Skip to main content
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2022 Mar 2;289(1970):20212261. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2021.2261

Prey encounters and spatial memory influence use of foraging patches in a marine central place forager

Virginia Iorio-Merlo 1,, Isla M Graham 1, Rebecca C Hewitt 1, Geert Aarts 2,3, Enrico Pirotta 4,5, Gordon D Hastie 6, Paul M Thompson 1
PMCID: PMC8889173  PMID: 35232237

Abstract

Given the patchiness and long-term predictability of marine resources, memory of high-quality foraging grounds is expected to provide fitness advantages for central place foragers. However, it remains challenging to characterize how marine predators integrate memory with recent prey encounters to adjust fine-scale movement and use of foraging patches. Here, we used two months of movement data from harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) to quantify the repeatability in foraging patches as a proxy for memory. We then integrated these data into analyses of fine-scale movement and underwater behaviour to test how both spatial memory and prey encounter rates influenced the seals' area-restricted search (ARS) behaviour. Specifically, we used one month's GPS data from 29 individuals to build spatial memory maps of searched areas and archived accelerometery data from a subset of five individuals to detect prey catch attempts, a proxy for prey encounters. Individuals were highly consistent in the areas they visited over two consecutive months. Hidden Markov models showed that both spatial memory and prey encounters increased the probability of seals initiating ARS. These results provide evidence that predators use memory to adjust their fine-scale movement, and this ability should be accounted for in movement models.

Keywords: area-restricted search, spatial memory, Hidden Markov Model, accelerometer, harbour seals, repeatability

1. Introduction

While key aspects of predator movements can be explained by theoretical search strategies [1], it is recognized that factors such as cognitive and perceptual abilities may also influence movement patterns [13]. Predator movements can be characterized into different modes (e.g. oriented versus non-oriented, exploratory versus area-restricted search (ARS)), with switches between these modes characterizing temporal and spatial variation in foraging effort [4]. ARS movement is widely recognized as a strategy by which predators concentrate their search activity in areas rich in resources [5,6]. Specifically, predators are expected to decrease their speed and increase turning angles upon encountering prey, thereby increasing time spent in areas where the probability of encountering further prey items is high [5,7,8]. Thus, an increase in prey encounters has been hypothesized to drive the initiation of ARS behaviour [9,10]. However, prey encounters are often highly stochastic, and since most predators have well-developed cognitive and sensory abilities, they are also expected to use other information sources to initiate ARS [3,11,12].

Many terrestrial and marine species display site fidelity to foraging and breeding locations, supporting their ability to store information on habitat quality [1315]. Furthermore, mechanistic movement models that include spatial memory can successfully replicate observed patterns of site fidelity [16,17]. Given the patchiness and high spatio-temporal predictability of marine resources, site fidelity and memory of foraging grounds is hypothesized to provide fitness advantages over an individual's lifespan [1820]. In particular, animals may use spatial memory to target patches of resources outside their perceptual ranges [2,21,22]. For example, black-browed albatrosses (Thalassarche melanophris) targeted areas of <1 km2 where they had previously encountered fishing vessels, despite these being >100 km from their colony [23]. Predators may thus use spatial memory to identify foraging areas, within which they then focus searching activity using ARS movement [24].

Previous studies considering both memory and the influence of prey encounters on searching strategies are based either on terrestrial systems [25,26] or simulations [2729]. Despite evidence of marine predators returning to foraging grounds [11,12,30], it is only recently that advances in biologging and acoustic technologies have provided finer resolution data to empirically test the effect of prey encounter events on marine mammal and seabird movements [9,31]. To date, we are aware of no study that has directly explored how marine predators combine longer term spatial memory and contemporary prey encounters to adjust their fine-scale movements.

Here, we used movement data from biologgers deployed on coastal harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) to test the influence of both spatial memory and prey encounters, and their interaction, on ARS behaviour in this central place forager [32]. First, movement data were used to classify seal activities at sea [33]. To provide initial support that seals have memory of foraging areas, we explored individual repeatability of foraging patches over two consecutive months. As a proxy for memory, data on the seal's activities were used to build spatial memory maps representing the areas in which seals concentrated their searching effort over a one month period. For a subset of animals, we then used fine-scale accelerometer data [34,35], to infer prey encounter events while the animal was diving. Finally, we fitted two hidden Markov models (HMMs) [36] to test whether spatial memory alone, or in combination with prey encounters, increased the probability of an animal initiating ARS behaviour during a foraging trip.

2. Methods

(a) . Case study species and data collection

Harbour seals are central place foragers inhabiting temperate coastal waters [37]. During February and March 2017, 31 adult harbour seals (11 males and 20 females) were captured and tagged in Loch Fleet, NE Scotland (57.935° N, 4.042° W) (see [13] for background on the study site and population). Seal capture and handling occurred in accordance with the Home Office Licence issued to the Sea Mammal Research Unit (licence no. 192CBD9F) with local licence approval from the University of St Andrews Animal Welfare and Ethics Committee. Fastloc GPS-GSM phone tags (Sea Mammal Research Unit Instrumentation, University of St Andrews, UK) were attached to the pelage at the back of the neck, using the capture and handling methods detailed in Russell et al. [38]. Tags were equipped with a GPS (Global Positioning System) receiver, wet–dry sensor and pressure sensor, providing geo-referenced summaries of activity and diving patterns via the GSM (Global System for Mobile communications) phone network [39]. Tags also collected tri-axial accelerometer data that were archived onboard, subject to digital storage limitations, but not relayed through the GSM network due to the volume of data from the high sampling frequencies used. Tags from a subset of five individuals were subsequently recovered on the shore after tags detached during the moult, allowing archived tri-axial accelerometer data to be downloaded.

Tags were programmed to record GPS information every time a seal surfaced. However, due to variation in satellite availability, this resulted in an irregular time series. On average, locations were recorded every 15 min. When the wet–dry sensor determined that the animal was at sea, the pressure sensor also recorded depth. Below a depth threshold of 1.5 m, time–depth data were recorded every 4 s and stored in the tags. Dives were summarized using depth bins at 23 equally spaced time points throughout the dives. For each dive, the maximum diving depth, duration and time–depth summary were transmitted through the GSM network. The tri-axial accelerometer measured the g-force at a frequency of 12.5 Hz. Because the accelerometers were not calibrated prior to release, a post hoc calibration was applied to the data, described in detail in electronic supplementary material, appendix A. Next, a box-moving average (window width of 12 Hz) of each of the three axes was calculated. These smoothed values represent an approximation for the gravitational component, which can be used to derive the pitch angle. Finally, these smoothed estimates were subtracted from the measured raw g-forces to obtain the dynamic or specific acceleration, which can be used to determine prey capture attempts [40].

(b) . Identification of ARS behaviour

We fitted a total of three HMMs (table 1) to classify at-sea activities and to build spatial memory maps of searched areas (Model 1), to assess the influence of memory alone on all individuals (Model 2) and to assess the simultaneous influence of spatial memory and prey encounters on the subset of five individuals for which accelerometer data were available (Model 3). All models were fitted using the momentuHMM package [41].

Table 1.

Overview of the three HMM models, showing the number of individuals included in the model, the time period for which movement data were used, the covariates that were included in the model to assess the influence on the transition probabilities and a summary of the objectives and what was the output used for.

model number of individuals time period covariates objective and output
Model 1 31 Feb to June none —identification of ARS locations to be used in the repeatability analysis
—spatial memory maps with the proportion of dive batches spent searching for the month of Apr and a month prior to the beginning of the accelerometer data
Model 2 29 May —spatial memory of ARS behaviour in April —test the influence of memory on the transition probability between ARS and transit
Model 3 5 April to June —spatial memory of ARS behaviour during the month prior to the beginning of the accelerometer data —test the influence of memory and prey encounters on the transition probability between ARS and transit
—mean number of prey encounters per dive in each dive batch

To ensure our analysis focused on central place foraging trips, we selected round-trips from and to the same haul-out site location, which were (i) >12 h and (ii) included locations that were >2 km from the haul-out site. This avoided the inclusion of shorter periods in the water which typically represent resting near intertidal haul-out sites [13,42].

We used batches of five dives as the unit of analysis to avoid potential numerical problems in estimating the maximum-likelihood and extreme residual autocorrelation associated with a dive-by-dive analysis [43]. The mean dive cycle (i.e. dive and subsequent period at surface, a dive being the time spent below 1.5 m depth) was 4.46 (± 6.68) minutes, and the 90th percentile of the time interval between GPS locations was 25 min. Dive locations were estimated by linearly interpolating between the GPS positions using the manufacturer software. However, due to gaps in the GPS datasets, there might be uncertainty around some dive locations (electronic supplementary material, appendix B, figure B1). Therefore, in the analyses, we only used batches of five dives that were associated with at least one raw GPS location (for more details, see electronic supplementary material, appendix B).

Seal activities at sea were classified into two behavioural states using an HMM based on the step length and turning angle between consecutive dive batches. The two states are assumed to represent transit and ARS movement, which are characterized by long directional displacement or short tortuous movement, respectively [44]. We calculated the step length and turning angle between the locations of the first dive of each batch and assumed these observations resulted from state-dependent gamma and wrapped Cauchy distributions [45], respectively. Following the methodology described by Russell et al. [46] and Carter et al. [47], if any dive batch was not associated with a raw GPS location, the step length and turning angle were set to ‘not available’ (NA) [36]; thus, the state was assigned solely based on the Markov property (for more details, see electronic supplementary material, appendix B). Finally, we selected the initial values of the parameters using the estimates from the model with the lowest Akaike information criterion (AIC) score among 50 iterations with randomly selected initial values. The most likely state sequence given the final model was decoded using the Viterbi algorithm [48].

(c) . Spatial memory of foraging patches

Searching areas were defined using the locations of dive batches that were classified as ARS by Model 1. To quantify how consistently seals visited the same areas over time, we calculated the spatial overlap between searched areas visited during two consecutive months [20]; here, April and May. Kernel distributions (UD) for each of the two months were calculated using the adehabitatHR package [49] using a grid size of 500 × 500 m. The most appropriate kernel bandwidth was estimated using the First Passage Time method described in Lascelles et al. [50]. Overlap between 50% UD was estimated using the Bhattacharyya's affinity (BA) index [51], where 0 indicates no overlap and 1 identical distributions. To compare the observed overlap with a null distribution of BA values, we used a pairwise comparison to calculate the overlap between a seal's UD in May with the UD in April of another randomly selected individual.

As a proxy for spatial memory, we built memory grids using the proportion of dive batches classified as ARS by Model 1, in a 1 × 1 km grid over the study area. Two sets of memory grids were built to be used in Model 2 and Model 3, respectively (table 1). We first created a set of spatial memory grids representing the individual's ARS behaviour during the previous month of the data included in Model 2 (table 1). Then, we created a second set of grids representing the areas used during one month prior to data included in Model 3 (table 1). Due to the differences in accelerometer data availability between individuals (electronic supplementary material, table S1), the month used to build the spatial memory grid for each of these five individuals varied.

(d) . Prey encounters

We inferred prey encounter events from the accelerometer data while animals were at sea. In coastal waters, harbour seals most frequently dive to the seabed and perform U-shaped dives through all phases of their foraging trips [52,53]. Therefore, we used accelerometery data to detect prey encounters during the bottom phase of each of these dives [53], characterized as the period when seals were within 20% of the maximum dive depth [54].

We used two different methodologies to detect prey encounters. First, we identified sudden peaks in dynamic acceleration resulting from rapid head and body movements [34,55,56]. This method has been validated with captive harbour seals and was able to identify prey capture attempts [34,35]. We calculated the standard deviation in dynamic acceleration over a moving window of 1.5 s for each axis and used a k-means cluster analysis to group the standard deviation values into two activity states, ‘high’ and ‘low’. We assumed an animal made a prey capture attempt and thus encountered a prey item, when its activity was determined to be ‘high’ on all three axes [34,55,56]. Second, we identified changes in body pitch angle, which have been used as indicators of the more subtle movements that harbour seals may use to catch benthic prey in shallow coastal waters [57]. The pitch angle was calculated based on the estimated gravitational component of the measured g-forces [34]. We calculated the differences between peaks and troughs in the time series of body pitch angle during each dive. Prey capture attempts were identified when a change in pitch angle greater than 20° occurred within a window of 5 s [57]. As these two methodologies have not previously been used together, we assessed whether the identified foraging attempts derived from the two methods (i.e. bursts in dynamic acceleration and drops in body pitch angle) occurred at the same time. To avoid counting the same event twice, we then calculated the total number of prey encounter events in each dive by summing the number of independent attempts detected by either method.

(e) . Assessing the drivers of area-restricted search behaviour

To assess which factors influenced the initiation of ARS behaviour, we ran two separate models Model 2 and Model 3 (table 1). Model 2 was based on foraging trips occurring in May and included the spatial memory grids of the seals' activities during the month of April as covariates on the transition probabilities between transit and ARS state [58]. In Model 3, we included the spatial memory grid of activities during the month prior to the beginning of the accelerometer data and the mean number of prey encounters per dive in each dive batch as covariates (see ‘Identification of ARS behaviour’). Note that although five individuals were represented in both models, the memory grids differed between models (see ‘Spatial memory of foraging patches section’). After assessing the correlation between the two covariates, we investigated both their additive effect and the effect of an interaction between the two. To assess the influence of each covariate, we fitted the models including both covariates or each covariate separately and ranked them based on AIC and BIC [59]. Covariates were retained in the model if their inclusion reduced the information criteria by at least two units [59].

3. Results

Between February and July 2017, each of the 31 tagged seals performed on average 44 foraging trips, which extended across the NE of Scotland (figure 1a). Foraging trips lasted on average 38.65 h (±34.79 h), with the longest trip performed by a male lasting 6.36 days. There was large inter-individual variation in at-sea distribution (figure 1a). However, the ranging patterns and characteristics of the trips of the five individuals for which accelerometer data were available fell within the range of all tagged individuals (figure 1b; electronic supplementary material, table S2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

(a) Maps displaying the movements of the 31 tagged harbour seals in the Moray Firth (Scotland), showing data from the five retrieved tags in yellow. (b) Tracks of the five focal seals where tags were recovered. The trips with accelerometer data that were included in the analysis are highlighted in red (Model 3), while the time period before and after is shown in blue. (Online version in colour.)

(a) . Memory of foraging patches

The first HMM (Model 1) assigned the dive batches into two states: state 1 (step length: 1026.98 ± 193.83 m, angle: µ = 0, γ = 0.80) and state 2 (step length: 587.81 ± 172.48 m, angle: µ = 0, γ = 0.027) (electronic supplementary material, figure S1). Based upon the combination of short step length and low concentration (i.e. high variability) in turning angle, state 2 was assumed to represent ARS behaviour.

We were able to compare the areas animals visited in May with those visited in April for 29 seals (two tags stopped recording during May). On average these seals performed 10 (±5.61) foraging trips in each month. We found 5.57 km to be the most appropriate h smoothing value to calculate individual's 50% UD (electronic supplementary material, figure S2). Individuals were highly consistent in the areas they visited in April and May (electronic supplementary material, table S3; figure 2), showing much higher overlap than the null distribution (figure 2). From the output of Model 1, dive batches classified as state 2 were used to create the spatial memory grids to be used as covariates in Model 2 and Model 3 (e.g. figure 3b).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

(a) Centroid location of the areas animals searched in April (red) in relation to the centroid location of the ones visited in May (yellow) for 29 individuals. (b) Frequency distribution of the observed overlap (green) of an individual's searched areas in consecutive month, estimated using Bhattacharyya's affinity index, and the null distribution of Bhattacharyya's affinity values from the overlap with the areas searched by another randomly selected individual. (Online version in colour.)

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Example of the spatial variation in prey encounters and proxy for spatial memory in relation to the behavioural state classification of Model 3 for the foraging trips of seal 242. (a) Locations of dive batches, colour-coded by the mean number of prey encounters per dive batch. (b) 1 × 1 km memory grid, showing the proportion of dive batches classified as ARS by Model 1 in each grid cell during the month prior to the trips in Model 3. (c) Tracks of the trips used in Model 3, colour-coded by the decoded HMM state. Missing parts of the tracks are due to unreliable dive batches (see electronic supplementary material, appendix B). (Online version in colour.)

(b) . Detection of prey encounters

Prey encounters were detected in all 51 foraging trips for which we had accelerometer data (electronic supplementary material, table S1; figure 3a). Within each of these trips, 69.45% of dives had at least one prey encounter identified by one of the two methods. In total, 51 586 encounters were identified from peaks in acceleration and 78 441 encounters were identified from changes in body pitch angle towards the seabed (electronic supplementary material, figure S3). Of these, only 981 events (0.008% of the total attempts identified) overlapped in time, possibly suggesting that the methods had identified the same event. There was inter-individual variability in the detection of prey encounters by the two methods (electronic supplementary material, figure S4).

(c) . Drivers of area-restricted search behaviour

The second model (Model 2) assigned dive batches during foraging trips occurring in May into two behavioural states: (i) the first was characterized by long step length and small turning angle (step: 1049.335 ± 556.832, angle: µ = 0, γ = 0.826), which we assumed represents an animal transiting and (ii) the second was characterized by short step length and large turning angle (step: 207.162 ± 181.983, angle: µ = 0, γ = 0.424), which we assumed represents ARS behaviour (electronic supplementary material, figure S5). Both model selection criteria supported the inclusion of spatial memory, based on seal movements in April, as a covariate in the model (table 2). The proportion of foraging batches spent searching in the same area during the previous month increased an individual's probability of initiating ARS behaviour (figure 4—Model 2).

Table 2.

Comparison of the models based on AIC and BIC, with covariates and removing one variable at a time for both Model 2 and Model 3. The memory covariate represents the number of dive batches spent searching in a grid cell during the previous month, and prey encounters indicates the mean number of catch attempts per dive for each batch.

covariates log-likelihood AIC BIC ΔAIC ΔBIC
Model 2
 with memory −164 875 329 759 329 848 0 0
 without memory −165 000 330 017 330 090 258 242
Model 3
 memory + prey encounters −26 816 53 657 53 739 0 0
 memory × prey encounters −26 814 53 657 53 751 0 12
  memory −26 845 53 780 53 781 54 41
  prey encounters −26 882 53 909 53 910 129 116

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Transition probability (mean and 95% CI) of remaining in a transit state or switching to an ARS state for the covariates included in Model 2 and Model 3. Model 2: influence of proportion of dive batches spent searching in the previous month (proxy for spatial memory) on the 29 individuals included in the model. Model 3: (left) influence of proportion of dive batches spent searching in the previous month and (right) influence of the mean batch prey encounters on the five individuals included in the model. (Online version in colour.)

Model 3 assigned movement between the dive batches into a transit state (step: 893.543 m ± 623.451, angle: µ = 0, γ = 0.827) and an ARS state (step: 164.869 m ± 150.729, angle: µ = 0, γ = 0.397) (figure 3c; electronic supplementary material, figure S6). We found no correlation (Kendall τ = 0.14) between the prey encounters detected and the memory maps of the ARS behaviour during the previous month (electronic supplementary material, figure S7). Based upon the HMM output, the seals spent 27.35% (±9.22%) of the dive batches transiting and 57.27% (±21.68%) in ARS behaviour, while 15.98% (±15.72%) of the dive batches could not be classified due to a lack of GPS locations. Both model selection criteria suggested that including prey encounter events and a proxy for memory of previous ARS movement (i.e. the proportion of dive batches spent searching in the area) improved the model (table 2). We found no improvement in the model by including an interaction between the two covariates (table 2). Model 3 showed that the probability of an individual initiating ARS behaviour was associated with prey encounters and areas where individuals spent time searching before (figure 4—Model 3). Finally, the variation we observed in mean prey encounters per batch during times classified as ARS shows that animals spent time actively searching within the foraging patch (electronic supplementary material, figure S8).

4. Discussion

Understanding the drivers of animal movement and foraging behaviour remains a central topic in movement ecology [60,61]. We found that individuals repeatedly used the same areas over time, which supports the reliance on spatial memory by predators to return to previously visited foraging grounds [13]. Therefore, we explored how marine predators use information both within and outside their perceptual ranges to adjust their behaviour and movement, showing that both memory and prey encounters influenced animals' foraging decisions [5,11]. Specifically, our model shows that encountering prey and having memory of searched areas coincides with an increased probability of an individual initiating ARS behaviour.

It is challenging to quantify the distribution and variability of prey encounters at scales that are relevant to marine predators [e.g. 62,63]. We overcame this challenge by using animal-borne accelerometer data to identify prey catch attempts, which can be used as a proxy for prey encounters [35]. As predators may adapt prey capture strategies according to prey size or type [64,65], we used two previously defined proxies for prey catch attempts. Using either methodology alone would have reduced detections by 60% [55] and 40% [57], respectively. The number of prey encounters showed a positive relationship with the probability of seals initiating searching behaviour. These findings provide support for the hypothesis that predators increase their residence time in foraging patches where encounter success is high [66]. However, individual residence times could increase either due to longer search time between prey encounters or higher prey capture rate and handling time. While we were unable to make inferences about foraging success and handling times from accelerometer data alone, this may be possible in the future using auxiliary sensors [67,68].

Previous studies have also found that predators adjust their foraging behaviour to the density of resources encountered [69]. For example, prey capture rate of double-crested cormorant (Phalacrocorax auritus) was a good indicator of prey density [70]. Similarly, blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus) adjusted the number of feeding lunges per dive to krill density [71]. The results of our study show a similar positive relationship, with a higher probability of transitioning to ARS when more prey encounters occurred. This further suggests that predators might be using the number of prey encounters to assess the profitability of the foraging patch. Therefore, we can hypothesize that the probability of initiating ARS behaviour is indeed indicative of the quality of the foraging site.

Many marine central place foragers repeatedly move between and return to terrestrial breeding and resting sites [72,73] and foraging areas [22,23,74]. It is increasingly recognized that individual foraging decisions are modified by the memory of previous experience in different foraging areas [75]. In our results, we showed that the seals displayed a high level of repeatability in the areas they searched for prey in over the span of two months. By contrast, previous research on repeatability in otariids found little overlap of foraging areas between trips within a year [20]. Furthermore, our dataset was not limited to a specific sex or life-history class (e.g. lactating females only as in [20,76]), but included both sexes, as well as pregnant and non-pregnant females. The observed repeatability in this study seems to be a common trait shared across sexes. All seals tagged in this study were adults, for which a higher repeatability is expected compared to young individuals [77].

Given that individuals in this population showed high repeatability of searched areas, we tested whether memory influenced fine-scale movement decisions by including spatial memory in the HMM. We found that the probability of initiating ARS behaviour was linked with individuals’ spatial memory. Similarly, Thums et al. [11] found that southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina) had a high probability of engaging in ARS behaviour along the shelf edge, independent of prey capture attempts recorded while diving. In our study, individuals changed their behaviour in anticipation of profitable foraging areas. The differences observed between Model 2 (with data from 31 individuals) and Model 3 (with data from just five individuals) could indicate individual differences in the importance of memory which should be investigated further. Our analysis assessed the influence of spatial memory associated with a 1 × 1 km grid without making any assumptions about what features the animals might be using to recognize the areas [11] or which cues they might be following to return to these areas [78].

Short- and long-term memory of encountered resources can also vary through the lifetime of an individual, with acquisition of new information and memory decay over time [79]. In our study, we compared multiple foraging trips occurring over consecutive months, building upon earlier studies that have investigated the role of memory over a series of dives or paired trips [31,80]. Our analysis focused on two months in spring/summer; future research should aim to extend this approach to explore the role of memory over longer temporal scales using movement data across different seasons [81,82]. For example, seasonal changes in prey distribution might affect the foraging areas targeted by individuals, causing a mismatch between the areas visited in consecutive months and the persistence of memory at longer timescales [83]. Comparison of the movements of individuals during similar time periods in different years would be needed to observe long-term memory-driven behaviour [20,22]. Spatial and temporal information on prey distribution is also needed to understand how memory of prey patches may vary within or between years.

Having prior knowledge on prey distribution can be particularly useful for predators that feed on cryptic prey species with low encounter rates. In this case, predators should adopt a Bayesian foraging strategy, whereby historic prey encounters are used as prior information that is updated while encountering prey [84,85]. In our study, predators appeared to adjust their movement in response to both prior knowledge and current experience to initiate ARS. However, the same drivers could also influence predators patch departure [86]; the marginal value theorem predicts that foragers should only leave a patch and switch back to transit movement when intake rate drops below the average intake rate of the entire area [87]. Here, we were only able to incorporate archival accelerometery data from the subset of tags that were recovered. However, with improvements in onboard processing [34], data on prey encounters can now be accessed in near real time with the associated GPS data, allowing these models to be tested over ecologically relevant spatial and temporal scales.

In conclusion, this study gives new insights into another driver of ARS behaviour. These findings provide empirical evidence that predators use other information, such as spatial memory, to guide movement decisions and to initiate ARS behaviour. Previous studies showed that predators responded to their recent prey encounters, but this was insufficient to fully explain observed movement patterns [3,75]. The ability of predators to memorize the distribution of predictable resources has been predicted to have evolved to cope with environmental variability and to maximize their long-term energy intake [18,19,88]. These results reinforce the importance of accounting for this ability within movement models [17,89].

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the staff and students who have been involved in the data collection. Matt Carter for his support in the early stages of the analysis, Sophie Brasseur for her valuable input and ideas on harbour seal feeding strategies and Thomas Bodey who provided suggestions and comments on the manuscript draft. We dedicate this study to Dr Bernie McConnell, who did so much to drive the development of the telemetry devices that revealed these and many other insights into the behaviour of seals at sea.

Ethics

All research activities were conducted under the Home Office Licence issued to the Sea Mammal Research Unit (licence no. 192CBD9F) with local licence approval from the University of St Andrews Animal Welfare and Ethics Committee.

Data accessibility

All data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.6q573n601 [90], and all processing codes are available from the GitHub Repository: https://github.com/virginiaIorio/Drivers-of-seal-ARS-behaviour.

Authors' contributions

V.I.-M.: conceptualization, formal analysis, methodology, visualization, writing—original draft; I.M.G.: conceptualization, data curation, investigation, supervision, writing—review and editing; R.C.H.: investigation; G.A.: methodology, writing—review and editing; E.P.: formal analysis, methodology, writing—review and editing; G.D.H.: investigation, methodology, writing—review and editing; P.M.T.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, project administration, supervision, writing—review and editing.

All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

Competing interests

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

This study was carried out as part of the Moray Firth Marine Mammal Monitoring Programme, a joint industry, academic and government strategic research project with funding from Beatrice Offshore Wind Ltd and Moray Offshore Renewables Ltd (MORL).

References

  • 1.Sims DW, Witt MJ, Richardson AJ, Southall EJ, Metcalfe JD. 2006. Encounter success of free-ranging marine predator movements across a dynamic prey landscape. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 1195-1201. ( 10.1098/rspb.2005.3444) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fagan WF, Gurarie E, Bewick S, Howard A, Cantrell RS, Cosner C. 2017. Perceptual ranges, information gathering, and foraging success in dynamic landscapes. Am. Nat. 189, 474-489. ( 10.1086/691099) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Auger-Methe M, Derocher AE, DeMars CA, Plank MJ, Codling EA, Lewis MA. 2016. Evaluating random search strategies in three mammals from distinct feeding guilds. J. Anim. Ecol. 85, 1411-1421. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.12562) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fagan WF, Hoffman T, Dahiya D, Gurarie E, Cantrell RS, Cosner C. 2019. Improved foraging by switching between diffusion and advection: benefits from movement that depends on spatial context. Theor. Ecol. 13, 127-136. ( 10.1007/s12080-019-00434-w) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kareiva P, Odell G. 1987. Swarms of predators exhibit preytaxis if individual predators use area-restricted search. Am. Nat. 130, 233-270. ( 10.1086/284707) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Benhamou S. 1992. Efficiency of area-concentrated searching behavior in a continuous patchy environment. J. Theor. Biol. 159, 67-81. ( 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80768-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ward D, Saltz D. 1994. Forging at different spatial scales: dorcas gazelles foraging for lilies in the Negev Desert. Ecology 75, 48-58. ( 10.2307/1939381) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Schoener TW. 1971. Theory of feeding strategies. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 2, 369-404. ( 10.1146/annurev.es.02.110171.002101) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bailey H, Lyubchich V, Wingfield J, Fandel A, Garrod A, Rice AN. 2019. Empirical evidence that large marine predator foraging behavior is consistent with area-restricted search theory. Ecology 100, e02743. ( 10.1002/ecy.2743) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hamer KC, Humphreys EM, Magalhaes MC, Garthe S, Hennicke J, Peters G, Grémillet D, Skov H, Wanless S. et al. 2009. Fine-scale foraging behaviour of a medium-ranging marine predator. J. Anim. Ecol. 78, 880-889. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01549.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Thums M, Bradshaw CJ, Hindelli MA. 2011. In situ measures of foraging success and prey encounter reveal marine habitat-dependent search strategies. Ecology 92, 1258-1270. ( 10.1890/09-1299.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Davoren GK, Montevecchi WA, Anderson JT. 2003. Search strategies of a pursuit-diving marine bird and the persistence of prey patches. Ecol. Monogr. 73, 463-481. ( 10.1890/02-0208) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Cordes LS, Duck CD, Mackey BL, Hall AJ, Thompson PM. 2011. Long-term patterns in harbour seal site-use and the consequences for managing protected areas. Anim. Conserv. 14, 430-438. ( 10.1111/j.1469-1795.2011.00445.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gonzalez-Gomez PL, Vasquez RA. 2006. A field study of spatial memory in green-backed firecrown hummingbirds (Sephanoides sephaniodes). Ethology 112, 790-795. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01223.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cameron MD, Joly K, Breed GA, Mulder CPH, Kielland K. 2020. Pronounced fidelity and selection for average conditions of calving area suggestive of spatial memory in a highly migratory ungulate. Front. Ecol. Evol. 8, 14. ( 10.3389/fevo.2020.564567) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Van Moorter B, Visscher D, Benhamou S, Börger L, Boyce MS, Gaillard J-M. 2009. Memory keeps you at home: a mechanistic model for home range emergence. Oikos 118, 641-652. ( 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2008.17003.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Nabe-Nielsen J, Tougaard J, Teilmann J, Lucke K, Forchhammer MC. 2013. How a simple adaptive foraging strategy can lead to emergent home ranges and increased food intake. Oikos 122, 1307-1316. ( 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2013.00069.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bradshaw CJA, Hindell MA, Sumner MD, Michael KJ. 2004. Loyalty pays: potential life history consequences of fidelity to marine foraging regions by southern elephant seals. Anim. Behav. 68, 1349-1360. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.12.013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Abrahms B, et al. 2019. Memory and resource tracking drive blue whale migrations. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 5582-5587. ( 10.1073/pnas.1819031116) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Arthur B, Hindell M, Bester M, Trathan P, Jonsen I, Staniland I, Oosthuizen WC, Wege M, Lea M-A. 2015. Return customers: foraging site fidelity and the effect of environmental variability in wide-ranging antarctic fur seals. PLoS ONE 10, e0120888. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0120888) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Weimerskirch H, Pinaud D, Pawlowski F, Bost CA. 2007. Does prey capture induce area-restricted search? A fine-scale study using GPS in a marine predator, the wandering albatross. Am. Nat. 170, 734-743. ( 10.1086/522059) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wakefield ED, Cleasby IR, Bearhop S, Bodey TW, Davies RD, Miller PI, Newton J, Votier SC, Hamer KC. 2015. Long-term individual foraging site fidelity—why some gannets don't change their spots. Ecology. 96, 3058-3074. ( 10.1890/14-1300.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Collet J, Weimerskirch H. 2020. Albatrosses can memorize locations of predictable fishing boats but favour natural foraging. Proc. R. Soc. B 287, 20200958. ( 10.1098/rspb.2020.0958) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Regular PM, Hedd A, Montevecchi WA. 2013. Must marine predators always follow scaling laws? Memory guides the foraging decisions of a pursuit-diving seabird. Anim. Behav. 86, 545-552. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.06.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Merkle JA, Fortin D, Morales JM. 2014. A memory-based foraging tactic reveals an adaptive mechanism for restricted space use. Ecol. Lett. 17, 924-931. ( 10.1111/ele.12294) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Schlagel UE, Merrill EH, Lewis MA. 2017. Territory surveillance and prey management: wolves keep track of space and time. Ecol. Evol. 7, 8388-8405. ( 10.1002/ece3.3176) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mueller T, Fagan WF. 2008. Search and navigation in dynamic environments—from individual behaviors to population distributions. Oikos 117, 654-664. ( 10.1111/j.0030-1299.2008.16291.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bracis C, Gurarie E, Rutter JD, Goodwin RA. 2018. Remembering the good and the bad: memory-based mediation of the food-safety trade-off in dynamic landscapes. Theor. Ecol. 11, 305-319. ( 10.1007/s12080-018-0367-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Schlägel UE, Lewis MA, Börger L. 2014. Detecting effects of spatial memory and dynamic information on animal movement decisions. Methods Ecol. Evol. 5, 1236-1246. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.12284) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Meese EN, Lowe CG. 2020. Active acoustic telemetry tracking and tri-axial accelerometers reveal fine-scale movement strategies of a non-obligate ram ventilator. Mov. Ecol. 8, 8. ( 10.1186/s40462-020-0191-3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Iwata T, Sakamoto KQ, Edwards EW, Staniland IJ, Trathan PN, Goto Y, Sato K, Naito Y, Takahashi A. 2015. The influence of preceding dive cycles on the foraging decisions of Antarctic fur seals. Biol. Lett. 11, 20150227. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0227) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Orians G, Pearson N. 1979. On the theory of central place foraging. In Analysis of ecological systems (eds Horn DH, Mitchell R, Stairs GR). Columbus, OH: Ohio State University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dragon AC, Bar-Hen A, Monestiez P, Guinet C. 2012. Comparative analysis of methods for inferring successful foraging areas from Argos and GPS tracking data. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Seri. 452, 253-267. ( 10.3354/meps09618) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Cox SL, Orgeret F, Gesta M, Rodde C, Heizer I, Weimerskirch H, Guinet C. 2018. Processing of acceleration and dive data on-board satellite relay tags to investigate diving and foraging behaviour in free-ranging marine predators. Methods Ecol. Evol. 9, 64-77. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.12845) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ydesen KS, Wisniewska DM, Hansen JD, Beedholm K, Johnson M, Madsen PT. 2014. What a jerk: prey engulfment revealed by high-rate, super-cranial accelerometry on a harbour seal (Phoca vitulina). J. Exp. Biol. 217, 2239-2243. ( 10.1242/jeb.100016) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Langrock R, King R, Matthiopoulos J, Thomas L, Fortin D, Morales JM. 2012. Flexible and practical modeling of animal telemetry data: hidden Markov models and extensions. Ecology 93, 2336-2342. ( 10.1890/11-2241.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Thompson D, Duck CD, Morris CD, Russell DJF. 2019. The status of harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) in the UK. Aquat. Conserv.-Mar. Freshw. Ecosyst. 29, 40-60. ( 10.1002/aqc.3110) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Russell DJ, Hastie GD, Thompson D, Janik VM, Hammond PS, Scott-Hayward LA, Matthiopoulos J, Jones EL, Mcconnell BJ. 2016. Avoidance of wind farms by harbour seals is limited to pile driving activities. J. Appl. Ecol. 53, 1642-1652. ( 10.1111/1365-2664.12678) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.McConnell B, Bryant E, Hunter C, Lovell P, Hall A. 2004. Phoning home—a new GSM mobile phone telemetry system to collect mark-recapture data. Marine Mammal Science. 20, 274-283. ( 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2004.tb01156.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Shepard ELC, et al. 2008. Identification of animal movement patterns using tri-axial accelerometry. Endanger. Species Res. 10, 47-60. ( 10.3354/esr00084) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.McClintock BT, Michelot T. 2018. momentuHMM: R package for generalized hidden Markov models of animal movement. Methods Ecol. Evol. 9, 1518-1530. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.12995) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Thompson PM, Mackay A, Tollit DJ, Enderby S, Hammond PS. 1998. The influence of body size and sex on the characteristics of harbour seal foraging trips. Can. J. Zool. 76, 1044-1053. ( 10.1139/z98-035) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.van Beest FM, et al. 2019. Classifying grey seal behaviour in relation to environmental variability and commercial fishing activity—a multivariate hidden Markov model. Sci. Rep. 9, 5642. ( 10.1038/s41598-019-42109-w) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.McClintock BT, Russell DJF, Matthiopoulos J, King R. 2013. Combining individual animal movement and ancillary biotelemetry data to investigate population-level activity budgets. Ecology 94, 838-849. ( 10.1890/12-0954.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.McClintock BT, London JM, Cameron MF, Boveng PL. 2017. Bridging the gaps in animal movement: hidden behaviors and ecological relationships revealed by integrated data streams. Ecosphere 8, e01751. ( 10.1002/ecs2.1751) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Russell DJF, et al. 2015. Intrinsic and extrinsic drivers of activity budgets in sympatric grey and harbour seals. Oikos 124, 1462-1472. ( 10.1111/oik.01810) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Carter MID, McClintock BT, Embling CB, Bennett KA, Thompson D, Russell DJF. 2020. From pup to predator: generalized hidden Markov models reveal rapid development of movement strategies in a naive long-lived vertebrate. Oikos 129, 630-642. ( 10.1111/oik.06853) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Zucchini W, MacDonald I. 2009. Hidden markov modelsfor time series: and introduction using R. London, UK: Chapman and Hall. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Calenge C. 2006. The package "adehabitat" for the R software: a tool for the analysis of space and habitat use by animals. Ecol. Modell. 197, 516-519. ( 10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2006.03.017) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Lascelles BG, et al. 2016. Applying global criteria to tracking data to define important areas for marine conservation. Divers. Distrib. 22, 422-431. ( 10.1111/ddi.12411) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bhattacharyya A. 1943. On a measure of divergence between two statistical populations defined by their probability distributions. Bull. Calcutta Math. Soc. 35, 99-109. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Tollit DJ, Black AD, Thompson PM, Mackay A, Corpe HM, Wilson B, Parijs SM, Grellier K, Parlane S. 1998. Variations in harbour seal Phoca vitulina diet and dive-depths in relation to foraging habitat. J. Zool. 244, 209-222. ( 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1998.tb00026.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Vance HM, Hooker SK, Mikkelsen L, van Neer A, Teilmann J, Siebert U, Johnson M. 2021. Drivers and constraints on offshore foraging in harbour seals. Sci. Rep. 11, 6514. ( 10.1038/s41598-021-85376-2) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bailleul F, Pinaud D, Hindell M, Charrassin J-B, Guinet C. 2008. Assessment of scale-dependent foraging behaviour in southern elephant seals incorporating the vertical dimension: a development of the first passage time method. J. Anim. Ecol. 77, 948-957. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2008.01407.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Viviant M, Trites AW, Rosen DAS, Monestiez P, Guinet C. 2010. Prey capture attempts can be detected in steller sea lions and other marine predators using accelerometers. Polar Biol. 33, 713-719. ( 10.1007/s00300-009-0750-y) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Guinet C, Vacquie-Garcia J, Picard B, Bessigneul G, Lebras Y, Dragon AC, Viviant M, Arnould JPY, Bailleul F. 2014. Southern elephant seal foraging success in relation to temperature and light conditions: insight into prey distribution. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 499, 285-301. ( 10.3354/meps10660) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Brasseur S, Aarts G, Meesters E, Petel T, Dijkman E, Cremer J, Reijnders P. 2012. Habitat preferences of harbour seals in the Dutch coastal area: analysis and estimate of effects of offshore wind farms (report no. OWEZ R 252T1 20120130 C043-10). IMARES, Wageningen, The Netherlands, NoordZeeWind.
  • 58.Leos-Barajas V, Photopoulou T, Langrock R, Patterson TA, Watanabe YY, Murgatroyd M, Papastamatiou YP. 2016. Analysis of animal accelerometer data using hidden Markov models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 8, 161-173. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.12657) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Burnham KP, Anderson DR. 2002. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach, 2nd edn. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Hays GC, et al. 2016. Key questions in marine megafauna movement ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 31, 463-475. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2016.02.015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Holyoak M, Casagrandi R, Nathan R, Revilla E, Spiegel O. 2008. Trends and missing parts in the study of movement ecology. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 19060-19065. ( 10.1073/pnas.0800483105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Davoren G. 2013. Distribution of marine predator hotspots explained by persistent areas of prey. Mar. Biol. 160, 3043-3058. ( 10.1007/s00227-013-2294-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Yoshino K, Takahashi A, Adachi T, Costa DP, Robinson PW, Peterson SH, Hückstädt LA, Holser RR, Naito Y. 2020. Acceleration-triggered animal-borne videos show a dominance of fish in the diet of female northern elephant seals. J. Exp. Biol. 223, jeb212936. ( 10.1242/jeb.212936) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hocking DP, Salverson M, Fitzgerald EM, Evans AR. 2014. Australian fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus) use raptorial biting and suction feeding when targeting prey in different foraging scenarios. PLoS ONE 9, e112521. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0112521) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hocking DP, Fitzgerald EMG, Salverson M, Evans AR. 2016. Prey capture and processing behaviors vary with prey size and shape in Australian and subantarctic fur seals. Mar. Mammal Sci. 32, 568-587. ( 10.1111/mms.12285) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Stephens DW, Krebs JR. 1986. Foraging theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Volpov BL, Hoskins AJ, Battaile BC, Viviant M, Wheatley KE, Marshall G, Abernathy K, Arnould JPY. 2015. Identification of prey captures in Australian fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus) using head-mounted accelerometers: field validation with animal-borne video cameras. PLoS ONE 10, e0128789. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0128789) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Goulet P, Guinet C, Swift R, Madsen PT, Johnson M. 2019. A miniature biomimetic sonar and movement tag to study the biotic environment and predator-prey interactions in aquatic animals. Deep-Sea Res. I-Oceanogr. Res. Pap. 148, 1-11. ( 10.1016/j.dsr.2019.04.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Mori Y, Mitani Y, Watanabe Y, Sato K. 2007. A behavioral indicator of prey patch richness derived from diving behavior: the proportion of residence time to the standard time. Isr. J. Ecol. Evol. 53, 347-354. ( 10.1560/IJEE.53.3.347) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Enstipp MR, Gremillet D, Jones DR. 2007. Investigating the functional link between prey abundance and seabird predatory performance. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 331, 267-279. ( 10.3354/meps331267) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Hazen EL, Friedlaender AS, Goldbogen JA. 2015. Blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus) optimize foraging efficiency by balancing oxygen use and energy gain as a function of prey density. Sci. Adv. 1, e1500469. ( 10.1126/sciadv.1500469) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Cordes LS, Thompson PM. 2013. Variation in breeding phenology provides insights into drivers of long-term population change in harbour seals. Proc. Biol. Sci. 280, 20130847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Naves LC, Yves Monnat J, Cam E. 2006. Breeding performance, mate fidelity, and nest site fidelity in a long-lived seabird: behaving against the current? Oikos 115, 263-276. ( 10.1111/j.2006.0030-1299.14883.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Augé AA, Chilvers BL, Moore AB, Davis LS. 2014. Importance of studying foraging site fidelity for spatial conservation measures in a mobile predator. Anim. Conserv. 17, 61-71. ( 10.1111/acv.12056) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Bracis C, Gurarie E, Van Moorter B, Goodwin RA. 2015. Memory effects on movement behavior in animal foraging. PLoS ONE 10, e0136057. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0136057) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Call KA, Ream RR, Johnson D, Sterling JT, Towell RG. 2008. Foraging route tactics and site fidelity of adult female northern fur seal (Callorhinus ursinus) around the Pribilof Islands. Deep-Sea Res. II- Top. Stud. Oceanogr. 55, 1883-1896. ( 10.1016/j.dsr2.2008.04.022) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Grecian WJ, Lane JV, Michelot T, Wade HM, Hamer KC. 2018. Understanding the ontogeny of foraging behaviour: insights from combining marine predator bio-logging with satellite-derived oceanography in hidden Markov models. J. R Soc. Interface 15, 20180084. ( 10.1098/rsif.2018.0084) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Reynolds AM, Cecere JG, Paiva VH, Ramos JA, Focardi S. 2015. Pelagic seabird flight patterns are consistent with a reliance on olfactory maps for oceanic navigation. Proc. Biol. Sci. 282, 20150468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Fagan WF, et al. 2013. Spatial memory and animal movement. Ecol. Lett. 16, 1316-1329. ( 10.1111/ele.12165) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Votier SC, et al. 2017. Effects of age and reproductive status on individual foraging site fidelity in a long-lived marine predator. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20171068. ( 10.1098/rspb.2017.1068) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Sharples RJ, Moss SE, Patterson TA, Hammond PS. 2012. Spatial variation in foraging behaviour of a marine top predator (Phoca vitulina) determined by a large-scale satellite tagging program. PLoS ONE 7, e37216. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0037216) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Thompson PM, McConnell BJ, Tollit DJ, Mackay A, Hunter C, Racey PA. 1996. Comparative distribution, movements and diet of harbour and grey seals from Moray Firth, N. E. Scotland. J. Appl. Ecol. 33, 1572. ( 10.2307/2404795) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Lesage W, Harnmill MO, Kovacs KM. 2004. Long-distance movements of harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) from a seasonally ice-covered area, the St. Lawrence River estuary, Canada. Can. J. Zool. 82, 1070-1081. ( 10.1139/z04-084) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.McNamara JM, Green RF, Olsson O. 2006. Bayes' theorem and its applications in animal behaviour. Oikos 112, 243-251. ( 10.1111/j.0030-1299.2006.14228.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Biernaskie JM, Walker SC, Gegear RJ. 2009. Bumblebees learn to forage like Bayesians. Am. Nat. 174, 413-423. ( 10.1086/603629) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Marshall HH, Carter AJ, Ashford A, Rowcliffe JM, Cowlishaw G. 2013. How do foragers decide when to leave a patch? A test of alternative models under natural and experimental conditions. J. Anim. Ecol. 82, 894-902. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.12089) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Charnov EL. 1976. Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theor. Popul. Biol. 9, 129-136. ( 10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Polansky L, Kilian W, Wittemyer G. 2015. Elucidating the significance of spatial memory on movement decisions by African savannah elephants using state-space models. Proc. Biol. Sci. 282, 20143042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Liukkonen L, Ayllon D, Kunnasranta M, Niemi M, Nabe-Nielsen J, Grimm V, Nyman AM. 2018. Modelling movements of Saimaa ringed seals using an individual-based approach. Ecol. Modell. 368, 321-335. ( 10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2017.12.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Iorio-Merlo V, Graham IM, Hewitt RC, Aarts G, Pirotta E, Hastie GD, Thompson PM. 2022. Data from: Prey encounters and spatial memory influence use of foraging patches in a marine central place forager. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.6q573n601) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Citations

  1. Iorio-Merlo V, Graham IM, Hewitt RC, Aarts G, Pirotta E, Hastie GD, Thompson PM. 2022. Data from: Prey encounters and spatial memory influence use of foraging patches in a marine central place forager. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.6q573n601) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

All data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.6q573n601 [90], and all processing codes are available from the GitHub Repository: https://github.com/virginiaIorio/Drivers-of-seal-ARS-behaviour.


Articles from Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES