Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Jan 21.
Published in final edited form as: Eat Behav. 2022 Jan 21;44:101596. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2022.101596

Typology of eating episodes in children and adolescents with overweight/obesity

Andrea B Goldschmidt a, Tyler B Mason b, Kathryn E Smith c, Amy H Egbert a, E Scott G Engel d,e, Alissa Haedt-Matt f
PMCID: PMC8885845  NIHMSID: NIHMS1774788  PMID: 35091193

Abstract

Background:

Research suggests that youth with overweight/obesity can be subtyped according to disinhibited eating tendencies. No research has attempted to subtype classes of eating episodes along sensational, psychological, and hedonic dimensions.

Methods:

Youth (N=39; 55% female) aged 8–14y with overweight/obesity completed a 2-week ecological momentary assessment protocol in which they reported on all eating episodes and their sensational, environmental, affective, and interpersonal contexts. Latent profile analysis (LPA) was used to classify episodes based on loss of control (LOC) while eating, self-reported overeating, food palatability, hunger, and cravings. Classes were compared on affective, interpersonal, appearance-related, and environmental correlates using Wald chi-square tests.

Results:

LPA identified three classes of eating episodes involving high levels of LOC/self-reported overeating (“binge-like” class), low levels of hunger (“eating-in-the-absence-of-hunger” class), and high hunger and cravings (“appetitive eating” class). Binge-like eating was associated with the highest levels of body dissatisfaction, interpersonal distress, and positive affect, and was most likely to occur in the after-school hours.

Conclusions:

Binge-like eating tends to occur in a psychological context relative to other types of eating episodes in youth with overweight/obesity. Future research should explore whether certain classes of eating episodes are associated with distinct weight trajectories over time.

Keywords: Ecological momentary assessment, loss of control, overeating, hedonic, eating in the absence of hunger, obesity

1. Introduction

Pediatric overweight/obesity affect >30% of youth [1] and lead to physical and psychosocial disability [2, 3], with evidence of racial/ethnic disparities in prevalence and comorbidities [4]. Although overweight/obesity are multifactorial conditions, excess energy intake is a major determinant of weight status/regulation across the lifespan [5]. Yet, multiple intra- and extra-personal factors promote excess energy intake at a given eating episode or throughout a day, week, month, and beyond. These factors are rarely studied using momentary, naturalistic designs in youth [6], particularly those from underrepresented backgrounds, and overlap among factors is under-explored. Understanding contexts in which excess energy ingestion is likely could assist in developing novel, targeted interventions to improve eating and weight regulation in youth.

In the most basic sense, eating behavior is driven by homeostatic energy needs, mediated by metabolic (e.g., glucose depletion), hormonal (e.g., ghrelin/leptin signaling) and neural pathways [e.g., hypothalamic hunger signals; 7]. Yet, multiple environmental and psychological factors determine energy intake [5] and may drive eating in the absence of hunger (EAH, traditionally conceptualized via laboratory assessment of energy ingestion following eating to satiation [8], and more recently via momentary, naturalistic designs [9, 10]), binge and loss of control (LOC) eating [characterized by a lack of control over what/how much one is eating, with or without ingestion of a large amount of food, i.e., overeating; 11], and hedonic eating (eating driven by motivational or reinforcing properties of food, such as cravings [12] and/or perceived palatability of food [13]). These factors may overlap to some extent [14, 15], may occur independently or interactively in relation to eating episodes parameters (e.g., timing/duration, content), and in some cases differ across racial/ethnic dimensions [e.g., 16]. Little research has characterized momentary dimensions of eating episodes, and their confluence and correlates, in youth with overweight/obesity [17], who are vulnerable to eating behavior that is not determined solely by energy deficit needs [11].

In addition to characterizing real-time, naturalistic eating episodes, a clearer understanding of the environmental and psychological context in which these episodes occur could help identify targets and optimal timing of momentary interventions. Hunger and cravings in adults tends to coincide with lunch and dinner [18], while overeating and LOC eating are more likely to occur during evening hours in both adults and children [1921]. Furthermore, overeating is more likely to co-occur with environmental and social cues, such as being away from home [22] and being around others [23, 24], particularly for Black youth [25], while LOC eating tends to occur in secret/while alone [21]. Although adult research supports an association between negative affect and LOC eating [26], the pediatric literature is inconsistent [21, 24, 27, 28]. However, body dissatisfaction and interpersonal distress may contribute to LOC eating or overeating in predominantly Black pediatric samples [24, 28].

This study utilized latent profile analysis (LPA) to identify naturalistically occurring eating episode subtypes, and assessed their environmental and psychosocial features, among racially and ethnically diverse youth with overweight/obesity. LPA is an atheoretical classification approach which is typically used to classify subtypes of individuals, but has been used to classify subtypes of eating episodes in other populations with aberrant eating [29]. Based on the previous literature [5], we aimed to characterize eating episodes along sensational (i.e., hunger), appetitive (i.e., food palatability, cravings), and psychological dimensions (i.e., LOC, overeating). However, because LPA is data driven, we had no specific hypotheses about the types of eating episodes that would be classified or their unique combinations of features. In terms of correlates, we expected that episodes involving higher levels of LOC and/or overeating (independently or interactively) would be associated with momentary psychological correlates, while those involving higher levels of hunger, cravings, and/or food palatability (independently or interactively) would be associated with environmental features. However, we also anticipated that unique combinations of eating-related indicators (e.g., high LOC accompanied by low cravings) could demonstrate unexpected associations with psychosocial and environmental correlates. Therefore, despite being informed by a large body of research on independent determinants of eating behavior [5], this study was largely exploratory in its novel approach to understanding naturalistic eating episodes and their contextual correlates.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedures

Participants between 8–14y with overweight/obesity [body mass index (BMI)≥85th age- and sex-adjusted percentile; 30] were recruited from The University of Chicago Medicine and Illinois Institute of Technology, both of which provided ethical approval. Recruitment involved community advertisements, pediatrician referrals, and phone logs from previous studies where families agreed to be re-contacted [31]. Exclusion criteria included: medical conditions or medications influencing weight or appetite; eating disorder other than binge eating disorder (BED); inability to read/understand English fluently; and concurrent participation in obesity treatment. Caregivers of interested individuals were phone screened to assess basic inclusion/exclusion criteria, and eligible participants were invited with caregivers to attend a baseline study visit. A total of 94 families contacted the research team to learn about the study, of whom 11 were uninterested in being screened, and 83 were screened via phone. Of these 83 families, 44 presented for a baseline evaluation. The remaining 39 either lost contact or failed to attend the evaluation (n=13; 33.3%), were uninterested/unavailable (n=11; 28.2%), did not meet inclusion criteria due to age, weight status, or medical/psychiatric conditions (n=4; 10.3%), or were not evaluated in person for unknown reasons (n=11; 28.2%). Of the 44 families completing a baseline evaluation, 39 met inclusion criteria and provided adequate EMA data (e.g., at least 1 week of EMA recording) to be included in the current analyses. The 44 excluded youth did not differ from the 39 who were included on gender (p=.98) or age (p=.87). After providing written informed assent/consent, participants had height and weight measured and completed assesments of eating behavior and psychosocial functioning. Participants and caregivers were then trained to complete EMA recordings.

The EMA protocol was administered through the ReTAINE platform (REal Time Assessment In the Natural Environment) using a secure link delivered to children’s mobile device which provided access to surveys on ReTAINE’s webpage. Surveys were meant to be completed independently via mobile device, but some children may have had assistance from caregivers, or completed surveys on a non-study device (e.g., desktop computer). All items were appropriate for a 3rd grade reading level. Participants were instructed to complete recordings after any eating episodes (event-contingent); before bedtime (interval-contingent); and at 3–5 semi-random times throughout the day [signal-contingent; 32]. Signaled prompts occurred every 2–3 hours between 8:00am-9:00pm on weekends, and 7:00–8:00am, 3:00–4:00pm, and 6:00–7:00pm on weekdays to avoid coinciding with school. Recordings were automatically time-stamped. During all recordings, participants were instructed to report on current affect, interpersonal events, weight-/shape-related concerns, and characteristics of any recent eating episode that had not been reported during prior recordings. This combination of signal-, event-, and interval-contingent recordings has been successfully implemented in prior EMA studies of youth with overweight [27, 28].

Children were required to complete a 1-day trial with ≥70% compliance to initiate the 14-day EMA study period; these data were not used in statistical analyses to reduce concerns about reactivity. Research staff contacted participants by phone after the first day of EMA recording, and every 2–3 days thereafter, to discuss compliance rates and address questions or concerns regarding assessment procedures.

Upon completion of the daily assessment phase, participants returned to the original research institution to return loaner Smartphones (if applicable); complete a brief, final assessment; and receive a final incentive payment. Participants received $50 for the initial evaluation; $50 for completing the 2-week protocol; and $1 for each response for up to $50 total (50 semi-random signals over the 2-week protocol).

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Baseline measures.

Height and weight were measured in light indoor clothing by trained staff via stadiometer and calibrated digital scale, respectively. Children’s standardized body mass index (kg/m2; z-BMI) was calculated using Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) growth charts [30]. Demographic data included children’s age, gender, race/ethnicity, current medications, and medical problems.

2.2.2. Eating episode indicators.

At eating episode recordings, participants reported on several features of the episode. For this small exploratory study (the first of its kind in a heterogeneous sample of youth with overweight/obesity), we adapted existing, validated measures [Child Eating Disorder Examination (Child EDE); 33] drawn from previous EMA studies of youth [28] to develop our momentary items assessing LOC and overeating. Ratings for self-reported overeating (“To what extent do you feel that you overate?”) and LOC (“While you were eating, did you feel… a sense of loss of control? … that you could not stop eating once you had started? … like you could not resist eating? … like a car without brakes, you just kept eating and eating?”) were made on a 1- to 5-point Likert-type scale (1=“no, not at all,” and 5=“yes, extremely”). The four LOC items were averaged to form a composite score based on high internal consistency (α=.91). Cravings and hunger levels (“Please rate how much you agree with the following statement: I am craving food” and “I am hungry,” respectively) both were rated on a 1- to 5-point Likert-type scale (1=“disagree strongly,” and 5=“agree strongly”), while food palatability was rated on a 10-point scale (“On a scale of 1 [terrible] to 10 [the best thing you have ever tasted] how good did the food you ate taste?”). Hunger and craving ratings were lagged to represent pre-episode scores (i.e., ratings were drawn from a separate EMA survey occuring closest in time prior to that in which the eating episode was reported).

2.2.3. Eating episode validators.

Across all EMA signal types, if participants reported that an eating episode had occurred since the last prompt, they then reported on the type of episode they experienced (meal, snack, or binge). Environmental variables included location (home, car, school, cafeteria, restaurant, outside, friend’s house, other), and presence of dining companions (“Did you eat alone/with other people?”). Time of day when eating occurred included morning (0000h–1159h), afternoon (1200h–1659h), and evening (1700h–2359h). The PANAS [34] was used to measure mood state. The PANAS is a brief, reliable, and valid measure [35] that has been used in several EMA studies [36, 37], including studies involving children [27, 28]. Each affect item (e.g., distressed, excited) was rated on a 5-point scale (“1”=“Not at all”; “5”=“Extremely”) and summed to form composite negative affect and positive affect scales (range=0–50). Shape/weight concerns were measured using five items adapted from the Child EDE that assessed feelings of fatness, dissatisfaction with shape/weight, and discomfort with shape/weight via a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from “Not at all” to “Very much.” Items were summed into a single scale (range=5–35) based on high internal consistency (α=.94). The Social Adjustment Scale-Self Report [SAS-SR; 38] was used to assess interpersonal context. Five items assessing loneliness, social rejection, friendship quality, and recent arguments, all rated on a 5-point scale ranging from “Not at all” to “A lot,” were combined into a single scale (range=5–25) based on high internal consistency (α=.74).

2.3. Statistical Analyses

LPA was used to identify eating episodes classes using indicator variables (self-reported overeating severity, LOC severity, perceived palatability of food being consumed, and pre-episode craving and hunger), accounting for clustering of data by participant. Missing data were handled via full information maximum-likelihood. The best-fitting model was determined using the log-likelihood (LL), Akaike information criterion (AIC), Bayesian information criterion (BIC), sample-size adjusted BIC (aBIC), and entropy. Lower BIC, AIC, and aBIC indicate better fitting models [3941] and higher entropy values indicate better classification accuracy [42]. After determining the number of classes, multinomial logistic regressions were used to compare eating episode classes on episode type (meal, snack, binge, or other) and signal type (event-, signal-, or interval-contingent). The modified Bolck, Croon, & Hagenaar (BCH) method [43, 44] was used to compare classes on validator variables (negative and positive affect, body dissatisfaction, interpersonal problems, being alone versus not, being at home versus not, and time of day. This method remedies biases in the identifying latent class variables [43, 44]. Overall comparisons between episode classes and EMA-measured validators were conducted using Wald chi-square tests. Mplus 7 and SPSS 25 were used for analyses.

3. Results

Participants were 39 youth [n=22 females (56.4%); M age=11.2±1.9y] with overweight/obesity (M z-BMI=2.06±0.49; range=1.15–2.89) who self-identified as African-American (64.1%; n=25), Hispanic (15.4%; n=6), non-Hispanic Caucasian (15.4%; n=6), or Asian (2.6%; n=1), or did not report race/ethnicity (n=1), reflecting the demographics of the study location. Participants completed 13.83 (SD=1.74) days of EMA recordings, on average, including 1,119 (67.6%) signal-contingent, 151 (9.1%) event-contingent, and 387 (23.3%) bedtime recordings. There were 689 distinct eating episodes included in LPA, of which 419 were reported at signal-contingent recordings (60.8%), 123 at interval-contingent recordings (17.9%), and 147 at event-contingent recordings (21.3%). Children reported an average of 1.89±1.15 eating episodes per day, with most reported as meals (n=545; 79.1%) or snacks (n=118; 17.1%); one episode was labeled a binge (0.2%) and 25 were labeled “other” (3.6%). A 4-class solution displayed the best fit with the lowest values on model fit statistics (see Table 1). However, two of the classes in the 4-class solution had <5% of the sample within the class. Thus, we selected the 3-class solution for stability and ability to test for differences on validators, which is what we report on in the following text. The 3-class solution included: 1) eating in the absence of hunger (EAH; 83%; n=574), characterized by low scores on self-reported overeating severity, LOC severity, hunger, and cravings, and moderate scores on food palatability; 2) appetitive eating (8%; n=55), characterized by low scores on self-reported overeating and LOC severity, moderate scores on food palatability, and high scores on hunger and cravings; and 3) binge-like eating (9%; n=60), characterized by the highest scores on all 5 indicators (see Table 2). Compared to event-contingent recordings, binge-like eating was more likely to be reported at signal-continent recordings compared to EAH (B=1.75, SE=0.61, p=.004) and appetitive eating (B=1.52, SE=0.70, p=.03). Compared to event-contingent recordings, binge-like eating was more likely to be reported at interval-contingent recordings compared to EAH (B=1.79, SE=0.65, p=.006). There were no significant differences between EAH and appetitive episodes with respect to signal type. There were no differences among the three classes with respect to eating episode type (meal, snack, binge, or other).

Table 1.

Model fit statistics for latent profile analysis

Number of Classes BIC aBIC AIC LL Entropy
1 9602.58 9570.83 9557.23 −4768.61 -
2 8275.49 8224.69 8202.92 −4085.46 .98
3 7529.46 7459.61 7429.69 −3692.84 .99
4 6326.59 6237.68 6199.60 −3071.80 .99

Note. BIC=Bayesian information criteria; aBIC=adjusted Bayesian information criteria; AIC=Akaike information criteria; LL=log likelihood

Table 2.

Descriptive characteristics of eating-related indicators (M unless otherwise indicated)

EAH
(n=574)
Appetitive eating
(n=55)
Binge-like eating
(n=60)
Proportion (%) of sample endorsing at 39/39 (100) 21/39 (53.8) 14/39 (35.9)
least one eating episode
Self-reported overeating 1.14a 1.19a 1.91b
LOC eating 1.07a 1.12a 1.73b
Taste 6.88a 6.88a 7.93b
Pre-episode (lagged) craving 1.00a 2.56b 4.71c
Pre-episode (lagged) hunger 1.54a 2.98b 4.36c

Note. EAH=eating in the absence of hunger; LOC=loss of control. Differing superscript letters indicate significant differences (p<.05).

As illustrated in Table 3, binge-like eating was associated with higher positive affect compared to EAH (χ2=4.22, p=.04) and with higher interpersonal problems compared to appetitive eating (χ2=3.93, p=.047). EAH was associated with lower body dissatisfaction compared to appetitive eating (χ2=7.21, p=.007) and binge-like eating (χ2=4.74, p=.03). Appetitive eating episodes were less likely to occur at home compared to EAH (χ2=6.99, p=.008) and binge-like episodes (χ2=9.08, p=.003). Binge-like eating occurred later in the day compared to EAH (χ2=4.15, p=.04). There were no differences for negative affect or being alone.

Table 3.

Mean comparisons between latent classes on contextual validators

EAH (n=574) Appetitive eating (n=55) Binge-like eating (n=60)
Negative affect 12.28a 12.84a 12.62a
Positive affect 23.36a 27.17ab 32.91b
Body dissatisfaction 11.05a 16.09b 18.43b
Interpersonal problems 5.87ab 5.62b 10.00a
Time of day 1.30a 1.44ab 1.48b
Alone (proportion) 0.30a 0.35a 0.18a
Home (proportion) 0.75a 0.55b 0.75a

Note. EAH=eating in the absence of hunger. Means with different superscripts are significantly different at p<.05

4. Discussion

This study identified classes of eating episodes occuring in the natural environment, and their contextual correlates, among majority Black youth with overweight/obesity. We identified three episode subtypes, one resembling EAH, one resembling binge-like eating, and one characterized by appetitive states. Consistent with the previous literature, binge-like episodes tended to co-occur with psychosocial factors [i.e., interpersonal problems, body dissatisfaction; 27, 28] and in the evening [21], possibly reflecting that these types of episodes facilitate a transition from school to home (e.g., “winding down” from school), differing access to food at home versus school, or greater distress in the context of the family versus school peers. Interestingly, binge-like episodes also were associated with higher positive affect (but not negative affect), consistent with earlier work using this dataset [45]. This suggests that dysregulated eating may function to sustain positive mood [46], rather than decrease negative mood, or that binge-like eating often occurs during celebrations or other social events where excessive eating is commonplace [21]. Appetitive eating was more likely to co-occur with psychosocial factors than EAH, and also tended to occur away from home. Overall, findings suggest that weight control interventions for youth should consider contextual factors specific to different types of eating episodes, although future research is needed to clarify if these different episodes have varying qualitative (e.g., macronutrient content) or quantitative (e.g., energy composition) features.

EAH was the most common type of episode, which may reflect the sample since EAH has been associated with excess weight in youth [8]. These episodes were less likely to co-occur with psychosocial factors (i.e., body dissatisfaction, interpersonal problems) and tended to occur earlier in the day and at home. EAH episodes were also more likely than binge-like episodes to be reported at event-contingent surveys, suggesting that retrospective recall biases did not play a prominent role in reporting these episodes (although hunger ratings were derived from time-lagged recordings at the previous EMA signals). Taken together, EAH may reflect “normative” eating in racially diverse youth with overweight/obesity, meaning that youth with these characteristics experience infrequent eating episodes solely driven by homeostatic energy needs. Perceptions of hunger and its correspondence with objective markers of eating (e.g., meal size) have received surprisingly little empirical attention with respect to weight status [47], particularly in youth, and recent data suggest that the association between objective physiological indicators of appetite (i.e., pre-prandial glucose) and self-reported hunger may be distorted in individuals with overweight/obesity [48, 49]. Thus, it is unclear whether “hunger” in this sample reflects a physiological state of energy depletion or some combination of subjective appetitive factors. Further characterization of hunger and “normative” eating in youth with overweight/obesity is therefore warranted.

Past EMA research in youth has not supported negative affect as a momentary correlate or precipitant of dysregulated eating [27, 28], which contrasts findings in adults [26]. Yet, in the current study, binge-like episodes tended to co-occur with shape-/weight-related and interpersonal distress. It is possible that youth with binge-like eating are less attuned to their affective state than to cues that trigger these states (e.g., alexithymia [50]). Indeed, our group found that youth tend to report “numbing out” during LOC episodes [21]. This may be especially relevant in the late afternoon/evening, when youth are unwinding from the school day and may be processing or ruminating on aversive events from earlier in the day. Assisting these youth in recognizing affective consequences of intra- and interpersonal events may be crucial in helping them develop more adaptive coping strategies.

Although EAH and hedonic eating are associated with excess weight status [11, 51], and may co-occur with LOC and emotional eating [14, 15], they are not typically considered pathological eating behaviors in their own right. This was mostly borne out in the current study, as both types of episodes were similarly associated with most psychosocial contextual factors. One exception was that appetitive eating was associated with greater body dissatisfaction than EAH (albeit lower than binge-like eating). Eating episodes involving high levels of craving and hunger, accompanied by consumption of palatable foods, may trigger activation of the brain’s reward system [51], and hence function to distract attention from uncomfortable body-related cognitions and attitudes. Alternatively, in the presence of low levels of hunger, gastric distress (e.g., bloating) may trigger shape-/weight-related concerns or make EAH a less effective coping strategy for shape-/weight-related distress.

This study had several strengths, including the diverse, community-based sample of majority Black and Hispanic youth, and the use of ecologically valid assessment methods that reduced retrospective recall biases. Furthermore, the use of LPA to characterize eating episodes according to sensational, appetitive, and psychological characteristics was novel. Limitations included the modest between-subjects sample size, which was somewhat offset by a large within-subjects sample size (i.e., 689 distinct eating episodes). However, the average number of eating episodes reported by participants (~1.9/day) may reflect under-reporting, potentially due to children forgetting to initiate recordings, being in an inopportune place/time to complete surveys (including school, where participants were not receiving semi-random prompts and/or may not have had possession of their phones), or feeling embarrassed about eating behavior. Taken together, results should be replicated in a larger independent sample of youth with additional strategies devoted to improving compliance, such as offering higher incentives for self-initiating eating episode ratings. Further, the sample was restricted to youth aged 8–14, precluding generalizability to younger children and older adolescents. In addition, participants did not receive EMA signals during the school day, limiting the range of contextual factors that could be investigated. Other limitations included the absence of non-overweight controls; reliance on self-report/subjective assessment of indicator and validator constructs; and the cross-sectional, observational nature of data collection, which limits ability to infer causation/directionality. Finally, we did not examine the dietary composition of eating episodes making it unclear if certain classes of episodes were more likely to contribute to the maintenance of excess weight status than others.

Despite these limitations, findings are highly novel and suggest that youth with overweight/obesity may engage in eating episodes characterized by varying levels of sensational, psychological, and appetitive characteristics. An improved understanding of the contexts in which these episodes occur can inform development of targeted and optimally timed interventions for obesogenic eating behavior. Future research is needed to understand if different eating episodes classes are associated with varying levels of risk for obesity onset/weight gain (including degree of excess weight), and whether they require different approaches to treatment.

Highlights.

  • We used latent profile analysis (LPA) to identify classes of eating episodes

  • Classes were “binge” eating, eating-in-the-absence-of-hunger, and appetitive eating

  • Classes differed on environmental and psychological correlates

Role of Funding Sources

This research was funded by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (UL1-TR000430). The funding source had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Abbreviations:

LPA

latent profile analysis

LOC

loss of control

EAH

eating in the absence of hunger

BED

binge eating disorder

EMA

ecological momentary assessment

PANAS

Positive and Negative Affect Scale

EDE

Eating Disorder Examination

SAS-SR

Social Adjustment Scale-Self-Report

LL

log likelihood

AIC

Akaike information criterion

BIC

Bayesian information criterion

aBIC

adjusted Bayesian information criterion

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Conflicts of Interest

ABG has consulted for Sunovion Pharmaceuticals. None of the other authors have any conflicts of interest to declare.

References

  • 1.Ogden CL, et al. , Trends in obesity prevalence among children and adolescents in the United States, 1988–1994 through 2013–2014. Jama, 2016. 315(21): p. 2292–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Finkelstein EA, Graham WCK, and Malhotra R, Lifetime direct medical costs of childhood obesity. Pediatrics, 2014. 133(5): p. 854–862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hoelscher DM, et al. , Position of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics: interventions for the prevention and treatment of pediatric overweight and obesity. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 2013. 113(10): p. 1375–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Falkner B and Cossrow ND, Prevalence of metabolic syndrome and obesity-associated hypertension in the racial ethnic minorities of the United States. Curr Hypertens Rep, 2014. 16(7): p. 449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.French SA, et al. , Eating behavior dimensions. Associations with energy intake and body weight. A review. Appetite, 2012. 59: p. 541–549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Maugeri A and Barchitta M, A Systematic Review of Ecological Momentary Assessment of Diet: Implications and Perspectives for Nutritional Epidemiology. Nutrients, 2019. 11(11). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Shin AC, Zheng H, and Berthoud HR, An expanded view of energy homeostasis: neural integration of metabolic, cognitive, and emotional drives to eat. Physiol Behav, 2009. 97(5): p. 572–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lansigan RK, Emond JA, and Gilbert-Diamond D, Understanding eating in the absence of hunger among young children: A systematic review of existing studies. Appetite, 2015. 85: p. 36–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Goldschmidt AB, et al. , Contextual factors associated with eating in the absence of hunger among adults with obesity. Eat Behav, 2017. 26: p. 33–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Boggiano MM, et al. , Real-time sampling of reasons for hedonic food consumption: Further validation of the Palatable Eating Motives Scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 2015. 6: p. 744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Shomaker LB, Tanofsky-Kraff M, and Yanovski J, Disinhibited eating and body weight in youth, in Handbook of Behavior, Food and Nutrition, Preedy VR, Watson RR, and Martin CR, Editors. 2011, Springer: New York, NY. p. 2183–2199. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Myers CA, Martin CK, and Apolzan JW, Food cravings and body weight: a conditioning response. Curr Opin Endocrinol Diabetes Obes, 2018. 25(5): p. 298–302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lowe MR and Butryn ML, Hedonic hunger: a new dimension of appetite? Physiol Behav, 2007. 91(4): p. 432–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Boutelle KN, et al. , Overeating phenotypes in overweight and obese children. Appetite, 2014. 76(0): p. 95–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vannucci A, et al. , Latent profile analysis to determine the typology of disinhibited eating behaviors in children and adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2013. 81(3): p. 494–507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lee-Winn AE, et al. , Gender and racial/ethnic differences in binge eating symptoms in a nationally representative sample of adolescents in the United States. Eat Behav, 2016. 22: p. 27–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Verbeken S, et al. , Editorial: Eating Behavior Research in Children’s and Adolescent’s Naturalistic Environment. Front Psychol, 2019. 10: p. 2139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Reichenberger J, et al. , It’s craving time: time of day effects on momentary hunger and food craving in daily life. Nutrition, 2018. 55–56: p. 15–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.de Castro JM, The time of day of food intake influences overall intake in humans. J Nutr, 2004. 134(1): p. 104–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Masheb RM and Grilo CM, Eating patterns and breakfast consumption in obese patients with binge eating disorder. Behav Res Ther, 2006. 44(11): p. 1545–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Tanofsky-Kraff M, et al. , A multisite investigation of binge eating behaviors in children and adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2007. 75(6): p. 901–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.de Castro JM, et al. , Overweight and obese humans overeat away from home. Appetite, 2012. 59(2): p. 204–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Herman CP, The social facilitation of eating. A review. Appetite, 2015. 86: p. 61–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Goldschmidt AB, et al. , Ecological momentary assessment of maladaptive eating in children and adolescents with overweight or obesity. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2018. 51(6): p. 549–557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Palmberg AA, et al. , Adolescent girls and their mothers talk about experiences of binge and loss of control eating. Journal of child and family studies, 2014. 23(8): p. 1403–1416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Haedt-Matt AA and Keel PK, Revisiting the affect regulation model of binge eating: A meta-analysis of studies using ecological momentary assessment. Psychological Bulletin, 2011. 137(4): p. 660–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hilbert A, et al. , Loss of control eating and psychological maintenance in children: An ecological momentary assessment study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2009. 47(1): p. 26–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ranzenhofer LM, et al. , Using ecological momentary assessment to examine interpersonal and affective predictors of loss of control eating in adolescent girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2014. 47(7): p. 748–757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Goldschmidt AB, et al. , Latent profile analysis of eating episodes in anorexia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 2014. 53: p. 193–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kuczmarski RJ, et al. , CDC growth charts: United States. Advance Data, 2000. 314: p. 1–27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Goldschmidt AB, et al. , Executive functioning in a racially diverse sample of children who are overweight and at risk for eating disorders. Appetite, 2018. 124: p. 43–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Wheeler L and Reis HT, Self-recording of everyday life events: Origins, types, and uses. Journal of Personality, 1991. 59(3): p. 339–354. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bryant-Waugh RJ, et al. , The use of the Eating Disorder Examination with children: A pilot study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 1996. 19(4): p. 391–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Watson D, Clark LA, and Tellegen A, Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1988. 54(6): p. 1063–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Laurent J, et al. , A measure of positive and negative affect for children: Scale development and preliminary validation. Psychological Assessment, 1999. 11(3): p. 326–338. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Smyth JM, et al. , Daily and momentary mood and stress are associated with binge eating and vomiting in bulimia nervosa patients in the natural environment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2007. 75(4): p. 629–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Engel SG, et al. , The role of affect in the maintenance of anorexia nervosa: Evidence from a naturalistic assessment of momentary behaviors and emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2013. 122(3): p. 709–719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Weissman MM and Bothwell S, Assessment of social adjustment by patient self-report. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1976. 33(9): p. 1111–1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Akaike H, Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 1987. 52(3): p. 317–332. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Schwarz G, Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistic, 1978. 6(2): p. 461–464. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sclove SL, Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. 1987, Springer: Germany. p. 333–343. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bozdogan H, Model selection and Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC): The general theory and its analytical extensions. Psychometrika, 1987. 52(3): p. 345–370. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bakk Z, Tekle FB, and Vermunt JK, Estimating the Association between Latent Class Membership and External Variables Using Bias-adjusted Three-step Approaches. Sociological Methodology, 2013. 43(1): p. 272–311. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Vermunt J, Latent class modeling with covariates: Two improved three-step approaches. Psycho-oncology, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Egbert AH, et al. , Momentary associations between positive affect dimensions and dysregulated eating during puberty in a diverse sample of youth with overweight/obesity. Int J Eat Disord, 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Mason TB, et al. , Anhedonia, positive affect dysregulation, and risk and maintenance of binge-eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord, 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Gibbons C, et al. , Issues in Measuring and Interpreting Human Appetite (Satiety/Satiation) and Its Contribution to Obesity. Curr Obes Rep, 2019. 8(2): p. 77–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Schembre SM, et al. , Using pre-prandial blood glucose to assess eating in the absence of hunger in free-living individuals. Eat Behav, 2020. 38: p. 101411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Howard LM, et al. , Examining the ecological validity of the Power of Food Scale. Eat Weight Disord, 2021. 26(2): p. 717–721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Shank LM, et al. , The association between alexithymia and eating behavior in children and adolescents. Appetite, 2019. 142: p. 104381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Freitas A, et al. , Appetite-Related Eating Behaviours: An Overview of Assessment Methods, Determinants and Effects on Children’s Weight. Ann Nutr Metab, 2018. 73(1): p. 19–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES