Abstract
The cerebellum has been traditionally disregarded in relation to nonmotor functions, but recent findings indicate it may be involved in language, affective processing, and social functions. Mentalizing, or Theory of Mind (ToM), is the ability to infer mental states of others and this skill relies on a distributed network of brain regions. Here, we leveraged large-scale multimodal neuroimaging data to elucidate the structural and functional role of the cerebellum in mentalizing. We used functional activations to determine whether the cerebellum has a domain-general or domain-specific functional role, and effective connectivity and probabilistic tractography to map the cerebello-cerebral mentalizing network. We found that the cerebellum is organized in a domain-specific way and that there is a left cerebellar effective and structural lateralization, with more and stronger effective connections from the left cerebellar hemisphere to the right cerebral mentalizing areas, and greater cerebello-thalamo-cortical and cortico-ponto-cerebellar streamline counts from and to the left cerebellum. Our study provides novel insights to the network organization of the cerebellum, an overlooked brain structure, and mentalizing, one of humans’ most essential abilities to navigate the social world.
Keywords: cerebellum, effective connectivity, human connectome project, structural connectivity, theory of mind
Introduction
The cerebellum has traditionally been considered a motor brain area, involved in the coordination of voluntary movements, gait, posture, and speech (Glickstein 1992, 1993; Brodal and Bjaalie 1997; Fine et al. 2002; Ito 2002). In more recent years there has been a newfound interest in nonmotor functions of the cerebellum (Leiner et al. 1986; Schmahmann and Pandya 1989, 1997; Schmahmann 1991, 1998; Middleton and Strick 1994). This work has clinical grounding in findings showing that cerebellar damage from stroke or injury could at times cause the “Cerebellar Cognitive Affective Syndrome” which includes language deficits such as mutism, dysprosodia, and anagrammatism, problems with executive functioning, as well as personality changes, blunting of affect, and inappropriate social behavior (Schmahmann and Sherman 1998). More strikingly, resection of the most common type of pediatric brain tumors which are found in the cerebellum, at times results in dramatic and long-lasting changes in cognition as well as personality, affect, and overall social behavior (Albazron et al. 2019).
Buckner et al. (2011) used resting-state analyses at the group level and showed, that the cerebellum has a distinct mapping of all the large-scale networks found in the cerebrum with each resting-state network represented thrice, specifically in lobules VI-Crus I, Crus II-VIIb, and lobule IX. Guell et al. (2018a) confirmed and extended Buckner’s demonstration using task-based fMRI data from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) dataset. Lastly, King et al. (2019) used a multidomain task battery and derived a comprehensive functional parcellation of the cerebellar cortex. We can distill these findings into a few take-homes: (1) motor functions are localized to the anterior lobe and inferior posterior lobe; cognitive functions to the superior posterior lobe; emotion/affect potentially to the vermis; (2) there are finer-scale functional subregions within each lobe; and (3) the functional subregions do not neatly follow sulcal/gyral boundaries.
Early studies in nonhuman animals found axonal connections between portions of the parietal and superior aspects of the temporal lobe and the cerebellum (Glickstein et al. 1985; Schmahmann and Pandya 1991). Subsequent studies provided insight to the cerebello-cerebral white matter connectivity profile. These studies found that the white matter connections of this structural network are predominately contralateral and form closed loops (Kelly and Strick 2003). The cerebral cortex projects to areas of the contralateral cerebellum via the pons, and then receives input from the same cerebellar areas which project back to the cerebral cortex via the thalamus, bypassing the pons. More recent animal studies confirmed this finding but also found a small number of ipsilateral cerebello-cerebral connections (Suzuki et al. 2012). These findings were confirmed by diffusion imaging research in humans which showed that contralateral connections comprise ~70–80% of cerebello-cerebral white matter connectivity with the remainder being ipsilateral (Salmi et al. 2010; Sokolov et al. 2014).
What is the best way to describe the role of the cerebellum in social cognition? Van Overwalle et al. (2015a; 2015b) performed meta-analyses on fMRI data and showed that portions of the cerebellum are consistently activated in mentalizing tasks. Mentalizing, or Theory of Mind (ToM), is the ability to attribute mental states to others, and to interpret their intentions, perspectives, and beliefs (Frith and Frith 2006; Blakemore 2008; Mar 2011; Schurz et al. 2014) and is an essential social skill. This finding is interesting because there has been a concerted effort in social neuroscience over the last 20 years to identify brain areas involved in mentalizing. Researchers have found that the extended “mentalizing network” is comprised of portions of the temporoparietal junction, precuneus, amygdala, anterior temporal lobe, occipital gyrus, fusiform gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, and medial prefrontal cortex (Gobbini et al. 2007; Uddin et al. 2007; Lahnakoski et al. 2012; Bzdok et al. 2013, 2015; Jack and Pelphrey 2015; Yang et al. 2015; Hartwright et al. 2016) and these regions are interconnected by a system of white matter pathways (Wang et al. 2021). However, with the exception of work by Van Overwalle et al. (2015b; 2016; 2017; 2019b; 2019c, 2020c; Heleven et al. 2021), the cerebellum has been largely ignored by researchers interested in mentalizing and it is not included in the classic mentalizing network.
In sum, the lesion findings are compelling, but they do little to illuminate what part of the cerebellum is involved in social and affective behavior. The neuroimaging findings are scarce, with few in-depth studies looking at large scale networks, and even fewer using large sample sizes, so we lack an understanding of how the cerebellum interacts with social and affective regions in the cerebrum. In the present study, we use a large multimodal neuroimaging dataset to investigate the cerebellar foundations of mentalizing. Using clustering methods, effective connectivity, and diffusion imaging with probabilistic tractography we show that the cerebellum is divided into distinct functional parcels, strongly connected to individual tasks. Furthermore, we show that there are mentalizing cerebellar areas effectively connected specifically to regions of the cerebral cortex that have a known role in mentalizing. Last, some—but not all—portions of this functional network are supported by an underlying white matter network.
Materials and Methods
Dataset and Participants
All data used in this study are part of the HCP dataset, specifically the WU-Minn HCP Consortium S900 Release (WU-Minn HCP Consortium 2015). This dataset is publicly available, accessible at https://www.humanconnectome.org. Only subjects that completed all imaging sessions of interest (T1/T2, task fMRI (tfMRI), and diffusion magnetic resonance imaging (dMRI)) were included in this study. To reduce variance in structural organization (McKay et al. 2017) we restricted our population to only right-handed subjects using the Edinburgh Handedness questionnaire (Oldfield 1971), which resulted in 679 healthy young adults. Additionally, 7 subjects were excluded due to lack of enough robust signal in all bilateral regions of interest (ROIs) in the mentalizing localizer task, and one more subject for missing all the explanatory variable files with timing information necessary for psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analyses, thus leading to a final sample of 671 healthy young adults (377 females, M = 28.8, SD = 3.7). Unless otherwise stated, all significant results reported in this study were corrected for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995).
Overview of HCP Protocol
Due to the complexity of the HCP data acquisition and preprocessing pipelines, listing all scanning protocols and data analysis procedures in detail is beyond the scope of this article. Full detailed description of all protocols and procedures can be found elsewhere (Van Essen et al. 2012; Barch et al. 2013; Glasser et al. 2013; Smith et al. 2013). Briefly, the HCP protocol includes acquisition of structural MRI, resting-state and task-state fMRI, dMRI, and extensive behavioral testing. Task-state fMRI encompasses 7 major domains: (1) social cognition (mentalizing); (2) motor (visual, motion, somatosensory, and motor systems); (3) gambling; (4) working memory/cognitive control systems and category specific representations; (5) language processing (semantic and phonological processing); (6) relational processing; and (7) emotion processing (Barch et al. 2013). The imaging data used in this article are the “minimally preprocessed” included in the WU-Minn HCP Consortium S900 Release (WU-Minn HCP Consortium 2015). Details of imaging protocols, preprocessing pipelines, and in-scanner task protocols can be found in the Supplementary Material.
As the main task of interest, participants in the mentalizing task viewed 20s video clips of geometrical shapes that either interacted with each other in a socially meaningful way or moved purposelessly on the screen. The video clips of this Heider and Simmel-type task were developed by Castelli et al. (2000) and Wheatley et al. (2007) and have been validated as a measure of mentalizing given evidence that they generate task-related activation in brain regions associated with mentalizing with reliable results across subjects (Castelli et al. 2000, 2002; Gobbini et al. 2007; Wheatley et al. 2007; White et al. 2011; Barch et al. 2013; Jack and Pelphrey 2015). Both the emotion and the mentalizing task require some degree of mentalizing. The mentalizing task asked participants to implement mental state attributions when watching the geometrical shapes interact, hence the mentalizing is intentional. On the other hand, in the emotion task (Hariri et al. 2006; Barch et al. 2013), participants are presented with blocks of trials that either ask them to decide which of 2 faces presented on the bottom of the screen match the face at the top of the screen, or which of 2 shapes presented at the bottom of the screen match the shape at the top of the screen. The faces have either an angry or fearful expression. Since the instruction and goal of the task is matching the bottom faces to the one on the top, participants only implicitly attribute mental states to the faces they view, hence the emotion task has a far smaller degree of mentalizing (Van Overwalle and Vandekerckhove 2013; Kliemann and Adolphs 2018).
Regions of Interest
We used 3 sets of ROIs for our analyses: 1 set in the cerebrum and 2 in the cerebellum. The set of cerebral brain regions were drawn from prior work on mentalizing (Schurz et al. 2014; Wang et al. 2021) thus the ROIs included the anterior temporal lobe (ATL; Olson et al. 2007; Ross and Olson 2010; Wang et al. 2017), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC; Van Overwalle 2009; Van Overwalle and Baetens 2009; Van Overwalle and Vandekerckhove 2013; Schurz and Perner 2015), precuneus (PreC; Cavanna and Trimble 2006; Uddin et al. 2007; Schiller et al. 2009; Schurz et al. 2014; Peer et al. 2015), temporoparietal junction (TPJ; Van Overwalle 2009; Koster-Hale and Saxe 2013), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC; (Molenberghs et al. 2016; Koster-Hale et al. 2017). Although other brain areas support the mentalizing network, we focused on regions that have been reliably and consistently implicated across various social tasks (Schurz et al. 2014) and are considered to be involved in basic, core social processes. To ensure sensitivity within each individual, we defined individual-specific ROIs using the social cognition task as a mentalizing ROI localizer. To precisely localize each mentalizing ROI, the social cognition task was processed on the “grayordinate-based” space (cortical surface vertices and subcortical voxels) using the MSM-All registration (Robinson et al. 2014). We used the Connectome Workbench software (Marcus et al. 2011) to manually extract the vertices (which were later transformed to MNI coordinates) of the peak activations of the bilateral 10 predefined ROIs (as well as their magnitudes) from the contrast “ToM > random” (socially meaningful interaction of the shapes > random movement of the shapes) for each individual separately. First, we loaded file [subjectID].MSMAll.32k_fs_LR.wb.spec and functional map [subjectID]_tfMRI_SOCIAL_level2_hp200_s4_MSMAll.dscalar.nii on Connectome Workbench and selected to view only the positive data on the map. We then increased the threshold manually until only the peak activation was visible for each mentalizing area of interest. Subsequently, the coordinates of each peak activation for each mentalizing area of interest and each subject were extracted. Twelve people worked in independent pairs to extract all mentalizing peak coordinates (4 people on the ATL, 4 on the PreC, DMPFC, and VMPFC, and 4 on the TPJ). After everyone extracted all coordinates, an independent researcher confirmed the reliability of the results and made adjustments whenever there was a coordinate discrepancy. These individual-specific cluster peak coordinates were used as input (spheres, 6 mm radius) for subsequent seed-based brain connectivity analyses at the individual level (PPI and probabilistic tractography). It is important to note that, based on the meta-analysis by Schurz et al. (2014), which showed that activation from the social or intentional interactions > physical movements contrast spans broadly on the TPJ, the TPJ was defined broadly, falling within the inferior parietal lobule (IPL, i.e., perspective taking) and/or posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS, i.e., biological motion perception). In the event that the TPJ had multiple clusters (falling either into what is traditionally considered to be the TPJ, or IPL or pSTS), we selected the cluster with the largest effect size, which represents the mean signal change in the ROI (z-score from the peak voxel) across all subjects, to avoid excessive intersubject inconsistency.
For the ROIs in the cerebellum, we used a different approach. The cerebellum has uniform cytoarchitecture and although several studies have attempted to functionally map the cerebellar cortex (Krienen and Buckner 2009; Buckner et al. 2011; Diedrichsen and Zotow 2015; Riedel et al. 2015; Marek et al. 2018; Guell et al. 2018a; 2018b; King et al. 2019), there is no consensus about functional boundaries within the cerebellum. Hence, it was impossible to employ the same approach as we did with the predefined cerebral ROIs. Instead, we used a data-driven approach. Following the method used by Guell et al. (2018a), we transformed individual level 2 cope files (results of within-subject fixed-effects grayordinate-based analyses which generate output files that index mean effects for an individual subject averaged across the 2 scan runs for a task) into Cohen’s d group maps by first transforming the grayordinate.dscalar.nii files to NIfTI. We then used FSL commands fslselectvols to extract the contrast of interest “ToM > random” for each individual, and fslmerge, fsmaths -Tmean, -Tstd, and -div to merge the individual contrast images, extract the mean, and the standard deviation, and divide the two, ultimately getting group Cohen’s d maps for the contrasts “ToM > random” (mentalizing), “faces > shapes” (emotion), “story > math” (language), “2-back > 0-back” (working memory), “reward > punishment” (gambling), “relational > match” (relational processing), and “average” (motor) based on our 671 subjects. The HCP S900 Release provides level 3 group z-maps, but Cohen’s d maps made it possible to observe the effect size of each task contrast rather than the significance of the BOLD signal change. A sample of 671 subjects ensures that a d value higher than 0.5 (Cohen 1988) will be statistically significant even after correction for multiple comparisons (d = z/sqrt(n), d > 0.5 we have z > 12.95 for N = 671; analysis of 17 853 cerebellar voxels would require P < 0.000028 after Bonferroni correction, and P < 0.000028 is equivalent to z > 4.026). Accordingly, we used FSL’s cluster tool, the Cohen’s d maps, and a threshold of 0.5 to extract clusters of activation for each task and local maxima within each cluster. After using a whole cerebellar mask to retain only the clusters and local maxima within the cerebellum, clusters smaller than 100 mm3 were further removed in order to omit very small clusters that were considered to be noninformative and would make a comprehensive description of the results too extensive. The coordinates of the remainder local maxima from the “ToM > random” (mentalizing) contrast within the cerebellum were used to create group cerebellar ROIs (spheres, 6 mm radius). Eleven mentalizing ROIs were created in the left cerebellar hemisphere and 7 ROIs were created in the right cerebellar hemisphere. The same method was used to extract the cerebellar motor ROIs which were used in our effective and structural connectivity control analyses. Seven motor ROIs were created in the left cerebellar hemisphere and 7 were created in the right cerebellar hemisphere.
Cluster Overlap and Euclidean Distances
Given that the HCP dataset uses FNIRT registration to the MNI template, we calculated the percentage of overlap of each cerebellar cluster, in reference to the cerebellar lobules, by using Diedrichsen’s FNIRT MNI maximum probability map (Diedrichsen et al. 2009). It has been shown though that functional parcellation of the cerebellum does not match its anatomical parcellation into lobules (King et al. 2019). Because of this, we created an atlas of cerebellar lobes (anterior, posterior, flocculonodular, and vermis) by combining the lobules from Diedrichsen’s FNIRT MNI maximum probability map (Diedrichsen et al. 2009) that belong in each lobe and used these maps for our analyses. We used FSL’s atlasq tool to determine the percentage of overlap of each cerebellar cluster to the cerebellar lobes, hence determining the primary location of each cluster. The Sørensen–Dice coefficient, which is a statistic measuring the similarity of 2 samples (Dice 1945; Sørensen 1948), was then used to calculate the percentage of overlap between the functional clusters generated from all tasks and determine their similarity, and Euclidean distances were calculated to estimate the distances of local maxima within and between clusters. At the individual level, we thresholded Z-scored β-weights of each subject’s activation map for each task contrast to >0 to retain only increased activation during the tasks and then ran Wilcoxon signed-rank tests between each task pair to examine whether there was a statistical difference between them.
Psychophysiological Interaction Analyses
PPI analyses were used to understand effective connectivity—a way to capture stimulus-driven patterns of directional influence among neural areas (Friston et al. 1997)—by identifying brain regions whose activity depends on an interaction between psychological context (the task) and physiological state (the time course of brain activity) of the seed region (O’Reilly et al. 2012; Gerchen et al. 2014; Smith et al. 2016). A PPI analysis allows you to go beyond an investigation into which individual brain areas are involved in a task, by measuring whether brain regions exchange information (Stephan 2004; Friston 2011; Smith et al. 2012) and how they change their connectivity to become part of different networks depending on the task or behavioral occasion (Cacioppo and Decety 2011; Smith et al. 2012). A standard PPI analysis does not allow inferences to be made about the directionality of information flow, but rather it detects increases in the relationship between a seed region of interest and the rest of the brain which are specific to the task. The measurement of these increases are in terms of the strength of regression of activity in one region on another (O’Reilly et al. 2012). We built a generalized PPI model (McLaren et al. 2012) using a nondeconvolution method (Di and Biswal 2017) in FSL for each cerebellar and cerebral 6 mm ROI. We used the “minimally preprocessed” tfMRI data without any additional denoising. Our model had 5 separate regressors: 2 psychological regressors of task events (mental interaction and random interaction), one physiological regressor of the time series of the seed ROIs and 2 corresponding interaction regressors (task events × seed ROI’s time series). To estimate the effective connectivity, we used the contrast between the interaction regressors (PPI mental > PPI random). Z-scored β-weights were extracted, using FSL’s fslmeants tool, for each pair of cerebellar to contralateral cerebral ROIs, which resulted in an 11 × 5 matrix for the left cerebellar-right cerebral hemispheres and a 7 × 5 matrix for the right cerebellar–left cerebral hemispheres for each individual. We applied symmetrization to the matrices by averaging the Z-scored β-weights of each pair of cerebro-cerebellar and cerebello-cerebral ROIs (Tompson et al. 2020). At the group level of the effective connectivity, one-sample t-tests were performed across individuals at each pair of ROIs to detect any significant effective connections. What we expected to learn from this analysis is which cerebellar mentalizing regions present significant coupling with contralateral cerebral mentalizing regions due to mental state attribution (mentalizing task). The same effective connectivity analyses were performed between the mentalizing cerebral ROIs and motor (control) cerebellar ROIs.
Diffusion Analyses
Probabilistic tractography analyses were performed using FSL’s probtrackx2 (probabilistic tracking with crossing fibers) (Behrens et al. 2003, 2007) in each subject’s native space and then the results were transformed to MNI standard space. An ROI-to-ROI approach was used with cerebellar and contralateral cerebral ROIs used as seeds and targets to reconstruct each subject’s cerebello-cerebral white matter connections. Fiber tracking was initialized in both directions separately (from seed to target and vice versa) with modified euler streamlining, 0.2 curvature threshold, 2000 steps per sample, and 5000 streamlines were drawn from each voxel in each ROI. Tractographies were performed to delineate the cerebello-thalamo-cortical (CTC) (Middleton and Strick 1997; Schmahmann and Pandya 1997; Palesi et al. 2017) and cortico-ponto-cerebellar (CPC) (Ramnani 2006; Palesi et al. 2017) between left/right cerebellar hemispheres and right/left cerebral hemispheres. For the CTC tractographies, a binarized mask of the superior cerebellar peduncle in MNI space from the Johns Hopkins University ICBM-DTI-81 white matter labels atlas (Mori et al. 2006; Wakana et al. 2007; Hua et al. 2018) was used as a waypoint, while the binarized contralateral cerebellar and cerebral hemispheres were set as exclusion masks (Example: CTC tractography between a left cerebellar ROI and a right cerebral ROI would entail a left superior cerebellar peduncle waypoint, a right cerebellar hemisphere exclusion mask, and a left cerebral hemisphere exclusion mask). For the CPC tractographies, a binarized mask of the middle cerebellar peduncle in MNI space from the same atlas was used as a waypoint, and the contralateral cerebellar and cerebral hemispheres were used as exclusion masks (Example: CPC tractography between a left cerebellar ROI and a right cerebral ROI would entail the middle cerebellar peduncle waypoint mask, a right cerebellar hemisphere exclusion mask, and a left cerebral hemisphere exclusion mask). We chose this method based on the prior tractography work of Palesi et al. (2015, 2017). The pons was not selected as an inclusion mask due to lack of a pons mask in standardized space through a standardized atlas. We also chose not to include the thalamus as a waypoint mask for the CTC pathway to keep the CTC and CPC tractography methods as similar as possible. Using probabilistic tractography, we recreated the same white matter pathways between motor (control) cerebellar ROIs and mentalizing cerebral ROIs. The number of streamlines for each path was obtained and used as a way to quantify and estimate white matter connectivity (Jones et al. 2013).
Results
Functional Domains in the Cerebellum
We first examined whether regions of the cerebellum have domain-specific functions. At the group level, we extracted clusters of activation from the group activation maps of each task contrast (Fig. 1A) and local maxima (Fig. 1B) for each task. The Sørensen–Dice coefficient was used to calculate the overlap between the functional clusters of all tasks. Euclidean distances were then calculated to estimate the distances of local maxima within and between clusters. Lastly, at the individual level, we performed Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for each task pair to examine whether there was a statistical difference between them. All task contrasts generated activation clusters at the Cohen’s d > 0.5 threshold, except for the gambling task which was, subsequently, excluded from all analyses. Percentage of overlap between functional clusters from the tasks and cerebellar lobes are presented in Supplementary Table 1. Consistent with previous findings, the motor task was the only task that activated the anterior lobe (lobules I–VI), whereas emotion and mentalizing were the only tasks showing activation in the vermis (Gao et al. 2018; Albazron et al. 2019; Brady et al. 2019; Watson et al. 2019). Interestingly, mentalizing and emotion tasks also activated the flocculonodular lobe (lobule X), an area of the cerebellum known to regulate saccadic eye movements and balance (Cohen and Highstein 1972; Ito 1982; Schniepp et al. 2017). A detailed description of the overlap of each cluster of activation and each lobule is presented in Supplementary Table 2.
Percentage of overlap between task contrast activation clusters, calculated using the Sørensen–Dice coefficient, is presented in Table 1. The mentalizing and language clusters shared almost half of their voxels (45.17%), the mentalizing and emotion clusters share 18.66%, and mentalizing and working memory clusters shared 11.76%, while working memory shared voxels with motor and relational processing. The language and motor clusters were the only ones with zero overlap. The remainder task contrast cluster pairs had <10% overlap. To further examine the relationship between the task contrast activation clusters, we extracted the local maxima within each cluster and then calculated the mean Euclidean distance of the local maxima within each cluster and between clusters. About 96.6% of Euclidean distances were > 8.5 mm (whole sample of Euclidean distances M = 39.01 mm, SD = 8.41 mm) which means that only 78 pairs of local maxima, out of a total of 2278 local maxima pairs, were proximal. Out of the 78, only 58 were Euclidean distances of local maxima belonging to different clusters, bringing the percentage of local maxima that are proximal, yet belong to clusters of different task contrasts, to 2.55%. Despite shared activation, group-level Euclidean distances results indicate that overlapping clusters have distinct local maxima. These compelling findings at the group level require additional support from individual-level analyses to make a claim of generality vs specificity for cerebellar function.
Table 1.
Task | Emotion | Language | Motor | Relational processing | Mentalizing | Working memory |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Emotion | ||||||
Language | 0.09 | |||||
Motor | 1.08 | 0 | ||||
Relational processing | 8.37 | 0.63 | 1.09 | |||
Mentalizing | 18.66 | 45.17 | 5.50 | 2.89 | ||
Working memory | 7.50 | 0.41 | 20.99 | 19.61 | 11.76 |
At the single subject level, we extracted the averaged cerebellar activations for each task (i.e., across all cerebellar voxels with positive task contrast weights). Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests indicated that the task contrast-weights across subjects did not follow a normal distribution for all 6 tasks (P’s < 0.001; Supplementary Table 3) hence we used Spearman’s rho to examine the strength and direction of association between each task, and the Wilcoxon signed-rank test to examine whether there was a statistical difference in averaged cerebellar activation between each pair of tasks. We found significant but relatively weak positive correlations (P’s < 0.25) between emotion–language, emotion–relational, emotion–mentalizing, working memory–motor, working memory–language, and relational–working memory activations (Supplementary Table 4). Wilcoxon signed-rank tests showed that there was a statistically significant difference between contrast-weights of all task pairs (Supplementary Table 5) except for the relational-emotion pair. So, despite the correlation of some task pairs, which can be attributed to the few voxels they share, the cerebellar activation of each task is significantly different from each other task. Together, the group-level and individual-level findings suggest that the cerebellum functions in a domain-specific way with distinct cerebellar areas devoted to distinct cognitive functions.
Functional Interactions between the Cerebellum and Cerebrum
Do mentalizing regions in the cerebellum functionally interact with mentalizing regions in the cerebrum? To measure this, we created 6 mm spherical ROIs based on each mentalizing and motor cerebellar local maximum and on individual-subject peak activations of the mentalizing task for the ATL, DMPFC, PreC, TPJ, and VMPFC. We used PPI to examine the effective connectivity between the cerebellar and contralateral cerebral mentalizing areas and for the control analysis, cerebellar motor, and contralateral cerebral mentalizing areas. This resulted in 110 mentalizing connections between the left cerebellum and right cerebrum (55 cerebellum-to-cerebrum e.g., L_VIIB_1 to R_ATL, L_VIIB_1 to R_DMPFC, 55 cerebrum-to-cerebellum e.g., R_ATL to L_VIIB_1, R_DMPFC to L_VIIB_1) (see Fig. 2 for cerebellar local maxima/ROI naming convention) and 70 connections between the right cerebellum and left cerebrum (35 CTC, 35 CPC). We extracted Z-scored β-weights for each pair of cerebellar to contralateral cerebral ROIs for each subject, averaged them, and at the group level we performed one-sample t-tests for each pair of ROIs to detect significant effective connections.
Each local maximum was represented in both cerebellar hemispheres and named accordingly (e.g., L_VIIB_1—R_VIIB_2) with the exception of left hemisphere local maxima L_VI_2, L_VI_3, Vermis_IX_1 which did not have the right cerebellar equivalent local maxima. In some cases, 2 local maxima were found in close proximity (pairs L_IX_1-L_IX_2 (Euclidean distance = 8.485), L_X_1-L_X_2 (Euclidean distance = 7.21), L_VI_2-L_VI_3 (Euclidean distance = 4.90), and R_VIIB_1-R_VIIB_2 (Euclidean distance = 6.33). In order to understand whether these proximal ROIs are functionally distinct, we ran paired samples t-tests (or Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for connections that did not follow a normal distribution) on individual β-weights for all connections between the proximal ROIs (e.g., paired t-test comparing L_IX_1-ATL to L_IX_2-ATL). Effective connectivity results for all R_VIIB_1-R_VIIB_2 pairs were not significant but we found significant effective connectivity differences for 75% of proximal left ROI pairs (Supplementary Tables 6 and 7). These findings suggest that there is significantly different effective connectivity between mentalizing cerebellar and cerebral ROIs despite close proximity of the cerebellar local maxima.
Overall, we found more connections (Fig. 3) and stronger effective connectivity between the left cerebellum and right cerebrum compared to vice versa. Local maxima on each of the left cerebellar lobes were connected to all 5 right mentalizing cerebral areas. Οn the left cerebellar hemisphere all local maxima ROIs were significantly connected to 3 or more right cerebral ROIs, with the exception of Vermis_IX_1 on the flocculonodular node which was only connected to the right ATL and DMPFC. The right DMPFC, ATL, and TPJ displayed significant connections with most left cerebellar ROIs (10 out of 11 left cerebellar ROIs for the DMPFC and 9 out of 11 for the ATL and TPJ). The right cerebellar hemisphere displayed a different pattern of connectivity. With the exception of R_VIIB_2 and R_VIIB_1, which combined were significantly connected to all left cerebral ROIs, all other right cerebellar ROIs were significantly functionally connected to either one or two left cerebral ROIs. The left TPJ was the brain area which displayed significant effective connectivity with all right cerebellar ROIs, except R_VIIB_3. Combining the significant connections of all the right cerebellar ROIs, we find that the right superior posterior, inferior posterior, and flocculonodular lobes display significant connectivity to all 5 left mentalizing brain areas. The right superior posterior cerebellar lobe is significantly connected to all 5 left cerebral brain areas when we combine the significant connectivity results of all its cerebellar ROIs (R_VIIB_2-R_VIIB_3), but the right inferior posterior lobe is only connected to the left ATL and TPJ, and the right flocculonodular lobe is only connected to the left TPJ (Fig. 3).
To test the specificity of these functional connections, we ran PPI analyses between cerebellar motor ROIs and the contralateral mentalizing cerebral ones and performed the same analyses as above. We found that, with minor exceptions, cerebellar motor to cerebral mentalizing effective connections were not significant (P’s > 0.06; Supplementary Tables 8 and 9). Additionally, we ran a two-sample binomial proportion test between the mentalizing and motor effective connections for both cerebellar hemispheres and found that the proportion of mentalizing–mentalizing significant connections differed significantly from the proportion of motor–mentalizing connections with significantly more mentalizing–mentalizing connections in both cerebellar hemispheres (Left cerebellum to right cerebrum: z = 4.2, P < 0.0001, Right cerebellum to left cerebrum: z = 2.7, P < 0.01).
These results indicate that there is a clear functional connection between regions sensitive to mentalizing in the cerebellum and regions sensitive to mentalizing in the cerebrum. There is some lateralization of function with the left cerebellar lobes communicating with all right mentalizing cerebral areas whereas the right cerebellum displays more limited connectivity. In addition, all left cerebellar local maxima were functionally connected to the right DMPFC, but only one right local maximum in the cerebellum was (weakly) connected to the left DMPFC. Notably, local maxima in both cerebellar hemispheres showed strongest connectivity to the TPJ.
Structural Connections between the Cerebellum and Cerebrum
Are cerebellar mentalizing areas structurally connected to cerebral mentalizing areas? To test this, we ran probabilistic tractography to reconstruct the CTC and CPC white matter pathways. We used the same 6 mm spherical ROIs from the PPI analyses as seeds and targets for the tractographies, the superior cerebellar peduncle as a waypoint for the CTC white matter pathway, and the middle cerebellar peduncle for the CPC white matter pathway (Palesi et al. 2015, 2017). We then extracted streamline counts for each pathway (Supplementary Tables 10 and 11).
Overall, the right/left CTC and right/left CPC pathways showed similar connectivity patterns and average streamline counts (Fig. 4). We combined the individual average streamline counts to create group average streamline counts for the superior posterior, inferior posterior, and flocculonodular lobes for the right and left CTC and CPC pathways (Table 2). We used Spearman’s rho (the white matter streamline counts did not follow a normal distribution; P’s < 0.001; Supplementary Table 12) to examine the strength and direction of association between left and right CTCs and CPCs, and the Wilcoxon signed-rank test to examine whether there was a statistical difference between them. For both the CTC and CPC pathways we found that the right superior posterior lobe streamline count is correlated to the left superior posterior, inferior, and flocculonodular lobe streamlines counts but it shows greater correlation to the left superior posterior lobe. The same applies for the left superior posterior lobe streamline counts and the inferior posterior and flocculonodular lobe streamline counts for both the right and left cerebellar hemispheres (Supplementary Tables 13 and 14). Wilcoxon signed-rank tests showed that there was a statistically significant difference in streamline counts between each lobe pair (e.g., left superior posterior—right superior posterior) for CTC and CPC tracks, with the majority of subjects having significantly larger CTC streamline counts in the right superior posterior lobe rather than the left, but significantly smaller streamline counts in the right inferior and flocculonodular lobes compared to the respective left. The exact same pattern was observed in the CPC tracks. We also found that the overall left CTC streamline count was significantly larger compared to the right CTC streamline count and the same was true for the CPC streamline counts. Additionally, we found that CTC streamline counts (from the left and right cerebellar hemispheres) were significantly larger compared to the ipsilateral CPC streamline counts (Supplementary Table 15). CTC pathways from both the left and right cerebellar hemisphere and toward all 5 contralateral mentalizing cerebral ROIs displayed the exact same streamline count pattern (DMPFC>VMPFC>PreC>ATL > TPJ). The CPC pathways displayed a different pattern. In the CPC pathways projecting to the left cerebellar hemisphere, the right TPJ was the brain area with the second largest streamline count toward all left cerebellar ROIs. Also, in the CPC pathways projecting to the right cerebellar hemisphere, the left VMPFC displayed disproportionally smaller streamline counts toward all right cerebellar ROIs compared both to the left CPC pattern and both CTC patterns (Fig. 4; Supplementary Tables 10 and 11). Overall, the largest streamline count was both to and from the DMPFC.
Table 2.
Streamline count | Streamline count | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Left CTC | 35.52 | (72.83) | Right CTC | 32.76 | (65.63) |
Left superior posterior lobe | 24.27 | (53.58) | Right superior posterior lobe | 32.26 | (66.79) |
Left inferior posterior lobe | 51.04 | (95.21) | Right inferior posterior lobe | 22.04 | (44.38) |
Left flocculonodular lobe | 46.02 | (78.17) | Right flocculonodular lobe | 45.99 | (47.81) |
Left CPC | 19.46 | (49.33) | Right CPC | 18.73 | 44.76 |
Left superior posterior lobe | 15.56 | (35.55) | Right superior posterior lobe | 17.34 | (36.14) |
Left inferior posterior lobe | 5.77 | (12.44) | Right inferior posterior lobe | 1.36 | (3.62) |
Left flocculonodular lobe | 51.69 | (86.19) | Right flocculonodular lobe | 43.10 | (81.07) |
The average streamline counts of whole mentalizing CTC and CPC pathways (streamlines of all local maxima combined) are in bold.
To examine the specificity of these white matter connections we ran probabilistic tractographies between the motor cerebellar areas and contralateral mentalizing cerebral ones. Results showed that the white matter pathways from the motor cerebellar areas to the mentalizing cerebral areas had low streamline counts (Supplementary Table 16) and Wilcoxon signed-rank test comparing the mentalizing–mentalizing and motor–mentalizing connections showed that the mentalizing–mentalizing white matter pathways had significantly larger streamline counts in each cerebellar lobe but also overall for the CTC and CPC (P’s < 0.001; Supplementary Table 17).
These results indicate that there are structural connections between the mentalizing cerebellar and cerebral areas. The feedforward (CTC) and feedback (CPC) pathway streamline counts between the right and left cerebellar lobes differed significantly. The right cerebellar superior posterior lobe had a larger streamline count than its left counterpart, but the right inferior and flocculonodular lobes had smaller streamline counts compared to their left counterparts. Additionally, there was a left lateralization in white matter connectivity with the overall left CTC streamline count being significantly larger compared to the right CTC streamline count, and the same being true for the CPC streamline counts. Of all ROIs in the classic mentalizing network, the DMPFC had the largest streamline count in both directions.
Discussion
In this study, we explored whether the cerebellum plays a role in mentalizing. We used a large-scale dataset and mapped out the cerebellum’s functional organization, functional connectivity, and structural connectivity. First, we investigated whether there is functional specificity in the cerebellum for mentalizing. To assess this, we looked at activation overlap between mentalizing activations and 5 other task activations: emotion processing, language, motor, visual relational processing, and working memory. There was sizeable overlap between mentalizing, emotion, and language. The mentalizing–emotion overlap occurred in the vermis and bilateral flocculonodular lobes (Fig. 1) and was anticipated. In nonhuman animals the vermis and flocculonodular lobes are structurally connected to the amygdala, hippocampus, septum, and hypothalamus (Heath and Harper 1974; Snider et al. 1976; Hu et al. 2008) thus these regions have long been thought of as the limbic system of the cerebellum (Schmahmann, 1991). Vermal stimulation in mice, cats, and primates causes cells to fire in these regions (Babb et al. 1974; Berman, 1997; Bobée et al. 2000; Zanchetti and Zoccolini 1954). There is suggestive evidence that deep-brain stimulation over the vermis can ameliorate aggression in human patients (Heath et al. 1979). Damage to the vermis can lead to atypical social and emotional behaviors (Schmahmann 1991; Pollack 1997; Schmahmann and Sherman 1998; Levisohn et al. 2000; Tavano et al. 2007; Manto and Mariën 2015) and some neuroimaging studies have reported that the vermis and flocculonodular lobes are active during emotion learning or change of affect tasks (Lane et al. 1997; Beauregard et al. 1998; Gündel et al. 2003) and social processing (Van Overwalle et al. 2014; Guell et al. 2018a). This overlap also makes sense given that these tasks contained overlapping features and processes. For instance, the emotion task involved some degree of implicit mentalizing because participants were required to match 2 faces that were presented with either an angry or fearful expression thus implicitly attributing emotion (Wang et al. 2021). One could also argue that language tasks containing any sort of narrative require mentalizing (for a meta-analysis see Van Overwalle et al. 2020a). The language task in the HCP dataset has an unusually high mentalizing load since the stimuli were variants of Aesop’s fables, which involved animals or humans interacting in social situations (Binder et al. 2011). Whether language tasks that are less social—for instance, single word processing—would show overlap with mentalizing activations in the cerebellum is not known.
It is important to note that task activation clusters and their overlap are heavily dependent on thresholding decisions. Guell et al. (2018a) also used the HCP S900 dataset to examine the task representations on the cerebellum but they analyzed the 2 mm smoothed fMRI data (here we used 4 mm smoothed data). Although their task clustering results are similar to ours, the cluster overlaps were smaller, with overlap found solely between the mentalizing and language tasks. They conclude that the cerebellum has domain-specific representations of different kinds of cognition and emotion. We believe this is a reasonable conclusion, but we wanted to provide additional evidence which would strengthen the claim to a domain-specific cerebellar role. The task cluster overlaps led us to explore in more depth the relationship between the activation clusters. Our Euclidean distance analysis showed that each cluster had distinct local maxima. We also analyzed the strength of cerebellar activation for each task at the individual level and found that each task elicited different degrees of cerebellar activations across subjects. Overall, our results provide support for the hypothesis that the cerebellum has domain-specific functions.
Second, we reasoned that if mentalizing clusters in the cerebellum truly play a role in mentalizing behavior, they should show effective connectivity with known mentalizing regions in the cerebrum. We tested this and found that (1) there was strong effective connectivity with all mentalizing regions in the cerebral cortex (Fig. 3); and (2) there were laterality differences, with clusters in the left cerebellum displaying more and stronger effective connections to the right cerebral mentalizing areas, compared to the opposite (right cerebellar–left cerebral hemisphere effective connections). These results are consistent with the right hemisphere bias of the mentalizing network in the cerebrum (Wang et al. 2021), with multiple studies showing both task activity and within-cerebrum connectivity lateralized in the right ATL (Gainotti 2007; Olson et al. 2007, 2013; Acres et al. 2009; Rice et al. 2015) and right TPJ (Saxe and Wexler 2005; Van Overwalle and Baetens 2009; Saxe 2010; Santiesteban et al. 2015; Karolis et al. 2019). In our study, the ATL displayed this lateralization with the right ATL being significantly connected to all but 2 left mentalizing cerebellar ROIs. Interestingly, the TPJ did not display this lateralization. The role of the TPJs in mentalizing is debated with several studies showing unilateral right TPJ activation in mentalizing, but others showing bilateral involvement (Gallagher et al. 2000; Van Overwalle 2009; Jenkins and Mitchell 2010; Bzdok et al. 2012; Schurz et al. 2014; Molenberghs et al. 2016). Despite this debate, we attribute the bilateral TPJ activation to the fact that the mentalizing task involved videos of socially meaningful biological motion. In their study on the underlying neural responses of anthropomorphism in autism spectrum disorder and typically developing individuals, Ammons et al. (2018) found bilateral TPJ activation during observation of social movement regardless of the type of agent (human figure or geometrical shape) in typically developing adults as well as individuals with autism. Furthermore, in our study we used a broader definition of the TPJ which could include parts of the IPL, known to be involved in perspective taking, and portions of the pSTS, involved in biological motion perception. We believe that the nature of the stimuli used to activate the mentalizing areas, combined with our selection method of the TPJ ROI, could explain the significant bilateral cerebellar effective connectivity of almost all cerebellar local maxima and the contralateral TPJs.
Third, we examined the structural connectivity profile using probabilistic tractography between cerebellar and cerebral mentalizing areas. The cerebellum is connected to the cerebral cortex through polysynaptic ouroboros loops. The CTC pathway sends projections from the cerebellum to the cortex, then that cortical region projects back to the same cerebellar region through the CPC pathway. Anatomical tracer studies in nonhuman animals have shown that cerebello-cerebral structural connectivity is different between primates and other vertebrates, with primate cerebellar output fibers extending to the frontal association cortex (Ito 1993; Strick et al. 2009; Stoodley and Schmahmann 2010). Only a few studies have tracked these fiberpaths using diffusion-weighted imaging techniques and tractography. Jissendi et al. (2008) were the first to isolate the complete cerebellar projections to prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices, albeit in a small sample and using rudimentary techniques. Similarly, Sokolov et al. (2014) and Keser et al. (2015) delineated these tracts in small samples. Karavasilis et al. (2019) confirmed the results by Keser et al. (2015) by replicating their study with a larger number of participants (N = 60). All of the above findings are promising evidence that the cerebellum is anatomically connected with nonmotor cerebral areas, but the in vivo anatomical evidence for specific cerebellar and cerebral mentalizing areas is still extremely limited.
We recreated the CTC and CPC white matter pathways using probabilistic tractography. There were 3 primary findings. First, we found that the CTC pathways had greater streamline counts compared to the CPC pathways. Second, the streamline counts emerging to and from the left cerebellum were greater than those emerging from the right, indicating some lateralization. This result matches nicely with the lateralization observed in our effective connectivity results. Third, streamline counts were dramatically higher between cerebellar mentalizing regions and the DMPFC, followed by the VMPFC, compared to regions outside of the frontal lobe (see Fig. 4). Several studies using histology methods have found fiber connections between the cerebellum and nonmotor regions of the frontal lobe (Ito 1993; Leiner et al. 1993; Middleton and Strick 1997, 2001; Schmahmann and Pandya 1997; Dum and Strick 2003; Kelly and Strick 2003; Clower et al. 2005) thus the ground truth for the DMPFC and VMPFC DTI findings is strong. The posterior cingulate cortex also projects to the cerebellum, although connections are fewer than to frontal regions (Glickstein et al. 1985). It is more difficult to rely on histology findings when interpreting the TPJ findings. Several histology studies in macaques have found connections between the cerebellum and BA 5 and 7 in the superior parietal/inferior parietal lobe (Glickstein et al. 1985; Clower et al. 2005). It is difficult to say whether the TPJ would fall within the bounds of these regions given that the term “temporoparietal junction” is not used in neuroanatomy texts. Across-species comparisons are also fraught because the human inferior parietal lobe is relatively expanded. In regards to the ATL, robust connections exist between the entire length of the superior temporal gyrus (excepting A1), as well as the depths of the superior temporal sulcus, and the cerebellum (Schmahmann and Pandya 1991). However there are little to no connections between the middle temporal gyrus and inferior temporal gyrus and the cerebellum (Schmahmann and Pandya 1991). These findings would predict that we should find weak structural connectivity between the human ATL mentalizing region and the cerebellum, given that the functionally defined region tends to fall along the anterior STS/middle temporal gyrus.
Even if direct pathways via the CTC/CPC are not present, other polysynaptic routes between the TPJ and ATL to the cerebellum likely exist. For instance, the uncinate fasciculus connects the ATL to the VMPFC (Von Der Heide et al. 2013) and from there, the CPC pathway begins. The presence of polysynaptic routes can explain the strong effective connectivity found between the cerebellar mentalizing regions and the TPJ and ATL. We should mention that our findings showed a mismatch. The DTI findings showed the most connections to the DMPFC and then VMPFC, while the effective connectivity findings showed that the TPJ was the cerebral area with the most effective connections in our analysis, followed by the DMPFC, the ATL, and then the VMPFC. There is a well-known quantitative mismatch between structural and functional connectivity (Huang and Ding 2016; Suárez et al. 2020) but the reason for this is not known.
Our findings add to a small but growing literature linking the cerebellum to social cognition. However, a key unanswered question is “what is the cerebellum’s mechanistic or computational process?”. In the motor literature there is agreement that the cerebellum contributes to the regulation of the rhythm, rate, accuracy, and force of movements (for a review see Manto et al. 2012). Similarly, it has been theorized that the cerebellum regulates the capacity, speed, appropriateness, and consistency of mental processes around a homeostatic baseline. This theory, termed the “dysmetria of thought theory” (Schmahmann 1998) is analogous to the dysmetria of movement theory. The latter is presented with overshooting and lack of control in the motor system. In the dysmetria of thought theory, these are equated with unpredictability in social interactions, a mismatch between reality and perceived reality, and unsuccessful, illogical efforts to correct errors in behavior and thought (Schmahmann 2019). It is clear that dysmetria of movement is matched with dysmetria of thought and this match in functions is explained by the theory of the Universal Cerebellar Transform (Schmahmann 2000, 2001, 2004) according to which there is a unique cerebellar computation which is applied to all its functions (movement- and cognition-related) (Schmahmann 2000, 2001, 2004; Guell et al. 2018a) due to its uniform cortical cytoarchitectonic organization (Ito, 1993; Voogd and Glickstein 1998). However, it is also possible that the cerebellar circuitry performs multiple diverse computations, given that the cerebellum appears with striking functional heterogeneity (Diedrichsen et al. 2019). The most prominent theory about the nature of the cerebellar computation posits that the cerebellum is encoding internal models that reproduce the dynamic properties of body parts for controlling these body parts without any sensory feedback, and for mental processes, that it encodes internal models which reproduce the essential properties of mental representations in the cerebral cortex (Ito 2008; Sokolov & Miall (2017); Raymond and Medina 2018; Sokolov 2018; Van Overwalle et al. 2020b). In a recent paper, Van Overwalle et al (2019) formed a hypothesis positing that, when it comes to social cognition, the cerebellum acts as a “forward controller” predicting how actions by the self and other people will be executed, what are the most likely responses to these actions, and what is the typical sequence of these actions. So, in the realm of motor behavior, the cerebellum calibrates the kinematics of movement, potentially by comparing an internal model to reality, computing an error, and making online adjustments. It has been proposed that something similar occurs for social cognition but direct tests of this are rare, with only few important research efforts in the past few years (Van Overwalle et al. 2019a; 2019b; Pu et al. 2020; Heleven et al. 2021).
Does cerebellar damage cause social cognition or mentalizing deficits? Our review of the lesion literature (Olson et al. 2021) found that the answer to this is complex, depending on the developmental stage during which the injury occurs, the location of injury, and how mentalizing is tested. Some of the mixed findings may be caused by choosing mentalizing tasks that are ill-suited for the computational processes that occur in the cerebellum. A Heider and Simmel-type task, such as the one used here, may be one of the best tasks for evoking cerebellar activations because the stimuli are novel and the shapes move in unpredictable ways, which triggers a constant stream of rapid predictions and error signals. At the same time, there is a social narrative that is being played out, so internal models are activated. However, most lesion studies use Reading the Eyes in the Mind (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001; Fernández-Abascal et al. 2013) or some variant of false-belief stories. In addition, the fact that adults with any type of cerebellar damage are included in these studies adds noise to the measure. As we saw in the present study, mentalizing clusters are limited to a small region of the posterior cerebellum so individuals with lesions that preserve this area should not be expected to exhibit social deficits or problems performing mentalizing tasks.
Limitations and Future Directions
First, connectivity analyses are inherently vulnerable to conceptual and methodological issues (Buckner et al. 2013) and their results depend on various factors (e.g., ROI selection methods, stimuli features, thresholding choices, etc.) (Uddin et al. 2008; Turchi et al. 2018). Accordingly, our findings should be interpreted with caution. PPI is a simple model of effective connectivity providing relational information via a linear model of coupling between a seed and target region (Friston et al. 1997; Stephan 2004; Friston 2011; Gerchen et al. 2014; Smith et al. 2016). However, post hoc interpretation of PPI results must be done with caution as a significant increase in coupling from one region to another may be not significant when running a PPI analysis in the opposite direction (Smith et al. 2016). Furthermore, we defined the spatial location of each mentalizing ROI by using peak activations in the HCP social task and examined effective connectivity in the same task. Although activation and connectivity were analytically orthogonal, this might introduce bias to our results. Future research should compare our results with connectivity patterns derived from other ToM tasks and other nonbiased ROI selection methods. Additionally, dMRI tractography has been criticized for returning high rates of false positives (Thomas et al. 2014; Reveley et al. 2015; Maier-Hein et al. 2017). Regardless, these connectivity methods have provided valuable insight into the anatomical and functional organization of the mentalizing network, and as both techniques improve, we hope other researchers replicate and extend our cerebellar findings. We also did not explore sex differences (Li et al. 2020).
Second, the task set included in the HCP dataset is limited. Whether other tasks with mentalizing demands—sarcasm processing, deception, or false beliefs—engage the same regions of the cerebellum and are functionally and structurally connected with the greater mentalizing network in the cerebrum needs to be explored by future researchers. Related to this question, it is unclear what aspect of mentalizing is driving this network. There are different types of mentalizing: conceptual (e.g., false-belief), perceptual (e.g., face-based), affective (e.g., empathy, moral reasoning), cognitive (e.g., perspective taking), and motion/motoric (e.g., Heider-Simmel biological motion trajectory understanding) (Luyten et al. 2020). Is the cerebellum implementing the same computational mechanism regardless of the particular mentalizing task? This seems implausible, given the wide variety of computational mechanisms used to perform the various tasks that fall into this category. It has recently been proposed that mentalizing tasks with a sequential structure—like the one used in this study—are the only ones linked to cerebellar function (Van Overwalle et al. 2019b), potentially because they draw more heavily on predictive modeling computations (Gonzalez and Chang 2021) linked to cerebellar function more generally. These hypotheses need to be further explored in the future.
Conclusions
This multimodal neuroimaging study investigated the structural and functional connectivity between mentalizing cerebellar and cerebral areas. We found that the cerebellum functions in a domain-specific way with distinct cerebellar areas devoted to distinct cognitive functions, among which is mentalizing. Furthermore, we mapped out the effective connectivity between the mentalizing cerebellum and cerebrum and found cerebellar hemispherical differences in cerebello-cerebral effective connectivity. The left cerebellar hemisphere displayed more and stronger significant effective connections to the right cerebral mentalizing areas. Additionally, we recreated the CTC and CPC pathways using probabilistic tractography and found that the CTC and CPC pathways from and to the left cerebellum had larger streamline counts compared to the right CTC and CPC pathways. Lastly, we found that the bilateral CTC pathways had greater streamline counts compared to the CPC pathways. All the above findings suggest that regions of the posterior cerebellum play a key role in mentalizing abilities.
Supplementary Material
Contributor Information
Athanasia Metoki, Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA; Department of Neurology,Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63108, USA.
Yin Wang, State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, and IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, 100875, China.
Ingrid R Olson, Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA.
Data and Code Availability
All data used in the present study were obtained from the WU-Minn HCP Consortium S900 Release. They are publicly available at https://www.humanconnectome.org. Most analyses were conducted using well established commonly used in the community softwares (FSL, Connectome Workbench, SUIT). Custom codes can be accessed at https://github.com/nasiametoki/HCP_cerebellum_mentalizing and are available from the corresponding authors on request.
Notes
We would like to thank Geoffrey Wright, Vishnu Murty, Jason Chein, Huiling Peng, and Nico Dosenbach for theoretical input at the dissertation phase that benefited this publication, and Dr Axel Kohlmeyer for assistance with high performance cluster computing. We would also like to thank 4 anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful commentary. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health or the National Institutes of Health. Conflict of Interest: None declared.
Funding
National Institutes of Health (grants to I.R.O. [R01 MH091113; R21 HD098509; R01 HD099165; 2R56MH091113-11]) and a Temple University dissertation completion (grant to A.M.). HCP data were provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (principal investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 National Institutes of Health and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research; and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University. National Natural Science Foundation of China (32000782 to Y.W.). This research includes calculations carried out on High Performance Computing resources supported in part by the National Science Foundation through major research instrumentation (grant 1625061) and by the US Army Research Laboratory (W911NF-16-2-0189).
References
- Acres K, Taylor KI, Moss HE, Stamatakis EA, Tyler LK. 2009. Complementary hemispheric asymmetries in object naming and recognition: a voxel-based correlational study. Neuropsychologia. 47:1836–1843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albazron FM, Bruss J, Jones RM, Yock TI, Pulsifer MB, Cohen AL, Nopoulos PC, Abrams AN, Sato M, Boes AD. 2019. Pediatric postoperative cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome follows outflow pathway lesions. Neurology. 93:E1561–E1571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ammons CJ, Doss CF, Bala D, Kana RK. 2018. Brain responses underlying anthropomorphism, agency, and social attribution in autism spectrum disorder. Open Neuroimag J. 12:16–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babb TL, Mitchell AG, Crandall PH. 1974. Fastigiobulbar and dentatothalamic influences on hippocampal cobalt epilepsy in the cat. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 36:141–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch DM, Burgess GC, Harms MP, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL, Corbetta M, Glasser MF, Curtiss S, Dixit S, Feldt C, et al. 2013. Function in the human connectome: task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior. Neuroimage. 80:169–189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. 2001. The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 42:241–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beauregard M, Leroux JM, Bergman S, Arzoumanian Y, Beaudoin G, Bourgouin P, Stip E. 1998. The functional neuroanatomy of major depression: an fMRI study using an emotional activation paradigm. Neuroreport. 9:3253–3258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrens TEJ, Berg HJ, Jbabdi S, Rushworth MFS, Woolrich MW. 2007. Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: what can we gain? Neuroimage. 34:144–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, Nunes RG, Clare S, Matthews PM, Brady JM, Smith SM. 2003. Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion-weighted MR imaging. Magn Reson Med. 50:1077–1088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. 1995. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R I State Dent Soc. 57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- Berman AJ. 1997. Amelioration of agression: response to selective cerebellar lesions in the rhesus monkey. International review of neurobiology. 41:111–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Binder JR, Gross WL, Allendorfer JB, Bonilha L, Chapin J, Edwards JC, Grabowski TJ, Langfitt JT, Loring DW, Lowe MJ, et al. 2011. Mapping anterior temporal lobe language areas with fMRI: a multicenter normative study. Neuroimage. 54:1465–1475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakemore SJ. 2008. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 9(4):267–277. 10.1038/nrn2353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bobée S, Mariette E, Tremblay-Leveau H, Caston J. 2000. Effects of early midline cerebellar lesion on cognitive and emotional functions in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 112:107–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brady RO, Gonsalvez I, Lee I, Öngür D, Seidman LJ, Schmahmann JD, Eack SM, Keshavan MS, Pascual-Leone A, Halko MA. 2019. Cerebellar-prefrontal network connectivity and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 176:512–520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodal P, Bjaalie JG. 1997. Salient anatomic features of the cortico-ponto-cerebellar pathway. Prog Brain Res. 114:227–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo BTT. 2011. The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 106:2322–2345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Yeo BTT. 2013. Opportunities and limitations of intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Nat Neurosci. 16(7):832–837. 10.1038/nn.3423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bzdok D, Heeger A, Langner R, Laird AR, Fox PT, Palomero-Gallagher N, Vogt BA, Zilles K, Eickhoff SB. 2015. Subspecialization in the human posterior medial cortex. Neuroimage. 106:55–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bzdok D, Langner R, Schilbach L, Jakobs O, Roski C, Caspers S, Laird AR, Fox PT, Zilles K, Eickhoff SB. 2013. Characterization of the temporo-parietal junction by combining data-driven parcellation, complementary connectivity analyses, and functional decoding. Neuroimage. 81:381–392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bzdok D, Schilbach L, Vogeley K, Schneider K, Laird AR, Langner R, Eickhoff SB. 2012. Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Struct Funct. 217:783–796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Decety J. 2011. Social neuroscience: challenges and opportunities in the study of complex behavior. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1224:162–173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castelli F, Frith C, Happe F, Frith U. 2002. Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain. 125:1839–1849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castelli F, Happé F, Frith U, Frith C. 2000. Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage. 12:314–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. 2006. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 129(3):564–583. 10.1093/brain/awl004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clower DM, Dum RP, Strick PL. 2005. Basal ganglia and cerebellar inputs to AIP. Cereb Cortex. 15:913–920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen B, Highstein SM. 1972. Cerebellar control of the vestibular pathways to oculomotor neurons. Prog Brain Res. 37:411–425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioural science. 2nd ed. New York, New York: Statistical Power Anaylsis for the Behavioral Sciences. [Google Scholar]
- Di X, Biswal BB. 2017. Psychophysiological interactions in a visual checkerboard task: reproducibility, reliability, and the effects of deconvolution. Front Neurosci. 11:573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dice LR. 1945. Measures of the amount of ecologic association between species. Ecology. 26:297–302. [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J, Balsters JH, Flavell J, Cussans E, Ramnani N. 2009. A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage. 46:39–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J, King M, Hernandez-Castillo C, Sereno M, Ivry RB. 2019. Universal transform or multiple functionality? Understanding the contribution of the human cerebellum across task domains. Neuron. 102:918–928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J, Zotow E. 2015. Surface-based display of volume-averaged cerebellar imaging data. PLoS One. 10:e0133402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dum RP, Strick PL. 2003. An unfolded map of the cerebellar dentate nucleus and its projections to the cerebral cortex. J Neurophysiol. 89:634–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández-Abascal EG, Cabello R, Fernández-Berrocal P, Baron-Cohen S. 2013. Test-retest reliability of the “reading the mind in the eyes” test: a one-year follow-up study. Mol Autism. 4:33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fine EJ, Ionita CC, Lohr L. 2002. The history of the development of the cerebellar examination. Semin Neurol. 22:375–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ. 2011. Functional and effective connectivity: a review. Brain Connect. 1:13–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ. 1997. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage. 6:218–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith CD, Frith U. 2006. The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron. 50(4):531–534. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gainotti G. 2007. Different patterns of famous people recognition disorders in patients with right and left anterior temporal lesions: a systematic review. Neuropsychologia. 45:1591–1607. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher HL, Happé F, Brunswick N, Fletcher PC, Frith U, Frith CD. 2000. Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of “theory of mind” in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia. 38:11–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao Z, Davis C, Thomas AM, Economo MN, Abrego AM, Svoboda K, De Zeeuw CI, Li N. 2018. A cortico-cerebellar loop for motor planning. Nature. 563:113–116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerchen MF, Bernal-Casas D, Kirsch P. 2014. Analyzing task-dependent brain network changes by whole-brain psychophysiological interactions: a comparison to conventional analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 35:5071–5082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasser MF, Sotiropoulos SN, Wilson JA, Coalson TS, Fischl B, Andersson JL, Xu J, Jbabdi S, Webster M, Polimeni JR, et al. 2013. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the human connectome project. Neuroimage. 80:105–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glickstein M. 1992. The cerebellum and motor learning. J Cogn Neurosci. 2:802–806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glickstein M. 1993. Motor skills but not cognitive tasks. Trends Neurosci. 16:450–451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glickstein M, May JG, Mercier BE. 1985. Corticopontine projection in the macaque: the distribution of labelled cortical cells after large injections of horseradish peroxidase in the pontine nuclei. J Comp Neurol. 235:343–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gobbini MI, Koralek AC, Bryan RE, Montgomery KJ, Haxby JV. 2007. Two takes on the social brain: a comparison of theory of mind tasks. J Cogn Neurosci. 19:1803–1814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gonzalez B, Chang L. 2021. Computational models of mentalizing. In: Gilead M, Ochsner KN, editors. The neural basis of mentalizing. Cham: Springer. 10.1007/978-3-030-51890-5_15. [DOI]
- Guell X, Gabrieli JDE, Schmahmann JD. 2018a. Triple representation of language, working memory, social and emotion processing in the cerebellum: convergent evidence from task and seed-based resting-state fMRI analyses in a single large cohort. Neuroimage. 172:437–449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guell X, Schmahmann JD, Gabrieli JDE, Ghosh SS. 2018b. Functional gradients of the cerebellum. Elife. 7:1–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gündel H, O’Connor M-F, Littrell L, Fort C, Lane RD. 2003. Functional neuroanatomy of grief: an fMRI study. Am J Psychiatry. 160:1946–1953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hariri AR, Brown SM, Williamson DE, Flory JD, De Wit H, Manuck SB. 2006. Preference for immediate over delayed rewards is associated with magnitude of ventral striatal activity. J Neurosci. 26:13213–13217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartwright CE, Hansen PC, Apperly IA. 2016. Current knowledge on the role of the inferior frontal gyrus in theory of mind – a commentary on Schurz and Tholen (2016). Cortex. 85:133–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heath RG, Franklin D, Shraberg D. 1979. Gross pathology of the cerebellum in patients diagnosed and treated as functional psychiatric disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis. 167:585–592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heath RG, Harper JW. 1974. Ascending projections of the cerebellar fastigial nucleus to the hippocampus, amygdala, and other temporal lobe sites. Exp Neurol. 45:268–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heleven E, van Dun K, De Witte S, Baeken C, Van Overwalle F. 2021. The role of the cerebellum in social and non-social action sequences: a preliminary LF-rTMS study. Front Hum Neurosci. 15:85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu D, Shen H, Zhou Z. 2008. Functional asymmetry in the cerebellum: a brief review. Cerebellum. 7(3):304–313. 10.1007/s12311-008-0031-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hua K, Zhang J, Wakana S, Jiang H, Li X, Reich DS, Calabresi PA, Pekar JJ, van Zijl PCM, Mori S. 2018. Tract probability maps in stereotaxic spaces: analyses of white matter anatomy and tract-specific quantification. Neuroimage. 39:336–347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang H, Ding M. 2016. Linking functional connectivity and structural connectivity quantitatively: a comparison of methods. Brain Connect. 6:99–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M. 1982. Cerebellar control of the vestibulo-ocular reflex--around the flocculus hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci. 5:275–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M. 1984. The cerebellum and neural control. New York: Raven. [Google Scholar]
- Ito M. 1993. Movement and thought: identical control mechanisms by the cerebellum. Trends Neurosci. 16:448–450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M. 2002. Historical review of the significance of the cerebellum and the role of purkinje cells in motor learning. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 978:273–288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M. 2008. Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nat Rev Neurosci. 9:304–313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack A, Pelphrey KA. 2015. Neural correlates of animacy attribution include neocerebellum in healthy adults. Cereb Cortex. 25:4240–4247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins AC, Mitchell JP. 2010. Mentalizing under uncertainty: dissociated neural responses to ambiguous and unambiguous mental state inferences. Cereb Cortex. 20:404–410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jissendi P, Baudry S, Balériaux D. 2008. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and tractography of the cerebellar projections to prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices: a study at 3T. J Neuroradiol. 35:42–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones DK, Knösche TR, Turner R. 2013. White matter integrity, fiber count, and other fallacies: the do’s and don’ts of diffusion MRI. Neuroimage. 73:239–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karavasilis E, Christidi F, Velonakis G, Giavri Z, Kelekis NL, Efstathopoulos EP, Evdokimidis I, Dellatolas G. 2019. Ipsilateral and contralateral cerebro-cerebellar white matter connections: a diffusion tensor imaging study in healthy adults. J Neuroradiol. 46:52–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karolis VR, Corbetta M, Thiebaut de Schotten M. 2019. The architecture of functional lateralisation and its relationship to callosal connectivity in the human brain. Nat Commun. 10:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly RM, Strick PL. 2003. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J Neurosci. 23:8432–8444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keser Z, Hasan KM, Mwangi BI, Kamali A, Ucisik-Keser FE, Riascos RF, Yozbatiran N, Francisco GE, Narayana PA. 2015. Diffusion tensor imaging of the human cerebellar pathways and their interplay with cerebral macrostructure. Front Neuroanat. 9:1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King M, Hernandez-Castillo CR, Poldrack RA, Ivry RB, Diedrichsen J. 2019. Functional boundaries in the human cerebellum revealed by a multi-domain task battery. Nat Neurosci. 22:1371–1378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kliemann D, Adolphs R. 2018. The social neuroscience of mentalizing: challenges and recommendations. Curr Opin Psychol. 24:1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koster-Hale J, Richardson H, Velez N, Asaba M, Young L, Saxe R. 2017. Mentalizing regions represent distributed, continuous, and abstract dimensions of others’ beliefs. Neuroimage. 161:9–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koster-Hale J, Saxe R. 2013. Theory of mind: a neural prediction problem. Neuron. 79:836–848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krienen FM, Buckner RL. 2009. Segregated fronto-cerebellar circuits revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Cereb Cortex. 19:2485–2497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahnakoski JM, Glerean E, Salmi J, Jääskeläinen IP, Sams M, Hari R, Nummenmaa L. 2012. Naturalistic fMRI mapping reveals superior temporal sulcus as the hub for the distributed brain network for social perception. Front Hum Neurosci. 6:1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane RD, Reiman EM, Bradley MM, Lang PJ, Ahern GL, Davidson RJ, Schwartz GE. 1997. Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuropsychologia. 35:1437–1444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leiner HC, Leiner AL, Dow RS. 1986. Does the cerebellum contribute to mental skills? Behav Neurosci. 100:443–454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leiner HC, Leiner AL, Dow RS. 1993. Cognitive and language functions of the human cerebellum. Trends Neurosci. 16:444–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levisohn L, Cronin-Golomb A, Schmahmann JD. 2000. Neuropsychological consequences of cerebellar tumour resection in children. Cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome in a paediatric population. Brain. 123:1041–1050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li G, Chen Y, Wang W, Dhingra I, Zhornitsky S, Tang X, Li CSR. 2020. Sex differences in neural responses to the perception of social interactions. Front Hum Neurosci. 14:376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luyten P, Campbell C, Allison E, Fonagy P. 2020. The mentalizing approach to psychopathology: state of the art and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 16:297–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier-Hein KH, Neher PF, Houde J-C, Côté M-A, Garyfallidis E, Zhong J, Chamberland M, Yeh F-C, Lin Y-C, Ji Q, et al. 2017. The challenge of mapping the human connectome based on diffusion tractography. Nat Commun. 8:1349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manto M, Bower JM, Conforto AB, Delgado-García JM, Da Guarda SNF, Gerwig M, Habas C, Hagura N, Ivry RB, Marien P, et al. 2012. Consensus paper: roles of the cerebellum in motor control—the diversity of ideas on cerebellar involvement in movement. The Cerebellum. 11:457–487. 10.1007/s12311-011-0331-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manto M, Mariën P. 2015. Schmahmann’s syndrome - identification of the third cornerstone of clinical ataxiology. Cerebellum Ataxias. 2:2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mar RA. 2011. The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annu Rev Psychol. 62:103–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcus DS, Harwell J, Olsen T, Hodge M, Glasser MF, Prior F, Jenkinson M, Laumann T, Curtiss SW, Van Essen DC. 2011. Informatics and data mining tools and strategies for the human connectome project. Front Neuroinform. 5:1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marek S, Siegel JS, Gordon EM, Raut RV, Gratton C, Newbold DJ, Ortega M, Laumann TO, Adeyemo B, Miller DB, et al. 2018. Spatial and temporal organization of the individual human cerebellum. Neuron. 100:977–993.e7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKay NS, Iwabuchi SJ, Häberling IS, Corballis MC, Kirk IJ. 2017. Atypical white matter microstructure in left-handed individuals. Laterality. 22:257–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaren DG, Ries ML, Xu G, Johnson SC. 2012. A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches. Neuroimage. 61:1277–1286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middleton FA, Strick PL. 1994. Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. Science. 266:458–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middleton FA, Strick PL. 1997. Cerebellar output channels. Int Rev Neurobiol. 41:61–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middleton FA, Strick PL. 2001. Cerebellar projections to the prefrontal cortex of the primate. J Neurosci. 21:700–712. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molenberghs P, Johnson H, Henry JD, Mattingley JB. 2016. Understanding the minds of others: a neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 65:276–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori S, Wakana S, Van Zijl PC, Nagae-Poetscher LM. 2005. MRI atlas of human white matter. Am J Neuroradiol. 27:1384. [Google Scholar]
- O’Reilly JX, Woolrich MW, Behrens TEJ, Smith SM, Johansen-Berg H. 2012. Tools of the trade: psychophysiological interactions and functional connectivity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 7:604–609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldfield RC. 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 9:97–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson IR, Hoffman LJ, Metoki A, Popal H, Wang Y. 2021. Theory of mind and the cerebellum: essential or irrelevant? (Unpublished manuscript). Department of Psychology, Temple University. [Google Scholar]
- Olson IR, McCoy D, Klobusicky E, Ross LA. 2013. Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes: a review and theoretical framework. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 8:123–133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson IR, Plotzker A, Ezzyat Y. 2007. The enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain. 130:1718–1731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palesi F, De Rinaldis A, Castellazzi G, Calamante F, Muhlert N, Chard D, Tournier JD, Magenes G, D’Angelo E, Wheeler-Kingshott CAMG. 2017. Contralateral cortico-ponto-cerebellar pathways reconstruction in humans in vivo: implications for reciprocal cerebro-cerebellar structural connectivity in motor and non-motor areas. Sci Rep. 7:1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palesi F, Tournier JD, Calamante F, Muhlert N, Castellazzi G, Chard D, D’Angelo E, Wheeler-Kingshott CAM. 2015. Contralateral cerebello-thalamo-cortical pathways with prominent involvement of associative areas in humans in vivo. Brain Struct Funct. 220:3369–3384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peer M, Salomon R, Goldberg I, Blanke O, Arzy S. 2015. Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 112:11072–11077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollack IF. 1997. Posterior fossa syndrome. Int Rev Neurobiol. 41:411–732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pu M, Heleven E, Delplanque J, Gibert N, Ma Q, Funghi G, Van Overwalle F. 2020. The posterior cerebellum supports the explicit sequence learning linked to trait attribution. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 20:798–815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramnani N. 2006. The primate cortico-cerebellar system: anatomy and function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 7:511–522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raymond JL, Medina JF. 2018. Computational principles of supervised learning in the cerebellum. Annu Rev Neurosci. 41:233–253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reveley C, Seth AK, Pierpaoli C, Silva AC, Yu D, Saunders RC, Leopold DA, Ye FQ. 2015. Superficial white matter fiber systems impede detection of long-range cortical connections in diffusion MR tractography. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 112:E2820–E2828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice GE, Ralph MAL, Hoffman P. 2015. The roles of left versus right anterior temporal lobes in conceptual knowledge: an ALE meta-analysis of 97 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. 25:4374–4391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riedel MC, Ray KL, Dick AS, Sutherland MT, Hernandez Z, Fox PM, Eickhoff SB, Fox PT, Laird AR. 2015. Meta-analytic connectivity and behavioral parcellation of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage. 117:327–342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson EC, Jbabdi S, Glasser MF, Andersson J, Burgess GC, Harms MP, Smith SM, Van Essen DC, Jenkinson M. 2014. MSM: a new flexible framework for multimodal surface matching. Neuroimage. 100:414–426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross LA, Olson IR. 2010. Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes. Neuroimage. 49:3452–3462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmi J, Pallesen KJ, Neuvonen T, Brattico E, Korvenoja A, Salonen O, Carlson S. 2010. Cognitive and motor loops of the human cerebro-cerebellar system. J Cogn Neurosci. 22:2663–2676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santiesteban I, Banissy MJ, Catmur C, Bird G. 2015. Functional lateralization of temporoparietal junction - imitation inhibition, visual perspective-taking and theory of mind. Eur J Neurosci. 42:2527–2533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxe R. 2010. The right temporo-parietal junction: a specific brain region for thinking about thoughts. Handb Theory Mind. 1–35. [Google Scholar]
- Saxe R, Wexler A. 2005. Making sense of another mind: the role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia. 43:1391–1399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller D, Freeman JB, Mitchell JP, Uleman JS, Phelps EA. 2009. A neural mechanism of first impressions. Nat Neurosci. 12:508–514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD. 1991. An emerging concept: the cerebellar contribution to higher function. Arch Neurol. 48:1178–1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD. 1998. Dysmetria of thought: clinical consequences of cerebellar dysfunction on cognition and affect. Trends Cogn Sci. 2:362–371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD. 2000. The role of the cerebellum in affect and psychosis. J Neurolinguistics. 13:189–214. [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD. 2001. The cerebrocerebellar system: anatomic substrates of the cerebellar contribution to cognition and emotion. Int Rev Psychiatry. 4:174–198. [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD. 2004. Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 16(3):367–378. 10.1176/jnp.16.3.367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD. 2019. The cerebellum and cognition. Neurosci Lett. 688:62–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN. 1989. Anatomical investigation of projections to the basis pontis from posterior parietal association cortices in rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 289:53–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN. 1991. Projections to the basis pontis from the superior temporal sulcus and superior temporal region in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 308:224–248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN. 1997. The cerebrocerebellar system. Int Rev Neurobiol. 41:31–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC. 1998. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain. 121:561–579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schniepp R, Möhwald K, Wuehr M. 2017. Gait ataxia in humans: vestibular and cerebellar control of dynamic stability. J Neurol. 264:87–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schurz M, Perner J. 2015. An evaluation of neurocognitive models of theory of mind. Front Psychol. 6:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schurz M, Radua J, Aichhorn M, Richlan F, Perner J. 2014. Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 42:9–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Miller KL, Moeller S, Xu J, Auerbach EJ, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Andersson J, Glasser MF, et al. 2012. Temporally-independent functional modes of spontaneous brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 109:3131–3136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Vidaurre D, Beckmann CF, Glasser MF, Jenkinson M, Miller KL, Nichols TE, Robinson EC, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Woolrich MW, et al. 2013. Functional connectomics from resting-state fMRI. Trends Cogn Sci. 17:666–682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith DV, Gseir M, Speer ME, Delgado MR. 2016. Toward a cumulative science of functional integration: a meta-analysis of psychophysiological interactions. Hum Brain Mapp. 37:2904–2917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snider RS, Maiti A, Snider SR. 1976. Cerebellar pathways to ventral midbrain and nigra. Exp Neurol. 53:714–728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokolov AA. 2018. The cerebellum in social cognition. Front Cell Neurosci. 12:145. [Google Scholar]
- Sokolov AA, Erb M, Grodd W, Pavlova MA. 2014. Structural loop between the cerebellum and the superior temporal sulcus: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Cereb Cortex. 24:626–632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokolov AA, Miall RC, Ivry RB. 2017. The cerebellum: adaptive prediction for movement and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 21 5 313–332 10.1016/j.tics.2017.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sørensen Τ. 1948. Α method of establishing groups of equal amplitude in plant sociology based on similarity of species and its application to analyses of the vegetation on Danish commons. K Danske Vidensk Selsk. 5:1–34. [Google Scholar]
- Stephan KE. 2004. On the role of general system theory for functional neuroimaging. J Anat. 205:443–470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. 2010. Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. Cortex. 46:831–844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strick PL, Dum RP, Fiez JA. 2009. Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 32:413–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suárez LE, Markello RD, Betzel RF, Misic B. 2020. Linking structure and function in macroscale brain networks. Trends Cogn Sci. 24(4):302–315. 10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki L, Coulon P, Sabel-Goedknegt EH, Ruigrok TJH. 2012. Organization of cerebral projections to identified cerebellar zones in the posterior cerebellum of the rat. J Neurosci. 32:10854–10869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tavano A, Grasso R, Gagliardi C, Triulzi F, Bresolin N, Fabbro F, Borgatti R. 2007. Disorders of cognitive and affective development in cerebellar malformations. Brain. 130:2646–2660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas C, Ye FQ, Irfanoglu MO, Modi P, Saleem KS, Leopold DA, Pierpaoli C. 2014. Anatomical accuracy of brain connections derived from diffusion MRI tractography is inherently limited. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 111:16574–16579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tompson SH, Kahn AE, Falk EB, Vettel JM, Bassett DS. 2020. Functional brain network architecture supporting the learning of social networks in humans. Neuroimage. 210:116498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turchi J, Chang C, Ye FQ, Russ BE, Yu DK, Cortes CR, Monosov IE, Duyn JH, Leopold DA. 2018. The basal forebrain regulates global resting-state fMRI fluctuations. Neuron. 97:940–952.e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uddin LQ, Iacoboni M, Lange C, Keenan JP. 2007. The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends Cogn Sci. 11:153–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uddin LQ, Mooshagian E, Zaidel E, Scheres A, Margulies DS, Kelly AMC, Shehzad Z, Adelstein JS, Castellanos FX, Biswal BB, et al. 2008. Residual functional connectivity in the split-brain revealed with resting-state functional MRI. Neuroreport. 19:703–709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC, Ugurbil K, Auerbach E, Barch D, Behrens TEJ, Bucholz R, Chang A, Chen L, Corbetta M, Curtiss SW, et al. 2012. The human connectome project: a data acquisition perspective. Neuroimage. 62:2222–2231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F. 2009. Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 30:829–858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Baetens K. 2009. Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage. 48:564–584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Baetens K, Mariën P, Vandekerckhove M. 2014. Social cognition and the cerebellum: a meta-analysis of over 350 fMRI studies. Neuroimage. 86:554–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Baetens K, Mariën P, Vandekerckhove M. 2015a. Cerebellar areas dedicated to social cognition? A comparison of meta-analytic and connectivity results. Soc Neurosci. 10:337–344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, D’aes T, Mariën P. 2015b. Social cognition and the cerebellum: a meta-analytic connectivity analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 36:5137–5154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, De Coninck S, Heleven E, Perrotta G, Ben TNO, Manto M, Mariën P. 2019a. The role of the cerebellum in reconstructing social action sequences: a pilot study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 14:549–558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Heleven E, Ma N, Mariën P. 2017. Tell me twice: a multi-study analysis of the functional connectivity between the cerebrum and cerebellum after repeated trait information. Neuroimage. 144:241–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Ma Q, Heleven E. 2020a. The posterior crus II cerebellum is specialized for social mentalizing and emotional self-experiences: a meta-analysis. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 15:905–928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Manto M, Cattaneo Z, Clausi S, Ferrari C, Gabrieli JDE, Guell X, Heleven E, Lupo M, Ma Q, et al. 2020b. Consensus paper: cerebellum and social cognition. Cerebellum. 19:833–868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Manto M, Leggio M, Delgado-García JM. 2019b. The sequencing process generated by the cerebellum crucially contributes to social interactions. Med Hypotheses. 128:33–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Mariën P. 2016. Functional connectivity between the cerebrum and cerebellum in social cognition: a multi-study analysis. Neuroimage. 124:248–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Van de Steen F, Mariën P. 2019c. Dynamic causal modeling of the effective connectivity between the cerebrum and cerebellum in social mentalizing across five studies. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 19:211–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Van de Steen F, van Dun K, Heleven E. 2020c. Connectivity between the cerebrum and cerebellum during social and non-social sequencing using dynamic causal modelling. Neuroimage. 206:116326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Vandekerckhove M. 2013. Implicit and explicit social mentalizing: dual processes driven by a shared neural network. Front Hum Neurosci. 7:560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Von Der Heide RJ, Skipper LM, Klobusicky E, Olson IR. 2013. Dissecting the uncinate fasciculus: disorders, controversies and a hypothesis. Brain. 136:1692–1707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voogd J, Glickstein M. 1998. The anatomy of the cerebellum. Trends Cogn Sci. 2(9):307–313. 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01210-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakana S, Caprihan A, Panzenboeck MM, Fallon JH, Perry M, Gollub RL, Hua K, Zhang J, Jiang H, Dubey P, et al. 2007. Reproducibility of quantitative tractography methods applied to cerebral white matter. Neuroimage. 36:630–644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Collins JA, Koski J, Nugiel T, Metoki A, Olson IR. 2017. Dynamic neural architecture for social knowledge retrieval. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 114:E3305–E3314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Metoki A, Xia Y, Zang Y, He Y, Olson IR. 2021. A large-scale structural and functional connectome of social mentalizing. Neuroimage. 236:118115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson TC, Obiang P, Torres-Herraez A, Watilliaux A, Coulon P, Rochefort C, Rondi-Reig L. 2019. Anatomical and physiological foundations of cerebello-hippocampal interaction. Elife. 8:e41896. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wheatley T, Milleville SC, Martin A. 2007. Understanding animate agents: distinct roles for the social network and mirror system: research report. Psychol Sci. 18:469–474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White SJ, Coniston D, Rogers R, Frith U. 2011. Developing the Frith-Happé animations: a quick and objective test of theory of mind for adults with autism. Autism Res. 4:149–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- WU-Minn HCP Consortium . 2015. WU-Minn HCP 900 subjects release: reference manual, appendix I – protocol guidance and HCP session protocols, pp. 1–55.
- Yang DYJ, Rosenblau G, Keifer C, Pelphrey KA. 2015. An integrative neural model of social perception, action observation, and theory of mind. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 51:263–275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanchetti A, Zoccolini A. 1954. Autonomic hypothalamic outbursts elicited by cerebellar stimulation. J Neurophysiol. 17:475–483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data used in the present study were obtained from the WU-Minn HCP Consortium S900 Release. They are publicly available at https://www.humanconnectome.org. Most analyses were conducted using well established commonly used in the community softwares (FSL, Connectome Workbench, SUIT). Custom codes can be accessed at https://github.com/nasiametoki/HCP_cerebellum_mentalizing and are available from the corresponding authors on request.