Abstract
Cooperation is crucial for the success of social interactions. Given its importance, humans should readily be able to use available cues to predict how likely others are to cooperate. Here, we review the empirical literature on how accurate such predictions are. To this end, we distinguish between three classes of cues: behavioral (including past decisions), personal (including gender, attractiveness, and group membership) and situational (including the benefits to cooperation and the ability to communicate with each other). We discuss (i) how each cue correlates with future cooperative decisions and (ii) whether people correctly anticipate each cue's predictive value. We find that people are fairly accurate in interpreting behavioral and situational cues. However, they often misperceive the value of personal cues.
Keywords: Perception, Cooperation, Social behavior, Cues
When people cooperate, they make an individual effort to benefit others [1]. Such voluntary acts of costly cooperation are crucial for most social interactions, affecting the well-being of families, the success of scientific collaborations, and the output of team work. Cooperative behavior can be explored from various angles. Theoretical studies analyze which social environments are most conducive to cooperation [2, 3, 4]. Similarly, experimental work explores under which conditions people actually cooperate and how they adapt their own behavior to the actual or expected behavior of their peers [5, 6, 7, 8]. Here, we review this literature, focusing on the following two questions: how do people use available cues to predict the cooperativeness of their interaction partners ahead of an interaction, and which cues are most reliable for predicting cooperativeness?
We distinguish between three broad categories of cues that may be used to make such predictions. (i) Behavioral cues refer to the interaction partners' past actions. Examples of such cues include whether these partners cooperated on previous occasions or whether they enforced cooperative social norms. (ii) Personal attributes comprise, for example, the interaction partner's gender or perceived attractiveness, among other characteristics. (iii) Situational cues define the environment in which the individual's next interaction takes place, including how costly cooperation will be and whether pre-play communication is possible. For each of the three categories, we ask whether the given cue is in fact a reliable predictor of cooperativeness and whether people judge the cue's predictive value accurately. Although the existing literature covers a wide range of relevant cues, there also exist notable gaps in the literature, which we encourage scholars to explore further.
There are various ways to elicit how individuals perceive each other's cooperativeness. For the purpose of this review, we consider evidence from three approaches. The first approach is to elicit perceived cooperativeness directly by asking participants to estimate how likely others will cooperate. The two other approaches are more indirect by either asking participants to choose a group member for future interactions (i.e. partner selection) or by asking participants how much money they would transfer to the respective group member in a trust game [9]. Our assumption is that the more inclined a participant is to choose an interaction partner or to transfer more money to a partner, with whom they will interact in the future, the more cooperative they perceive the partner to be —and indeed, some empirical work has found this to be the case [10].
Behavioral cues of cooperativeness
Perhaps the most immediate cue to predict future cooperative behavior is whether, and how often, the respective individual cooperated in the past. Experimental research suggests that people exhibit a stable and consistent ‘cooperative phenotype’: an individual's decision in one cooperative game is indicative of what that individual will subsequently do in different games [11,12]. Participants in laboratory experiments, in turn, seem to expect others to be consistent in their cooperative behavior. When people need to choose an interaction partner, they strongly prefer partners who have been cooperative in the past [13]. Similar evidence comes from studies on charitable giving and pro-environment behavior. Donors to charity are trusted more as well as chosen more often as interaction partners, and in many cases, they indeed turn out to be more cooperative in subsequent social dilemmas [14,15]; but see also [16].
Another — more indirect — cue of cooperative behavior is whether individuals previously engaged in the enforcement of social norms. As per this account, people who punish selfishness may signal that they are not selfish. To test this hypothesis, Jordan et al. [17] consider an interaction that consists of two stages. In the first stage, a ‘signaler’ witnesses a transgressor who refuses to help a recipient. The signaler can then decide whether to engage in third-party punishment by reducing the payoff of the transgressor. In the second stage, a ‘chooser’ decides how much money to send to the signaler in a trust game. The experiment shows that signalers who punish transgressors are indeed entrusted with more money, which turns out to be justified: these signalers also return more money to the chooser. Interestingly, when in the first stage signalers have a choice between punishing the transgressor or helping the recipient, signalers are less likely to punish. Instead, helping turns out to be the more frequently chosen (and more accurate) signal of trustworthiness. This result is in line with an earlier experiment reported by Rockenbach and Milinski [18]: when individuals need to choose group members for a cooperative task, they place more weight on how often group members cooperated, rather than how often they enforced cooperation. Overall, punishment may not necessarily be taken as a cue of altruism, as it may also imply aggressiveness. As a result, punishment is judged more appropriate if it is implemented by the entire group, rather than by a single individual [19].
