515 | 105.11(16.55) | 55-142 |
Covariates | |||
Child sex (female) | 3597 | 49 | |
Gestational age (weeks) | 3597 | 37.57 (2.83) | 23-42 |
Sleep problem | |||
18 months | 2788 | 11 | |
24 months | 2557 | 9 | |
30 months | 2475 | 12 | |
History of GERD (4-24 months) | 3251 | 17 | |
Maternal age (years) | 3597 | 31.27 (2.83) | 23-42 |
Non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race | 3597 | 84 | |
College educated | 3597 | 59 | |
Married or civil union | 3454 | 83 | |
Infertility treatment | 3597 | 37 | |
Nulliparous | 3565 | 50 | |
Plurality | 3597 | 35 | |
Maternal depression (24 months) | 2724 | 2.21 (2.40) | 0-15 |
Note: ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. GERD = gastroesophageal reflux disease.
Weighted frequencies based on multiply imputed data.
In unadjusted models of the feeding problem scale with ASQ delay, there was always a significant or marginally significant feeding problem by age interaction (24 month vs. 18 month ORs=1.12–1.36; 30 month vs. 18 month ORs=1.67–2.78) so analyses were stratified by child age. In stratified analyses (Table 3), feeding problems were associated with an increased risk of total ASQ delay and delay in nearly all subdomains at all ages. Reflecting the interactions, point estimates indicated stronger associations between feeding problems and ASQ delay as the children aged (ORs=1.50–2.45 at 18 months, ORs=2.36–3.12 at 24 months, and ORs=4.50–6.60 at 30 months).
Table 3.
18 months | 24 months | 30 months | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | |
Unadjusted | |||||||||
ASQ Total | 1.77 | 1.26, 2.47 | <.001 | 2.41 | 1.85, 3.15 | <.001 | 4.50 | 3.08, 6.58 | <.001 |
Fine Motor | 2.45 | 1.46, 4.12 | <.001 | 3.11 | 2.03, 4.75 | <.001 | 6.08 | 3.52, 10.52 | <.001 |
Gross Motor | 1.50 | 0.92, 2.44 | .102 | 2.36 | 1.48, 3.77 | <.001 | 5.38 | 3.37, 8.60 | <.001 |
Communication | 2.39 | 1.42, 4.03 | .001 | 2.57 | 1.84, 3.58 | <.001 | 5.40 | 3.33, 8.76 | <.001 |
Personal-Social | 1.99 | 1.20, 3.29 | .008 | 2.92 | 1.99, 4.29 | <.001 | 6.60 | 3.79, 11.49 | <.001 |
Problem Solving | 2.23 | 1.39, 3.58 | .001 | 3.12 | 1.96, 4.96 | <.001 | 4.78 | 2.83, 8.06 | <.001 |
Adjusted | |||||||||
ASQ Total | 1.49 | 1.05, 2.11 | .027 | 2.10 | 1.58, 2.79 | <.001 | 3.90 | 2.61, 5.82 | <.001 |
Fine Motor | 1.98 | 1.16, 3.38 | .012 | 2.69 | 1.74, 4.15 | <.001 | 5.32 | 3.01, 9.42 | <.001 |
Gross Motor | 1.30 | 0.78, 2.16 | .304 | 2.07 | 1.25, 3.43 | .005 | 4.92 | 2.98, 8.11 | <.001 |
Communication | 1.76 | 1.05, 2.97 | .033 | 2.14 | 1.51, 3.03 | <.001 | 4.42 | 2.65, 7.36 | <.001 |
Personal-Social | 1.55 | 0.92, 2.61 | .101 | 2.38 | 1.60, 3.54 | <.001 | 5.64 | 3.14, 10.12 | <.001 |
Problem Solving | 1.87 | 1.14, 3.07 | .012 | 2.63 | 1.65, 4.20 | <.001 | 4.08 | 2.37, 7.03 | <.001 |
Note: GLMM = generalized linear mixed model. ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. Adjusted models control for child sex, gestational age, sleep problems (time-varying), history of gastroesophageal reflux disease, maternal age, non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race, college education, marital status, infertility treatment, parity, plurality, and maternal depression. Models are stratified by child age because interactions between child age and feeding problems were significant or marginally significant.
In adjusted analyses (Table 3), children with a one-point increase in the feeding problems score were more than twice as likely to have a delay on the ASQ total and all subscales at 24 months and four or more times as likely to have a delay at 30 months. Associations were weaker at 18 months, but consistent with the directions observed at 24 and 30 months. Full model results are in Tables 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, and 9.
Table 4.
18 months | 24 months | 30 months | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | |
ASQ total | |||||||||
Feeding problems | 1.49 | 1.05, 2.11 | .027 | 2.10 | 1.58, 2.79 | <.001 | 3.90 | 2.61, 5.82 | <.001 |
Child sex | 0.77 | 0.59, 1.00 | .046 | 0.62 | 0.49, 0.78 | <.001 | 0.60 | 0.46, 0.78 | <.001 |
Gestational age | 0.90 | 0.86, 0.95 | <.001 | 0.87 | 0.83, 0.90 | <.001 | 0.87 | 0.84, 0.91 | <.001 |
Sleeping problem | 1.07 | 0.72, 1.59 | .724 | 1.17 | 0.81, 1.67 | .403 | 1.30 | 0.88, 1.91 | .186 |
History of GERD | 1.62 | 1.15, 2.29 | .006 | 1.68 | 1.23, 2.28 | .001 | 1.40 | 0.99, 1.99 | .055 |
Maternal age | 0.98 | 0.96, 1.00 | .115 | 1.00 | 0.97, 1.02 | .808 | 0.98 | 0.96, 1.01 | .148 |
Non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race | 0.81 | 0.57, 1.03 | .233 | 0.79 | 0.58, 1.08 | .136 | 0.68 | 0.50, 0.93 | .015 |
College educated | 0.77 | 0.57, 1.03 | .080 | 0.80 | 0.62, 1.04 | .100 | 0.94 | 0.70, 1.26 | .679 |
Married or civil union | 0.78 | 0.54, 1.15 | .210 | 0.90 | 0.64, 1.26 | .539 | 0.62 | 0.43, 0.87 | .006 |
Infertility treatment | 1.25 | 0.92, 1.68 | .149 | 1.06 | 0.80, 1.40 | .685 | 1.01 | 0.75, 1.36 | .942 |
Nulliparous | 0.69 | 0.52, 0.91 | .009 | 0.74 | 0.57, 0.97 | .027 | 0.69 | 0.52, 0.91 | .008 |
Plurality | 1.35 | 0.99, 1.84 | .056 | 1.04 | 0.79, 1.36 | .789 | 1.43 | 1.06, 1.91 | .017 |
Maternal depression | 1.08 | 1.01, 1.14 | .020 | 1.08 | 1.02, 1.14 | .006 | 1.07 | 1.01, 1.14 | .029 |
Note: GLMM = generalized linear mixed model. ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. GERD = gastroesophageal reflux disease.
Table 5.
18 months | 24 months | 30 months | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | |
ASQ Fine Motor | |||||||||
Feeding problems | 1.98 | 1.16, 3.38 | .012 | 2.69 | 1.74, 4.15 | >.001 | 5.32 | 3.01, 9.42 | >.001 |
Child sex | 0.65 | 0.45, 0.94 | .023 | 0.87 | 0.63, 1.21 | .406 | 0.65 | 0.45, 0.93 | .020 |
Gestational age | 0.94 | 0.88, 1.01 | .075 | 0.88 | 0.83, 0.93 | >.001 | 0.90 | 0.85, 0.96 | >.001 |
Sleeping problem | 1.18 | 0.67, 2.09 | .565 | 1.41 | 0.86, 2.33 | .177 | 1.20 | 0.71, 2.03 | .487 |
History of GERD | 1.47 | 0.89, 2.44 | .132 | 1.47 | 0.95, 2.27 | .082 | 1.28 | 0.76, 2.15 | .352 |
Maternal age | 0.97 | 0.94, 1.01 | .121 | 0.98 | 0.95, 1.01 | .167 | 0.99 | 0.96, 1.03 | .727 |
Non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race | 0.65 | 0.41, 1.04 | .075 | 0.79 | 0.51, 1.22 | .289 | 0.60 | 0.40, 0.89 | .012 |
College educated | 0.74 | 0.48, 1.14 | .174 | 0.84 | 0.59, 1.21 | .354 | 0.88 | 0.60, 1.30 | .514 |
Married or civil union | 0.78 | 0.45, 1.35 | .374 | 0.85 | 0.53, 1.37 | .512 | 0.56 | 0.35, 0.90 | .017 |
Infertility treatment | 1.18 | 0.76, 1.82 | .467 | 1.15 | 0.77, 1.70 | .497 | 1.04 | 0.70, 1.53 | .860 |
Nulliparous | 0.71 | 0.47, 1.08 | .106 | 0.64 | 0.44, 0.93 | .019 | 0.51 | 0.34, 0.75 | .001 |
Plurality | 1.33 | 0.85, 2.08 | .205 | 1.22 | 0.85, 1.76 | .274 | 1.57 | 1.07, 2.30 | .020 |
Maternal depression | 1.08 | 0.99, 1.18 | .072 | 1.07 | 0.99, 1.17 | .095 | 1.04 | 0.96, 1.14 | .320 |
Note: GLMM = generalized linear mixed model. ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. GERD = gastroesophageal reflux disease.
Table 6.
18 months | 24 months | 30 months | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | |
ASQ Gross Motor | |||||||||
Feeding problems | 1.30 | 0.78, 2.16 | .304 | 2.07 | 1.25, 3.43 | .005 | 4.92 | 2.98, 8.11 | >.001 |
Child sex | 0.90 | 0.63, 1.29 | .576 | 0.84 | 0.60, 1.18 | .315 | 0.76 | 0.56, 1.04 | .084 |
Gestational age | 0.92 | 0.86, 0.98 | .009 | 0.87 | 0.82, 0.92 | >.001 | 0.86 | 0.82, 0.91 | >.001 |
Sleeping problem | 0.99 | 0.54, 1.80 | .969 | 1.23 | 0.73, 2.06 | .438 | 1.30 | 0.82, 2.06 | .266 |
History of GERD | 1.73 | 1.09, 2.75 | .020 | 1.53 | 0.98, 2.37 | .060 | 1.44 | 0.96, 2.17 | .077 |
Maternal age | 0.98 | 0.95, 1.02 | .337 | 0.99 | 0.96, 1.02 | .575 | 0.98 | 0.95, 1.01 | .142 |
Non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race | 0.82 | 0.50, 1.36 | .444 | 0.76 | 0.49, 1.18 | .223 | 0.82 | 0.56, 1.21 | .322 |
College educated | 0.83 | 0.55, 1.25 | .365 | 0.97 | 0.67, 1.41 | .878 | 1.12 | 0.79, 1.60 | .528 |
Married or civil union | 0.86 | 0.50, 1.50 | .603 | 0.79 | 0.49, 1.28 | .343 | 0.56 | 0.37, 0.85 | .007 |
Infertility treatment | 1.27 | 0.85, 1.90 | .249 | 1.39 | 0.92, 2.11 | .114 | 1.24 | 0.88, 1.75 | .220 |
Nulliparous | 0.74 | 0.51, 1.09 | .132 | 0.62 | 0.42, 0.90 | .013 | 0.52 | 0.37, 0.72 | >.001 |
Plurality | 1.22 | 0.80, 1.84 | .353 | 1.33 | 0.90, 1.98 | .155 | 1.55 | 1.10, 2.20 | .012 |
Maternal depression | 1.04 | 0.96, 1.14 | .326 | 1.05 | 0.96, 1.14 | .305 | 1.03 | 0.96, 1.11 | .401 |
Note: GLMM = generalized linear mixed model. ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. GERD = gastroesophageal reflux disease.
Table 7.
18 months | 24 months | 30 months | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | |
ASQ Communication | |||||||||
Feeding problems | 1.76 | 1.05, 2.97 | .033 | 2.14 | 1.51, 3.03 | <.001 | 4.42 | 2.65, 7.36 | <.001 |
Child sex | 0.57 | 0.37, 0.87 | .010 | 0.52 | 0.38, 0.71 | <.001 | 0.53 | 0.38, 0.75 | <.001 |
Gestational age | 0.94 | 0.87, 1.02 | .144 | 0.89 | 0.85, 0.94 | <.001 | 0.89 | 0.85, 0.94 | <.001 |
Sleeping problem | 1.03 | 0.56, 1.87 | .933 | 1.40 | 0.91, 2.15 | .130 | 1.19 | 0.74, 1.93 | .470 |
History of GERD | 2.41 | 1.47, 3.94 | .001 | 1.79 | 1.22, 2.62 | .003 | 1.54 | 1.01, 2.37 | .047 |
Maternal age | 0.98 | 0.94, 1.01 | .182 | 1.01 | 0.98, 1.04 | .620 | 0.99 | 0.95, 1.02 | .356 |
Non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race | 0.68 | 0.41, 1.12 | .126 | 0.82 | 0.54, 1.23 | .327 | 0.60 | 0.42, 0.86 | .006 |
College educated | 0.91 | 0.57, 1.45 | .685 | 0.75 | 0.53, 1.06 | .100 | 0.82 | 0.57, 1.18 | .280 |
Married or civil union | 0.61 | 0.36, 1.03 | .065 | 0.73 | 0.49, 1.08 | .114 | 0.59 | 0.39, 0.89 | .012 |
Infertility treatment | 1.06 | 0.68, 1.66 | .789 | 0.75 | 0.52, 1.08 | .122 | 0.96 | 0.66, 1.39 | .821 |
Nulliparous | 0.61 | 0.38, 0.96 | .034 | 0.80 | 0.58, 1.10 | .171 | 0.69 | 0.48, 0.98 | .040 |
Plurality | 1.70 | 1.09, 2.65 | .020 | 1.32 | 0.94, 1.85 | .112 | 1.37 | 0.95, 1.99 | .091 |
Maternal depression | 1.13 | 1.03, 1.24 | .009 | 1.11 | 1.04, 1.18 | .001 | 1.08 | 1.00, 1.16 | .059 |
Note: GLMM = generalized linear mixed model. ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. GERD = gastroesophageal reflux disease.
Table 8.
18 months | 24 months | 30 months | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | |
ASQ Personal-Social | |||||||||
Feeding problems | 1.55 | 0.92, 2.61 | .101 | 2.38 | 1.60, 3.54 | <.001 | 5.64 | 3.14, 10.12 | <.001 |
Child sex | 0.62 | 0.43, 0.91 | .014 | 0.58 | 0.42, 0.81 | .001 | 0.61 | 0.43, 0.88 | .007 |
Gestational age | 0.91 | 0.85, 0.98 | .018 | 0.89 | 0.84, 0.94 | <.001 | 0.88 | 0.84, 0.93 | <.001 |
Sleeping problem | 1.26 | 0.72,2.21 | .414 | 1.54 | 0.99, 2.42 | .058 | 1.28 | 0.77, 2.13 | .331 |
History of GERD | 1.61 | 1.01, 2.59 | .047 | 1.68 | 1.12, 2.53 | .013 | 1.68 | 1.08, 2.61 | .021 |
Maternal age | 0.99 | 0.95, 1.02 | .437 | 0.99 | 0.96, 1.02 | .593 | 0.99 | 0.95, 1.02 | .420 |
Non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race | 0.85 | 0.51, 1.41 | .519 | 0.58 | 0.39, 0.84 | .005 | 0.74 | 0.50, 1.10 | .137 |
College educated | 0.76 | 0.49, 1.19 | .229 | 0.90 | 0.63, 1.27 | .533 | 0.91 | 0.62, 1.35 | .651 |
Married or civil union | 0.75 | 0.45, 1.25 | .269 | 0.85 | 0.56, 1.29 | .446 | 0.55 | 0.35, 0.88 | .012 |
Infertility treatment | 1.29 | 0.85, 1.96 | .227 | 1.09 | 0.75, 1.57 | .656 | 1.17 | 0.80, 1.72 | .410 |
Nulliparous | 0.67 | 0.44, 1.03 | .066 | 0.73 | 0.52, 1.02 | .068 | 0.60 | 0.41, 0.87 | .008 |
Plurality | 1.38 | 0.91, 2.11 | .132 | 1.30 | 0.92, 1.85 | .139 | 1.39 | 0.95, 2.04 | .087 |
Maternal depression | 1.11 | 1.02, 1.22 | .018 | 1.11 | 1.02, 1.20 | .011 | 1.03 | 0.95, 1.12 | .436 |
Note: GLMM = generalized linear mixed model. ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. GERD = gastroesophageal reflux disease.
Table 9.
18 months | 24 months | 30 months | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | |
ASQ Problem Solving | |||||||||
Feeding problems | 1.87 | 1.14, 3.07 | .013 | 2.63 | 1.65, 4.20 | >.001 | 4.08 | 2.37, 7.03 | >.001 |
Child sex | 0.71 | 0.50, 1.01 | .056 | 0.72 | 0.50, 1.03 | .072 | 0.71 | 0.51, 0.99 | .042 |
Gestational age | 0.96 | 0.90, 1.03 | .257 | 0.90 | 0.85, 0.95 | >.001 | 0.90 | 0.85, 0.95 | >.001 |
Sleeping problem | 0.97 | 0.56, 1.70 | .924 | 1.44 | 0.85, 2.42 | .174 | 1.17 | 0.71, 1.93 | .533 |
History of GERD | 1.59 | 0.98, 2.57 | .061 | 1.64 | 1.03, 2.61 | .036 | 1.42 | 0.93, 2.18 | .104 |
Maternal age | 0.99 | 0.95, 1.02 | .498 | 1.00 | 0.96, 1.03 | .860 | 0.99 | 0.96, 1.02 | .487 |
Non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race | 0.85 | 0.52, 1.36 | .490 | 0.85 | 0.53, 1.36 | .487 | 0.75 | 0.51, 1.09 | .130 |
College educated | 0.68 | 0.45, 1.01 | .055 | 0.81 | 0.55, 1.19 | .285 | 1.04 | 0.72, 1.51 | .846 |
Married or civil union | 0.84 | 0.50, 1.39 | .490 | 0.77 | 0.47, 1.25 | .288 | 0.56 | 0.37, 0.87 | .009 |
Infertility treatment | 1.08 | 0.72, 1.62 | .716 | 1.10 | 0.73, 1.65 | .649 | 1.02 | 0.71, 1.47 | .918 |
Nulliparous | 0.90 | 0.62, 1.30 | .576 | 0.72 | 0.49, 1.08 | .112 | 0.61 | 0.43, 0.86 | .005 |
Plurality | 1.16 | 0.73, 1.83 | .530 | 1.43 | 0.97, 2.10 | .073 | 1.50 | 1.05, 2.16 | .027 |
Maternal depression | 1.10 | 1.01, 1.20 | .027 | 1.10 | 1.00, 1.20 | .047 | 1.07 | 0.99, 1.15 | .089 |
Note: GLMM = generalized linear mixed model. ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. GERD = gastroesophageal reflux disease.
In adjusted analyses of persistent feeding problems with ASQ at 30 months (Table 10), compared with children who never experienced high (≥ 90th percentile) feeding problems, children who experienced high feeding problems at one or two time points were more than twice as likely to have an ASQ delay, and children who experienced high feeding problems at all three time points were four or more times as likely to have an ASQ delay.
Table 10.
OR | 95% CI | p | |
---|---|---|---|
ASQ Total | |||
Sometime | 2.10 | 1.53, 2.89 | <.001 |
Always | 3.94 | 1.89, 8.21 | <.001 |
Fine Motor | |||
Sometime | 2.28 | 1.47, 3.54 | <.001 |
Always | 5.05 | 2.24, 11.41 | <.001 |
Gross Motor | |||
Sometime | 2.17 | 1.49, 3.17 | <.001 |
Always | 3.99 | 1.77, 9.01 | <.001 |
Communication | |||
Sometime | 2.28 | 1.55, 3.35 | <.001 |
Always | 4.22 | 1.93, 9.19 | <.001 |
Personal-Social | |||
Sometime | 2.50 | 1.64, 3.80 | <.001 |
Always | 4.34 | 1.91, 9.86 | <.001 |
Problem Solving | |||
Sometime | 2.10 | 1.40, 3.15 | <.001 |
Always | 4.23 | 1.93, 9.28 | <.001 |
Note: ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. Models control for child sex, gestational age, sleep problems (time-varying), history of gastroesophageal reflux disease, maternal age, non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race, college education, marital status, infertility treatment, parity, plurality, and maternal depression.
Sensitivity Analyses
Removing 244 children with congenital malformations, 1259 twins and higher order multiples, and 887 preterm (gestational age < 37 weeks) children, the adjusted findings were similar to those reported above (Tables 11 and 12).
Table 11.
18 months | 24 months | 30 months | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | |
A. Removing congenital malformations | |||||||||
ASQ Total | 1.48 | 1.02, 2.17 | .041 | 2.15 | 1.60, 2.90 | <.001 | 3.95 | 2.62, 5.97 | <.001 |
Fine Motor | 1.97 | 1.11, 3.50 | <.001 | 2.66 | 1.69, 4.17 | <.001 | 5.06 | 2.83, 9.04 | <.001 |
Gross Motor | 1.34 | 0.76, 2.35 | .102 | 2.24 | 1.30, 3.85 | .004 | 5.19 | 3.10, 8.69 | <.001 |
Communication | 1.82 | 1.03, 3.21 | .001 | 2.05 | 1.43, 2.93 | <.001 | 4.41 | 2.61, 7.46 | <.001 |
Personal-Social | 1.53 | 0.87, 2.70 | .008 | 2.25 | 1.49, 3.40 | <.001 | 5.65 | 3.13, 10.21 | <.001 |
Problem Solving | 1.84 | 1.08, 3.14 | .001 | 2.61 | 1.61, 4.25 | <.001 | 4.11 | 2.36, 7.16 | <.001 |
B. Removing multiples | |||||||||
ASQ Total | 1.70 | 1.05, 2.74 | .030 | 2.14 | 1.51, 3.03 | <.001 | 4.53 | 2.77, 7.40 | <.001 |
Fine Motor | 2.19 | 1.10, 4.39 | .027 | 3.27 | 1.88, 5.66 | <.001 | 4.84 | 2.47, 9.47 | <.001 |
Gross Motor | 1.87 | 0.96, 3.64 | .065 | 2.58 | 1.42, 4.71 | .002 | 5.74 | 3.04, 10.83 | <.001 |
Communication | 2.10 | 0.99, 4.45 | .052 | 1.90 | 1.22, 2.97 | .005 | 4.21 | 2.31, 7.70 | <.001 |
Personal-Social | 2.13 | 1.02, 4.45 | .043 | 2.45 | 1.49, 4.02 | <.001 | 5.91 | 3.01, 11.59 | <.001 |
Problem Solving | 1.99 | 1.05, 3.78 | .035 | 2.99 | 1.62, 5.52 | <.001 | 4.25 | 2.24, 8.07 | <.001 |
C. Removing preterm | |||||||||
ASQ Total | 1.69 | 1.07, 2.65 | .023 | 2.33 | 1.64, 3.30 | <.001 | 4.59 | 2.84, 7.43 | <.001 |
Fine Motor | 2.20 | 1.09, 4.43 | .027 | 3.41 | 2.00, 5.82 | <.001 | 5.34 | 2.74, 10.44 | <.001 |
Gross Motor | 1.63 | 0.86, 3.10 | .132 | 2.66 | 1.47, 4.81 | .001 | 6.44 | 3.46, 12.02 | <.001 |
Communication | 2.04 | 1.00, 4.15 | .050 | 2.20 | 1.40, 3.44 | <.001 | 4.97 | 2.74, 9.00 | <.001 |
Personal-Social | 1.82 | 0.91, 3.64 | .092 | 2.66 | 1.62, 4.37 | <.001 | 6.45 | 3.27, 12.71 | <.001 |
Problem Solving | 1.96 | 1.07, 3.59 | .030 | 3.37 | 1.86, 6.11 | <.001 | 4.58 | 2.46, 8.55 | <.001 |
Note: GLMM = generalized linear mixed model. ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. Adjusted models control for child sex, gestational age (not in preterm model), sleep problems (time-varying), history of gastroesophageal reflux disease, maternal age, non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race, college education, marital status, infertility treatment, parity, plurality (not in multiples model), and maternal depression.
Table 12.
A. Removing congenital malformations |
B. Removing multiples | C. Removing preterm | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | OR | 95% CI | p | |
ASQ Total | |||||||||
Sometime | 2.15 | 1.55, 2.99 | <.001 | 2.07 | 1.37, 3.11 | <.001 | 2.29 | 1.56, 3.37 | <.001 |
Always | 4.22 | 1.96, 9.10 | <.001 | 4.17 | 1.61, 10.84 | .004 | 3.74 | 1.41, 9.90 | .008 |
Fine Motor | |||||||||
Sometime | 2.19 | 1.39, 3.43 | <.001 | 2.26 | 1.28, 3.98 | .005 | 2.23 | 1.30, 3.81 | .004 |
Always | 5.11 | 2.18, 12.02 | <.001 | 4.40 | 1.44, 13.42 | .010 | 3.26 | 0.97, 11.00 | .057 |
Gross Motor | |||||||||
Sometime | 2.30 | 1.56, 3.39 | <.001 | 2.32 | 1.38, 3.88 | .002 | 2.55 | 1.58, 4.10 | <.001 |
Always | 4.58 | 2.01, 10.45 | <.001 | 4.41 | 1.44, 13.49 | .009 | 4.19 | 1.33, 13.21 | .015 |
Communication | |||||||||
Sometime | 2.31 | 1.55, 3.45 | <.001 | 1.97 | 1.17, 3.30 | .010 | 2.43 | 1.49, 3.96 | <.001 |
Always | 4.44 | 1.95, 10.13 | <.001 | 3.17 | 1.07, 9.39 | .038 | 3.28 | 1.06, 10.14 | .039 |
Personal-Social | |||||||||
Sometime | 2.59 | 1.69, 3.99 | <.001 | 2.57 | 1.49, 4.44 | <.001 | 2.78 | 1.62, 4.76 | <.001 |
Always | 4.49 | 1.88, 10.72 | <.001 | 3.23 | 0.91, 11.44 | .070 | 2.83 | 0.71, 11.25 | .139 |
Problem Solving | |||||||||
Sometime | 2.15 | 1.41, 3.27 | <.001 | 2.05 | 1.21, 3.48 | .008 | 2.20 | 1.33, 3.64 | .002 |
Always | 4.46 | 1.94, 10.28 | <.001 | 3.55 | 1.12, 11.27 | .031 | 3.44 | 1.11, 10.61 | .032 |
Note: ASQ = Ages and Stages Questionnaire©. Models control for child sex, gestational age (not in preterm model), sleep problems (time-varying), history of gastroesophageal reflux disease, maternal age, non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race, college education, marital status, infertility treatment, parity, plurality (not in multiples model), and maternal depression.
The food refusal and mechanical/distress feeding problem subscales identified with PCA were moderately correlated at each time point, rs=.47-.52, ps<.001. When mechanical and distress items were further delineated into mechanical (can't chew solid foods, gags on food, holds food in mouth) and distress/behavioral (spits food out, throws food, cries/screams), correlations were slightly larger between the two, rs=.51-61, ps<.001, than between food refusal and mechanical, rs=.34-.43, ps<.001, and food refusal and distress/behavioral, rs=.47-.50, ps<.001. The food refusal, mechanical, and distress/behavioral subscales (as well as the combined mechanical/distress subscale) were similarly associated with developmental delays and no clear pattern emerged of differential associations of the subscales with developmental delays. All effects were in the same direction and similar strength as the total scale (data not shown).
Subgroup Analyses
In analyses of 516 children who completed the assessment at age 4, a one-point increase in feeding problems at 30 months was associated with scoring 4-6 points lower on the Battelle total and subscales (Table 13). Adjusting for child and maternal factors, the reduction in Battelle scores was 3-4 points.
Table 13.
Unadjusted | Adjusted | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | 95% CI | p | B | 95% CI | p | |
Battelle Total | −6.72 | −10.87, −2.58 | .002 | −4.48 | −8.40, −0.56 | .025 |
Motor | −6.30 | −10.39, −2.21 | .003 | −4.29 | −8.32, −0.26 | .037 |
Communication | −5.62 | −9.97, −1.27 | .011 | −3.84 | −8.08, 0.39 | .075 |
Cognitive | −4.16 | −8.11, −0.22 | .038 | −2.98 | −6.81, 0.85 | .128 |
Personal-Social | −5.98 | −9.62, −2.33 | .001 | −3.89 | −7.44, −0.34 | .032 |
Adaptive | −6.58 | −10.33, −2.84 | <.001 | −4.51 | −8.09, −0.94 | .013 |
Note: GLMM = generalized linear mixed model. Adjusted models control for child sex, gestational age, sleep problems at 30 months, history of GERD, maternal age, non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race, college education, marital status, infertility treatment, parity, plurality, and maternal depression.
Discussion
The results of this study suggest that feeding problems, broadly defined to include meal-time distress, food refusal, and mechanical issues, are increasingly associated with developmental delays from 18 to 30 months, and feeding problems at 30 months are associated with somewhat lower developmental scores at 4 years. Children who had high feeding problems at one or two time points were more than twice as likely to have a probable developmental delay on the ASQ, and children who had high feeding problems at all three time points were 4 or more times more likely to have a delay on the ASQ at 30 months. Removing children with congenital malformations, twins and higher-order multiples, and preterm children did not change these conclusions.
This study expands beyond previous studies that have found associations between feeding problems and development in children at risk1, 21, 23 as well as population studies that have found associations between picky eating and development.19, 20 This study used a large cohort of children at three time points, measured a range of feeding problems and developmental delays, and included a full in-person developmental assessment for a subsample of children (rather than relying exclusively on maternal reports of child development).
Much of the literature on feeding problems in normative samples focuses on food refusal or picky eating.4, 11, 17, 18, 20, 40 Picky eating is distressing for parents17 and may put children at risk for growth faltering.9 However, sensitivity analyses in this study did not indicate that a particular kind of feeding problem was more strongly associated with developmental delays. The risk for developmental delays was similar across food refusal and mechanical/distress subscales.
The associations between feeding problems and development were not limited to a particular developmental domain. In this study, we explored feeding problems as an early indicator of developmental delays because feeding problems may be easier to identify early, not because we believe feeding problems are the cause of later delays. Except in rare cases of extreme feeding problems that lead to growth faltering and failure to thrive,8, 9, 21 it is more likely that feeding problems in the general population are simply a symptom of an underlying developmental issue. Feeding problems may be indicative of broad delays, or may have associations with a particular developmental domain for different children. For example, children may experience feeding problems because of neurological problems that impact the ability to coordinate oral-motor, communication, and social feeding skills.41 Children may experience feeding problems because of the inability to communicate with the caregiver, leading the child to exhibit feeding problem behaviors instead.42, 43 Lacking fine motor skills may also lead to feeding problems by impacting the ability to self-feed and increasing frustration during meals.
There were somewhat weaker associations between feeding problems and developmental delays at 18 months compared with 30 months. Children experience improvements in oral-motor and self-feeding skills in the second year of life, but there is wide variation in individual rates of development.44, 45 The wide range of normative individual differences at 18 months may have attenuated associations between feeding problems and developmental delays. For example, some typically developing children may be late in acquiring a particular oral-motor skill (e.g., coordination of chewing juicy foods) at 18 months, but by 30 months, most children have strong oral-motor and self-feeding skills unless there is a lingering developmental problem.45
The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommends screening for developmental disabilities at 9, 18, and 30 months.46 If AAP screening guidelines are followed, most children at risk for developmental delays will be identified before the pattern of feeding problems is clear. However, an estimated 37% of pediatricians did not screen all children under 3 years in 2016,47 and screening varies widely by state.48 Two common perceived barriers to universal screening were the amount of time the screenings take and inadequate reimbursement.47 Many other countries, including low- and middle-income countries, do not have routine developmental screening programs.49 There may also be disparities in referral rates for children who screen positive for developmental concerns. In a large U.S. hospital system, over half of children who screened positive for a developmental problem were not referred to early intervention services, and referral rates differed by child sex, age, and race.50 Screening toddlers whose mothers raise concerns about feeding behavior could help to identify children at risk for developmental delays. However, the weaker associations between feeding problems and delays earlier in development suggest that more weight should be given to feeding problems that persist into the third year. Pediatricians could also use information about feeding problems to help decide whether a child who screens positive for a developmental delay should be referred to early intervention services.
Both feeding problems and the ASQ were derived from maternal reports, which may inflate associations among constructs due to common source variance. However, results were largely supported by independent testing of a subset of children at age 4 years. Maternal reports of feeding problems have been validated against observational feeding data in other studies.40 Mothers’ reports of developmental status have also been shown to relate to independent testing of the child.51 The 90th percentile cut-point used to identify children with persistently high feeding problems was sample-specific, but these analyses were complimented by those using the full range of scores.
The sample in this study was largely European-American and well educated. These demographics were reflected in children’s high average standard scores on the Battelle, but there was large variation (Table 2). It is unclear to what extent the results of this study will generalize to other groups. Finally, there is always the possibility of confounding due to unmeasured variables such as detailed indicators of growth that might be associated with both feeding behavior and developmental delay.52 However, we did include a range of important child and family covariates (see Appendix for details).
The subsample that completed the BDI-2 was not random. Only 28% of those invited chose to participate, and invitations were stratified according to various characteristics including performance on developmental screeners. It is unclear what effect this may have had on the BDI-2 findings and they should be interpreted with caution.
Despite feeding problems being a common occurrence in early childhood,2, 3 15, 16 children who are reported to have high levels of feeding problems in early childhood are also more likely to have a delay on multiple domains of a developmental screening test and score lower in independent testing. High levels of feeding problems translated to mothers reporting between rarely and sometimes or more in occurrence of 9 to 12 different feeding issues. Should our findings be confirmed, taking notes of such reports in pediatric records may be a practical way to add information for targeted screening. The relatively stronger results as children age from 18 to 30 months combined with the finding that children with persistently high feeding problems from 18 to 30 months are 4 or more times as likely to have a delay on the ASQ suggest that children who experience feeding problems into their third year may be at the greatest risk and should be screened for developmental delays.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Supported by the Intramural Research Program of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (contracts #HHSN275201200005C, #HHSN267200700019C). The sponsor played no role in the study design, data collection, data analysis or interpretation, writing of the manuscript, or the decision to submit the article for publication. The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Abbreviations:
- ASQ
Ages and Stages Questionnaire
- BDI-2
Battelle Developmental Inventory, second edition
- MCHAT
Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers
- GERD
gastroesophageal reflux disease
- GLMM
generalized linear mixed models
- OR
odds ratio
- PCA
principal component analysis.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- [1].Kleinert JO. Pediatric feeding disorders and severe developmental disabilities. Seminars in Speech and Language. 2017;38:116–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Aldridge VK, Dovey TM, Martin CI, Meyer C. Identifying clinically relevant feeding problems and disorders. Journal of Child Health Care. 2010;14:261–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Lindberg L, Bohlin G, Hagekull B. Early feeding problems in a normal population. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1991;10:395–405. [Google Scholar]
- [4].Machado BC, Dias P, Lima VS, Campos J, Goncalves S. Prevalence and correlates of picky eating in preschool-aged children: A population-based study. Eating Behaviors. 2016;22:16–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Benjasuwantep B, Chaithirayanon S, Eiamudomkan M. Feeding problems in healthy young children: Prevalence, related factors and feeding practices. Pediatric Reports. 2013;5:38–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Kovacic K, Rein LE, Szabo A, Kommareddy S, Bhagavatula P, Goday PS. Pediatric feeding disorder: A nationwide prevalence study. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2021;228:126–31 e3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Delaney AL, Arvedson JC. Development of swallowing and feeding: Prenatal through first year of life. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2008;14:105–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Wright CM, Parkinson KN, Drewett RF. How does maternal and child feeding behavior relate to weight gain and failure to thrive? Data from a prospective birth cohort. Pediatrics. 2006;117:1262–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Wright CM, Parkinson KN, Shipton D, Drewett RF. How do toddler eating problems relate to their eating behavior, food preferences, and growth? Pediatrics. 2007;120:e1069–e75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Powell FC, Farrow CV, Meyer C. Food avoidance in children: The influence of maternal feeding practices and behaviours. Appetite. 2011;57:683–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Zohar AH, Lev-Ari L, Bachner-Melman R. Child and maternal correlates of picky eating in young children. Psychology. 2019;10:1249–61. [Google Scholar]
- [12].Abulizi X, Pryor L, Michel G, Melchior M, van der Waerden J, Group EM-CCS. Temperament in infancy and behavioral and emotional problems at age 5.5: The EDEN mother-child cohort. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0171971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Rybak A. Organic and nonorganic feeding disorders. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism. 2015;66:16–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Kerzner B, Milano K, MacLean WC Jr., Berall G, Stuart S, Chatoor I. A practical approach to classifying and managing feeding difficulties. Pediatrics. 2015;135:344–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Krantz J, Gievers L, Khaki S. Investigating parental concerns at the first well-child visit. Clinical Pediatrics. 2020;59:83–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Norlin C, Crawford MA, Bell CT, Sheng X, Stein MT. Delivery of well-child care: A look inside the door. Academic Pediatrics. 2011;11:18–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Trofholz AC, Schulte AK, Berge JM. How parents describe picky eating and its impact on family meals: A qualitative analysis. Appetite. 2017;110:36–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Mascola AJ, Bryson SW, Agras WS. Picky eating during childhood: A longitudinal study to age 11 years. Eating Behaviors. 2010;11:253–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Schmid G, Schreier A, Meyer R, Wolke D. A prospective study on the persistence of infant crying, sleeping and feeding problems and preschool behaviour. Acta Paediatrica. 2010;99:286–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Cardona Cano S, Hoek HW, van Hoeken D, de Barse LM, Jaddoe VW, Verhulst FC, et al. Behavioral outcomes of picky eating in childhood: A prospective study in the general population. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2016;57:1239–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Chatoor I, Surles J, Ganiban J, Beker L, McWade Paez L, Kerzner B. Failure to thrive and cognitive development in toddlers with infantile anorexia. Pediatrics. 2004;113:e440–e7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Dahl M Early feeding problems in an affluent society Ill. Follow-up at two years: Natural course, health, behaviour and development. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica. 1987;76:872–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Berlin KS, Lobato DJ, Pinkos B, Cerezo CS, LeLeiko NS. Patterns of medical and developmental comorbidities among children presenting with feeding problems: A latent class analysis. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2011;32:41–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Carruth BR, Ziegler PJ, Gordon A, Barr SI. Prevalence of picky eaters among infants and toddlers and their caregivers’ decisions about offering a new food. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2004;104:S57–S64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Buck Louis GM, Hediger ML, Bell EM, Kus CA, Sundaram R, McLain AC, et al. Methodology for establishing a population-based birth cohort focusing on couple fertility and children's development, the Upstate KIDS Study. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology. 2014;28:191–202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Yeung EH, Sundaram R, Bell EM, Druschel C, Kus C, Ghassabian A, et al. Examining infertility treatment and early childhood development in the Upstate KIDS study. JAMA Pediatrics. 2016;170:251–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].Squires J, Bricker D. Ages & Stages Questionnaires®, Third Edition (ASQ®-3): A Parent-Completed Child Monitoring System. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc.; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- [28].Robins DL, Fein D, Barton ML, Green JA. The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers: An initial study investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2001;31:131–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Wright CM, Gurney JM, Mutoro AN, Shum C, Khan A, Milligan B, et al. Development of a scale to measure infant eating behaviour worldwide. Nutrients. 2021; 13:2495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Limbos MM, Joyce DP. Comparison of the ASQ and PEDS in screening for developmental delay in children presenting for primary care. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2011;32:499–511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Yue A, Jiang Q, Wang B, Abbey C, Medina A, Shi Y, et al. Concurrent validity of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire and the Bayley Scales of Infant Development III in China. PLoS One. 2019;14:e0221675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Newborg J Battelle Developmental Inventory– Second Edition. Itasca, IL: Riverside; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- [33].Elbaum B, Gattamorta KA, Penfield RD. Evaluation of the Battelle Developmental Inventory, screening test for use in states’ child outcomes measurement systems under the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act. Journal of Early Intervention. 2010;32:255–73. [Google Scholar]
- [34].New York State Department of Health. Statistical summary of children bom in 2008 and diagnosed through 2010. Congenital malformations registry. Albany, NY: New York State Department of Health; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- [35].Glinianaia SV, Rankin J, Wright C. Congenital anomalies in twins: A register-based study. Human Reproduction. 2008;23:1306–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Rubin DB. Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- [37].Abadie V, Couly G. Congenital feeding and swallowing disorders. In: Dulac O, Lassonde M, Sarnat HB, editors. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Pediatric Neurology Part III. 2013/April/30 ed. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier; 2013. p. 1539–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Hvelplund C, Hansen BM, Koch SV, Andersson M, Skovgaard AM. Perinatal risk factors for feeding and eating disorders in children aged 0 to 3 years. Pediatrics. 2016;137:e20152575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].Lorenz JM. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of twins. Seminars in Perinatology. 2012;36:201–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Fernandez C, DeJesus JM, Miller AL, Appugliese DP, Rosenblum KL, Lumeng JC, et al. Selective eating behaviors in children: An observational validation of parental report measures. Appetite. 2018;127:163–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [41].Cooper-Brown L, Copeland S, Dailey S, Downey D, Petersen MC, Stimson C, et al. Feeding and swallowing dysfunction in genetic syndromes. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2008;14:147–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Rescorla L, Ross GS, McClure S. Language delay and behavioral/emotional problems in toddlers: Findings from two developmental clinics. Joural of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2007;50:1063–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [43].Satter E The feeding relationship: Problems and interventions. The Journal of Pediatrics 1990;117:S181–S9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [44].Carruth BR, Ziegler PJ, Gordon A, Hendricks K. Developmental milestones and self-feeding behaviors in infants and toddlers. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2004;104:51–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [45].Carruth BR, Skinner JD. Feeding behaviors and other motor development in healthy children (2-24 months). Journal of the American College of Nutrition. 2002;21:88–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [46].Lipkin PH, Macias MM, AAP Council on Children with Disabilities Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. Promoting optimal development: Identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders through developmental surveillance and screening. Pediatrics. 2020;145:e20193449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [47].Lipkin PH, Macias MM, Baer Chen B, Coury D, Gottschlich EA, Hyman SL, et al. Trends in pediatricians' developmental screening: 2002-2016. Pediatrics. 2020;145:e20190851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [48].Hirai AH, Kogan MD, Kandasamy V, Reuland C, Bethell C. Prevalence and variation of developmental screening and surveillance in early childhood. JAMA Pediatrics. 2018;172:857–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [49].Faruk T, King C, Muhit M, Islam MK, Jahan I, Baset KU, et al. Screening tools for early identification of children with developmental delay in low- and middle-income countries: a systematic review. BMJ Open. 2020;10:e038182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [50].Wallis KE, Davis Rivera LB, Guthrie W, Bennett AE, Mandell DS, Miller JS. Provider responses to positive developmental screening: Disparities in referral practices? Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2021;42:23–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [51].Gollenberg AL, Lynch CD, Jackson LW, McGuinness BM, Msall ME. Concurrent validity of the parent-completed Ages and Stages Questionnaires, 2nd Ed. with the Bayley Scales of Infant Development II in a low-risk sample. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2010;36:485–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [52].Jaekel J, Sorg C, Baeuml J, Bartmann P, Wolke D. Head growth and intelligence from birth to adulthood in very preterm and term born individuals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2019;25:48–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.