Skip to main content
Brain and Neuroscience Advances logoLink to Brain and Neuroscience Advances
. 2022 Feb 24;6:23982128221079573. doi: 10.1177/23982128221079573

Emotion-driven problem behaviour: The predictive utility of positive and negative urgency

Christopher Willie 1, Peter Richard Gill 1,, Robert Teese 2, Vasileios Stavropoulos 1, Andrew Jago 1
PMCID: PMC8883363  PMID: 35237728

Abstract

Impulsive urgency describes the tendency to act rashly when experiencing extreme emotions. This Australian study aimed to investigate the predictive utility of impulsivity, including impulsive urgency (positive and negative), across a range of problem behaviours. Data from two community samples, one retrospective (n = 281) and one current (n = 604), were analysed using hierarchical regression to determine which facets of impulsivity, as assessed with a comprehensive scale (i.e. negative urgency, positive urgency, lack or premeditation, lack of perseverance, and sensation seeking), best predicted a series of problem behaviours (i.e. problem gambling, disorderly alcohol use, online gambling disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder behaviours, and social media addiction). The impulsive urgency facets were shown to be significant predictors across the behaviours examined. More specifically, negative urgency was the strongest predictor of disorderly alcohol use, obsessive-compulsive disorder behaviours, and social media addiction. Positive urgency was associated with problem gambling and online gambling disorder behaviours. These findings suggest that impulsive urgency is a key contributing factor in many behavioural problems and that the valence of the urgency is an important consideration when addressing a broad range of psychopathologies.

Keywords: Impulsivity, negative urgency, problem gambling, alcohol abuse, obsessive-compulsive disorder, social media addiction


Impulsivity can be broadly defined as ‘behaviour that incorporates a component of rashness, lack of foresight or planning’ (Dawe and Loxton, 2004: 343). As noted by Cyders (2015), impulsivity is the most common criterion in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) of the American Psychiatric Association (APA, 2013) and as such has clear implications for a diverse range of clinical disorders and abnormal patterns of behaviour, including attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Dawe and Loxton, 2004; Whiteside and Lynam, 2001), problem gambling (Blain et al., 2015; Dussault et al., 2011; Teese et al., 2020; Whiteside et al., 2005), and substance abuse (Gullo et al., 2011; Whiteside et al., 2005). While the majority of the related empirical evidence provided is cross-sectional, prospective longitudinal studies (e.g. Littlefield et al., 2010; Thompson et al., 2015) have also demonstrated the predictive utility of impulsivity in accounting for problem behaviour/psychopathology.

While the above reinforce the importance of impulsivity to problem behaviours clinically and empirically, the understanding of its exact impact has been thwarted by a lack of consensus regarding impulsivity conceptualisation and measurement (Sharma et al., 2014). There have been a number of different conceptualisations of impulsivity resulting in conjecture over its dimensionality. Early conceptualisations such as those underpinning the Eysenck Impulsiveness Inventory (EII; Eysenck and Eysenck, 1978), noted impulsivity as a unidimensional behaviour/construct, measured alongside the continuum of venturesomeness and empathy, while acknowledging the potential role of emotional aspects impacting impulsivity. Later models of impulsivity adopted a multi-faceted understanding, eventually leading to the development of the popular Barratt Impulsiveness Inventory version 11 (BIS-11; Patton et al., 1995). The BIS-11 broadly splits impulsivity into attentional, motor, and non-planning aspects, but does not directly measure emotional impulsivity. In contrast to the BIS-11, the subsequent Urgency, Premeditation (lack of), Perseverance (lack of), Sensation Seeking, Positive Urgency, Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS; Whiteside and Lynam, 2001, UPPS-P; Lynam et al., 2006) introduced emotional urgency alongside both sensation seeking and cognitive impulsivity aspects.

While similar in underlying concept, the UPPS-P focuses on different variations of emotional drive than the EII. Where the EII focuses on empathy, as an externalised form of emotion (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1978), the UPPS-P focuses on urgency, as an internalised form of emotion. In that context, positive urgency (PU) refers to acting rashly when experiencing extreme positive emotions, and negative urgency (NU) refers to acting rashly when experiencing extreme negative emotions (Lynam et al., 2006). Both PU and NU have since demonstrated robust support within the literature as predictors of problem behaviour (Billieux et al., 2010), problem gambling (Teese et al., 2020), substance abuse (Cyders and Smith, 2008; Cyders et al., 2010), eating disorders (Magel and Ranson, 2021), and have also been shown to interact with pathological conditions including post-traumatic stress, whereby they exacerbated problematic alcohol consumption (Brown et al., 2021).

From a neurobiological perspective, functional changes in brain activity within neural regions associated with reward sensitivity and impulse control (Crone et al., 2016; Harden and Mann, 2015; Harden et al., 2017; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2016) provide evidence of the importance of the prefrontal cortex in the association between impulsivity and problem behaviour. More recent evidence has provided insight into possible mechanisms by which urgency might impact on problem behaviour. For example, urgency can excessively tax response inhibition pathways (e.g. Chester et al., 2016; Johnson et al., 2020), heighten reward sensitivity across the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and ventral striatum (Edmiston et al., 2020), and increase activity within the anterior insular when making risky decisions (Johnson et al., 2020).

Despite this emerging evidence, conjecture remains within the literature as to whether the emotional component of impulsivity is a unitary construct or whether the valence of the impulsive urgency creates two constructs (positive and negative; see Johnson et al., 2020, for a review). This debate is yet to be resolved, with some indicating that valence plays little part in the impact that impulsivity plays on behaviour (e.g. Johnson et al., 2020), proposing that it is more one’s inability to maintain control under emotional states that is the issue. Such a position is supported by strong similarity in the predictive utility of PU and NU for disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (e.g. Price et al., 2017), as well as similar effect sizes in the predictive utility of PU and NU across a number of other forms of psychopathology and problem behaviour (e.g. Johnson et al., 2017). In contrast, alternative findings imply important distinctions between PU and NU, especially when evaluating the predictive utility of NU in relation to bulimia and substance use (e.g. Cyders and Smith, 2008), or the importance of PU to problem gambling behaviour (e.g. Teese et al., 2020). In addition, those experiencing manic episodes have demonstrated elevated levels of PU but not NU (Muhtadie et al., 2014), suggesting there may be some key similarities and differences in the impact of urgency valence, and a clear need to gain further insight into the extent to which these similarities and differences impact on problematic behaviour and psychopathology, as well as the possible mechanisms underlying any differences.

To add to this area of knowledge, the present research presents findings from two studies, one retrospectively analysed (Teese et al., 2020) and a current study, exploring the utility of emotional impulsivity as predictors of problem behaviours including problem gambling, online gambling disorder, alcohol abuse, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) behaviours, and social media addiction (SMA). It was hypothesised that either NU or PU would be significant unique predictors of all problem behaviours assessed within the context of a UPPS-P five-factor regression model.

Method

Participants

The participants for this study are drawn from two different studies. Study 1 comprised a sample of 281 Australian emerging adults (18–32) recruited via convenience using online media platforms in 2017. Study 2 comprised 604 all adult paid participants recruited in early 2021, sampled using Prolific (2020) in English from Australia, New Zealand, the United Kingdom, Canada, and the United States. Demographics for all studies are shown in Table 1. After cases with missing data were removed, there were fewer full responses to the Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test (AUDIT; n = 111) for study 1. For study 2, only participants who had gambled online completed the Online Gambling Disorder Questionnaire (OGDQ; n = 241). All behavioural problem variables from these two studies were analysed and are reported below.

Table 1.

Demographics for all tests.

Test scale Biological gender N (n with age) M (SD) age Age range
Study 1 PGSI Male 116 (51) 26.941 (9.702) 18–57
Female 129 (66) 24.197 (7.663) 18–50
AUDIT Unspecified 36 (0)
Male 51 (48) 27.438 (9.785) 18–57
Female 60 (55) 24.291 (8.254) 18–50
Study 2 OGDQ Male 165 24.04 (3.62) 18–32
Female 69 25.30 (3.41) 19–30
Unspecified 7 22.57 (2.51) 19–26
All other scales Male 303 23.86 (3.63) 18–32
Female 288 24.03 (3.68) 18–30
Unspecified 13 23.69 (3.35) 19–29

SD: standard deviation; PGSI: Problem Gambling Severity Index; AUDIT: Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test; OGDQ: Online Gambling Disorder Questionnaire.

Measures

Urgency, Premeditation (lack of), Perseverance (lack of), Sensation Seeking, Positive Urgency, Impulsive Behavior Scale

The UPPS-P (Lynam et al., 2006) is a 59-item 4-point Likert-type impulsivity measure (1 = strongly agree to 4 = strongly disagree). This scale consists of five factors: negative urgency (NU; 12 items, for example, ‘I have trouble controlling my impulses’), positive urgency (PU; 14 items, for example, ‘I tend to lose control when I am in a great mood’), lack of premeditation (11 items, for example, ‘My thinking is usually careful and purposeful’), lack of perseverance (10 items, for example, ‘I tend to give up easily’), and sensation seeking (12 items, for example, ‘I generally seek new and exciting experiences and sensations’). Each scale is calculated as the mean of all the allocated items. A higher score represents higher impulsivity. Cronbach’s alpha for the UPPS-P ranged from 0.794 to 0.937 for the 2017 sample and 0.861 to 0.943 for 2021 sample.

Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale Short

The Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale Short (MCSD; Reynolds, 1982) is a true/false measure of socially desirable responding and was used as a control variable in both samples (e.g. ‘I like to gossip at times’). Two similar but different 10-item versions were used for the 2017 and 2021 data. Cronbach’s alpha for MCSD for 2017 was 0.569 for the AUDIT study and 0.618 for the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI) study. Cronbach’s alpha for 2021 was 0.653 for the OGDQ study and 0.616 for all remaining studies.

Problem Gambling Severity Index

The PGSI (Ferris and Wynne, 2001) is a 4-point Likert-type, nine-item measure of problem gambling (0 = never to 3 = almost always; for example, ‘Have you bet more than you could really afford?’). This scale is unidimensional with a higher score indicating higher problem gambling. The current studies scored the scale as a continuous variable rather than categorical risk categories. Cronbach’s alpha for the PGSI was 0.912.

Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test

The AUDIT (Babor et al., 1992) is a 10-item measure of disordered alcohol use. The first eight questions are scored on a 5-point Likert-type scale and the last two questions on a 3-point Likert-type scale (0 through 4 and 0-2-4, respectively; for example, ‘How often during the last year have you had a feeling of guilt or remorse after drinking?’). This scale is unidimensional with a higher score indicating an individual is more likely to have an alcohol use disorder. Cronbach’s alpha for the AUDIT was 0.752.

Online Gambling Disorder Questionnaire

The OGDQ (González-Cabrera et al., 2020) is a 12-item measure of online problem gambling scored on a 5-point Likert-type response scale (1 = Never to 5 = Every day; for example, ‘Have you ever tried to control, reduce or stop gambling online and have not been able to do so?’). Higher score indicates higher disordered online gambling, with a score of four or more in any item considered a ‘problem indicator’, and four or more problem indicators suggesting an online gambling disorder. The current studies used the scale as a continuous dependent variable. Cronbach’s alpha for the OGD-Q was 0.938.

Obsessive Compulsive Inventory–Revised

The Obsessive Compulsive Inventory–Revised (OCI-R; Foa et al., 2002) is an 18-item measure of obsessive-compulsive behaviours scored on a 5-point Likert-type response scale (0 = not at all, to 4 = extremely; for example, ‘I check things more often than necessary’). This scale is unidimensional with a higher score indicating higher obsessive-compulsive tendencies. Cronbach’s alpha for the OCI-R was 0.905.

Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale

The Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS; Andreassen et al., 2012) is a six-item measure of social media addiction scored on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = very rarely to 5 = very often; for example, ‘How often in the last 6 months did you feel an urge to use social media more and more?’). This scale is unidimensional with a higher score indicating that an individual demonstrates higher social media addiction. Cronbach’s alpha for the BSMAS was 0.887.

Procedure

University ethics approval was granted for both studies. Participants for study 1 received a link via social media platforms that took them to a Qualtrics survey. Upon informed consent, participants completed a series of digital questionnaires including the UPPS-P, MCSD short, PGSI, and AUDIT. For study 2, participants received an invitation link via Prolific that redirected them to a Qualtrics survey. Upon informed consent, participants completed a series of digital questionnaires including the UPPS-P, MCSD, mindful attention awareness scale (MAAS), OCI-R, personal wellbeing index (PWI), and BSMAS. Participants who indicated they had previously gambled online were also asked to complete the OGDQ. Upon completing the study, a reimbursement for their time was transferred to their Prolific accounts, approximately £7.50/h based on average completion time.

Analysis

With alpha set at 0.05, a series of hierarchical regressions were run predicting a range of behavioural problems from the five facets of impulsivity measured, while controlling for social desirability. In each of the presented tests, the following assumptions were met. Linearity was met through visual inspection of plots and partial plots. Homoscedasticity of residuals was met through visual inspection of plots of standardised residuals against standardised predicted values. Multicollinearity was met with acceptable variance influence factor (VIF) and tolerance levels (below 10 and above 1, respectively). No univariate outliers were present as assessed by leverage values (below 0.2 for all participants) and Cook’s values (below 1 for all participants). No multivariate outliers were present as assessed by a chi-square of Mahalanobis distance (above 0.001). Normality was met as all values of skewness and kurtosis were within acceptable range (±3). After evaluating the cost of type 1 versus type 2 errors, it was decided not to adjust alpha levels on multiple comparisons (Di Stefano, 2003). Statistical power is reported for each regression.

Results

With a minimum possible score of 1 and a maximum of 4 on any of the UPPS-P scales, Tables 2 and 3 show that both studies had participants exhibiting both higher and lower levels of impulsivity traits.

Table 2.

Study 1 descriptives.

Scale N M (SD) Min-Max
OGDQ MCSD 241 4.494 (2.164) 0–10
OGDQ 241 18.851 (8.860) 12–61
Lack of Prem 241 1.962 (0.505) 1–3.73
Lack of Pers 241 2.012 (0.482) 1–3.40
Sens Seek 241 2.653 (0.571) 1–3.92
NU 241 2.622 (0.610) 1–4
PU 241 2.211 (0.630) 1–4
All other tests MCSD 604 4.657 (2.096) 0–10
OCI-R 604 21.306 (12.851) 0–72
BSMAS 604 14.505 (5.956) 6–30
Lack of Prem 604 1.886 (0.475) 1–3.73
Lack of Pers 604 2.000 (0.478) 1–3.40
Sens Seek 604 2.525 (0.613) 1–4
NU 604 2.588 (0.613) 1–4
PU 604 2.103 (0.637) 1–4

SD: standard deviation; OGDQ: Online Gambling Disorder Questionnaire; MCSD: Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale Short; NU: negative urgency; PU: positive urgency; OCI-R: Obsessive Compulsive Inventory–Revised; BSMAS: Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale.

Table 3.

Study 2 descriptives.

Scale N M (SD) Min-Max
PGSI PGSI 281 1.911 (3.766) 0–27
MCSD 281 4.737 (2.123) 10–20
Lack of Prem 281 1.990 (0.482) 1–4
Lack of Pers 281 1.838 (0.450) 1–3.30
Sens Seek 281 2.620 (0.669) 1–4
NU 281 2.340 (0.603) 1–3.92
PU 281 1.939 (0.647) 1–4
AUDIT AUDIT 111 8.288 (5.547) 0–25
MCSD 111 4.658 (1.961) 10–19
Lack of Prem 111 2 (0.464) 1–3.27
Lack of Pers 111 1.924 (0.422) 1–3.10
Sens Seek 111 2.743 (0.656) 1.08–4
NU 111 2.361 (0.598) 1–3.75
PU 111 1.954 (0.649) 1–4

SD: standard deviation; PGSI: Problem Gambling Severity Index; MCSD: Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale Short; NU: negative urgency; PU: positive urgency; AUDIT: Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test.

Tables 48 display the main results for the hierarchal regressions of the UPPS-P impulsivity traits against problem gambling, disorderly alcohol use, online gambling disorder, OCD behaviours, and social media addiction, when controlling for participant social desirability.

Table 4.

Hierarchical regression on problem gambling.

B [95% CI] Standard error β t p Semi-partial
1 Constant 8.452
[4.785, 12.493]
1.951 5.535 <0.001
MCSD −0.444
[−0.701, −0.209]
0.125 −0.250 −4.327 0.001 −0.250
2 Constant 5.070
[0.230, 10.249]
2.566 2.006 0.047
MCSD −0.330
[−0.589, −0.078]
0.128 −0.186 −3.027 0.011 −0.168
Lack of Prem −0.342
[−1.512, 0.748]
0.580 −0.044 −0.645 0.564 −0.036
Lack of Pers −0.007
[−1.410, 1.408]
0.722 −0.001 −0.012 0.991 −0.001
Sens Seek −0.370
[−0.989, 0.258]
0.321 −0.066 −1.042 0.254 −0.058
NU −0.429
[−1.823, 0.713]
0.639 −0.069 −0.786 0.507 −0.044
PU 2.257
[0.789, 3.980]
0.823 0.388 4.273 0.008 0.237

CI: confidence interval; MCSD: Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale Short; NU: negative urgency; PU: positive urgency.

Bootstrapped to 5000.

Table 5.

Hierarchical regression on disorderly alcohol use.

B [95% CI] Standard error β t p Semi-partial
1 Constant 8.428
[−0.694, 17.293]
4.579 2.104 0.065
MCSD −0.010
[−0.611, 0.626]
0.314 −0.003 −0.035 0.975 −0.003
2 Constant −3.888
[−17.437, 9.861]
6.975 −0.641 0.571
MCSD 0.143
[−0.496, 0.817]
0.330 0.051 0.514 0.660 0.048
Lack of Prem 1.780
[−1.031, 4.535]
1.423 0.149 1.370 0.212 0.127
Lack of Pers −0.123
[−3.573, 1.537]
−1.003 −0.076 −0.698 0.432 −0.065
Sens Seek 1.240
[−0.583, 2.978]
0.913 0.147 1.394 0.178 0.129
NU 2.411
[0.047, 4.742]
1.188 0.260 1.991 0.042 0.184
PU −0.333
[−2.923, 2.457]
1.345 −0.039 −0.281 0.801 −0.026

CI: confidence interval; MCSD: Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale Short; NU: negative urgency; PU: positive urgency.

Bootstrapped to 5000.

Table 6.

Hierarchical regression on online gambling disorder.

B [95% CI] Standard error β t p Semi-partial
1 Constant 23.120
[20.271, 26.005]
1.462 18.004 <0.001
MCSD −0.950
[−1.458, −0.447]
0.257 −0.232 −3.689 <0.001 −0.232
2 Constant 6.473
[−2.009, 14.664]
4.229 1.471 0.125
MCSD −0.565
[−1.016, −0.109]
0.230 −0.138 −2.132 0.014 −0.124
Lack of Prem −0.0131
[−2.856, 2.959]
1.470 −0.007 −0.096 0.928 −0.006
Lack of Pers −0.725
[−3.420, 1.845]
1.338 −0.039 −0.517 0.588 −0.030
Sens Seek 1.397
[−0.753, 3.407]
1.044 0.090 1.384 0.178 0.080
NU 0.601
[−1.541, 2.856]
1.106 0.041 0.495 0.578 0.029
PU 5.133
[2.883, 7.482]
1.181 0.365 4.824 <0.001 0.280

CI: confidence interval; MCSD: Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale Short; NU: negative urgency; PU: positive urgency.

Bootstrapped to 5000.

Table 7.

Hierarchical regression on OCD behaviours.

B [95% CI] Standard error β t p Semi-partial
1 Constant 26.432
[23.803, 29.116]
1.370 21.060 <0.001
MCSD −1.101
[−1.616, −0.588]
0.263 −0.180 −4.478 <0.001 −0.180
2 Constant 1.348
[−6.064, 8.938]
3.826 0.365 0.738
MCSD −0.234
[−0.696, 0.211]
0.235 −0.038 −0.989 0.323 −0.035
Lack of Prem −0.4525
[−6.951, −0.2005]
1.257 −0.167 −3.704 <0.001 −0.131
Lack of Pers −1.524
[−3.743, 0.703]
1.132 −0.057 −1.344 0.175 −0.048
Sens Seek 0.685
[−0.966, 2.350]
0.842 0.033 0.832 0.409 0.029
NU 7.242
[5.231, 9.360]
1.055 0.345 7.031 <0.001 0.249
PU 5.781
[3.709, 7.677]
1.013 0.286 5.871 <0.001 0.208

CI: confidence interval; MCSD: Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale Short; NU: negative urgency; PU: positive urgency.

Bootstrapped to 5000.

Table 8.

Hierarchical regression on social media addiction.

B [95% CI] Standard error β t p Semi-partial
1 Constant 17.253
[16.080, 18.422]
0.597 29.828 <0.001
MCSD −0.590
[−0.809, −0.371]
0.112 −0.208 −5.209 <0.001 −0.208
2 Constant 7.405
[3.887, 10.948]
1.806 4.016 <0.001
MCSD −0.283
[−0.510, −0.062]
0.115 −0.100 −2.405 0.014 −0.092
Lack of Prem −1.590
[−0.2839, −0.269]
0.666 −0.127 −2.611 0.017 −0.099
Lack of Pers 0.668
[−0.446, 1.745]
0.559 0.054 1.183 0.221 0.045
Sens Seek 0.543
[−0.265, 1.347]
0.421 0.056 1.324 0.199 0.050
NU 2.354
[1.352, 3.379]
0.513 0.242 4.585 <0.001 0.174
PU 1.245
[0.210, 2.262]
0.521 0.133 2.537 0.016 0.097

CI: confidence interval; MCSD: Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale Short; NU: negative urgency; PU: positive urgency.

Bootstrapped to 5000.

As shown in Table 4 after controlling for social desirability, PU was the only significant predictor of problem gambling. The UPPS-P as a whole significantly predicted problem gambling, accounting for 15.5% of the variance, F(6, 274) = 8.425, p < 0.001, R2= 0.155, adj R2 = 0.137. Achieved power, as computed with G*Power (Faul et al., 2009), was >0.999, indicating more than adequate statistical power.

As shown in Table 5, after controlling for social desirability, NU was the only significant predictor of disorderly alcohol use. The UPPS-P as a whole did not significantly predicted disorderly alcohol use, accounting for 11% of the overall variance, F(6, 104) = 2.137, p = 0.055, R2 = 0.110, adj R2 = 0.058. Achieved power, as computed with G*Power, was 0.774, slightly below the 0.8 standard.

As shown in Table 6, after controlling for social desirability, only PU was a significant predictor of online gambling. The UPPS-P as a whole significantly predicted online gambling, accounting for 21.3% of the variance, F(6, 234) = 10.574, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.213, adj R2 = 0.193. Achieved power, as computed with G*Power, was >0.999, indicating more than adequate statistical power.

As shown in Table 7, after controlling for social desirability, and in order of predictive strength, NU, PU, and lack of premeditation were significant predictors of OCD behaviours. The UPPS-P as a whole significantly predicted OCD behaviours, accounting for 25.2% of the variance, F(6, 597) = 33.603, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.252, adj R2 = 0.245. Achieved power, as computed with G*Power, was >0.999, indicating more than adequate statistical power.

As shown in Table 8, after controlling for social desirability, and in order of predictive strength, NU, lack of premeditation, and PU were significant predictors of SMA. As a whole the UPPS-P significantly predicted social media addiction, accounting for 13.5% of the variance, F(6,597) = 15.529, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.135, adj R2 = 0.126. Achieved power, as computed with G*Power, was >0.999, indicating more than adequate statistical power. Across the five hierarchical regressions run, the strength of the predictors is displayed in Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Strength of predictors summary.

As summarised in Figure 1, across the five hierarchical regression run, lack of premeditation was a significant predictor twice, lack of perseverance was not a significant predictor, sensation seeking was not a significant predictor, negative urgency was a significant predictor in three regressions, and positive urgency was a significant predictor in four regressions. Of those, negative urgency was the strongest predictor in three regressions and positive urgency the strongest predictor in the remaining two.

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the predictive utility of impulsive urgency when compared to other facets of impulsivity, and the importance of valence in accounting for a range of problem behaviours. The hypothesis that either NU or PU would predict all behaviours examined in this study was supported. In all analyses, PU, NU, or both were shown to be significant unique predictors of a number of problematic behaviours assessed. Furthermore, for problem gambling, disorderly alcohol use, and online gambling disorder, PU and NU were the only significant predictors within the UPPS-P. NU was the strongest predictor of both SMA and OCD. These findings confirm the predictive utility of impulsive urgency in addressing a series of important problem behaviours and also the significance of considering the valence of urgency.

When comparing across PU and NU, there are some similarities and important contrasts in the results. Both positive and negative urgency significantly predicted OCD behaviours and SMA; however, NU was shown to be the more robust predictor in both cases. Similar to past studies, online gambling and problem gambling were only predicted by PU (Teese et al., 2020), where NU was the only predictor of disordered alcohol use (Cyders and Smith, 2008; Cyders et al., 2010). While similar in some aspects, these findings denote a clear difference between the predictive utility of positive and negative valences of urgency, suggesting the importance of their distinction, as noted in the original model design (Lynam et al., 2006) and by confirmatory factor analysis on the UPPS-P (Teese et al., 2020; Zsila et al., 2020).

While further research is needed in order to gain a full picture of how valence influences different forms of problem behaviour, past research provides some insight. For example, findings from Emery et al. (2014) in relation to problematic alcohol use suggest that PU may relate to reward sensitivity, whereas NU is associated with a lack of impulse control, which could imply that urgency facets may impact on behaviour via distinct neural pathways. Other research has suggested that the social context of behaviour may be important in understanding the influence of impulsivity facets on behaviour, especially in social situations that enhance sensitivity to emotional arousal (Crone et al., 2016; Steinberg, 2008). This could indeed impact on the types of behaviour associated with greater positive or negative emotions. Currently, studies are limited in the extent to which these issues have been investigated, as some studies appear to overlook the influence of one’s emotional state on their risky behaviours, despite the majority of these behaviours being more likely to occur in situations with greater emotional intensity (Millstein, 2003; Steinberg, 2004).

With clear support for the importance and valence of emotional aspects within the conceptualisation of impulsivity, a quandary arises with a few popular models of impulsivity. Some models of impulsivity, such as the popular BIS-11 (Patton et al., 1995), do not explicitly take into consideration emotional aspects of impulsivity. This is not the first criticism the BIS-11 has received, with it being recently questioned on its factor structure (e.g. Vasconcelos et al., 2012). As such, it is proposed that this research adds to a growing body of evidence indicating the need for a review of how impulsivity is conceptualised and studied/measured. Acknowledging that the BIS-11 was not part of this study, so no direct claims can be made on its structure from this study alone, future efforts should comparatively incorporate different impulsivity scales like the BIS-11 and the UPPS-P, that either do or do not explicitly address varying emotional aspects, to examine if distinct emotional qualities, such as the UPPS-P’s NU, are accounting for something uniquely different within impulsivity.

It is worth noting that as a cross-sectional study, causation cannot be inferred. The lack of a predictive model for alcohol use is also inconsistent with previous research and likely due to the small sample size for that variable (n = 111). Despite these limitations, the current study presents evidence of the utility of emotion-based impulsivity in predicting problem behaviours. It also suggests that emotional valence may be an important factor for one to consider when aiming to differentially predict/address certain problematic behaviours.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Christopher Willie: Conceptualisation, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Peter Richard Gill: Conceptualisation, Data curation Formal Analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Robert Teese: Conceptualisation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Vasileios Stavropoulos: Methodology, Supervision Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Andrew Jago: Data curation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Declaration of conflicting interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding: The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edn). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  2. Andreassen CS, Torsheim T, Brunborg GS, et al. (2012) Development of a Facebook Addiction Scale. Psychological Reports 110(2): 501–517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Saunders J, et al. (1992) AUDIT. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test. Guidelines for Use in Primary Health Care. Geneva: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
  4. Billieux J, Gay P, Rochat L, et al. (2010) The role of urgency and its underlying psychological mechanisms in problematic behaviours. Behaviour Research and Therapy 48(11): 1085–1096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blain B, Gill PR, Teese R. (2015) Predicting problem gambling in Australian adults using a multifaceted model of impulsivity. International Gambling Studies 15(2): 239–255. [Google Scholar]
  6. Brown R, Mortensen J, Hawn S, et al. (2021) Drinking patterns of post-deployment Veterans: The role of personality, negative urgency, and posttraumatic stress. Military Psychology 33(4): 240–249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chester DS, Lynam DR, Milich R, et al. (2016) How do negative emotions impair self-control? A neural model of negative urgency. NeuroImage 132: 43–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Crone EA, van Duijvenvoorde ACK, Peper JS. (2016) Annual research review: Neural contributions to risk-taking in adolescence – Developmental changes and individual differences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 5(3): 353–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cyders M, Smith G. (2008) Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: Positive and negative urgency. Psychological Bulletin 134(6): 807–828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cyders MA. (2015) The misnomer of impulsivity: Commentary on ‘choice impulsivity’ and ‘rapid-response impulsivity’ articles by Hamilton and colleagues. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 6(2): 204–205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cyders MA, Zapolski TCB, Combs JL, et al. (2010) Experimental effect of positive urgency on negative outcomes from risk taking and on increased alcohol consumption. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 24(3): 367–375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dawe S, Loxton N. (2004) The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 28(3): 343–351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Di Stefano J. (2003) How much power is enough? Against the development of an arbitrary convention for statistical power calculations. Functional Ecology 17(5): 707–709. [Google Scholar]
  14. Dussault F, Brendgen M, Vitaro F, et al. (2011) Longitudinal links between impulsivity, gambling problems and depressive symptoms: A transactional model from adolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52(2): 130–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Edmiston EK, Fournier JC, Chase HW, et al. (2020) Assessing relationships among impulsive sensation seeking, reward circuitry activity, and risk for psychopathology: A functional magnetic resonance imaging replication and extension study. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging 5(7): 660–668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Emery NN, Simons JS, Clarke CJ, et al. (2014) Emotion differentiation and alcohol-related problems: The mediating role of urgency. Addictive Behaviors 39(10): 1459–1463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Eysenck S, Eysenck H. (1978) Impulsiveness and venturesomeness: Their position in a dimensional system of personality description. Psychological Reports 43(3_suppl): 1247–1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, et al. (2009) Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods 41(4): 1149–1160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ferris J, Wynne H. (2001) The Canadian Problem Gambling Index: Final Report. Ottawa, ON, Canada: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse. [Google Scholar]
  20. Foa E, Huppert J, Leiberg S, et al. (2002) The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: Development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment 14(4): 485–496. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. González-Cabrera J, Machimbarrena J, Beranuy M, et al. (2020) Design and measurement properties of the Online Gambling Disorder Questionnaire (OGD-Q) in Spanish adolescents. Journal of Clinical Medicine 9(1): 120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gullo M, Ward E, Dawe S, et al. (2011) Support for a two-factor model of impulsivity and hazardous substance use in British and Australian young adults. Journal of Research in Personality 45(1): 10–18. [Google Scholar]
  23. Harden KP, Mann FD. (2015) Biological risk for the development of problem behavior in adolescence: Integrating insights from behavioral genetics and neuroscience. Child Development Perspectives 9(4): 211–216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Harden KP, Kretsch N, Mann FD, et al. (2017) Beyond dual systems: A genetically-informed, latent factor model of behavioural and self-report measures related to adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 25: 221–234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Johnson SL, Elliott MV, Carver CS. (2020) Impulsive responses to positive and negative emotions: Parallel neurocognitive correlates and their implications. Biological Psychiatry 87(4): 338–349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Johnson SL, Tharp JA, Peckham AD, et al. (2017) A path model of different forms of impulsivity with externalizing and internalizing psychopathology: Towards greater specificity. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology 56(3): 235–252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Littlefield AK, Sher KJ, Steinley D. (2010) Developmental trajectories of impulsivity and their association with alcohol use and related outcomes during emerging and young adulthood I. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 34(8): 1409–1416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lynam DR, Smith GT, Whiteside SP, et al. (2006) The UPPS-P: Assessing Five Personality Pathways to Impulsive Behavior. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University. [Google Scholar]
  29. Magel C, Ranson K. (2021) Negative urgency combined with negative emotionality is linked to eating disorder psychopathology in community women with and without binge eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders 54(5): 821–830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Millstein SG. (2003) Risk perception: Construct development, links to theory, correlates, and manifestations. In: Romer D. (ed.) Reducing Adolescent Risk: Toward and Integrated Approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE, pp. 35–43. [Google Scholar]
  31. Muhtadie L, Johnson SL, Carver CS, et al. (2014) A profile approach to impulsivity in bipolar disorder: The key role of strong emotions. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 129(2): 100–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Patton J, Stanford M, Barratt E. (1995) Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology 51(6): 768–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Price M, Connor J, Allen H. (2017) The moderating effect of childhood maltreatment on the relations among PTSD symptoms, positive urgency, and negative urgency. Journal of Traumatic Stress 30(4): 432–437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Reynolds W. (1982) Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology 38(1): 119–125. [Google Scholar]
  35. Sharma L, Markon KE, Clark LA. (2014) Toward a theory of distinct types of ‘impulsive’ behaviors: A meta-analysis of self-report and behavioural measures. Psychological Bulletin 140(2): 374–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Steinberg L. (2004) Risk taking in adolescence: What changes, and why? In: Dahl RE, Spear LP. (eds) Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 1021. Adolescent Brain Development: Vulnerabilities and Opportunities. New York: New York Academy of Sciences, pp. 51–58. [Google Scholar]
  37. Steinberg L. (2008) A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review 28(1): 78–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Teese R, Willie C, Jago A, et al. (2020) An investigation of Alternative factor models of Impulsivity using the UPPS-P. Journal of Personality Assessment 103(3): 324–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Thompson K, Roemer A, Leadbeater B. (2015) Impulsive personality, parental monitoring, and alcohol outcomes from adolescence through young adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Health 57(3): 320–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. van Duijvenvoorde ACK, Peters S, Braams BR, et al. (2016) What motivates adolescents? Neural responses to rewards and their influence on adolescents’ risk taking, learning, and cognitive control. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 70: 135–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Vasconcelos A, Malloy-Diniz L, Correa H. (2012) Systematic review of psychometric proprieties of Barratt impulsiveness scale version 11 (BIS-11). Clinical Neuropsychiatry 9(2): 61–74. [Google Scholar]
  42. Whiteside S, Lynam D. (2001) The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences 30(4): 669–689. [Google Scholar]
  43. Whiteside S, Lynam D, Miller J, et al. (2005) Validation of the UPPS impulsive behaviour scale: A four-factor model of impulsivity. European Journal of Personality 19(7): 559–574. [Google Scholar]
  44. Zsila Á, Bőthe B, Demetrovics Z, et al. (2020) Further exploration of the SUPPS-P impulsive behavior scale’s factor structure: Evidence from a large Hungarian sample. Current Psychology 39: 378–388. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Brain and Neuroscience Advances are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES