Abstract
Neurobehavioral task performance is modulated by the circadian and homeostatic processes of sleep/wake regulation. Biomathematical modeling of the temporal dynamics of these processes and their interaction allows for prospective prediction of performance impairment in shift-workers and provides a basis for fatigue risk management in 24/7 operations. It has been reported, however, that the impact of the circadian rhythm—and in particular its timing—is inherently task-dependent, which would have profound implications for our understanding of the temporal dynamics of neurobehavioral functioning and the accuracy of biomathematical model predictions. We investigated this issue in a laboratory study designed to unambiguously dissociate the influences of the circadian and homeostatic processes on neurobehavioral performance, as measured during a constant routine protocol preceded by three days on either a simulated night shift or a simulated day shift schedule. Neurobehavioral functions were measured every 2 h using three functionally distinct assays: a digit symbol substitution test, a psychomotor vigilance test, and the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale. After dissociating the circadian and homeostatic influences and accounting for inter-individual variability, peak circadian performance occurred in the late biological afternoon (in the “wake maintenance zone”) for all three neurobehavioral assays. Our results are incongruent with the idea of inherent task-dependent differences in the endogenous circadian impact on performance. Rather, our results suggest that neurobehavioral functions are under top-down circadian control, consistent with the way they are accounted for in extant biomathematical models.
Keywords: cognitive throughput, constant routine, inter-individual differences, simulated shift-work, sleep/wake homeostasis, subjective sleepiness, vigilant attention
1. Introduction
Neurobehavioral functioning is strongly influenced by the two processes of sleep/wake regulation: a homeostatic process (“Process S”), which degrades neurobehavioral functioning over time awake and restores it during sleep; and a circadian process (“Process C”), which cycles between promoting and depressing neurobehavioral functioning across time of day [1,2]. The combined effect of these two processes governs changes in waking neurobehavioral functioning over time, such that day shift-workers tend to exhibit stable, near-optimal performance through the workday [3,4], whereas night shift-workers typically experience a steady decline of performance through the night [5,6]. Inter-individual differences in the contributions of the two processes notwithstanding [7,8], these dynamics are well understood [9,10] and have led to the development of biomathematical models predicting neurobehavioral performance across a wide range of sleep/wake/work scenarios [11,12]. Such biomathematical models can be used to prospectively predict performance impairment in shift-workers [13,14] and may be implemented in 24/7 operations as a tool for fatigue risk management [15,16].
The importance of circadian rhythms for neurobehavioral performance has long been recognized [17,18,19]. It has been observed, however, that the circadian rhythmicity of neurobehavioral functioning may be task-dependent. Early studies reported that under conditions of sleep deprivation and forced desynchrony, the timing of optimal functioning varies across tasks [20,21]. For example, performance on complex cognitive tasks (e.g., verbal reasoning) was found to peak in the late morning [22,23], whereas simple perceptual-motor task performance was optimal in the late afternoon [24]. These differences suggested the involvement of multiple, distinct underlying brain oscillators [25,26] and led researchers to conclude that “it is as incorrect to speak of a single performance rhythm as it is to speak of single physiological rhythm” [26] (p. 544). Furthermore, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data from a recent sleep deprivation study, captured during a sustained attention test administered multiple times over the circadian cycle, showed variation in the phase of circadian rhythmicity between brain regions. According to the investigators, this “rules out a global task-independent circadian influence and suggests the influence of a local, region-specific, task-dependent circadian signal” [27] (p. 690). However, when performance was investigated under constant routine (CR) conditions, differences in the circadian rhythms of performance between short-term memory and calculation performance tasks and subjective alertness [28] as well as between serial search, verbal reasoning, manual dexterity, and visual vigilance tasks [29] were largely absent. As Monk and colleagues observed, their CR study “confirmed the suggestion made by Johnson et al. (1992) that inter-task differences, which may result in differences in time of peak and trough under a normal nychthemeral routine, can fail to do so when the sleep/wake cycle is suspended, and endogenous circadian rhythms are ‘unmasked’ using a constant conditions protocol” [29] (p. 14). These strictly controlled laboratory studies challenged the concept of task-dependent circadian rhythms in neurobehavioral functioning; yet, subsequent publications on the topic have led to renewed interest in the idea [30,31,32,33].
In studies investigating task-dependent variation in the timing of circadian rhythmicity in neurobehavioral functioning, the influence of the circadian process (time of day) needs to be dissociated from the influence of the homeostatic process (time awake) [34]. Because during periods of wakefulness, when neurobehavioral functioning is measured, time of day and time awake change hand in hand, disentangling the circadian (rhythmic) and homeostatic (trending) processes involved must be done post hoc. This is mathematically and statistically complicated and prone to misestimation of rhythm parameters [35]. The problem is compounded by the task-dependence of vulnerability to neurobehavioral impairment due to sleep loss [36,37], which may be mediated by use-dependent, pathway-specific degradation of neuronal processing [38] and implies that there are task-specific differences in the impact of the homeostatic process [39]. These issues can be overcome by explicitly accounting for inter-individual variability during data analysis [7] but, to date, that approach has not been applied to the evaluation of putative task differences tied to circadian rhythmicity. Various other potential confounds further cast doubt on the reliability of reports of task-specificity in the timing of circadian rhythms [34]. That is, different performance tasks are differentially susceptible to phenomena that can mask circadian rhythmicity, including practice effects [40], performance strategy changes [41], and temporal changes in speed/accuracy trade-offs [42]. These masking effects could create the appearance of task-dependent differences in the effect of circadian rhythmicity on neurobehavioral functioning even when no such differences are endogenously produced.
Here, we addressed these issues by means of a simulated shift-work protocol with randomization to either a day shift condition or a 12 h offset night shift condition, followed by a CR protocol, during a 7-day/6-night laboratory study. The CR protocol served to measure neurobehavioral functioning, at 2 h intervals, under strictly controlled conditions, and to expose the endogenous circadian rhythmicity [43]. The neurobehavioral assays we used were functionally distinct [36,40], suitable for repeated administration, and not subject to masking from practice, strategy changes, or dynamically shifting speed/accuracy trade-offs. The test battery included a psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) [44], a computerized digit symbol substitution test (DSST) [45], and the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) [46]. The two conditions of the simulated shift-work protocol that preceded the CR protocol maximized variability in circadian alignment and optimized the mathematical/statistical disentanglement of the effects of the circadian and homeostatic processes on task performance. With mixed-effects regression methodology developed specifically to account for differences between individuals [47], we investigated whether the effect of the circadian process on neurobehavioral functioning is, in fact, fundamentally task-dependent.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Subjects
Fourteen healthy adults (10 men and 4 women), ranging in age from 22 to 34 years (mean ± SD: 25.8 ± 3.2 years), participated in a highly controlled laboratory study. They were physically and psychologically healthy as determined by physical exam, history, and questionnaires. Subjects were normal sleepers with no sleep or circadian disorders as verified with questionnaires and baseline polysomnography, were neither extreme morning types nor extreme evening types as assessed by questionnaire [48], reported no shift-work in the prior three months, and did not travel across time zones during the month prior to participation. They reported habitual sleep durations between 6 h and 10 h per night and wake-up times between 06:00 and 09:00.
During the seven days before the laboratory study, subjects maintained their habitual sleep/wake schedule, without napping, as verified with wrist actigraphy, sleep diaries, and call-ins of bedtimes and waketimes on a time-stamped voice recorder. During the seven days prior and while in the laboratory, subjects refrained from caffeine and alcohol intake, smoking, and drug use (except oral contraceptives), as verified with breathalyzer and urine tests. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Washington State University. Subjects gave written, informed consent and were paid for their time.
2.2. Experimental Design
Subjects completed a 7-day/6-night in-laboratory study, which was designed as shown in Figure 1. Following a baseline day and night (8 h sleep opportunity: 22:00–06:00), subjects were randomized to one of two conditions (seven subjects each): three days of a simulated day shift schedule with 8 h nighttime sleep opportunities (22:00–06:00); or a daytime nap (14:00–18:00) followed by three days of a simulated night shift schedule with 8 h daytime sleep opportunities (10:00–18:00). In both conditions, light exposure during wakefulness was low (<50 lux). Immediately after the last sleep opportunity of the three-day simulated day or night shift schedule, subjects underwent a 24 h CR protocol, during which they stayed awake under continuous behavioral monitoring, ate a small isocaloric snack every hour, and maintained a semi-recumbent posture under constant dim light (<50 lux) and fixed ambient temperature (21 ± 1 °C). The study ended with a recovery period, after which subjects left the laboratory.
Sleep periods were recorded polysomnographically and scored visually using standard criteria promulgated by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine [49]. Sleep findings for this study have been documented previously [50]; here, the sleep records were used to assess the timing of final awakening before the start of the CR protocol (mean ± SD for the day shift condition: 05:59 ± 1 min, for the night shift condition: 17:53 ± 16 min).
During the CR protocol, blood was collected at intervals of 1–3 h through an intravenous catheter. Blood samples were used to measure circulating melatonin (radioimmunoassay IB88111, IBL), and the dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) was assessed as a marker of the timing of the endogenous circadian pacemaker as described previously [51]. For one subject in the simulated night shift condition, the subject-specific DLMO could not be assessed reliably, because the individual’s melatonin concentration did not reach the 10 pg/ml threshold used to define DLMO [51]. As our statistical approach involved using subject-specific circadian time based on DLMO (see below), this subject was removed from the data set as a whole, leaving data from a total of thirteen subjects (n = 7 in the day shift condition and n = 6 in the night shift condition) for analyses.
2.3. Neurobehavioral Assays
Throughout periods of wakefulness during the study, including the CR protocol, subjects completed three neurobehavioral assays every 2 h (Figure 1): the KSS, DSST, and PVT. The KSS was administered both before and after the DSST, which was followed by the PVT.
The KSS is a self-report scale of subjective sleepiness [52] with response options ranging from 1 (extremely alert) to 9 (very sleepy, fighting to stay awake). Scores on the two KSS administrations in each test bout were averaged to yield one KSS outcome measure. The KSS is highly sensitive to circadian rhythmicity [46] and functionally distinct from the DSST and PVT both within [45] and between [36] individuals.
The DSST requires subjects to match digit and symbol stimulus pairs as quickly as they can while prioritizing accuracy [53]. In the 3 min computerized version used here [45], a key is displayed at the top of the screen, indicating a series of different symbols (e.g., star, plus sign) paired with the digits 1–9. In the center of the screen, a symbol is presented, and the subject must type the corresponding digit using the number keys at the top of the keyboard. After each response, the symbol in the center is replaced with a new one; subjects determine the pacing of the task by the speed of their responses. The key remains the same throughout the task duration, but the pairings vary between test bouts. Cognitive throughput (number of correct responses) was used as the outcome measure of interest. The DSST is highly sensitive to circadian rhythmicity [34]. The task shows a minor but persistent practice effect [45]; however, at the start of the CR protocol the practice effect would have approached asymptote as, by that time, subjects had already performed the task more than two dozen times across the simulated shift work days. The components of cognition involved in the DSST are partially distinct from those involved in the PVT [40], and DSST performance has been shown to be functionally distinct from PVT performance both within [45] and between [36] individuals.
The PVT is a 10 min computer-paced, one-choice reaction time task requiring subjects to respond as fast as possible to a stimulus presented as a rolling millisecond counter that appears in random 2–10 s intervals [54]. Rather than the conventionally used number of lapses of attention [55], which exhibits a floor effect, an index of the fidelity of information processing (log-transformed signal-to-noise ratio; LSNR) was used as the outcome measure of interest [56]. The PVT does not show any meaningful practice effects [45], but is highly sensitive to circadian rhythmicity [34].
To account for any idiosyncratic differences between subjects in basal neurobehavioral functioning, outcome measures were expressed relative to the baseline average, calculated using the second through fourth test bouts on day two (at 08:15, 09:20, and 12:00), leaving enough time after the first in-laboratory sleep opportunity to dissipate sleep inertia [57] and preceding the daytime nap at the start of the simulated night shift condition. Difference scores for the DSST were inverted so that larger values corresponded to greater neurobehavioral impairment for all three assays.
2.4. Statistical Methods
Our statistical approach is illustrated in Figure 2. It is based on the equations for the two-process model [58] and our earlier work on the estimation of the contributions of the homeostatic and circadian processes to neurobehavioral impairment during continuous wakefulness [7]. As was shown, the temporal dynamics of performance yj across a period of wakefulness for a given task j can be described with the following regression model:
(1) |
where S and C represent the homeostatic and circadian processes, respectively; βj and γj are task-specific scaling factors for processes S and C, respectively; κj is a task-specific intercept; ta denotes time awake (waking time since final awakening immediately before the start of the CR protocol); tc denotes circadian time (time of day expressed relative to DLMO); and εj is residual variance assumed to be normally distributed over time with zero mean and task-specific variance σj2.
For a given individual i, the two-process model equation for the homeostatic process S during wakefulness [58] can be shown to be written as [7]:
(2) |
where τw = 18.2 h is the previously estimated time constant of homeostatic build-up during wakefulness [58]; ta,i is the subject-specific time awake (depending on the timing of the individual’s final awakening); and S0,i is the initial subject-specific state of the homeostatic process at the start of the waking period. Note that because our study had a between-subjects design with different individuals assigned to the day versus night shift condition, S0,i is automatically condition-specific (and thereby accounts for any sleep-related differences between conditions). Taking the homeostatic process and the intercept in the model together, we can write:
(3) |
where Bj is a task-specific scaling factor, Hi is a subject-specific scaling coefficient thereof, and Kj is a task-specific intercept.
Further, the subject-specific and (potentially) task-dependent endogenous circadian process C is given by [58]:
(4) |
where the A and am coefficients are previously assessed constants [58]; tc,i is the subject-specific circadian time (depending on the timing of the individual’s DLMO); and φij is a subject-specific and (potentially) task-dependent phase angle (i.e., circadian timing of the upward zero crossing of process C relative to the subject-specific DLMO). Note again that because our study had a between-subjects design, φij is automatically condition-specific.
Putting these pieces together, the temporal dynamics of performance yij across a period of wakefulness for a given individual i and given task j can be modeled as follows:
(5) |
where Gj is a task-specific scaling factor for the circadian process; and εij is error variance assumed to be normally distributed over subjects and time with zero mean and task-specific variance σj2. For the k = 3 different neurobehavioral assays considered in our study, this regression equation has k + 1 = 4 subject-specific parameters, namely φij (j = 1,…, k) and Hi. These can be modeled by independent, normally distributed random effects with parameter-specific mean Φj and zero and parameter-specific variance ωj2 and ϖ2, respectively. (Note that the normal distribution of Hi makes the overall scaling of the homeostatic process log-normally distributed over subjects.)
To allow for direct comparisons between neurobehavioral assays within the same regression model, we normalized the data yij for each of the assays using the task-specific grand mean and standard deviation. We then fitted the model to the overall data set of the three assays combined using non-linear mixed-effects regression [47] implemented in SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Contrasts were included to compare parameter estimates among the assays, with the comparison between the mean phase angles Φj being of primary interest.
3. Results
Following the three days of simulated night shift, compared to simulated day shift, there was a modest delay of 87 min (±45 min SE; t11 = 1.92, p = 0.082) in the circadian process as estimated by the timing of the DLMO, which occurred at 23:01 (±95 min SD) in the night shift condition versus 21:34 (±68 min SD) in the day shift condition. Our DLMO data are consistent with natural variability in circadian phase [59,60] and prior observations of limited circadian phase shifting in night shift protocols under dim light conditions [61]. Because the homeostatic process is a function of time awake and not time of day (and vice versa for the circadian process), it follows that the simulated night shift condition produced an average shift of 10.6 h in the alignment of the homeostatic process relative to the circadian process during the subsequent 24 h CR protocol. This considerable dispersion confirmed the effectiveness of the study protocol and helped to optimize the statistical analysis and parameter estimation of the circadian process. To verify that the statistical model of Equation (5) was well-parameterized for investigating endogenous circadian rhythmicity, we checked the parameter correlation matrix and made sure all estimated parameters (other than the intrinsically correlated homeostatic parameters Bj and Kj) had low pairwise correlations. They were between −0.37 and 0.36, confirming excellent parameter estimability.
Figure 3 shows the temporal profiles of the three neurobehavioral outcomes as observed during the 24 h CR protocol following the simulated day and night shift protocols (left and right panels, respectively), plotted against circadian time (i.e., relative to DLMO). The statistical model curves in the day shift condition (left panels) show the expected profile of low neurobehavioral impairment during the first ~16 h of wakefulness constituting the biological day, followed by a steady increase of impairment into the biological night [3]. Likewise, the statistical model curves in the night shift condition (right panels) show the expected profile of steadily increasing impairment through the biological night and into the subsequent biological day, followed by a partial (circadian-mediated) rebound toward the end of the biological day [4].
Figure 4 shows the endogenous circadian rhythm contribution to the temporal profiles of each of the three neurobehavioral outcomes during the 24 h CR protocol following the simulated day and night shift conditions, as estimated using Equation (4) as embedded in the statistical model of Equation (5). The magnitude of the contribution of the circadian process, to be interpreted relative to the homeostatic process, was smaller for the DSST than for the KSS and PVT (F2,9 = 5.61, p = 0.026). However, even though the statistical model was fit with task-specific and condition-specific parameters and also accounted for inter-individual differences, the timing of the effect of the circadian process on neurobehavioral functioning, relative to subject-specific DLMO, was consistent between the day and night shift conditions for all three neurobehavioral functioning outcomes. This was corroborated by comparisons of the subject-specific φij estimates, i.e., the empirical Bayes estimates (EBEs) for the task-specific random effects on the circadian phase angles relative to subject-specific DLMO in the statistical model of Equation (5). There were no significant differences between conditions in the EBEs for the KSS (F1,11 = 0.01, p = 0.90), DSST (F1,11 = 0.83, p = 0.38), and PVT (F1,11 = 0.37, p = 0.56), indicating no systematic shifting between conditions in the timing of the circadian contribution, relative to DLMO, to neurobehavioral functioning.
As such, the peak times of the endogenous circadian contribution to neurobehavioral functioning varied little between the neurobehavioral outcomes, and there was substantial overlap among the 95% confidence intervals (Figure 4). Notably, there was no significant effect of task on the timing of the circadian peaks relative to DLMO (F2,9 = 1.45, p = 0.28).
4. Discussion
This study provides proof of principle for our approach to investigating whether the circadian rhythmicity of neurobehavioral functioning is task-dependent, addressing limitations of earlier research on this topic. In contrast with previous reports that circadian rhythms peak in the morning or afternoon depending on the task at hand [21,26,30], we found no evidence of a task-based dissociation in optimal circadian timing for three functionally distinct neurobehavioral outcomes. During a 24 h CR protocol, preceded by either a three-day simulated day shift schedule or a three-day simulated night shift schedule, circadian peak times clustered approximately 16–20 h after DLMO—consistent with the height of endogenous circadian drive for alert wakefulness during the “wake maintenance zone” [62] in the late afternoon [63,64,65]. Differences in circadian peak times between the neurobehavioral functions we measured were within the margin of error and no greater than our 2 h sampling interval (Figure 4). Thus, we found no evidence that the circadian timing for optimal neurobehavioral functioning is inherently task-dependent.
By randomizing subjects to either a day shift condition with normal alignment of the homeostatic and circadian processes, or a night shift condition with nearly opposite (i.e., more than 10.5 h shifted) alignment of the homeostatic and circadian processes, we created favorable empirical conditions [66,67] for dissociating the contributions of these two biological processes to neurobehavioral functioning as measured in the strictly controlled setting of the subsequent CR protocol. By also accounting for inter-individual differences in the statistical analysis [47], we further optimized our ability to isolate the circadian component of the temporal dynamics of neurobehavioral functioning [7]. Additionally, by selecting well-characterized [40,44,46] and functionally distinct [36,40] neurobehavioral assays, we avoided a range of measurement confounds encountered in the literature (e.g., practice effects, strategy changes, and dynamic speed/accuracy trade-offs) and enhanced our ability to detect task-dependent differences in circadian timing if such differences were present. Thus, our failure to detect differences in the circadian peaks of the three neurobehavioral assays is not an artifact of insufficient statistical power from inadequate sample size—although arguably a larger sample would have enhanced our ability to demonstrate even smaller differences—but rather indicates that with regard to timing, these assays were not affected differentially by endogenous circadian rhythmicity.
A limitation of our study is that the sample consisted of healthy young adults only and was predominately male. Age and sex differences in circadian rhythms have been reported [68,69], and whether those might interact with inter-task differences remains unknown. Importantly, another limitation is the small number of different assays we deployed. The imperative of repeated testing and the need for rest breaks to avoid cumulative time-on-task effects, as well as time windows needed for blood sampling and hourly snacks during the CR protocol, meant that only three assays could be included in the test battery.
Further research into task-dependence of the endogenous circadian impact on performance should incorporate other neurobehavioral functions, but selecting the right assays is not a straightforward matter. The so-called “task impurity problem” implies that any neurobehavioral task involves a number of interrelated cognitive processes that must be dissociable and distinguished to be able to interpret which, if any, of these processes are differentially affected by circadian rhythmicity [40,70]. Based on dissociations demonstrated in recent sleep deprivation studies, neurobehavioral (or cognitive) functions of particular interest for future studies may include attentional control [71], memory binding [72] and maintenance [73], and emotion regulation [74]. Referring to the sleep deprivation literature is relevant here, because our results indicate that the differences we observed in the overall temporal profiles of the KSS, DSST, and PVT with the circadian and homeostatic processes still intertwined (Figure 3) are attributable at least in part to differences between neurobehavioral functions in the influence of time awake through the homeostatic process. This more holistic perspective is consistent with a theoretical model in which the circadian process exerts global, top-down control over neurobehavioral functioning (placing peak circadian functioning in the “wake maintenance zone” as we have found); whereas the homeostatic process exerts local, bottom-up control based on the neuronal pathways involved, in a use-dependent manner that gives rise to differential degrees of impairment [38,42]. Our results add to a growing body of evidence consistent with that theoretical model [75,76,77].
Our findings have practical implications for shift-workers and others involved in around-the-clock operations [78,79,80]. If the circadian timing of neurobehavioral functioning were inherently task-dependent, this would present a formidable challenge in the prediction, assessment, and management of workplace risks for errors, incidents, and accidents [81,82]. However, while there are substantial inter-individual differences in the magnitude of neurobehavioral impairment to contend with in such settings [83], our results provide some reassurance that circadian peaks and troughs in subjective assessments of sleepiness are temporally aligned with those in objective performance deficits. Thus, biologically driven, circadian changes in neurobehavioral functioning should be predictable regardless of the tasks people perform—consistent with how it is implemented in current biomathematical models predicting neurobehavioral performance [12]. As such, there does not appear to be a fundamental need to adjust biomathematical model predictions based on what activities a person may be engaged in.
5. Conclusions
If there are inherent differences between tasks in the timing of the endogenous circadian peak, our carefully conducted study with comprehensive disentanglement of the circadian and homeostatic processes as well as the inter-individual differences therein would have been expected to manifestly expose them—but for the three distinct neurobehavioral tasks we investigated we found peak circadian performance clustering in the late biological afternoon (in the “wake maintenance zone”) during the CR protocol after both simulated day and night shift schedules. Our findings are in line with reports from previous CR studies [28,29] where, as Monk and colleagues observed: “between-task heterogeneity in circadian performance rhythms appeared to be absent when the sleep/wake cycle was suspended” [29] (p. 9). However, results from a forced desynchrony study [28], and from our systematic dissociation of the endogenous circadian process from the homeostatic process via juxtaposition of prior exposure to simulated day versus night shift conditions, suggest that between-task heterogeneity in circadian performance rhythms may be more broadly absent; and that any apparent circadian timing differences in other studies may actually be attributable to differential homeostatic effects and/or measurement confounds. From the available evidence, therefore, we parsimoniously conclude that circadian rhythmicity in neurobehavioral functioning is governed by a single endogenous circadian driver emanating from the central biological clock [84]. Further research with larger and more diverse samples and a wider spectrum of distinct neurobehavioral assays is needed to explore the limits of generalizability of this conclusion.
Acknowledgments
We thank the staff of the Sleep and Performance Research Center at Washington State University for helping to conduct this experiment.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, S.G. and H.P.A.V.D.; Funding Acquisition, S.G. and H.P.A.V.D.; Methodology, S.G. and H.P.A.V.D.; Data Reduction and Presentation, R.A.M., A.N.H. and H.P.A.V.D.; Formal Analysis, R.A.M., A.N.H. and H.P.A.V.D.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, R.A.M., A.N.H. and H.P.A.V.D.; Writing—Review and Editing, all authors; Supervision, K.A.H. and H.P.A.V.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research was funded by CDMRP, grant numbers W81XWH-16-1-0319 and W81XWH-20-1-0442. This research was also funded by the Sleep and Performance Research Center and the College of Pharmacy and Pharmaceutical Sciences at Washington State University.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The Institutional Review Board of Washington State University approved the study under protocol number 14248 on 26 February 2015. The study was conducted in compliance with good clinical practice and applicable laws and regulations.
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement
Upon reasonable request from H.P.A.V.D. the data can be shared with researchers.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in: the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Daan S., Beersma D.G., Borbély A.A. Timing of human sleep: Recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker. Am. J. Physiol. 1984;246:R161–R183. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1984.246.2.R161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Dijk D.J., Duffy J.F., Czeisler C.A. Circadian and sleep/wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive performance. J. Sleep Res. 1992;1:112–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1992.tb00021.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dijk D.J., Czeisler C.A. Paradoxical timing of the circadian rhythm of sleep propensity serves to consolidate sleep and wakefulness in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 1994;166:62–68. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(94)90841-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Van Dongen H.P.A., Dinges D.F. Sleep, circadian rhythms, and psychomotor vigilance. Clin. Sports Med. 2005;24:237–249. doi: 10.1016/j.csm.2004.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wilson M., Permito R., English A., Albritton S., Coogle C., Van Dongen H.P.A. Performance and sleepiness in nurses working 12-h day shifts or night shifts in a community hospital. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2019;126:43–46. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.09.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gurubhagavatula I., Barger L.K., Barnes C.M., Basner M., Boivin D.B., Dawson D., Drake C.L., Flynn-Evans E.E., Mysliwiec V., Patterson P.D., et al. Guiding principles for determining work shift duration and addressing the effects of work shift duration on performance, safety, and health. Sleep. 2021;15:zsab161. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsab161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Van Dongen H.P.A., Bender A.M., Dinges D.F. Systematic individual differences in sleep homeostatic and circadian rhythm contributions to neurobehavioral impairment during sleep deprivation. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2012;45:11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Rusterholz T., Tarokh L., Van Dongen H.P.A., Achermann P. Interindividual differences in the dynamics of the homeostatic process are trait-like and distinct for sleep versus wakefulness. J. Sleep Res. 2017;26:171–178. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Achermann P., Borbély A.A. Simulation of daytime vigilance by the additive interaction of a homeostatic and a circadian process. Biol. Cybern. 1994;71:115–121. doi: 10.1007/BF00197314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Van Dongen H.P.A., Dinges D.F. Investigating the interaction between the homeostatic and circadian processes of sleep-wake regulation for the prediction of waking neurobehavioral performance. J. Sleep Res. 2003;12:181–187. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00357.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mallis M.M., Mejdal S., Nguyen T.T., Dinges D.F. Summary of the key features of seven biomathematical models of human fatigue and performance. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2004;75:A4–A14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Riedy S.M., Hursh S.R., Dawson D., Balkin T.J., Van Dongen H.P.A. Sleep, sleepiness, and performance prediction modeling. In: Kryger M., Roth T., Goldstein C.A., Dement W.C., editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 7th ed. Elsevier; Philadelphia, PA, USA: 2022. pp. 780–787. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Knock S.A., Magee M., Stone J.E., Ganesan S., Mulhall M.D., Lockley S.W., Howard M.E., Rajaratnam S.M.W., Sletten T.L., Postnova S. Prediction of shiftworker alertness, sleep, and circadian phase using a model of arousal dynamics constrained by shift schedules and light exposure. Sleep. 2021;44:zsab146. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsab146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.McCauley M.E., McCauley P., Riedy S.M., Banks S., Ecker A.J., Kalachev L.V., Rangan S., Dinges D.F., Van Dongen H.P.A. Fatigue risk management based on self-reported fatigue: Expanding a biomathematical model of fatigue-related performance deficits to also predict subjective sleepiness. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2021;79:94–106. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2021.04.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rangan S., Riedy S.M., Bassett R., Klinck Z.A., Hagerty P., Schek E., Zhang Y., Hursh S.R., Van Dongen H.P.A. Predictive and proactive fatigue risk management approaches in commercial aviation. Chronobiol. Int. 2020;37:1479–1482. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2020.1803902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Vital-Lopez F.G., Doty T.J., Reifman J. Optimal sleep and work schedules to maximize alertness. Sleep. 2021;44:zsab144. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsab144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hockey G.R.J., Colquhoun W.P. Diurnal variation in human performance: A review. In: Colquhoun W.P., editor. Aspects of Human Efficiency: Diurnal Rhythm and Loss of Sleep. English Universities Press; London, UK: 1972. pp. 1–23. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kirkcaldy B.D. Performance and circadian rhythms. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 1984;52:375–379. doi: 10.1007/BF00943366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Folkard S., Hume K., Minors D., Waterhouse J.M., Watson F.L. Independence of the circadian rhythm in alertness from the sleep/wake cycle. Nature. 1985;313:678–679. doi: 10.1038/313678a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Monk T.H., Fookson J.E., Moline M.L., Pollak C.P. Diurnal variation in mood and performance in a time-isolated environment. Chronobiol. Int. 1985;2:185–193. doi: 10.3109/07420528509055558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Monk T.H., Leng V.C. Interactions between inter-individual and inter-task differences in the diurnal variation of human performance. Chronobiol. Int. 1986;3:171–177. doi: 10.3109/07420528609066364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Folkard S. Diurnal variation in logical reasoning. Br. J. Psychol. 1975;66:1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01433.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Folkard S., Monk T.H. Circadian rhythms in human memory. Br. J. Psychol. 1980;71:295–307. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1980.tb01746.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Colquhoun W.P. Rhythms in performance. In: Aschoff J., editor. Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology. Volume 4. Plenum Press; New York, NY, USA: 1981. pp. 333–348. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Folkard S., Wever R.A., Wildgruber C.M. Multi-oscillatory control of circadian rhythms in human performance. Nature. 1983;305:223–226. doi: 10.1038/305223a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Monk T.H., Weitzman E.D., Fookson J.E., Moline M.L., Kronauer R.E., Gander P.H. Task variables determine which biological clock controls circadian rhythms in human performance. Nature. 1983;304:543–545. doi: 10.1038/304543a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Muto V., Jasper M., Meyer C., Kussé C., Chellappa S.L., Degueldre C., Balteau E., Shaffii-Le Bourdiec A., Luxen A., Middleton B., et al. Local modulation of human brain responses by circadian rhythmicity and sleep debt. Science. 2016;353:687–690. doi: 10.1126/science.aad2993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Johnson M.P., Duffy J.F., Dijk D.J., Ronda J.M., Dyal C.M., Czeisler C.A. Short-term memory, alertness and performance: A reappraisal of their relationship to body temperature. J. Sleep Res. 1992;1:24–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1992.tb00004.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Monk T.H., Buysse D.J., Reynolds C.F., III, Berga S.L., Jarrett D.B., Begley A.E., Kupfer D.J. Circadian rhythms in human performance and mood under constant conditions. J. Sleep Res. 1997;6:9–18. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1997.00023.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Owens D.S., Macdonald I., Tucker P., Sytnik N., Minors D., Waterhouse J., Totterdell P., Folkard S. Diurnal trends in mood and performance do not all parallel alertness. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health. 1998;24((Suppl. 3)):109–114. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Chellappa S.L., Morris C.J., Scheer F.A.J.L. Daily circadian misalignment impairs human cognitive performance task-dependently. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:3041. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20707-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.McMahon W.R., Ftouni S., Drummond S.P.A., Maruff P., Lockley S.W., Rajaratnam S.M.W., Anderson C. The wake maintenance zone shows task dependent changes in cognitive function following one night without sleep. Sleep. 2018;41:zsy148. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Collet J., Ftouni S., Clough M., Cain S.W., Fielding J., Anderson C. Differential impact of sleep deprivation and circadian timing on reflexive versus inhibitory control of attention. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:7270. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-63144-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Skeiky L., Hudson A.N., Van Dongen H.P.A. Circadian rhythms in sleepiness, alertness, and performance. In: Kryger M., Roth T., Goldstein C.A., Dement W.C., editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 7th ed. Elsevier; Philadelphia, PA, USA: 2022. pp. 407–414. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bendat J.S., Piersol A.G. Random Data: Analysis and Measurement Procedures. 4th ed. John Wiley & Sons; New York, NY, USA: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Van Dongen H.P.A., Baynard M.D., Maislin G., Dinges D.F. Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: Evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep. 2004;27:423–433. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.3.423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Van Dongen H.P.A., Caldwell J.A., Caldwell J.L. Investigating systematic individual differences in sleep-deprived performance on a high-fidelity flight simulator. Behav. Res. Meth. 2006;38:333–343. doi: 10.3758/BF03192785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Van Dongen H.P.A., Belenky G., Krueger J.M. A local, bottom-up perspective on sleep deprivation and neurobehavioral performance. Curr. Top. Med. Chem. 2011;11:2414–2422. doi: 10.2174/156802611797470286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Zhou X., Ferguson S.A., Matthews R.W., Sargent C., Darwent D., Kennaway D.J., Roach G.D. Interindividual differences in neurobehavioral performance in response to increasing homeostatic sleep pressure. Chronobiol. Int. 2010;27:922–933. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2010.488958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Honn K.A., Halverson T., Jackson M.L., Krusmark M., Chavali V.P., Gunzelmann G., Van Dongen H.P.A. New insights into the cognitive effects of sleep deprivation by decomposition of a cognitive throughput task. Sleep. 2020;43:zsz319. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Folkard S., Monk T.H. Time of day and processing strategy in free recall. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1979;31:461–475. doi: 10.1080/14640747908400739. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hudson A.N., Hansen D.A., Hinson J.M., Whitney P., Layton M.E., DePriest D.M., Van Dongen H.P.A., Honn K.A. Speed/accuracy trade-off in the effects of acute total sleep deprivation on a sustained attention and response inhibition task. Chronobiol. Int. 2020;37:1441–1444. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2020.1811718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Duffy J.F., Dijk D.J. Getting through to circadian oscillators: Why use constant routines? J. Biol. Rhythms. 2002;17:4–13. doi: 10.1177/074873002129002294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lim J., Dinges D.F. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008;1129:305–322. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Van Dongen H.P.A., Maislin G., Mullington J.M., Dinges D.F. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: Dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26:117–126. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.2.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Åkerstedt T., Anund A., Axelsson J., Kecklund G. Subjective sleepiness is a sensitive indicator of insufficient sleep and impaired waking function. J. Sleep Res. 2014;23:240–252. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Olofsen E., Dinges D.F., Van Dongen H.P.A. Nonlinear mixed-effects modeling: Individualization and prediction. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2004;75:A134–A140. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Smith C.S., Reilly C., Midkiff K. Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. J. Appl. Psychol. 1989;74:728–738. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.74.5.728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Iber C., Ancoli-Israel S., Chesson A.L., Jr., Quan S.F. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology, and Technical Specifications. 1st ed. American Academy of Sleep Medicine; Westchester, IL, USA: 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Skornyakov E., Gaddameedhi S., Paech G.M., Sparrow A.R., Satterfield B.C., Shattuck N.L., Layton M.E., Karatsoreos I., Van Dongen H.P.A. Cardiac autonomic activity during simulated shift work. Ind. Health. 2019;57:118–132. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.2018-0044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Skene D., Skornyakov E., Chowdhury N., Gajula R.P., Middleton B., Satterfield B.C., Porter K.I., Van Dongen H.P.A., Gaddameedhi S. Separation of circadian- and behavior-driven metabolite rhythms in humans provides a window on peripheral oscillators and metabolism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2018;115:7825–7830. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1801183115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Åkerstedt T., Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int. J. Neurosci. 1990;52:29–37. doi: 10.3109/00207459008994241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised. Psychological Corp.; New York, NY, USA: 1981. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Dinges D.F., Powell J.W. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 1985;17:652–655. doi: 10.3758/BF03200977. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Dorrian J., Rogers N.L., Dinges D.F. Psychomotor vigilance performance: Neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleep loss. In: Kushida C.A., editor. Sleep Deprivation: Clinical Issues, Pharmacology, and Sleep Loss Effects. Marcel Dekker, Inc.; New York, NY, USA: 2005. pp. 39–70. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Chavali V.P., Riedy S.M., Van Dongen H.P.A. Signal-to-noise ratio in PVT performance as a cognitive measure of the effect of sleep deprivation on the fidelity of information processing. Sleep. 2017;40:zsx016. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Lundholm K.R., Honn K.A., Skeiky L., Muck R.A., Van Dongen H.P.A. Trait interindividual differences in the magnitude of subjective sleepiness from sleep inertia. Clocks Sleep. 2021;3:298–311. doi: 10.3390/clockssleep3020019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Borbély A.A., Achermann P. Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation. J. Biol. Rhythm. 1999;14:557–568. doi: 10.1177/074873099129000894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kerkhof G.A., Van Dongen H.P.A. Morning-type and evening-type individuals differ in the phase position of their endogenous circadian oscillator. Neurosci. Lett. 1996;218:153–156. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(96)13140-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Kantermann T., Eastman C.I. Circadian phase, circadian period and chronotype are reproducible over months. Chronobiol. Int. 2018;35:280–288. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2017.1400979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Eastman C.I., Liu L., Fogg L.F. Circadian rhythm adaptation to simulated night shift work: Effect of nocturnal bright-light duration. Sleep. 1995;18:399–407. doi: 10.1093/sleep/18.6.399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Strogatz S.H., Kronauer R.E., Czeisler C.A. Circadian pacemaker interferes with sleep onset at specific times each day: Role in insomnia. Am. J. Physiol. 1987;253:R172–R178. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1987.253.1.R172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lavie P. Ultrashort sleep-waking schedule. III. ‘Gates’ and ‘forbidden zones’ for sleep. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1986;63:414–425. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(86)90123-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Shekleton J.A., Rajaratnam S.M., Gooley J.J., Van Reen E., Czeisler C.A., Lockley S.W. Improved neurobehavioral performance during the wake maintenance zone. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2013;9:353–362. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.2588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.De Zeeuw J., Wisniewski S., Papakonstantinou A., Bes F., Wahnschaffe A., Zaleska M., Kunz D., Münch M. The alerting effect of the wake maintenance zone during 40 hours of sleep deprivation. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:11012. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29380-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Mackie J.L. Causes and conditions. Am. Philos. Q. 1965;2:245–264. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Rubin D.B. Estimating causal effects of treatments in randomized and nonrandomized studies. J. Educ. Psychol. 1974;66:688–701. doi: 10.1037/h0037350. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Santhi N., Lazar A.S., McCabe P.J., Lo J.C., Groeger J.A., Dijk D.J. Sex differences in the circadian regulation of sleep and waking cognition in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2016;113:E2730–E2739. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1521637113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Dijk D.J., Landolt H.P. Sleep physiology, circadian rhythms, waking performance and the development of sleep-wake therapeutics. Handb. Exp. Pharmacol. 2019;253:441–481. doi: 10.1007/164_2019_243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Whitney P., Hinson J.M. Measurement of cognition in studies of sleep deprivation. Prog. Brain Res. 2010;185:37–48. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00003-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Whitney P., Hinson J.M., Satterfield B.C., Grant D.A., Honn K.A., Van Dongen H.P.A. Sleep deprivation diminishes attentional control effectiveness and impairs flexible adaptation to changing conditions. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:16020. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16165-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kurinec C.A., Whitney P., Hinson J.M., Hansen D.A., Van Dongen H.P.A. Sleep deprivation impairs binding of information with its context. Sleep. 2021;44:zsab113. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsab113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Stepan M.E., Fenn K.M., Altmann E.M. Effects of sleep deprivation on procedural errors. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2019;148:1828–1833. doi: 10.1037/xge0000495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Stenson A.R., Kurinec C.A., Hinson J.M., Whitney P., Van Dongen H.P.A. Total sleep deprivation reduces top-down regulation of emotion without altering bottom-up affective processing. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0256983. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Krueger J.M., Huang Y.H., Rector D.M., Buysse D.J. Sleep: A synchrony of cell activity-driven small network states. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2013;38:2199–2209. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Nir Y., Andrillon T., Marmelshtein A., Suthana N., Cirelli C., Tononi G., Fried I. Selective neuronal lapses precede human cognitive lapses following sleep deprivation. Nat. Med. 2017;23:1474–1480. doi: 10.1038/nm.4433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Thomas C.W., Guillaumin M.C., McKillop L.E., Achermann P., Vyazovskiy V.V. Global sleep homeostasis reflects temporally and spatially integrated local cortical neuronal activity. eLife. 2020;9:e54148. doi: 10.7554/eLife.54148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Smith M.R., Eastman C.I. Shift work: Health, performance and safety problems, traditional countermeasures, and innovative management strategies to reduce circadian misalignment. Nat. Sci. Sleep. 2012;4:111–132. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S10372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Ganesan S., Magee M., Stone J.E., Mulhall M.D., Collins A., Howard M.E., Lockley S.W., Rajaratnam S.M.W., Sletten T.L. The impact of shift work on sleep, alertness and performance in healthcare workers. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:4635. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-40914-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Rosa D.E., Marot L.P., de Mello M.T., Marqueze E.C., Narciso F.V., de Araújo L.B., Crispim C.A. Association between chronotype and psychomotor performance of rotating shift workers. Sci. Rep. 2021;11:6919. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-86299-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Raslear T.G., Hursh S.R., Van Dongen H.P.A. Predicting cognitive impairment and accident risk. Prog. Brain Res. 2011;190:155–167. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53817-8.00010-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Wong I.S., Popkin S., Folkard S. Working Time Society consensus statements: A multi-level approach to managing occupational sleep-related fatigue. Ind. Health. 2019;57:228–244. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.SW-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Van Dongen H.P.A., Belenky G. Individual differences in vulnerability to sleep loss in the work environment. Ind. Health. 2009;47:518–526. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.47.518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Moore R.Y. The suprachiasmatic nucleus and the circadian timing system. Prog. Mol. Biol. Transl. Sci. 2013;119:1–28. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-396971-2.00001-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Upon reasonable request from H.P.A.V.D. the data can be shared with researchers.