Finally, another potential cue may come from how a person makes cooperative decisions. For example, based on a game-theoretic model, Hoffman et al. [20] suggest that people who collect additional information to carefully compare the advantages and disadvantages of a cooperative decision are considered less reliable and less cooperative. As per this account, people who deliberately refuse to learn payoff-relevant information may seem more committed to cooperation even when defection happens to be profitable. In line with this view, Jordan et al. [21] show that study participants who ignore the precise costs of cooperation are indeed (and accurately) predicted to be more trustworthy. Participants in turn seemed to be well-aware of the reputational benefit of strategic ignorance: when the cooperation costs can be learnt secretly, participants were more likely to do so. Similar evidence comes from a study reported by Levine et al. [22] who compare the reputational consequences of emotion-based versus reason-based decision-making. Players who state having made a decision based on emotion are perceived as more cooperative by their partner and indeed turn out to be more cooperative. Interestingly, however, players who state having used reason were not perceived as any less cooperative than a control group.
Personal cues of cooperativeness
A second category of cues pertains to personal characteristics of individuals. The idea that visible characteristics are used as signals of cooperativeness has its roots in evolutionary biology: computer simulations and game-theoretic models suggest that individuals can use visible cues to identify potential cooperation partners. This theoretical work suggests that partner choice based on visible cues can in turn be an important mechanism for the evolution of cooperation [23].
Arguably, one of the most salient personal cues is gender. Although some scholars document gender differences in cooperativeness in the dictator game [24] and the prisoner's dilemma [25], the literature remains notoriously mixed [26, 27, 28]. Indeed, Exley et al. [29] show that gender only inconsistently predicts cooperativeness across seven economic games. Nevertheless, across all economic games, they find robust evidence that people consistently believe that women are expected to be fairer, more generous, and more cooperative than men, in line with studies reported by Aguiar et al. [30] and Brañas-Garza et al.[31].
Other cues about a person include their physical appearance, such as an interaction partner's face and facial expressions [32, 33, 34∗], their voice [35] and, in particular, their attractiveness [36,37]. Although there is no evidence that attractiveness is a reliable predictor of cooperativeness, good looks nonetheless positively impact people's perception of others in many domains of economic life, known as the ‘beauty premium’ [38]. Indeed, Anderoni and Petrie [36] and Wilson and Eckel [37] find that people expect more cooperation and reciprocation from attractive partners. Interestingly, when such expectations are not met, attractive interaction partners incur a ‘beauty penalty,’ receiving less reciprocation than less attractive players.
Another factor that may affect a partner's cooperativeness is wealth. Here, the experimental evidence remains inconclusive. For example, Piff et al. [39] find that subjects who consider their own socioeconomic rank to be low tend to be more generous and charitable. In contrast, Smeets et al. [40] report that millionaires are considerably more generous in dictator games than usual participants, especially if they are paired with a low-income partner. A similarly conflicting picture emerges on the level of perceived cooperativeness. Some experiments find that wealthy participants are perceived to be more trustworthy and cooperative [41,42], while at the same time, people seem to systematically underestimate the generosity of the extremely rich [43].
A person's political, religious, or ethical convictions can also serve as potential cues of cooperativeness. Research on political affiliation finds that left-leaning participants tend to cooperate more than right-leaning participants. However, the effect is small at best [44,45], and it seems to be moderated by the fact that left-leaning participants expect more cooperation from others [46]. Political ideology in turn shapes how people are perceived. Balliet et al. [47] find that among United States participants, Democrats are perceived as more cooperative by both sides of the political spectrum, even though this belief is inaccurate. Similar to political ideology, religiosity appears to be correlated with prosocial behavior [48,49]. As a result, when Christians show overt religious cues (e.g. a necklace with a cross), they are perceived as more trustworthy [50]. Finally, people are considered more trustworthy when they make deontological rather than consequentialist judgments [51,52]. Interestingly, however, deontological participants are not necessarily more cooperative [53].
More often than not, when it comes to social attributes, perceptions of cooperativeness are partially shaped by in-group bias, as group membership is itself an important cue. A large literature demonstrates that participants cooperate more with, and preferentially reward, ‘in-group’ members, both in the laboratory [54] and in the field [55]. Democrats and Republicans both tend to cooperate more with in-group members [47], and participants cooperate more with a partner that shares their nationality [56]. However, group membership may largely serve as a coordination device: although participants do not believe that in-group members are intrinsically more cooperative than others, Balliet et al [57] find that more cooperation is expected from in-group members.
Situational cues of cooperativeness
Our final category of cues covers situational aspects surrounding a cooperative decision. These include factors outside an individual's control that are often determined by the structure or context of the interaction. For example, some social interactions create more mutual benefits to the participants than others. In laboratory studies, these factors can be studied in isolation, holding everything else constant. For instance, in the repeated prisoner's dilemma, people tend to be more cooperative when the mutual benefit of cooperation increases [58]. This is consistent with evidence from a study reported by Charness et al. [59]: as the mutual benefit of cooperation increases, participants also expect to see more cooperativeness, especially those participants who later choose to cooperate. This suggests that individuals are able to ‘read’ a situation and that they adjust their willingness to cooperate accordingly.
There has been a debate on whether cooperativeness is affected by whether or not decisions need to be made under time pressure. One account holds that when individuals are forced to decide quickly, they tend to be more cooperative [60]. However, the causal evidence is mixed [61]. Alternatively, it has been argued that fast decisions may not necessarily result in more cooperation. Instead, it may result in more extreme outcomes, either toward cooperation or defection [62]. This is also what participants themselves seem to expect: when asked to predict the outcome of a fast cooperation decision, participants are more likely to expect an extreme (but not necessarily a cooperative) outcome [63].
Finally, communication has long been found to enhance cooperation [64]. However, there is debate as to what is the precise mechanism that allows communication to be favorable. He et al. [65] rule out several potential explanations, namely, that communication reduces social distance or that it offers opportunities to make promises. Instead, they argue that most importantly, communication allows people to recognize the other person as a cooperative type. Indeed, Sparks et al [66] find that people can accurately predict cooperative behavior in a prisoner's dilemma after only a short in-person interaction, even when participants do not discuss the game itself.
Conclusion and future directions
People are quick to form an impression, sometimes in just a few milliseconds [32], but these impressions do not need to be reliable. In this review, we have summarized how individuals perceive different cues to predict others’ cooperativeness. Among the three categories of cues we considered, people most accurately use behavioral and situational cues to predict future cooperative behavior. In contrast, predictions seem to be least accurate when they are based on personal attributes. In fact, with the possible exception of political affiliation and religiosity, personal attributes are often not a good predictor of actual cooperation behavior, yet many such attributes — such as gender and attractiveness — are nonetheless (and inaccurately) perceived as predictors of cooperativeness.
These findings raise a number of interesting questions. For example, theoretical work could explore which kinds of environments and cues allow people to form reliable expectations. Similarly, experimental work could investigate how persistent certain misperceptions are and whether they disappear with more experience. Although we summarized results on a number of different cues, several others seem to have received limited attention, including age and ethnicity.
Further interesting problems arise when several (possibly conflicting) cues are available or when people need to aggregate cues from different domains. For example, Kumar et al. [56] show that when people learn both their interaction partner's nationality and gender, the former cue becomes dominant: people cooperate more with own nationality partners, and they expect own nationality partners to be more cooperative.
More generally, any given cue seems to become less relevant once more salient information — such as actual cooperative behavior — is available [17,36,67,68]. These findings suggest that people rank different cues according to each cue's predictive value. They only make use of unreliable cues when no other cues are available.
Conflict of interest statement
Nothing declared.
Acknowledgements
Christian Hilbe acknowledges generous funding by the European Research Council Starting Grant 850529: E-DIRECT, and Oliver Hauser is grateful to the UKRI Future Leaders Fellowship and the University of Exeter Business School for their generous support.
This review comes from a themed issue on People-Watching: Interpersonal Perception and Prediction
Edited by Kate Barasz and Tami Kim
Contributor Information
Charlotte S.L. Rossetti, Email: rossetti@evolbio.mpg.de.
Christian Hilbe, Email: hilbe@evolbio.mpg.de.
Oliver P. Hauser, Email: o.hauser@exeter.ac.uk.
References
- 1.Rand D.G., Nowak M.A. Human cooperation. Trends Cognit Sci. 2013;17:413–425. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sigmund K. Princeton University Press; 2010. Calculus of selfishness. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Apicella C.L., Silk J.B. The evolution of human cooperation. Curr Biol. 2019;29:R447–R450. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hauser O.P., Hilbe C., Chatterjee K., Nowak M.A. Social dilemmas among unequals. Nature. 2019;572:524–527. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1488-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Fehr E., Fischbacher U. The nature of human altruism. Nature. 2003;425:785–791. doi: 10.1038/nature02043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Pletzer J.L., Balliet D., Joireman J., Kuhlman D.M., Voelpel S.C., van Lange P.A.M. Social value orientation, expectations, and cooperation in social dilemmas: a meta-analysis. Eur J Pers. 2018;32:62–83. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Santa J.C., Exadaktylos F., Soto-Faraco S. Beliefs about others' intentions determine whether cooperation is the faster choice. Sci Rep. 2018;8:7509. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25926-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Aoyagi M., Fréchette G.R., Yuksel S. 2021. Beliefs in repeated games; pp. 1–77. Working paper. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Alos-Ferrer C., Farolfi F. Trust games and beyond. Front Neurosci. 2019;13:887. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ferrin D.L., Bligh M.C., Kohles J.C. It takes two to tango: an interdependence analysis of the spiralling of perceived trustworthiness and cooperation in interpersonal and intergroup relationships. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2008;107:161–178. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Peysakhovich A., Nowak M.A., Rand D.G. Humans display a ‘cooperative phenotype’ that is domain general and temporally stable. Nat Commun. 2014;5:4939. doi: 10.1038/ncomms5939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Reigstad A., Strømland E., Tinghög G. Extending the cooperative phenotype: assessing the stability of cooperation across countries. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1990. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Barclay P. Biological markets and the effects of partner choice on cooperation and friendship. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015;7 [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fehrler S., Przepiorka W. Charitable giving as a signal of trustworthiness: disentangling the signaling benefits of altruistic acts. Evol Hum Behav. 2013;34:139–145. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Barclay P., Barker J. Greener than thou: people who protect the environment are more cooperative, compete to be environmental, and benefit from reputation. J Environ Psychol. 2020;72:101441. [Google Scholar]
- Vesely S., Klöckner C., Brick C. Pro-environmental behavior as a signal of cooperativeness: evidence from a social dilemma experiment. J Environ Psychol. 2020;67:101362. [Google Scholar]; Players who report performing more pro-environment behavior in their life are expected to be more cooperative in a PGG, are more likely to be chosen as partner for the game, and elicit more cooperation from their co-players. However, there is no evidence that they are actually more cooperative themselves.
- Jordan J., Hoffman M., Bloom P., Rand D.G. Third-party punishment as a costly signal of trustworthiness. Nature. 2016;530:473–476. doi: 10.1038/nature16981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; The study combines a game theoretical model and a behavioral experiment to show that participants use third-party punishment to indicate they are more trustworthy. The experiment finds that punishers are indeed and accurately perceived as more trustworthy. However, if participants could choose whether to help the victim rather than to punish the transgressor, punishment is no longer associated with more trust.
- Rockenbach B., Milinski M. To qualify as a social partner, humans hide severe punishment, although their observed cooperativeness is decisive. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108:18307–18312. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1108996108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Groups of five participants played two blocks of 15 rounds of a public goods game with a costly punishment opportunity. An observer could watch the participants behavior during the first block to decide which of these participants should join them to interact in the second block. The study finds that observers select high contributors, whereas they pay little attention to the participants' punishment behavior.
- 19.Ericsson K., et al. Cultural universals and cultural differences in meta-norms about peer punishment. Manag Organ Rev. 2017;13:851–870. [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman M., Yoeli E., Nowak M. Cooperate without looking: why we care what people think and not just what they do. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112:1727–1732. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1417904112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; The study presents a game-theoretical model to show that individuals may learn to deliberately ignore payoff-related information. By ignoring this information, individuals can credibly commit themselves to cooperate, even if the realized payoffs happen to make defection more profitable.
- 21.Jordan J., Hoffman M., Nowak M.A., Rand D.G. Uncalculating cooperation is used to signal trustworthiness. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113:8658–8663. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601280113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Levine E., Barasch A., Rand D.G., Berman J.Z., Small D.A. Signaling emotion and reason in cooperation. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2018;147:702–719. doi: 10.1037/xge0000399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gardner A. The greenbeard effect. Curr Biol. 2019;29:R425–R473. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.03.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Andreoni J., Vesterlund L. Which is the fair sex? Gender differences in altruism. Q J Econ. 2001;116:293–312. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ortmann A., Tichy L.K. Gender differences in the laboratory: evidence from prisoner's dilemma games. J Econ Behav Organ. 1999;39:327–339. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Croson R., Gneezy U. Gender differences in preferences. J Econ Lit. 2009;47:448–474. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Balliet D., Li N.P., Macfarlan S.J., Van Vugt M. Sex differences in cooperation: a meta-analytic review of social dilemmas. Psychol Bull. 2011;137:881. doi: 10.1037/a0025354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Dorrough A.R., Glöckner A. A cross-national analysis of sex differences in prisoner's dilemma games. Br J Soc Psychol. 2019;58:225–240. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Exley C., Hauser O.P., Moore M., Pezzuto J.-H. 2021. Beliefs about gender differences in social preferences. Working Paper. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Aguiar F., Brañas-Garza P., Cobo-Reyes R., Jimenez N., Miller L.M. Are women expected to be more generous? Exp Econ. 2009;12:93–98. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Brañas-Garza P., Capraro V., Rascon-Ramirez E. Gender differences in altruism on Mechanical Turk: expectations and actual behaviour. Econ Lett. 2018;170:19–23. [Google Scholar]
- 32.De Neys W., Hopfensitz A., Bonnefon J.-F. Split-second trustworthiness detection from faces in an economic game. Exp Psychol. 2017;64:231–239. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Danvers A.F., Shiota M.N. Dynamically engaged smiling predicts cooperation above and beyond average smiling levels. Evol Hum Behav. 2018;39:112–119. [Google Scholar]
- Jaeger B., Oud B., Williams T., Krumhuber E.G., Fehr E., Engelmann J.B. 2020. Can people detect the trustworthiness of strangers based on their facial appearance? pp. 1–20. Working paper. [Google Scholar]; Whether they were making a trustworthiness rating or trust decision (both incentivized), participants were unable to accurately perceive trustworthiness from faces and did not earn more than expected by chance. Neither gender or cropping moderated the results.
- 35.Knowles K., Little A. Vocal fundamental and formant frequencies affect perceptions of speaker cooperativeness. Q J Exp Psychol. 2016;69:1657–1675. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1091484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Andreoni J., Petri R. Beauty, gender and stereotypes: evidence from laboratory experiments. J Econ Psychol. 2008;29:73–93. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wilson R., Eckel C. Judging a book by its cover: beauty and expectations in the trust game. Polit Res Q. 2006;59:189–202. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hamermesh D.S., Biddle J.E. Beauty and the labor market. Am Econ Rev. 1994;84:1174–1194. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Piff P.L., Kraus M.W., Côté B.H., Cheng B.H., Keltner D. Having less, giving more: the influence of social class on prosocial behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010;99:771–784. doi: 10.1037/a0020092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Smeets P., Bauer R., Gneezy U. Giving behavior of millionaires. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112:10641–10644. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1507949112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Qi Y., Li Q., Du F. Are rich people perceived as more trustworthy? Perceived socioeconomic status modulates judgments of trustworthiness and trust behavior based on facial appearance. Front Psychol. 2018;9:512. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Martinangeli A.F.M. Do what (you think) the rich will do: inequality and belief heterogeneity in public good provision. J Econ Psychol. 2021;83:102364. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Trautmann S., Wang X., Wang Y., Xu Y. 2021. Noblesse oblige: holding high-status individuals to higher standards; pp. 1–14. Working paper. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Helénsdotter R. 2019. Experimental evidence on cooperation, political affiliation, and group size; pp. 1–66. Working paper. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Grünhage T., Reuter M. Political orientation is associated with behavior in public-goods and trust-games. Polit Behav. 2020 [Google Scholar]
- 46.Romano A., Sutter M., Liu J.H., Balliet D. Political ideology, cooperation and national parochialism across 42 nations. Phil Trans B. 2021;376:20200146. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Balliet D., Tybur J.M., Wu J., Antonellis C., Van Lange P.A.M. Political ideology, trust, and cooperation: in-group favoritism among republicans and democrats during a US national election. J Conflict Resolut. 2018;62:797–818. doi: 10.1177/0022002716658694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Norenzayan A., Shariff A.F. The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science. 2008;322:58–62. doi: 10.1126/science.1158757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Billingsley J., Gomes C.M., McCullough M. Implicit and explicit influences of religious cognition on dictator game transfers. R Soc Open Sci. 2018;5:170238. doi: 10.1098/rsos.170238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.McCullough M., Swartwout P., Shaver J.H., Carter E.C., Sosis R. Christian religious badges instill trust in Christian and non-Christian perceivers. Psychol Relig Spiritual. 2016;8:149–163. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Everett J., Pizarro D., Crockett M. Inference of trustworthiness from intuitive moral judgments. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2016;145:772–787. doi: 10.1037/xge0000165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bostyn D.H., Roets A. Trust, trolleys and social dilemmas: a replication study. J Exp Psychol. 2017;146 doi: 10.1037/xge0000295. e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capraro V., et al. People making deontological judgments in the Trapdoor dilemma are perceived to be more prosocial in economic games than they actually are. PloS One. 2018;13 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0205066. e0205066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Participants had to make a choice for the trolley problem. Those participants who made a deontological choice rather than a consequentialist one were perceived as more trustworthy and altruistic by their co-player, but they actually were not.
- 54.Chen Y., Li S.X. Group identity and social preferences. Am Econ Rev. 2009;99:431–457. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Goette L., Huffman D., Meier S. The impact of group membership on cooperation and norm enforcement: evidence using random assignment to real social groups. Am Econ Rev. 2006;96:212–216. [Google Scholar]
- Kumar M.M., Tsoi L., Lee M.S., Cone J., McAuliffe K. Nationality dominates gender in decision-making in the dictator and prisoner's dilemma games. PloS One. 2021;16 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244568. e0244568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Participants played a prisoner's dilemma (PD) and a dictator game (DG) with a hypothetical partner of the same or a different nationality and gender. Participants were more likely to cooperate in the PD, and gave more in the DG, with own nationality partners. They also reported expecting more cooperation from own nationality partners.
- 57.Balliet D., Wu J., De Dreu C.K.W. Ingroup favoritism in cooperation: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2014;140:1556. doi: 10.1037/a0037737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Dal Bó P., Fréchette G. The evolution of cooperation in infinitely repeated games: experimental evidence. Am Econ Rev. 2011;101:411–429. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Charness G., Rigotti L., Rustichini A. Social surplus determines cooperation rates in the one-shot Prisoner's Dilemma. Game Econ Behav. 2016;100:113–124. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Rand D.G., Greene J.D., Nowak M.A. Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature. 2012;489:427–430. doi: 10.1038/nature11467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bouwmeester S., et al. Registered replication report: rand, greene, and nowak (2012) Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017;12:527–542. doi: 10.1177/1745691617693624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Evans A.M., Dillon K.D., Rand D.G. Fast but not intuitive, slow but not reflective: decision conflict drives reaction times in social dilemmas. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2015;144:951–966. doi: 10.1037/xge0000107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans A.M., Van De Calseyde P. The effects of observed decision time on expectations of extremity and cooperation. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2017;68:50–59. [Google Scholar]; In hypothetical public good games, fast decisions are accurately perceived as more extreme, but they can either be more selfish or more cooperative. Slow decisions are perceived as more conflicted, and feelings of conflict correlate with extremity perception. The same results are found in a continuous prisoner's dilemma. Here, participants were more likely to make an extreme decision when the other player was a fast decider.
- 64.Balliet D. Communication and cooperation in social dilemmas: a meta-analytic review. J Conflict Resolut. 2010;54:39–57. [Google Scholar]
- 65.He S., Offerman T., van de Ven J. The sources of the communication gap. Manag Sci. 2017;63:2832–2846. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Sparks A., Burleigh T., Barclay P. We can see inside: accurate prediction of Prisoner's Dilemma decisions in announced games following a face-to-face interaction. Evol Hum Behav. 2016;37:210–216. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Jaeger B., Evans A.M., Stel M., van Beest I. Explaining the persistent influence of facial cues in social decision-making. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2019;148:1008–1021. doi: 10.1037/xge0000591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Zylbersztejn A., Babutsidze Z., Hanaki N. Preferences for observable information in a strategic setting: an experiment. J Econ Behav Organ. 2020;170:268–285. [Google Scholar]