Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Mar 9;17(3):e0264394. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0264394

Air pollution and individuals’ mental well-being in the adult population in United Kingdom: A spatial-temporal longitudinal study and the moderating effect of ethnicity

Mary Abed Al Ahad 1,*, Urška Demšar 1, Frank Sullivan 2, Hill Kulu 1
Editor: Bijaya Kumar Padhi3
PMCID: PMC8906596  PMID: 35263348

Abstract

Background

Recent studies suggest an association between ambient air pollution and mental well-being, though evidence is mostly fragmented and inconclusive. Research also suffers from methodological limitations related to study design and moderating effect of key demographics (e.g., ethnicity). This study examines the effect of air pollution on reported mental well-being in United Kingdom (UK) using spatial-temporal (between-within) longitudinal design and assesses the moderating effect of ethnicity.

Methods

Data for 60,146 adult individuals (age:16+) with 349,748 repeated responses across 10-data collection waves (2009–2019) from “Understanding-Society: The-UK-Household-Longitudinal-Study” were linked to annual concentrations of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants using the individuals’ place of residence, given at the local-authority and at the finer Lower-Super-Output-Areas (LSOAs) levels; allowing for analysis at two geographical scales across time. The association between air pollution and mental well-being (assessed through general-health-questionnaire-GHQ12) and its modification by ethnicity and being non-UK born was assessed using multilevel mixed-effect logit models.

Results

Higher odds of poor mental well-being was observed with every 10μg/m3 increase in NO2, SO2, PM10 and PM2.5 pollutants at both LSOAs and local-authority levels. Decomposing air pollution into spatial-temporal (between-within) effects showed significant between, but not within effects; thus, residing in more polluted local-authorities/LSOAs have higher impact on poor mental well-being than the air pollution variation across time within each geographical area. Analysis by ethnicity revealed higher odds of poor mental well-being with increasing concentrations of SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 only for Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other-ethnicities and non-UK born individuals compared to British-white and natives, but not for other ethnic groups.

Conclusion

Using longitudinal individual-level and contextual-linked data, this study highlights the negative effect of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being. Environmental policies to reduce air pollution emissions can eventually improve the mental well-being of people in UK. However, there is inconclusive evidence on the moderating effect of ethnicity.

1. Introduction

Mental health problems are rising noticeably world-wide causing serious socio-economic losses to the societies manifested in diminished work productivity and contributing to higher rates of criminal activity and lack of trust in governments [1, 2]. The global burden of mental diseases is estimated at 32% of years lived with disability and 13% of disability-adjusted life-years [3]. Mental disorders are mainly triggered by genetics and/or by psycho-social risk factors [1]. However, recent literature has shown a relationship between environmental factors including exposure to ambient air pollution and mental well-being that ranges from subjective stress and anxiety to more severe depression and suicidal ideation; though most of the evidence is fragmented and inconclusive [1, 47].

Ambient air pollution is a mixture of particles (e.g., black carbon and particulate matter with diameters less than or equal to 10 μm: PM10 and to 2.5 μm: PM2.5) and gaseous chemicals (e.g., sulphur dioxide: SO2, nitrogen dioxide: NO2, carbon monoxide: CO, and ozone) that are released to the atmosphere from natural processes (e.g., windblown soil, volcano ashes, pollen, dust) or from man-made activities including energy production, livestock farming, traffic exhaust, and industrial and mining processes [8, 9]. Inhalation of air pollutants can have major consequences on the human central nervous system and neuro-behavioural mechanisms [1, 5, 9, 10]. For example, particulate matter of small diameter such as PM1 or PM2.5 might initiate oxidative stress and lead to the formation of inflammatory cytokines that infiltrate the blood-brain barrier causing neurodegeneration and neuroinflammation [11]. Specifically, exposure to ambient PM2.5 results in depressive responses and increased hippocampal pro-inflammatory cytokines [11], while exposure to PM1 leads to increased inflammation and reactive oxygen species (ROS) generation and impacts learning and memory [12]. In this context, traffic related air pollution (e.g., particulate matter and NO2) have been linked by observational research to increased rates of mental health problems including: autism spectrum disorders [13], schizophrenia [14], dementia [15], psychotic experiences [16, 17], cognitive disabilities [18], anxiety and major depressive disorders [19]. Moreover, higher rates of emergency hospital admissions for depressive disorders have been found on more polluted days in Canada [2022]; and self-reported mental well-being was associated with long-term exposure to NO2, PM10, and carbon monoxide pollution in Korea [6].

Air pollution can also affect mental well-being indirectly through nuisance and individuals’ coping behaviours. Air pollution can result in cognitive anxiety, stress, and loneliness leading to general fatigue and perceived symptoms of poor mental well-being due to aesthetic/odorous nuisance and inhibition of psychological-supporting outdoor activities [2325]. For example, people may prefer to stay indoors rather than enjoying outdoor activities during periods of high air pollution, especially when air pollution is characterised by visible signs (e.g., colour) and/or bad odours [26]. In a recent systematic literature review of 178 published articles, air pollution was shown to decrease happiness and life satisfaction substantially, and to increase anxiety, annoyance, mental problems, suicidal ideation, and coping approaches such as avoidance behaviour and migration [2].

Air pollution has been also linked to stress and experiential anxiety emerging from worrying feelings about one’s physical health and future [27]. Due to the more conclusive research about the effect of air pollution on physical health including cardiopulmonary, immune system and cancer diseases [8, 9, 28], people living in highly polluted areas might experience stress and worrisome feelings of getting physical illness, which impairs their mental well-being.

Despite the establishment of linkages between air pollution and mental well-being in the literature, results are generally inconclusive and suffer from methodological drawbacks related to the chosen study design and methods of estimating air pollution [16]. Most of the studies are either cross-sectional studies or longitudinal studies that lack spatial-temporal specificity and a lengthy follow-up time. To date, no study has tried to address the association between long-term (11 years) air pollution exposure and mental well-being using a spatial-temporal (between-within) longitudinal design. A between-within analysis can determine the spatial time-constant cross-sectional (average 11 years air pollution) effect of air pollution on mental well-being between different geographical areas (e.g., local authorities or census output areas) as well as the temporal time-varying longitudinal (yearly air pollution deviation from the 11 years average) effect of air pollution on mental well-being within each geographical area. In other words, this analysis reveals whether living in more polluted local authorities or census output area is the driving cause for poor mental well-being (between) or whether it is the fluctuation in air pollution across time within each local authority or census output area (within) that is causing poor mental well-being. This type of analysis combines both cross-sectional and longitudinal designs; thus establishing a more robust measurement of the effect of air pollution [29].

Furthermore, published research has not yet covered population subgroups and the potential moderating effect that key demographic groups might have on the association between air pollution and mental well-being. To date, only age (young vs elderly population) and gender have been reported as effect modifiers for the association between air pollution and mental well-being. In Korea, a more pronounced risk of stress and depression from air pollution exposure was observed among men than among women and among people aged less than 65 [6]. In China, exposure to increased concentrations of ambient air pollution showed a greater risk for mental health problems and general well-being among female college students [30]. Therefore, examining how the effect of air pollution on mental well-being varies by other key demographic characteristics such as ethnicity can provide a more conclusive explanation for the association between air pollution and mental well-being. Earlier literature has shown that ethnic minorities suffer from relatively higher levels of stress, depression, and self-harm [31, 32]. This could be attributed to their lower socio-economic status and to living in more deprived ethnic communities with poor housing and neighbourhood conditions [3335]. Ethnicity was also examined in the literature from the lens of migration and being a non-native resident. A systematic literature review in Sweden showed increased risks of depression and psychotic problems among immigrants compared to the native population [36]. Given the higher observed risk of mental problems among ethnic minorities and immigrants, the effect of air pollution on mental well-being among ethnic groups should be investigated.

Accordingly, this study investigates longitudinally the overall and the spatial-temporal (between-within) effects of long-term (11 years) exposure to NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollution in the UK on individuals’ reported mental well-being measured using the 12 items General Health Questionnaire (GHQ12) scale. Unlike other studies that assess the effect of air pollution on well-being using one geographical scale, our study aims to assess the effect of air pollution exposure on mental well-being at two geographical scales, coarse local authorities (council areas in Scotland) and detailed Lower Super Output Areas (LSOAs; data zones in Scotland). This will allow us to compare the results between the two geographical scales and explore in more detail the local-contextual patterns of the effect of air pollution on mental well-being. Additionally, our study aims to consider both space and time by determining whether living in more polluted geographical areas (local authorities and LSOAs) is the driving cause for poor mental well-being (between) or whether it is the variation in air pollution across time within each geographical area (within) that is causing poor mental well-being; thus providing detailed spatial-temporal evidence for policymaking decisions. Finally, we aim to investigate whether ethnic minorities including Pakistani/Bangladeshi, Indians, Black/African/Caribbean, mixed and other ethnicities as well as non-UK born individuals suffer from a more pronounced risk for mental well-being with increasing concentrations of the four air pollutants compared to British-White ethnicity and UK-born individuals, respectively.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Study design and population

This study utilises individual-level data from the “Understanding Society: The UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS)” [37]. The UKHLS is a rich longitudinal dataset that is composed of 10 data collection panels/waves over a period of 12 years from (2009–2020) with about 40,000 households enrolled at the first wave from the four UK nations: England, Wales, Scotland, and Northern Ireland.

The dataset contains yearly information on the self-reported general health and mental well-being of individuals and on their socio-demographic characteristics including gender, age, educational qualification, marital status, occupation, perceived financial situation, ethnicity, and country of birth. In addition, the dataset collects yearly information on individuals’ lifestyle factors such as cigarette smoking and on contextual factors such as the local authority/council area and the Lower Super Output Areas (LSOAs)/data zones where households are located [37].

The UKHLS main survey sample is composed of four data collection sub-samples as summarised in Fig 1 and described in detail in other publications [3739].

Fig 1. A diagram showing the four data collection sub-samples of the UKHLS main survey.

Fig 1

The present study utilises individual-level data from the UKHLS survey on 60,146 adult (age: 16+) individuals with 349,748 repeated responses (at least 2 repeated responses per individual) collected within 10 waves over a period of 11 years (2009–2019). It should be noted, however, that the initial UKHLS adult survey includes a total of 87,045 individuals with 444,181 repeated responses. We dropped 94,433 observations due to the reasons described in Fig 2.

Fig 2. A diagram showing the reasons for dropping survey responses from the UKHLS longitudinal panel data.

Fig 2

2.2. Variables and measurements

2.2.1. Individuals’ reported mental well-being

Individuals’ reported mental well-being was measured using the 12-items “General Health Questionnaire (GHQ12)” scale [40] which is widely used in the population health research to capture non-psychotic psychiatric illness. The GHQ12 scale is composed of 12 questions about the individuals’ experience of 12 symptoms related to mental well-being in the past few weeks preceding data collection date. The 12 questions are: 1) Ability to concentrate; 2) Losing much sleep; 3) Playing a useful part; 4) Capability of taking decisions; 5) Being under stress; 6) Inability to overcome difficulties; 7) Enjoying normal activities; 8) Being able to face up problems; 9) Feeling unhappy and depressed; 10) Losing confidence; 11) Thinking of self as worthless; and 12) Feeling reasonably happy [41]. Individuals are then asked to rate the negative questions as 0 = ‘not at all’, 1 = ‘no more than usual’, 2 = ‘rather more than usual’, 3 = ‘much more than usual’ and the positive questions as 0 = ‘better than usual’, 1 = ‘same as usual’, 2 = ‘less than usual’, and 3 = ‘much less than usual’ [42]. Two methods are used by relevant literature to construct the overall GHQ12 well-being score. The first and most used method is the (0-0-1-1) method whereby responses for each of the 12 questions of the GHQ12 scale are dichotomised (0 and 1 into 0; 2 and 3 into 1) and then the 12 items are summed up resulting in a general mental well-being score ranging from 0 to 12 with higher scores indicating poorer mental well-being [4246]. The second method aims to construct a Likert scale score (0-1-2-3) by adding up all the 12 items of the GHQ12 scale resulting in a total mental well-being score ranging from 0 to 36 with higher scores indicating poorer mental well-being [47, 48].

For the present study, we used both methods for the GHQ12 scale, the (0-0-1-1) and the (0-1-2-3) method. Given that the scores of the GHQ12 scale are right-skewed and based on relevant literature, we dichotomised the overall GHQ12 scale using two cut off points for the GHQ12 (0–12): our sample mean GHQ12 (0–12) score = 1.8 ∼ 2 (GHQ0-12 ≥ 2) and the GHQ12 (0–12) score of greater than or equal to 4 (GHQ0-12 ≥ 4) as an indication of poor mental well-being [4346, 49, 50]. The GHQ12 (0–36) score was dichotomised based on one cut off point of greater than or equal to 12 (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12) as an indication of poor mental well-being [47, 48]. Therefore, GHQ0-12 and GHQ0-36 total scores were treated as binary variables: GHQ0-12 ≥ 2 (0 = scores <2, good mental well-being; 1 = scores ≥2, poor mental well-being); GHQ0-12 ≥ 4 (0 = scores <4, good mental well-being; 1 = scores ≥4, poor mental well-being); and GHQ0-36 ≥ 12 (0 = scores <12, good mental well-being; 1 = scores ≥12, poor mental well-being).

2.2.2. Air pollution data

Raster data of annual mean concentrations of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants available up to the year of 2019, measured in μg/m3, and estimated using air dispersion models at a 1x1 km spatial resolution were downloaded from the “Department for Environment Food and Rural Affairs (DEFRA)” online data repository [51]. These air pollution maps at 1x1 km resolution are modelled each year by DEFRA under the “Defra’s Modelling of Ambient Air Quality (MAAQ) contract” and are used to provide policy support in the UK and to fulfil the UK’s reporting obligations to Europe [51]. The 1x1 km air pollution raster data are the finest spatial resolution data that can be downloaded from DEFRA and are sufficient to obtain good modelling estimates [52, 53].

These data were first projected using the UK National Grid projection system in ArcGIS Pro software. Then for each of the 391 local authorities/council areas in the UK, we computed the average concentrations of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants from all the 1x1 km raster cells that fell within the boundaries of the respective local authorities/council areas for each year from 2009 up to 2019. Next, we linked the average concentrations of air pollution at the local authority level to the UKHLS dataset for each individual and each year of follow up (2009–2019).

To minimise exposure bias and establish more robust results from a spatial perspective, we also linked the 1x1 km raster air pollution data to the UKHLS data at the level of Lower Super Output Areas (LSOAs; data zones for Scotland and Super Output Areas for Northern Ireland), a finer geographical scale, for each individual and each year of follow up (2009–2019). LSOAs are used to decompose England and Wales based on the population size into areas with a minimum population size of 1000 people and are the lowest level of geography offered by the UKHLS dataset. The LSOAs in England and Wales are equivalent to data zones in Scotland and to Super Output Areas in Northern Ireland. For simplicity we refer to the joint LSOAs, data zones, and Super Output Areas as LSOAs. Using these smaller spatial units, we ran our analysis at a smaller geographic scale than local authorities, which allowed us to explore local patterns of the effect of air pollution.

A map showing the local authorities in the UK (council areas in Scotland) and an enlarged subset of 20 local authorities in the south-east of UK with an example of PM10 concentrations at 1x1 km resolution for the year of 2017 for Tower Hamlets local authority and its corresponding LSOAs is used to clarify the process of linking air pollution to the UKHLS dataset at the two geographical scales, the coarse local authorities and the detailed LSOAs (Fig 3).

Fig 3. A map showing the local authorities in the UK and an enlarged subset of 20 local authorities in the south-east of UK with an example of PM10 concentrations at 1x1 km grid for the year of 2017 for Tower Hamlets local authority and its corresponding LSOAs.

Fig 3

The map was constructed by the authors in ArcGIS Pro software using PM10 air pollution shapefile for the year of 2017 downloaded from the DEFRA online data repository [51], local authorities UK boundaries shapefile downloaded from the Office for National Statistics [54], and LSOAs and data zones UK boundaries also downloaded from the Office for National Statistics, National Records of Scotland, and Northern Ireland Statistics [55]. Both DEFRA and Office for National Statistics shapefiles are governed under the Open Government Licence v.3.0.

2.2.3. Socio-demographic and lifestyle covariates

A list of individual-level socio-demographic and lifestyle covariates summarised in Table 1 was selected a priori for this study based on what is available in the UKHLS dataset and based on the potential confounders considered by relevant literature. Most of the researchers in the field of air pollution and mental well-being have considered age [4, 6, 16], gender [2, 4, 6, 16], education [4, 6], marital status [6], socio-economic deprivation and occupation [16, 56], ethnicity [1, 7, 16], cigarette smoking [16, 56], alcohol drinking [4, 16], physical exercise [4, 16], and body mass index [4] as potential confounders. Specifically, poor mental well-being, stress, and depression have been associated with younger or older ages, women, cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking, physical inactivity, lower education, divorced/widowed individuals, lower household income, and belonging to an ethnic minority group [6, 3133], which in turn confounds the association between air pollution and mental well-being outcomes. In addition, environmental-contextual factors such as noise pollution [1, 16], neighbourhood disorder [16], green spaces availability [25, 57], air temperature [57], and seasonality [16, 58] have been also considered as potential confounders in the association between air pollution and mental well-being.

Table 1. The socio-demographics and lifestyle covariates selected for this study.
Covariates Coding
Gender 1 = Male
2 = Female
Age Coded as 16–18 and then in 5 years increments (19–23; 24–28; 29–33; 34–38; 39–43; 44–48; 49–53; 54–58; 59–63; 59–63; 64–68; 69–73; 74–78; >78)
Ethnicity 1 = British white
2 = Other white
3 = Indian
4 = Pakistani/Bangladeshi
5 = Black/African/Caribbean
6 = Mixed ethnicities
7 = Other ethnicities
Country of birth 1 = Born in UK
2 = Not born in UK
3 = No answer
Marital status 1 = Married
2 = Living as a couple
3 = Widowed
4 = Divorced/separated
5 = Single never married
6 = No answer
Educational qualification 1 = University degree
2 = High school degree
3 = Lower educational qualifications
4 = Other qualifications
5 = Still a student
Occupation 1 = Managers/Professionals/employers
2 = Non manual workers
3 = Manual workers
4 = Not applicable: Student/retired/Not working
5 = No answer
Perceived financial situation 1 = Living comfortably/doing alright
2 = Living difficultly
3 = No answer
Cigarette smokinga 0 = Non-smoker
1 = Smoker
2 = No answer

Cigarette smokinga: data for cigarette smoking were missing for wave 1 responses and for waves 3 and 4 among individuals aged more than 21 years. Therefore, we replicated the smoking status responses of wave 2 in wave 1 for each individual and in waves 3 and 4 for each individual aged more than 21 years. The smoking status in waves 1, 3, and 4 was coded into “no answer” for individuals who were not present in wave 2.

2.3. Data analysis

Individuals’ socio-demographic and lifestyle factors were described using percentages for each wave (waves 1 to 10) of the UKHLS sample.

The mean of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 concentrations and the Pearson’s correlation between the four pollutants was computed at both geographical scales, the coarse local authorities and the detailed LSOAs. Given the high observed correlations between NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants (Pearson’s coefficient ≥ 0.7 [59]; Tables 3 and 4), the association of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 with individuals’ reported mental well-being was analysed in separate regression models. Nevertheless, low to moderate correlation was observed between SO2 and each of the other three pollutants which enabled the construction of bi-pollutant models adjusting the NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 models for the SO2 pollutant.

Table 3. Correlation matrix of air pollutants at the LSOAs level (N = 42,619 LSOAs).

NO2 (μg/m3) SO2 (μg/m3) PM10 (μg/m3) PM2.5 (μg/m3)
NO2 (μg/m3) 1.00
SO2 (μg/m3) 0.37 1.00
PM10 (μg/m3) 0.76 0.28 1.00
PM2.5 (μg/m3) 0.79 0.32 0.97 1.00

Table 4. Correlation matrix of air pollutants at the local authority level (N = 391 local authorities).

NO2 (μg/m3) SO2 (μg/m3) PM10 (μg/m3) PM2.5 (μg/m3)
NO2 (μg/m3) 1.00
SO2 (μg/m3) 0.50 1.00
PM10 (μg/m3) 0.77 0.38 1.00
PM2.5 (μg/m3) 0.81 0.42 0.97 1.00

Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) were also calculated to assess the homogeneity in the mental well-being scores within individuals and household clusters; with an ICC of greater than 0.3 indicating moderate to fair homogeneity [60]. Given the presence of 42% and 49% homogeneity (ICC = 0.42 and ICC = 0.49; Table 5) for the mental well-being scores of GHQ0-12 and GHQ0-36 within individuals’ clusters across time, respectively, the mean score of mental well-being was computed from predictions of mixed effects linear models, adjusting for age in fixed effects and for the individual ID in random intercept.

Table 5. Intraclass correlation coefficient for within individual and household clusters.

Mental well-being GHQ12 scale (0–12) Mental well-being GHQ12 scale (0–36)
Individual ID ICC [95%CI] 0.42 [0.41, 0.42] 0.49 [0.48, 0.49]
N of surveys 349,748
N of individuals 60,146
Meana (SD) 1.81 (0.21) 11.14 (0.05)
Household ID ICC [95%CI] 0.16 [0.16, 0.17] 0.18 [0.18, 0.19]
N of surveys 349,748
N of households 217,009

Moderate to fair ICCs>0.3 are highlighted in italic-bold; Meana is based on predictions from mixed effects linear models which are adjusted for age in fixed effects and for the individual ID in random intercept.

The association between the three binary measures (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12; GHQ0-12 ≥ 2; GHQ0-12 ≥ 4) of individuals’ reported mental well-being and each of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants (linked at two geographical scales: once at the LSOAs and once at the local authorities level) was examined separately and in bi-pollutant models adjusted for SO2 using three-levels (repeated individual observations across time nested within LSOAs or local authorities) mixed-effect logit models, adjusting for the socio-demographic and lifestyle covariates and for the year (2009–2019) dummies. This type of analysis was chosen as it fits the longitudinal panel design of the study which involves repeated individual responses across time linked to air pollution data at the LSOA or local authority level whereby repeated individual responses are nested within LSOAs or local authorities. The individual-level random intercept is necessary in the multilevel models given the high homogeneity in the individual’s responses across time (ICC = 0.42 and ICC = 0.49; Table 5), while the local authority or LSOAs random intercept is needed to allow for less biased assessments of the contextual-linked air pollution effect on mental well-being [61]; resulting in three-levels mixed-effect logit models. In a supplementary analysis, we also show the association of individuals’ reported mental well-being with each of the socio-demographic and lifestyle covariates (Table 1 in S1 File). It should be noted, however, that our models did not account for the household clustering in random intercepts due to the low observed homogeneity in the well-being responses within each household cluster (ICC = 0.16 and ICC = 0.18; Table 5).

In additional analysis, we decomposed the overall effect of air pollution (linked at two geographical scales: LSOAs and local authorities) on mental well-being into between (spatial) and within (temporal) effects. Between effects were used to examine the spatial time-constant effect of air pollution between different geographical areas (local authorities and LSOAs) by calculating the average concentrations of NO2, SO2, PM10 and PM2.5 pollutants across the 11 years of follow up (2009–2019) for each local authority and each LSOA. On the other hand, within effects were used to assess the temporal time-varying effect of air pollution within each geographical area by calculating the annual air pollution deviation from the 11 years average air pollution for each local authority and LSOA. Therefore, two sets of four multilevel mixed effect logit models (one for each pollutant) were used to examine the overall (Eq 1) and the between-within (Eq 2) effects of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being, respectively, at the two geographical scales (coarse local authorities and detailed LSOAs).

ln(Ytij1Ytij)=β0+U0ij+U0j+β1overallpollutantconcentrationtij+β2Agetij+β3Gendertij+β4Ethnicitytij+β5Countryofbirthtij+β6Maritalstatustij+β7Educationtij+β8Occupationtij+β8Perceivedfinancialsituationtij+β9Smokingstatustij+β10Yeardummiesij (1)
ln(Ytij1Ytij)=β0+U0ij+U0j+β1Betweenpollutantconcentrationtij+β2Withinpollutantconcentrationtij+β3Agetij+β4Gendertij+β5Ethnicitytij+β6Countryofbirthtij+β7Maritalstatustij+β8Educationtij+β9Occupationtij+β10Perceivedfinancialsituationtij+β11Smokingstatustij+β12Yeardummiesij (2)

Where Ytij is the mental well-being outcome for individual i, in local authority or LSOA j at year t; β1, β2 …. β12 are the slopes of fixed effects; β0 is the fixed intercept; U0ij is level 2 random intercept of individuals nested in local authorities or LSOAs; U0j is level 3 random intercept of local authorities or LSOAs.

To assess the effect modification of ethnicity and country of birth on the association between air pollution and mental well-being, we added interaction terms between ethnicity and each of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants and between country of birth and each of the four pollutants. These interaction terms were added into the models investigating the overall effect of air pollution and into the models examining the between and within effects of air pollution at the two geographical scales of local authorities and LSOAs, separately (i.e., one interaction term at a time, once with the between effect and once with the within effect). The interaction results were visualised with coefficient plots.

To estimate the cohort effect and to balance the follow up time, we repeated the same multilevel mixed effect logit modelling in a sensitivity analysis only for individuals recruited during the first wave of the UKHLS.

STATA software (StataCorp. 2015. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP) was used for statistical analysis and ArcGIS Pro software was used for spatial pre-processing of air pollution data. Odd ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence (CIs) for every 10 μg/m3 increase in air pollution were used to report the study findings. Statistical significance was considered at a P-value < 0.05.

2.4. Ethical considerations

This paper was granted ethical approval on the 14th of May 2020 by the authors’ affiliated institution (School of Geography and Sustainable Development Ethics Committee, acting on behalf of the University Teaching and Research Ethics Committee (UTREC) at the University of St Andrews). The paper uses secondary adult (age 16+) fully anonymised data from the “Understanding Society: The UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS)” and authors did not have access to potentially identifying information; thus, obtaining participants’ informed consent is not applicable and was waved by the authors’ institution ethics committee. The University of Essex responsible for the UKHLS data collection and management has already obtained written informed consent from all the study participants [37]. Requesting consent for health record linkage was approved at Wave 1 by the National Research Ethics Service (NRES) Oxfordshire REC A (08/H0604/124), and at Wave 4 by NRES Southampton REC A (11/SC/0274). Approval for the collection of biosocial data by trained nurses in Waves 2 and 3 of the main survey was obtained from the National Research Ethics Service (Understanding Society—UK Household Longitudinal Study: A Biosocial Component, Oxfordshire A REC, Reference: 10/H0604/2).

3. Results

3.1. Individuals’ socio-demographic and lifestyle characteristics

A total of 60,146 adult individuals with 349,748 repeated responses over 11 years (2009–2019) of 10 data collection waves were included in this study. The mean of observations per individual was 5.81 (SD = 2.81) with a minimum of 2 observations per individual and the average follow up time was 5.58 (SD = 2.98) years.

Description of the individuals’ socio-demographic and lifestyle characteristics for the 10 data collection waves of the UKHLS are summarised in Table 2. For all waves, the majority of individuals were females, belonged to the middle-aged group (34–58 years), were married, had either a university or high school degree, were non-manual workers (if working), were living comfortably/doing alright financially, and were cigarette non-smokers (Table 2).

Table 2. Description of individual’s socio-demographic and lifestyle factors for each wave of the UKHLS data (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).

Wave1 (2009–2011) Wave2 (2010–2012) Wave3 (2011–2013) Wave4 (2012–2014) Wave5 (2013–2015) Wave6 (2014–2016) Wave7 (2015–2017) Wave8 (2016–2018) Wave9 (2017–2019) Wave10 (2018–2019)
N = 31,258 N = 39,858 N = 38,632 N = 37,315 N = 35,190 N = 36,349 N = 35,572 N = 34,348 N = 31,741 N = 29,485
Gender Male 43.2% 43.6% 43.8% 44.0% 44.3% 44.3% 44.4% 44.4% 44.3% 44.0%
Female 56.8% 56.4% 56.2% 56.0% 55.7% 55.7% 55.6% 55.6% 55.7% 56.0%
Age Young (<34) 26.2% 25.4% 25.8% 25.2% 25.1% 24.6% 24.0% 23.3% 22.5% 20.8%
Middle age (34–58) 46.1% 44.8% 45.3% 44.7% 44.5% 44.7% 44.3% 43.7% 43.4% 43.5%
Old (>58) 27.7% 29.7% 28.9% 30.1% 30.4% 30.7% 31.6% 33.0% 34.2% 35.7%
Ethnicity British white 81.4% 82.5% 81.8% 81.8% 82.0% 76.6% 76.5% 76.9% 78.2% 79.1%
Other white 4.1% 4.8% 4.7% 4.6% 4.4% 5.6% 5.5% 5.4% 5.3% 5.1%
Indian 3.1% 2.5% 2.6% 2.6% 2.6% 3.8% 3.9% 3.9% 3.7% 3.6%
Pakistani/Bangladeshi 3.5% 2.7% 3.0% 3.0% 3.1% 4.6% 4.7% 4.8% 4.6% 4.5%
Black/African/Caribbean 4.0% 3.0% 3.4% 3.3% 3.3% 4.3% 4.4% 4.0% 3.6% 3.3%
Mixed ethnicities 1.6% 1.3% 1.5% 1.5% 1.6% 1.8% 1.8% 1.8% 1.8% 1.7%
Other ethnicities 2.3% 3.1% 3.1% 3.0% 3.0% 3.3% 3.2% 3.1% 2.8% 2.8%
Country of birth Born in the UK 86.3% 67.7% 67.0% 68.0% 68.6% 66.3% 66.7% 67.1% 68.2% 68.4%
Not born in the UK 13.7% 10.5% 10.5% 10.5% 10.3% 14.3% 14.3% 13.9% 12.6% 12.0%
No answer 0.0% 21.8% 22.4% 21.5% 21.1% 19.5% 18.9% 19.0% 19.2% 19.6%
Marital status Married 53.2% 53.6% 52.7% 52.1% 51.7% 52.9% 52.8% 53.2% 53.8% 55.2%
Living as a couple 11.8% 11.5% 11.6% 11.8% 11.6% 10.8% 10.7% 10.5% 10.0% 9.6%
Widowed 5.5% 5.9% 5.7% 5.8% 5.8% 5.7% 5.7% 5.8% 5.9% 5.9%
Divorced/separated 9.1% 8.4% 8.5% 8.6% 8.5% 8.1% 8.0% 7.9% 7.9% 8.1%
Single never married 20.4% 20.6% 21.7% 21.5% 22.2% 22.2% 22.6% 22.5% 22.1% 20.8%
No answer 0.1% 0.0% 0.0% 0.1% 0.2% 0.4% 0.2% 0.2% 0.3% 0.4%
Educational qualification University degree 31.9% 25.5% 27.2% 28.1% 29.3% 29.8% 30.6% 31.6% 32.8% 34.3%
High school degree 32.9% 25.7% 26.2% 26.5% 26.7% 26.2% 26.4% 26.7% 27.0% 26.9%
Lower educational levels 1.4% 1.1% 1.1% 1.1% 1.1% 1.0% 1.0% 1.0% 1.0% 1.0%
Other qualifications 27.4% 40.8% 38.7% 37.6% 36.3% 36.5% 35.5% 34.7% 33.5% 33.3%
Still a student 6.3% 6.9% 6.8% 6.7% 6.6% 6.5% 6.5% 5.9% 5.6% 4.6%
Occupation Managers/Professionals/employers 12.4% 12.1% 12.0% 12.3% 12.4% 12.1% 12.1% 12.2% 12.1% 11.9%
Non manual workers 27.5% 27.1% 27.6% 27.3% 28.0% 27.5% 27.7% 27.1% 26.8% 26.5%
Manual workers 17.9% 17.9% 18.2% 17.9% 18.3% 18.2% 18.3% 17.8% 17.2% 16.3%
Not applicable: Student/ retired/Not working 42.0% 42.7% 41.6% 42.1% 40.9% 41.6% 41.3% 41.9% 41.8% 42.6%
No answer 0.2% 0.2% 0.7% 0.4% 0.3% 0.6% 0.6% 1.0% 2.2% 2.7%
Perceived financial situation living comfortably/doing alright 59.9% 62.3% 62.0% 64.5% 66.3% 70.8% 72.4% 73.1% 71.7% 71.6%
living difficultly 40.0% 37.6% 37.9% 35.4% 33.6% 29.0% 27.5% 26.7% 28.1% 28.2%
no answer 0.1% 0.1% 0.1% 0.1% 0.1% 0.3% 0.2% 0.2% 0.2% 0.2%
Cigarette smoking non-smoker 73.8% 79.2% 70.7% 69.3% 82.1% 77.3% 84.4% 85.3% 86.6% 87.0%
smoker 19.6% 20.8% 19.0% 18.6% 17.9% 15.6% 15.5% 14.6% 13.3% 12.8%
no answer 6.6% 0.1% 10.4% 12.1% 0.0% 7.1% 0.1% 0.1% 0.1% 0.2%
Nation England 83.1% 75.6% 75.8% 76.1% 76.9% 78.7% 78.7% 78.7% 78.2% 78.3%
Wales 5.0% 7.8% 7.9% 7.8% 7.5% 6.6% 6.4% 6.5% 6.6% 6.5%
Scotland 7.4% 9.5% 9.5% 9.3% 9.5% 8.5% 8.6% 8.5% 8.7% 8.8%
Northern Ireland 4.4% 7.1% 6.7% 6.8% 6.1% 6.3% 6.2% 6.2% 6.5% 6.4%

For ethnicity, most individuals were UK-born (86% in wave 1) and belonged to the British-white group (81% in wave 1). The description of other ethnic groups in wave 1 is as follows: Other-white (4%), Indians (3%), Pakistani/Bangladeshi (3.5%), Black/African/Caribbean (4%), mixed ethnicities (1.6%), and other ethnicities (2%) (Table 2).

3.2. Description of air pollution

3.2.1. Description of air pollution at the LSOAs level

Fig 4 shows the average yearly concentrations of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants across the 42,619 LSOAs in the UK from 2009 to 2019. Air pollution showed fluctuations across time with lower concentrations seen in the last 5 years (2015–2019) of observation compared to previous years for all four pollutants (Fig 4).

Fig 4. The annual mean of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants at the LSOAs level in the UK from the year of 2009 to 2019 (N = 42,619 LSOAs).

Fig 4

We also observed high correlations (Pearson’s coefficient ≥ 0.7) between NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants (Table 3), which could be attributed to the source of emission and the atmospheric chemical reactions between these pollutants. For example, the major source of NO2 and particulate matter (PM10 and PM2.5) emissions is traffic exhaust [62, 63], while industrial processes and power plants are the major sources of SO2 pollution [64].

3.2.2. Description of air pollution at the local authority level

Similar to the air pollution at the LSOAs level, air pollution at the local authority level also showed fluctuations across time with lower concentrations seen in the last 5 years (2015–2019) of observation compared to previous years (2009–2014) for all four pollutants (Fig 5). Likewise, high correlations (Pearson’s coefficient ≥ 0.7) between NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants were noticed (Table 4).

Fig 5. The annual mean of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants at the local authority level in the UK from the year of 2009 to 2019 (N = 391 local authorities).

Fig 5

3.3. Description of individuals’ reported mental well-being

The mean score for mental well-being GHQ12 (0–12) scale was 1.8 (SD = 0.2) with 30% of responses having a GHQ12 score of 2 or more and 19% having a GHQ12 score of 4 or more. The mean score for GHQ12 (0–36) Likert scale was 11.14 (SD = 0.05) with 36% of responses having a score of 12 or more. The ICC was 0.42 and 0.49 for GHQ12 (0–12) and GHQ12 (0–36), respectively, indicating a moderate homogeneity in the well-being responses within the individual clusters over time, whilst low homogeneity (ICC = 0.16 and ICC = 0.18) was detected within the household clusters (Table 5).

3.4. The effect of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being

3.4.1. The effect of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being at the LSOAs level

Higher odds of poor mental well-being were observed with every 10 μg/m3 increase in NO2 (ORs ranging between 1.12–1.19), SO2 (ORs ranging between 1.29–1.49), PM10 (ORs ranging between 1.19–1.34), and PM2.5 (ORs ranging between 1.30–1.53) air pollutants (Table 6).

Table 6. The association of individuals’ mental well-being with each of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants linked at the LSOAs level in separate models (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).
Mental well-being (GHQ0-36a ≥ 12) Mental well-being (GHQ0-12b ≥ 2) Mental well-being (GHQ0-12b ≥ 4)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2
OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI]
Overall pollution effect
NO2 (μg/m3) 1.13 [1.10, 1.16]** 1.12 [1.09, 1.15]** 1.19 [1.16, 1.21]** 1.14 [1.11, 1.17]** 1.16 [1.13, 1.20]** 1.12 [1.09, 1.16]**
SO2 (μg/m3) 1.45 [1.31, 1.61]** 1.30 [1.18, 1.44]** 1.44 [1.30, 1.59]** 1.29 [1.17, 1.42]** 1.49 [1.33, 1.67]** 1.31 [1.17, 1.47]**
PM10 (μg/m3) 1.19 [1.12, 1.27]** 1.22 [1.15, 1.30]** 1.34 [1.26, 1.42]** 1.28 [1.20, 1.36]** 1.25 [1.17, 1.34]** 1.23 [1.15, 1.31]**
PM2.5 (μg/m3) 1.30 [1.20, 1.42]** 1.35 [1.24, 1.47]** 1.53 [1.41, 1.66]** 1.44 [1.33, 1.56]** 1.41 [1.29, 1.55]** 1.38 [1.25, 1.51]**
Between pollution effect
NO2 (μg/m3) 1.12 [1.09, 1.15]** 1.11 [1.08, 1.15]** 1.18 [1.15, 1.22]** 1.13 [1.10, 1.17]** 1.16 [1.12, 1.19]** 1.12 [1.08, 1.15]**
SO2 (μg/m3) 3.78 [2.98, 4.79]** 2.21 [1.77, 2.76]** 2.58 [2.07, 3.23]** 1.59 [1.29, 1.96]** 3.51 [2.73, 4.50]** 1.94 [1.53, 2.45]**
PM10 (μg/m3) 1.17 [1.09, 1.25]** 1.21 [1.13, 1.30]** 1.34 [1.26, 1.43]** 1.28 [1.19, 1.36]** 1.24 [1.15, 1.33]** 1.21 [1.13, 1.31]**
PM2.5 (μg/m3) 1.29 [1.17, 1.42]** 1.36 [1.23, 1.50]** 1.57 [1.43, 1.72]** 1.47 [1.34, 1.61]** 1.40 [1.26, 1.55]** 1.36 [1.23, 1.51]**
Within pollution effect
NO2 (μg/m3) 1.06 [0.95, 1.18] 1.01 [0.91, 1.13] 1.08 [0.97, 1.20] 1.04 [0.94, 1.16] 1.10 [0.97, 1.25] 1.06 [0.94, 1.20]
SO2 (μg/m3) 0.93 [0.78, 1.12] 0.95 [0.80, 1.13] 0.99 [0.83, 1.18] 1.04 [0.87, 1.23] 0.93 [0.76, 1.14] 0.99 [0.81, 1.20]
PM10 (μg/m3) 1.15 [0.96, 1.38] 1.08 [0.91, 1.29] 1.19 [0.99, 1.42] 1.10 [0.92, 1.31] 1.33 [1.08, 1.64]** 1.23 [0.99, 1.51]
PM2.5 (μg/m3) 1.17 [0.95, 1.45] 1.09 [0.88, 1.35] 1.26 [1.02, 1.55]* 1.14 [0.92, 1.41] 1.35 [1.06, 1.73]* 1.24 [0.97, 1.59]

**P-value <0.01

*P-value<0.05

ORs and 95%CIs are expressed in terms of 10 μg/m3 increase in the air pollutants.

GHQ0-36a: GHQ scale composed of 12 questions, each scored using a Likert format: 0-1-2-3 and summed up by adding all the items generating a scale ranging from 0 to 36; the 0–36 scale is dichotomised using a cut-off score of 12 based on relevant literature into good mental well-being (score<12) and poor mental well-being (score ≥ 12)

GHQ0-12b: GHQ scale composed of 12 questions, each scored using a simple binary format: 0-0-1-1 and summed up by adding all the items generating a scale ranging from 0 to 12; the 0–12 scale is dichotomised using two cut-off scores of 2 and 4 based on relevant literature into good mental well-being (score<2 or score<4) and poor mental well-being (score ≥ 2 or score ≥ 4)

Model 1 is adjusted for age, gender and year dummies (2009–2019); Model 2 is adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, country of birth, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status and year dummies (2009–2019).

We noticed similar results of higher odds of poor mental well-being with every 10 μg/m3 increase in NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants in bi-pollutant models adjusted for SO2 (Table 7).

Table 7. The association of individuals’ mental well-being with each of NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants linked at the LSOAs level in bi-pollutant models adjusted for SO2 pollutant (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).
Mental well-being (GHQ0-36a ≥ 12) Mental well-being (GHQ0-12b ≥ 2) Mental well-being (GHQ0-12b ≥ 4)
OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI]
Overall pollution effect
NO2—SO2 Model NO2 (μg/m3) 1.11 [1.07, 1.14]** 1.13 [1.10, 1.16]** 1.11 [1.08, 1.15]**
SO2 (μg/m3) 1.18 [1.06, 1.31]** 1.14 [1.02, 1.26]* 1.17 [1.04, 1.31]**
PM10SO2 Model PM10 (μg/m3) 1.19 [1.12, 1.27]** 1.25 [1.18, 1.33]** 1.20 [1.12, 1.29]**
SO2 (μg/m3) 1.24 [1.12, 1.37]** 1.21 [1.09, 1.33]** 1.24 [1.11, 1.39]**
PM2.5SO2 Model PM2.5 (μg/m3) 1.31 [1.20, 1.43]** 1.40 [1.29, 1.52]** 1.34 [1.22, 1.47]**
SO2 (μg/m3) 1.23 [1.11, 1.37]** 1.20 [1.08, 1.33]** 1.23 [1.10, 1.38]**

**P-value <0.01

*P-value<0.05

ORs and 95%CIs are expressed in terms of 10 μg/m3 increase in the air pollutants.

GHQ0-36a: GHQ scale composed of 12 questions, each scored using a Likert format: 0-1-2-3 and summed up by adding all the items generating a scale ranging from 0 to 36; the 0–36 scale is dichotomised using a cut-off score of 12 based on relevant literature into good mental well-being (score<12) and poor mental well-being (score ≥ 12)

GHQ0-12b: GHQ scale composed of 12 questions, each scored using a simple binary format: 0-0-1-1 and summed up by adding all the items generating a scale ranging from 0 to 12; the 0–12 scale is dichotomised using two cut-off scores of 2 and 4 based on relevant literature into good mental well-being (score<2 or score<4) and poor mental well-being (score ≥ 2 or score ≥ 4)

Models are additionally adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, country of birth, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status and year dummies (2009–2019).

Decomposing the overall effect of air pollution on mental well-being into between (spatial: across LSOAs) and within (temporal: across years within each LSOA) effects, revealed significant associations with poor mental well-being for the between effects for all the four pollutants; while no significant associations were noted for the within effects despite the sign of the odds ratios being largely as expected. An exception was PM10 and PM2.5 pollutants which showed significant within effects on poor mental well-being (GHQ0-12) only in model 1; yet these significant effects disappeared after controlling for the sociodemographic and lifestyle covariates (Table 6). Therefore, living in more polluted LSOAs was the driving cause for poor mental well-being (between) rather than the variation in air pollution across time within each LSOA.

In a sensitivity analysis, results remained the same for the overall and for the between-within effects of the four pollutants on individuals’ mental well-being for individuals starting at wave 1 of the UKHLS survey (Table 2 in S1 File).

3.4.2. The effect of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being at the local authority level

Similar to the air pollution results at the LSOAs geographical scale, higher odds of poor mental well-being were also observed with every 10 μg/m3 increase in NO2 (ORs ranging between 1.10–1.16), SO2 (ORs ranging between 1.27–1.51), PM10 (ORs ranging between 1.13–1.22), and PM2.5 (ORs ranging between 1.21–1.36) pollutants at the local authority level (Table 8).

Table 8. The association of individuals’ mental well-being with each of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants linked at the local authority level in separate models (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).
Mental well-being (GHQ0-36a ≥ 12) Mental well-being (GHQ0-12b ≥ 2) Mental well-being (GHQ0-12b ≥ 4)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2
OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI]
Overall pollution effect
NO2 (μg/m3) 1.11 [1.07, 1.15]** 1.10 [1.07, 1.14]** 1.16 [1.12, 1.20]** 1.13 [1.09, 1.16]** 1.13 [1.08, 1.17]** 1.10 [1.06, 1.14]**
SO2 (μg/m3) 1.51 [1.27, 1.80]** 1.44 [1.22, 1.71]** 1.42 [1.20, 1.68]** 1.34 [1.14, 1.58]** 1.36 [1.12, 1.65]** 1.27 [1.05, 1.53]**
PM10 (μg/m3) 1.13 [1.04, 1.23]** 1.17 [1.09, 1.26]** 1.22 [1.13, 1.32]** 1.21 [1.13, 1.30]** 1.15 [1.06, 1.26]** 1.17 [1.08, 1.26]**
PM2.5 (μg/m3) 1.21 [1.08, 1.35]** 1.27 [1.15, 1.41]** 1.36 [1.23, 1.51]** 1.35 [1.23, 1.49]** 1.25 [1.12, 1.41]** 1.28 [1.15, 1.43]**
Between pollution effect
NO2 (μg/m3) 1.09 [1.04, 1.13]** 1.09 [1.05, 1.13]** 1.14 [1.10, 1.19]** 1.11 [1.07, 1.15]** 1.11 [1.07, 1.16]** 1.09 [1.05, 1.13]**
SO2 (μg/m3) 3.72 [2.53, 5.46]** 2.62 [1.87, 3.67]** 3.03 [2.08, 4.41]** 2.11 [1.50, 2.96]** 3.58 [2.40, 5.35]** 2.39 [1.67, 3.43]**
PM10 (μg/m3) 1.09 [0.99, 1.20] 1.16 [1.06, 1.26]** 1.23 [1.12, 1.34]** 1.22 [1.13, 1.33]** 1.13 [1.03, 1.24]* 1.16 [1.06, 1.26]**
PM2.5 (μg/m3) 1.16 [1.02, 1.33]* 1.26 [1.12, 1.42]** 1.39 [1.23, 1.58]** 1.38 [1.24, 1.54]** 1.23 [1.07, 1.41]** 1.27 [1.13, 1.44]**
Within pollution effect
NO2 (μg/m3) 1.07 [0.94, 1.22] 1.06 [0.93, 1.21] 1.14 [1.00, 1.30] 1.12 [0.99, 1.28] 1.08 [0.93, 1.26] 1.07 [0.92, 1.25]
SO2 (μg/m3) 1.08 [0.82, 1.44] 1.01 [0.77, 1.35] 1.13 [0.85, 1.49] 1.08 [0.81, 1.43] 0.95 [0.69, 1.32] 0.90 [0.65, 1.25]
PM10 (μg/m3) 1.11 [0.91, 1.35] 1.09 [0.89, 1.32] 1.05 [0.86, 1.28] 1.02 [0.84, 1.24] 1.22 [0.97, 1.54] 1.19 [0.94, 1.49]
PM2.5 (μg/m3) 1.09 [0.86, 1.39] 1.06 [0.83, 1.34] 1.09 [0.86, 1.39] 1.04 [0.82, 1.32] 1.17 [0.88, 1.55] 1.12 [0.85, 1.49]

**P-value <0.01

*P-value<0.05

ORs and 95%CIs are expressed in terms of 10 μg/m3 increase in the air pollutants.

GHQ0-36a: GHQ scale composed of 12 questions, each scored using a Likert format: 0-1-2-3 and summed up by adding all the items generating a scale ranging from 0 to 36; the 0–36 scale is dichotomised using a cut-off score of 12 based on relevant literature into good mental well-being (score<12) and poor mental well-being (score ≥ 12)

GHQ0-12b: GHQ scale composed of 12 questions, each scored using a simple binary format: 0-0-1-1 and summed up by adding all the items generating a scale ranging from 0 to 12; the 0–12 scale is dichotomised using two cut-off scores of 2 and 4 based on relevant literature into good mental well-being (score<2 or score<4) and poor mental well-being (score ≥ 2 or score ≥ 4)

Model 1 is adjusted for age, gender and year dummies (2009–2019); Model 2 is adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, country of birth, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status and year dummies (2009–2019).

Higher odds of poor mental well-being were also observed at the local authority level with every 10 μg/m3 increase in NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants in bi-pollutant models adjusted for SO2 (Table 9).

Table 9. The association of individuals’ mental well-being with each of NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants linked at the local authority level in bi-pollutant models adjusted for SO2 pollutant (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).
Mental well-being (GHQ0-36a ≥ 12) Mental well-being (GHQ0-12b ≥ 2) Mental well-being (GHQ0-12b ≥ 4)
OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI] OR [95%CI]
Overall pollution effect
NO2—SO2 Model NO2 (μg/m3) 1.09 [1.05, 1.12]** 1.12 [1.08, 1.16]** 1.10 [1.06, 1.14]**
SO2 (μg/m3) 1.25 [1.05, 1.49]* 1.10 [0.93, 1.31] 1.07 [0.88, 1.30]
PM10SO2 Model PM10 (μg/m3) 1.13 [1.05, 1.22]** 1.19 [1.10, 1.28]** 1.15 [1.06, 1.24]**
SO2 (μg/m3) 1.36 [1.14, 1.61]** 1.23 [1.04, 1.45]* 1.18 [0.97, 1.43]
PM2.5SO2 Model PM2.5 (μg/m3) 1.22 [1.10, 1.35]** 1.32 [1.19, 1.46]** 1.25 [1.12, 1.40]**
SO2 (μg/m3) 1.34 [1.13, 1.59]** 1.21 [1.02, 1.43]* 1.16 [0.96, 1.40]

**P-value <0.01

*P-value<0.05

ORs and 95%CIs are expressed in terms of 10 μg/m3 increase in the air pollutants.

GHQ0-36a: GHQ scale composed of 12 questions, each scored using a Likert format: 0-1-2-3 and summed up by adding all the items generating a scale ranging from 0 to 36; the 0–36 scale is dichotomised using a cut-off score of 12 based on relevant literature into good mental well-being (score<12) and poor mental well-being (score ≥ 12)

GHQ0-12b: GHQ scale composed of 12 questions, each scored using a simple binary format: 0-0-1-1 and summed up by adding all the items generating a scale ranging from 0 to 12; the 0–12 scale is dichotomised using two cut-off scores of 2 and 4 based on relevant literature into good mental well-being (score<2 or score<4) and poor mental well-being (score ≥ 2 or score ≥ 4)

Models are additionally adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, country of birth, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status and year dummies (2009–2019).

The between-within (spatial-temporal) analysis of the effect of air pollution on mental well-being at the local authority level revealed similar results to the between-within analysis at the LSOAs level (Table 8). Thus, living in more polluted local authorities was also the driving cause for poor mental well-being (between) rather than the variation in air pollution across time within each local authority.

In a sensitivity analysis for individuals starting at wave 1 of the UKHLS survey, results remained unchanged for the overall and for the between-within effects of the four pollutants on individuals’ mental well-being at the local authority level (Table 3 in S1 File).

3.5. The association of air pollution with individuals’ mental well-being by ethnicity and country of birth

3.5.1. The association of air pollution at the LSOAs level with individuals’ mental well-being by ethnicity and country of birth

At the LSOAs level, Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other-white, and other ethnicities group as well as non-UK born individuals showed higher odds of poor mental well-being compared to British-white and UK-born individuals, respectively, with every 10 μg/m3 increase in SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants only with the GHQ0-12 well-being measure (Fig 6). Nevertheless, most of these significant differences disappeared in a cohort sub-analysis for only individuals recruited at wave 1 of the UKHLS survey (Fig 1 in S1 File). In addition, we observed no significant ethnic differences for the between or within effects of air pollution at the LSOAs level on mental well-being with exception of between effects for PM10 and PM2.5 acting as protective factors (ORs<1) against poor mental well-being for people from Indian, Black/African/Caribbean and mixed ethnicities origin (Figs 3 and 4 in S1 File).

Fig 6. The overall effect of air pollution linked at the LSOAs level on individuals’ mental well-being by ethnicity and country of birth (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).

Fig 6

The dashed line is placed at OR = 1 as a cut-off for statistically insignificant results; The solid line separates between the air pollution-ethnicity interaction models and the air pollution-country of birth interaction models; Air pollution-ethnicity interaction models are adjusted for country of birth, age, gender, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status, and year dummies (2009 to 2019); Air pollution-country of birth interaction models are adjusted for ethnicity, age, gender, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status, and year dummies (2009 to 2019).

It is worth to note that examining the association between ethnicity and mental well-being revealed higher odds of poor mental well-being among people from Pakistani/Bangladeshi (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12: OR = 1.17, 95%CI = 1.06–1.29; GHQ0-12 ≥ 2: OR = 1.09, 95%CI = 1.00–1.19; GHQ0-12 ≥ 4: OR = 1.13, 95%CI = 1.02–1.25) and mixed ethnicities (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12: OR = 1.15, 95%CI = 1.01–1.32; GHQ0-12 ≥ 2: OR = 1.21, 95%CI = 1.08–1.37; GHQ0-12 ≥ 4: OR = 1.26, 95%CI = 1.10–1.43) origin in comparison to the British-white, even after adjusting for socio-demographics and lifestyle covariates (Table 1 in S1 File).

3.5.2. The association of air pollution at the local authority level with individuals’ mental well-being by ethnicity and country of birth

Similar to the LSOAs level, analysis at the local authority level showed higher odds of poor mental well-being with increasing concentrations of SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants among people from Pakistani/Bangladeshi and other ethnicities origin compared to British-white and among non-UK born people compared to natives. People from an Indian origin also showed higher odds of poor mental well-being than the British-white with every 10 μg/m3 increase in SO2 pollution at the local authority level (Fig 7). Yet, these significant differences disappeared in a cohort sub-analysis for only individuals recruited at wave 1 of the UKHLS survey (Fig 2 in S1 File). Similarly, no significant ethnic differences for the between or within effects of air pollution at the local authority level on mental well-being were observed with exception of between effects for PM10 and PM2.5 acting as protective factors (ORs<1) against poor mental well-being for people from Indian, Black/African/Caribbean and mixed ethnicities origin (Figs 5 and 6 in S1 File).

Fig 7. The overall effect of air pollution linked at the local authority level on individuals’ mental well-being by ethnicity and country of birth (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).

Fig 7

The dashed line is placed at OR = 1 as a cut-off for statistically insignificant results; The solid line separates between the air pollution-ethnicity interaction models and the air pollution-country of birth interaction models; Air pollution-ethnicity interaction models are adjusted for country of birth, age, gender, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status, and year dummies (2009 to 2019); Air pollution-country of birth interaction models are adjusted for ethnicity, age, gender, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status, and year dummies (2009 to 2019).

4. Discussion

This study showed the negative impact of NO2, SO2, PM10 and PM2.5 air pollution (linked at two geographical scales: coarse local authorities and detailed LSOAs) on mental well-being in the UK for individuals followed from the year 2009 up to 2019. These results are supported by relevant literature whereby exposure to ambient air pollution has been shown to affect negatively individuals’ mental well-being and contribute to increased rates of mental health problems such as autism spectrum disorders [13], schizophrenia [14], dementia [15], psychotic experiences [16, 17], cognitive disabilities [18], anxiety and major depressive disorders [19]. Evidence from China revealed an elevation in the rate of depressive symptoms and poorer self-reported mental well-being with long-term exposure to air pollution [6567]. Similarly, exposure to NO2 and particulate matter (PM10 and PM2.5) air pollution in the Netherlands was positively associated with poor mental health and prescription of anti-anxiety drugs [68]. In South Korea, increased exposure to NO2, SO2, and PM10 pollutants resulted in higher hazards for suicide death [69]. In addition, a recent systematic literature review and meta-analysis of 22 articles revealed that exposure to PM2.5 pollution increases the risk for depression and anxiety with a pooled odd ratio estimate of 1.10 for every 10 μg/m3 increase in PM2.5 concentration [70].

The observed positive association between air pollution and poor mental well-being in this study can be explained by four factors. The first explanation is through the biological mechanisms of air pollutants on the human central nervous system and neuro-behavioural processes [1, 5, 9, 10]. Air pollution particles of small diameters such as PM2.5 are capable of initiating oxidative stress and forming inflammatory cytokines that infiltrate the blood-brain barrier causing neurodegeneration and neuroinflammation [11]. The second explanation is through the aesthetic and odorous nuisance caused by air pollution, which results in avoidance behaviour and inhibition of psychological-supporting outdoor activities and sports. This in turn leads to reduced happiness and life satisfaction and to elevated levels of stress, anxiety, loneliness, and poor mental well-being [2, 2325]. The third explanation is related to experiential anxiety and worrying feelings about one’s physical health and future [27]. Through substantive research, people are made aware of the negative impact of air pollution on human’s physical health and the higher risk for acute and chronic diseases such as cardiovascular, respiratory, cancer and immune system diseases [8, 9, 28]. People living in highly polluted areas might experience stress and anxiety about physical illness, which is reflected in poorer mental well-being. The last explanation is manifested in the indirect effect of air pollution on mental well-being through the physical health of individuals. People who already suffer from physical illness often also suffer from poor mental well-being. Based on an international study that utilises data from the World Health Organisation, 9% to 23% of patients with one or more chronic physical health conditions displayed symptoms of depression [71]. Thus, air pollution might be associated with poor mental well-being because individuals are also suffering from a physical health condition. Conducting a simple t-test on the UKHLS data showed a higher GHQ12(0–12) mean score (mean = 2.44, SD = 3.46) among individuals with at least one physical health condition (e.g., asthma, arthritis, coronary heart disease, cancer, liver illness, chronic bronchitis, diabetes, blood pressure) in comparison to a GHQ12(0–12) mean score of 1.68 (SD = 2.92) for those with non-reported physical health condition (t-test P-value = 0.000).

Despite the existence of literature on the topic of air pollution and poor mental well-being, this study went a further step in analysing the spatial-temporal effects of air pollution on mental well-being using a between-within longitudinal design. Additionally, we carried out the analysis at two geographical scales, coarse local authorities and detailed LSOAs, which forms another novelty of the present study. The between-within analysis is extensively used in the fields of economics, behavioural finance, and strategic management [29], yet little used in health research [72]; and no previous study has examined the between-within effects of air pollution on mental well-being. Our study revealed significant between effects for NO2, SO2, PM10 and PM2.5 pollutants on poor mental well-being at both the LSOAs and the local authority geographical scales, while no significant within effects were noted. Thus, individuals residing in LSOAs or local authorities with higher average concentrations of the four pollutants across the 11 years of follow up exhibited poorer mental well-being than individuals residing in LSOAs or local authorities with lower pollution concentrations. This shows the importance of the spatial dimension in the association between air pollution and mental well-being whether at the coarse local authorities or at the detailed LSOAs geographical level given that both geographical scales resulted in similar findings. Nevertheless, the non-significant within (temporal) effects could be related to the low variation of yearly air pollution concentrations across the 11 years of follow up, particularly for SO2 pollutant as shown in Fig 4 for LSOAs and in Fig 5 for local authorities. Therefore, further research with longer follow up time is needed to allow for more temporal variation in air pollution which might result in significant within effects.

In the second part of this study, we attempted to examine the moderating effect of ethnicity and country of birth on the association between air pollution and individuals’ mental well-being. We hypothesised that the lower socio-economic status of ethnic minorities and living in more disadvantaged neighbourhoods, near major roads and transportation networks can result in higher levels of stress, anxiety, and mental health problems among ethnic minority groups; thus, moderating the association between air pollution and mental well-being. However, our findings did not reveal much difference in the overall and in the between and within effects of air pollution on mental well-being across the ethnic groups; with exception for Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other-white (only at the LSOAs level but not at the local authority level), Indians (only for SO2 pollution at the local authority level), other ethnicities, and non-UK born individuals who showed higher odds of poor mental well-being than the British-White and UK-born individuals with increasing concentrations of SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants. The poorer mental well-being observed among the Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other ethnicities group, and non-UK born individuals could be related to the socio-economic and lifestyle differences or to place-related contextual differences. Literature on ethnic inequalities in health has shown that ethnic minorities often live in more disadvantaged communities, and have lower socio-economic status, lower healthcare coverage and higher job/income insecurity [34, 73, 74], which increases their risk of physical and mental illness. Nevertheless, our analysis adjusted for the main socio-economic and lifestyle factors of individuals including age, gender, marital status, education, occupation, financial situation, and cigarette smoking. Therefore, the most likely explanation for the more pronounced effect of SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollution on poor mental well-being among Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other ethnicities, and non-UK born individuals could be related to the contextual factors and place of residence. Ethnic minorities and immigrants often choose to reside in large cities and highly urbanised regions, near major roads and key transportation networks to simplify their commuting and working conditions [75]. In addition, ethnic minorities often live in low-priced social housing offered by local authorities, which is often situated in more deprived ethnic concentration neighbourhoods [35]. These place-related factors can result in more pronounced effect of air pollution on mental well-being among ethnic minorities and immigrants due to greater exposure to air pollution resulting from vehicles, factories, and burning of fossil fuels. In a supplementary analysis, we show through Chi2 square tabulation that a very high percentage of non-UK born individuals (93%) and of ethnic minorities including Pakistani/Bangladeshi (99%) reside in urban areas; whereas this percentage is much lower for British-white (71%) and UK-born (75%) individuals (Table 4 in S1 File). It should be noted, however, that decomposing the overall effect of air pollution into between (spatial) and within (temporal) effects at both the LSOAs and local authority levels did not show significant differences among the ethnic groups, not even for the Pakistani/Bangladeshi and non-UK born individuals. Thus, we cannot be conclusive that residing in more polluted areas is the key explanation for the more pronounced effect of SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollution on mental well-being among Pakistani/Bangladeshi and non-UK born individuals. Furthermore, in a cohort sub-analysis for individuals recruited at wave 1 of the UKHLS survey (Figs 1 and 2 in S1 File), the differences noted with respect to the Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other ethnicities group, and non-UK born individuals disappeared. Therefore, our study shows no conclusive evidence of ethnicity or nativity differences in the association between air pollution and poor mental well-being.

Despite the new insights provided by this study, it is important to discuss its limitations. The first limitation is related to the design of the study in which individual-level data from the UKHLS survey was linked to yearly air pollution contextual data at the local authority level. Therefore, individuals residing within a respective local authority and a respective year were assigned to the same value of air pollution exposure. However, we also linked the air pollution data to the UKHLS at the LSOAs level (the lowest available geography level at the UKHLS due to ethical considerations) which minimised the exposure bias. Air pollution linked at both, the local authority and the LSOAs level, revealed similar results. For future research, we recommended the usage of data sources that allow linkages of air pollution at the postcode level, the lowest available geography in the UK. Second, our analysis included all individuals recruited at different waves of the UKHLS survey, that had at least two observations through the follow up time (2009–2019). Hence, the time window for follow up differed between the study participants, some were followed for 11 years across all the 10 waves while others entered the study at later waves and were followed for a shorter period of time. It should be noted, however, that similar results were observed in a sensitivity analysis on wave 1 cohort, except for the analysis by ethnic groups whereby Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other ethnicities, and non-UK born individuals did not show significant differences in the association of air pollution with mental well-being at both the LSOAs and local authority geographical scales (Tables 2, 3 and Figs 1 and 2 in S1 File). This can be explained by the UKHLS sample design which involved ethnic minority boost samples at waves 1 and 6 of data collection to enable ethnicity-focused research. Therefore, by conducting analysis on only wave 1 cohort, we are missing the second ethnic minority boost sample at wave 6 which resulted in the observed differences. Finally, the UKHLS survey included longitudinal weights that adjust for the overrepresentation of some groups, such as the ethnic minority groups; thus, allowing for greater generalisation of the estimates. However, we could not add the longitudinal weights into our analysis as this requires that all individuals be followed until the last wave (wave 10) of the survey, which was not the case.

5. Conclusion

Using a longitudinal panel design that involves linking individual to context-level data at two geographical scales (coarse local authorities and detailed LSOAs) and a between-within analysis, this study highlights the negative effect of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being over space and time and emphasises the importance of the spatial dimension in the shaping of this association. Thus, environmental policies to reduce air pollution emissions with a core of spatial planning can eventually improve the mental well-being of people residing in the UK. There is, however, less conclusive evidence on the moderating effect of ethnicity.

Supporting information

S1 File. Additional analysis and sensitivity check-ups.

(DOCX)

Data Availability

We cannot make the data underlying our analysis publicly available due to ethical and legal restrictions. We are using the “Understanding Society: The UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS)” dataset which is an initiative funded by the Economic and Social Research Council and various Government Departments, with scientific leadership by the Institute for Social and Economic Research, University of Essex, and survey delivery by NatCen Social Research and Kantar Public. These data are protected by a copyright license and strictly distributed by the UK Data Service which is the largest digital repository for quantitative and qualitative social science and humanities research data in the UK. Therefore, data underlying our analysis can only be accessed through the UK Data Service for authorized researchers from the following URL: https://beta.ukdataservice.ac.uk/datacatalogue/series/series?id=2000053. We confirm that authors did not have any special access privileges that others would not have. The data can be accessed following registration on the UK Data Archive. An application to access geography data such as the local authority and the Lower Super Output Areas is required using the UK Data Archive platform.

Funding Statement

This paper is part of a PhD project that is funded by the St Leonard’s PhD scholarship, University of St Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom.

References

  • 1.Dzhambov AM, Markevych I, Tilov B, Arabadzhiev Z, Stoyanov D, Gatseva P, et al. Pathways linking residential noise and air pollution to mental ill-health in young adults. Environmental Research. 2018;166:458–65. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2018.06.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lu JG. Air pollution: A systematic review of its psychological, economic, and social effects. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2020;32:52–65. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.06.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Vigo D, Thornicroft G, Atun R. Estimating the true global burden of mental illness. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(2):171–8. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00505-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Lim Y-H, Kim H, Kim Jin H, Bae S, Park Hye Y, Hong Y-C. Air Pollution and Symptoms of Depression in Elderly Adults. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2012;120(7):1023–8. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1104100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ventriglio A, Bellomo A, di Gioia I, Di Sabatino D, Favale D, De Berardis D, et al. Environmental pollution and mental health: a narrative review of literature. CNS Spectr. 2021;26(1):51–61. doi: 10.1017/S1092852920001303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Shin J, Park JY, Choi J. Long-term exposure to ambient air pollutants and mental health status: A nationwide population-based cross-sectional study. PLoS One. 2018;13(4):e0195607. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0195607 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Power MC, Kioumourtzoglou M-A, Hart JE, Okereke OI, Laden F, Weisskopf MG. The relation between past exposure to fine particulate air pollution and prevalent anxiety: observational cohort study. BMJ: British Medical Journal. 2015;350:h1111. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h1111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Abed Al Ahad M, Sullivan F, Demšar U, Melhem M, Kulu H. The effect of air-pollution and weather exposure on mortality and hospital admission and implications for further research: A systematic scoping review. PLoS One. 2020;15(10):e0241415. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241415 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Manisalidis I, Stavropoulou E, Stavropoulos A, Bezirtzoglou E. Environmental and Health Impacts of Air Pollution: A Review. Front Public Health. 2020;8:14–. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.00014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Genc S, Zadeoglulari Z, Fuss SH, Genc K. The Adverse Effects of Air Pollution on the Nervous System. Journal of Toxicology. 2012;2012:782462. doi: 10.1155/2012/782462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Calderón-Garcidueñas L, Calderón-Garcidueñas A, Torres-Jardón R, Avila-Ramírez J, Kulesza RJ, Angiulli AD. Air pollution and your brain: what do you need to know right now. Primary health care research & development. 2015;16(4):329–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Morris RH, Counsell SJ, McGonnell IM, Thornton C. Early life exposure to air pollution impacts neuronal and glial cell function leading to impaired neurodevelopment. BioEssays. 2021;43(9):2000288. doi: 10.1002/bies.202000288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Volk HE, Lurmann F, Penfold B, Hertz-Picciotto I, McConnell R. Traffic-related air pollution, particulate matter, and autism. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(1):71–7. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.266 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Pedersen CB, Raaschou-Nielsen O, Hertel O, Mortensen PB. Air pollution from traffic and schizophrenia risk. Schizophrenia Research. 2004;66(1):83–5. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(03)00062-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Yuchi W, Sbihi H, Davies H, Tamburic L, Brauer M. Road proximity, air pollution, noise, green space and neurologic disease incidence: a population-based cohort study. Environmental Health. 2020;19(1):8. doi: 10.1186/s12940-020-0565-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Bakolis I, Hammoud R, Stewart R, Beevers S, Dajnak D, MacCrimmon S, et al. Mental health consequences of urban air pollution: prospective population-based longitudinal survey. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2020. doi: 10.1007/s00127-020-01966-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Newbury JB, Arseneault L, Beevers S, Kitwiroon N, Roberts S, Pariante CM, et al. Association of Air Pollution Exposure With Psychotic Experiences During Adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76(6):614–23. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kilian J, Kitazawa M. The emerging risk of exposure to air pollution on cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease—Evidence from epidemiological and animal studies. Biomed J. 2018;41(3):141–62. doi: 10.1016/j.bj.2018.06.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Roberts S, Arseneault L, Barratt B, Beevers S, Danese A, Odgers CL, et al. Exploration of NO(2) and PM(2.5) air pollution and mental health problems using high-resolution data in London-based children from a UK longitudinal cohort study. Psychiatry Res. 2019;272:8–17. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Szyszkowicz M. Air pollution and emergency department visits for depression in Edmonton, Canada. Int J Occup Med Environ Health. 2007;20(3):241–5. doi: 10.2478/v10001-007-0024-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Szyszkowicz M, Kousha T, Kingsbury M, Colman I. Air Pollution and Emergency Department Visits for Depression: A Multicity Case-Crossover Study. Environmental Health Insights. 2016;10:EHI.S40493. doi: 10.4137/EHI.S40493 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Szyszkowicz M. Ambient sulfur dioxide and female ED visits for depression. Air Quality, Atmosphere & Health. 2011;4(3):259–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Claeson A-S, Lidén E, Nordin M, Nordin S. The role of perceived pollution and health risk perception in annoyance and health symptoms: a population-based study of odorous air pollution. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health. 2013;86(3):367–74. doi: 10.1007/s00420-012-0770-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Klæboe R, Amundsen AH, Fyhri A. Annoyance from vehicular air pollution: A comparison of European exposure–response relationships. Atmospheric Environment. 2008;42(33):7689–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Dzhambov A, Hartig T, Markevych I, Tilov B, Dimitrova D. Urban residential greenspace and mental health in youth: Different approaches to testing multiple pathways yield different conclusions. Environmental Research. 2018;160:47–59. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.09.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.von Lindern E, Hartig T, Lercher P. Traffic-related exposures, constrained restoration, and health in the residential context. Health & Place. 2016;39:92–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lu JG, Lee JJ, Gino F, Galinsky AD. Polluted Morality: Air Pollution Predicts Criminal Activity and Unethical Behavior. Psychological Science. 2018;29(3):340–55. doi: 10.1177/0956797617735807 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ab Manan N, Noor Aizuddin A, Hod R. Effect of Air Pollution and Hospital Admission: A Systematic Review. Ann Glob Health. 2018;84(4):670–8. doi: 10.9204/aogh.2376 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.McNeish D, Kelley K. Fixed effects models versus mixed effects models for clustered data: Reviewing the approaches, disentangling the differences, and making recommendations. Psychol Methods. 2019;24(1):20–35. doi: 10.1037/met0000182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Sui G, Liu G, Jia L, Wang L, Yang G. The association between ambient air pollution exposure and mental health status in Chinese female college students: a cross-sectional study. Environmental Science and Pollution Research. 2018;25(28):28517–24. doi: 10.1007/s11356-018-2881-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gater R, Tomenson B, Percival C, Chaudhry N, Waheed W, Dunn G, et al. Persistent depressive disorders and social stress in people of Pakistani origin and white Europeans in UK. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2008;44(3):198. doi: 10.1007/s00127-008-0426-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Brown G, Marshall M, Bower P, Woodham A, Waheed W. Barriers to recruiting ethnic minorities to mental health research: a systematic review. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2014;23(1):36–48. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1434 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Smith NR, Lewis DJ, Fahy A, Eldridge S, Taylor SJC, Moore DG, et al. Individual socio-demographic factors and perceptions of the environment as determinants of inequalities in adolescent physical and psychological health: the Olympic Regeneration in East London (ORiEL) study. BMC Public Health. 2015;15(1):150. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-1459-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Su JG, Jerrett M, de Nazelle A, Wolch J. Does exposure to air pollution in urban parks have socioeconomic, racial or ethnic gradients? Environmental Research. 2011;111(3):319–28. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2011.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.van Ham M, Manley D. Social housing allocation, choice and neighbourhood ethnic mix in England. Journal of Housing and the Built Environment. 2009;24(4):407–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gilliver SC, Sundquist J, Li X, Sundquist K. Recent research on the mental health of immigrants to Sweden: a literature review. European Journal of Public Health. 2014;24(suppl_1):72–9. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cku101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.UniversityofEssex. Understanding Society: Waves 1–10, 2009–2019 and Harmonised BHPS: Waves 1–18, 1991–2009. In: University of Essex IfSaER, NatCen Social Research, Kantar Public, editor. 13th Edition ed. UK Data Service2020.
  • 38.Lynn P. Sample design for understanding society. Underst Soc Work Pap Ser. 2009;2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lynn P, Nandi A, Parutis V, Platt L. Design and implementation of a high-quality probability sample of immigrants and ethnic minorities: Lessons learnt. Demographic Research. 2018;38:513–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Goldberg DP. User’s guide to the General Health Questionnaire. Windsor. 1988(NFER/Nelson). [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Zulkefly SN, Baharudin R. Using the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) to Assess the Psychological Health of Malaysian College Students. Global Journal of Health Science. 2010;2:73. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Daly M, Sutin AR, Robinson E. Longitudinal changes in mental health and the COVID-19 pandemic: evidence from the UK Household Longitudinal Study. Psychological Medicine. 2020:1–10. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720004432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hori D, Tsujiguchi H, Kambayashi Y, Hamagishi T, Kitaoka M, Mitoma J, et al. The associations between lifestyles and mental health using the General Health Questionnaire 12-items are different dependently on age and sex: a population-based cross-sectional study in Kanazawa, Japan. Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine. 2016;21(6):410–21. doi: 10.1007/s12199-016-0541-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Nerdrum P, Rustøen T, Rønnestad MH. Student Psychological Distress: A psychometric study of 1750 Norwegian 1st‐year undergraduate students. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research. 2006;50(1):95–109. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Godinho EL, Farias LC, Aguiar JC, Martelli-Júnior H, Bonan PR, Ferreira RC, et al. No association between periodontal disease and GHQ-12 in a Brazilian Police population. Med Oral Patol Oral Cir Bucal. 2011;16(6):e857–63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Puustinen PJ, Koponen H, Kautiainen H, Mäntyselkä P, Vanhala M. Psychological distress measured by the GHQ-12 and mortality: a prospective population-based study. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(6):577–81. doi: 10.1177/1403494811414244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Berglund E, Lytsy P, Westerling R. Health and wellbeing in informal caregivers and non-caregivers: a comparative cross-sectional study of the Swedish general population. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2015;13(1):109. doi: 10.1186/s12955-015-0309-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wrede O, Löve J, Jonasson JM, Panneh M, Priebe G. Promoting mental health in migrants: a GHQ12-evaluation of a community health program in Sweden. BMC Public Health. 2021;21(1):262. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-10284-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Maheswaran H, Kupek E, Petrou S. Self-reported health and socio-economic inequalities in England, 1996–2009: Repeated national cross-sectional study. Social Science & Medicine. 2015;136–137:135–46. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.05.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Holi MM, Marttunen M, Aalberg V. Comparison of the GHQ-36, the GHQ-12 and the SCL-90 as psychiatric screening instruments in the Finnish population. Nord J Psychiatry. 2003;57(3):233–8. doi: 10.1080/08039480310001418 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Modelled background pollution data [Internet]. 2020. Available from: https://uk-air.defra.gov.uk/data/pcm-data.
  • 52.Santiago JL, Rivas E, Gamarra AR, Vivanco MG, Buccolieri R, Martilli A, et al. Estimates of population exposure to atmospheric pollution and health-related externalities in a real city: The impact of spatial resolution on the accuracy of results. Science of The Total Environment. 2021:152062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lehtomäki H, Geels C, Brandt J, Rao S, Yaramenka K, Åström S, et al. Deaths Attributable to Air Pollution in Nordic Countries: Disparities in the Estimates. Atmosphere. 2020;11(5). [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Local Authority Districts (December 2017) Full Clipped Boundaries in United Kingdom [Internet]. 2017. Available from: https://data.gov.uk/dataset/f8f57716-72b4-4c93-a50d-30b2a443c2c1/local-authority-districts-december-2017-full-clipped-boundaries-in-united-kingdom-wgs84. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.2011 Census Geography boundaries (Lower Layer Super Output Areas and Data Zones) [Internet]. 2011. Available from: https://statistics.ukdataservice.ac.uk/dataset/2011-census-geography-boundaries-lower-layer-super-output-areas-and-data-zones.
  • 56.Yang Z, Song Q, Li J, Zhang Y, Yuan X-C, Wang W, et al. Air pollution and mental health: the moderator effect of health behaviors. Environmental Research Letters. 2021;16(4):044005. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Xue T, Zhu T, Zheng Y, Zhang Q. Declines in mental health associated with air pollution and temperature variability in China. Nature Communications. 2019;10(1):2165. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-10196-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Oudin A, Åström DO, Asplund P, Steingrimsson S, Szabo Z, Carlsen HK. The association between daily concentrations of air pollution and visits to a psychiatric emergency unit: a case-crossover study. Environmental Health. 2018;17(1):4. doi: 10.1186/s12940-017-0348-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Schober P, Boer C, Schwarte LA. Correlation Coefficients: Appropriate Use and Interpretation. Anesth Analg. 2018;126(5):1763–8. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000002864 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Lajos GJ, Haddad SM, Tedesco RP, Passini R Jr, Dias TZ, Nomura ML, et al. Intracluster correlation coefficients for the Brazilian Multicenter Study on Preterm Birth (EMIP): methodological and practical implications. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2014;14(1):54. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-14-54 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Jerrett M, McConnell R, Wolch J, Chang R, Lam C, Dunton G, et al. Traffic-related air pollution and obesity formation in children: a longitudinal, multilevel analysis. Environmental Health. 2014;13(1):49. doi: 10.1186/1476-069X-13-49 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Grazuleviciene R, Maroziene L, Dulskiene V, Malinauskiene V, Azaraviciene A, Laurinaviciene D, et al. Exposure to urban nitrogen dioxide pollution and the risk of myocardial infarction. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health. 2004;30(4):293–8. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.797 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Jyethi DS. Air Quality: Global and Regional Emissions of Particulate Matter, SOx, and NOx. In: Kulshrestha U, Saxena P, editors. Plant Responses to Air Pollution. Singapore: Springer Singapore; 2016. p. 5–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Vestreng V, Myhre G, Fagerli H, Reis S, Tarrasón L. Twenty-five years of continuous sulphur dioxide emission reduction in Europe. Atmos Chem Phys. 2007;7(13):3663–81. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Zhang X, Zhang X, Chen X. Happiness in the air: How does a dirty sky affect mental health and subjective well-being? Journal of Environmental Economics and Management. 2017;85:81–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jeem.2017.04.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Liu H, Chen Y, Ma L. Effect of time-varying exposure to air pollution on subjective well-being. Journal of Cleaner Production. 2021;281:125364. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Zu D, Zhai K, Qiu Y, Pei P, Zhu X, Han D. The Impacts of Air Pollution on Mental Health: Evidence from the Chinese University Students. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(18). doi: 10.3390/ijerph17186734 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Klompmaker JO, Hoek G, Bloemsma LD, Wijga AH, van den Brink C, Brunekreef B, et al. Associations of combined exposures to surrounding green, air pollution and traffic noise on mental health. Environment International. 2019;129:525–37. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2019.05.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Min J-y Kim H-J, Min K-b. Long-term exposure to air pollution and the risk of suicide death: A population-based cohort study. Science of The Total Environment. 2018;628–629:573–9. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2018.02.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Braithwaite I, Zhang S, Kirkbride James B, Osborn David PJ, Hayes Joseph F. Air Pollution (Particulate Matter) Exposure and Associations with Depression, Anxiety, Bipolar, Psychosis and Suicide Risk: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2019;127(12):126002. doi: 10.1289/EHP4595 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Moussavi S, Chatterji S, Verdes E, Tandon A, Patel V, Ustun B. Depression, chronic diseases, and decrements in health: results from the World Health Surveys. The Lancet. 2007;370(9590):851–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Dieleman JL, Templin T. Random-Effects, Fixed-Effects and the within-between Specification for Clustered Data in Observational Health Studies: A Simulation Study. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e110257. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110257 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Cézard G, Finney N, Kulu H, Marshall A. Ethnic differences in self-assessed health in Scotland: The role of socio-economic status and migrant generation. Population, Space and Place. 2020;n/a(n/a):e2403. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Egede LE. Race, ethnicity, culture, and disparities in health care. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2006;21(6):667. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.0512.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Lucas K, Stokes G, Bastiaanssen J, Burkinshaw J. Inequalities in Mobility and Access in the UK Transport System. Future of Mobility: Evidence Review, Government Office for Science. 2019. doi: 10.1126/science.aax3494 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Bijaya Kumar Padhi

9 Dec 2021

PONE-D-21-31292Air pollution and individuals’ mental well-being in the adult population in United Kingdom: A spatial-temporal longitudinal study and the moderating effect of ethnicityPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Abed Al Ahad,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 23 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Bijaya Kumar Padhi, PhD, MPH

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please amend your current ethics statement to address the following concerns:

a) Did participants provide their written or verbal informed consent to participate in this study?

b) If consent was verbal, please explain i) why written consent was not obtained, ii) how you documented participant consent, and iii) whether the ethics committees/IRB approved this consent procedure.

3. You indicated that you had ethical approval for your study. In your Methods section, please ensure you have also stated whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians of the minors included in the study or whether the research ethics committee or IRB specifically waived the need for their consent.

4. Thank you for stating the following in the Funding Section of your manuscript:

“: This paper is part of a PhD project that is funded by the St Leonard’s PhD scholarship, University of St Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom.”

We note that you have provided funding information that is not currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form.

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows:

“This paper is part of a PhD project that is funded by the St Leonard’s PhD scholarship, University of St Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

5. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

6. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

7. Please note that in order to use the direct billing option the corresponding author must be affiliated with the chosen institute. Please either amend your manuscript to change the affiliation or corresponding author, or email us at plosone@plos.org with a request to remove this option.

8. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please move it to the Methods section and delete it from any other section. Please ensure that your ethics statement is included in your manuscript, as the ethics statement entered into the online submission form will not be published alongside your manuscript.

9. We note that Figure 3 in your submission contain [map/satellite] images which may be copyrighted. All PLOS content is published under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which means that the manuscript, images, and Supporting Information files will be freely available online, and any third party is permitted to access, download, copy, distribute, and use these materials in any way, even commercially, with proper attribution. For these reasons, we cannot publish previously copyrighted maps or satellite images created using proprietary data, such as Google software (Google Maps, Street View, and Earth). For more information, see our copyright guidelines: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/licenses-and-copyright.

 We require you to either (1) present written permission from the copyright holder to publish these figures specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license, or (2) remove the figures from your submission:

 a. You may seek permission from the original copyright holder of Figure 3 to publish the content specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license. 

 We recommend that you contact the original copyright holder with the Content Permission Form (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=7c09/content-permission-form.pdf) and the following text:

“I request permission for the open-access journal PLOS ONE to publish XXX under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CCAL) CC BY 4.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Please be aware that this license allows unrestricted use and distribution, even commercially, by third parties. Please reply and provide explicit written permission to publish XXX under a CC BY license and complete the attached form.”

 Please upload the completed Content Permission Form or other proof of granted permissions as an ""Other"" file with your submission.

 In the figure caption of the copyrighted figure, please include the following text: “Reprinted from [ref] under a CC BY license, with permission from [name of publisher], original copyright [original copyright year].”

 b. If you are unable to obtain permission from the original copyright holder to publish these figures under the CC BY 4.0 license or if the copyright holder’s requirements are incompatible with the CC BY 4.0 license, please either i) remove the figure or ii) supply a replacement figure that complies with the CC BY 4.0 license. Please check copyright information on all replacement figures and update the figure caption with source information. If applicable, please specify in the figure caption text when a figure is similar but not identical to the original image and is therefore for illustrative purposes only.

The following resources for replacing copyrighted map figures may be helpful:

 USGS National Map Viewer (public domain): http://viewer.nationalmap.gov/viewer/

The Gateway to Astronaut Photography of Earth (public domain): http://eol.jsc.nasa.gov/sseop/clickmap/

Maps at the CIA (public domain): https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-world-factbook/index.html and https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/cia-maps-publications/index.html

NASA Earth Observatory (public domain): http://earthobservatory.nasa.gov/

Landsat: http://landsat.visibleearth.nasa.gov/

USGS EROS (Earth Resources Observatory and Science (EROS) Center) (public domain): http://eros.usgs.gov/#

Natural Earth (public domain): http://www.naturalearthdata.com/

:

Additional Editor Comments:

We have received reviews from two reviewers for your manuscript “Air pollution and individuals’ mental well-being in the adult population in United Kingdom: A spatial-temporal longitudinal study and the moderating effect of ethnicity”. At this time, the manuscript will require substantial revision before it can be considered for publication. The reviewers recommended that you make substantial amendments to your manuscript. Please respond within the next 14 days to all comments raised by the reviewers. You can also submit a revised version of your manuscript at that time. We encourage you to submit your documents with tracked changes to highlight the revisions.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This study is interesting because the authors utilized the datasets from a large cohort to investigate the associations between mental health outcomes and exposures to NO2, SO2, PM2.5, and PM10. The study is properly designed, and data analyses are adequate with new findings. The data interpretations and discussions are adequate.

Specific comments:

Line 99: Please break this sentence into two separately focusing impacts of PM1 and PM2.5. A part of PM2.5 cannot cross blood brain barriers.

Fig 4 title: correct subscripts.

Line 209: Please provide a reference justifying that 1x1 km spatial resolution is sufficient for selected air pollutants for this type of association studies. Please add more information on this.

Lines 224 – 234: Besides citing references please also add some information stating the connections between these factors and targeted pollutants and health outcomes.

Table 5: correct subscripts for air pollutants.

Reviewer #2: The authors tried to examine the effects of ambient air pollution on mental health by measuring mental well being by a suitable tool. I also agree with authors that evidence base of the association between air pollution and mental health across the globe is week. In that context this study is important and has good scientific merits. The largest advantage of the study is the large sample size with repeated measure over 11 years. Though exposure misclassification is an issue as authors highlighted as population exposure assigned to individual, but that was only available option to them. However, there are lot of methodological issues, unless that are addressed, I cannot recommend for publication. These are the following issues

1. I couldn’t understand why they dichotomized each item of GHQ12 tools. Instead, they could have added all considering each item measured in an ordinal scale of 4 points to have better precision in measurement. By doing so they reduced the true variability mechanical which led to decrease in precision of the measurement to some extent.

2. The most serious flaw of the analysis is not to report the distribution of the response of interest. As far as existing literature is concern that is supposed to be a negatively skewed distribution. Though I don’t know what their distribution was looked like, but my guess it is too both side truncated(bounded) negatively skewed distribution. Whatever means and sds reported by them in the discussion indicated presence of severe skewness. Question is how valid a Gaussian Mixed Effects Model is under this background of response distribution. No such attempt was made by the authors to check such validity of the multilevel model they used. Without that one cannot rely on their estimates or findings.

3. I recommend a Beta-Binomial mixed effects model to be used for this analysis which is commonly used by people with tool-based measurement of psychological features in the literature. Authors can convert the score into a proportion scale (0,1) by minimax normalization method. If any score attends exactly 0, or 1 do the necessary step to shrink them within (0,1). Report the results in terms OR per 10ug/m3 increase in pollutant. OR should be interpreted as the ratio of the odds of poor mental health wellbeing for every 10ug/m3 increase in ambient concentration of the pollutant. Please go through the relevant literature will guide you. As data wouldn’t be binary, the interpretation in terms of OR is a bit tricky.

4. After understanding the data structure, I feel multilevel model may not be needed. There is no reason to believe that the temporal effects will vary over geography. Both temporal and cluster effects can be assumed independent considering two independent random intercepts. Secondly, the levels should be finalized by testing the variances step by step (H0: sigma_b=0 vs H1:sigma_b!=0). If nested model, then nested variances should be tested. Look at random effects model literature. ICC based assessment may not be correct always as that ignores the nested structure.

5. I am confused with within vs between effects of pollutants. After reading their process of computation I understand they all are same measured with different level of precision. 1) manually removed the temporal variation and estimated effect size 2) manually removed spatial variation and estimated effect size 3) statistically removed spatiotemporal variation and estimated effects size. Where are the differences? First fix the questions you are asking. The possible questions could be 1) Do the effects of pollution vary over geography? 2) Do the effects vary over duration of exposure? Stratification by interaction term should be the solution of it.

6. Supplementary analysis should be done by either baseline logistic model or ordered logistic, not by binary logistic model. By multiple binary logistic models’ precision of the estimates would be compromised.

7. In the abstract authors reported regression coefficients but named as OR, I guess. As higher score means worst in mental wellbeing, one should expect a positive association with pollution, OR should be >1.

8. Line 385-389: Authors stating in text about high score but reporting regression coefficients doesn’t go with sentence. Is it the mean difference from reference ethnic group? If so, state accordingly.

9. Looking at the correlation matrix among pollutant, it is worthy to try multipollutant model with SO2, NO2 and PM2.5. Look at variance inflation factor, if less than 8/10, don’t bother, it will work. Best way to assess is add one at a time if AIC decreases you can retain. In case of multicollinearity AIC should increase after adding them in a single model.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Mar 9;17(3):e0264394. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


14 Jan 2022

PONE-D-21-31292

Air pollution and individuals’ mental well-being in the adult population in United Kingdom: A spatial-temporal longitudinal study and the moderating effect of ethnicity

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming.

2. Please amend your current ethics statement to address the following concerns:

a) Did participants provide their written or verbal informed consent to participate in this study?

Response: For this study, we didn’t need informed consent from participants because we did secondary analysis on the “Understanding Society: The UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS)” dataset. The UKHLS team have already obtained written informed consent from participants.

We have added into the manuscript a section on Ethical considerations under the methods which includes the above information.

b) If consent was verbal, please explain i) why written consent was not obtained, ii) how you documented participant consent, and iii) whether the ethics committees/IRB approved this consent procedure.

Response: Not applicable.

3. You indicated that you had ethical approval for your study. In your Methods section, please ensure you have also stated whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians of the minors included in the study or whether the research ethics committee or IRB specifically waived the need for their consent.

Response: We have added to the methods a section about Ethical considerations as follows: “This paper was granted ethical approval on the 14th of May 2020 by the authors’ affiliated institution (School of Geography and Sustainable Development Ethics Committee, acting on behalf of the University Teaching and Research Ethics Committee (UTREC) at the University of St Andrews). The paper uses secondary adult (age 16+) data from the “Understanding Society: The UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS)”; thus, participants’ informed consent is not applicable. The University of Essex responsible for the UKHLS data collection and management has already obtained written informed consent from all the study participants (37). Requesting consent for health record linkage was approved at Wave 1 by the National Research Ethics Service (NRES) Oxfordshire REC A (08/H0604/124), and at Wave 4 by NRES Southampton REC A (11/SC/0274). Approval for the collection of biosocial data by trained nurses in Waves 2 and 3 of the main survey was obtained from the National Research Ethics Service (Understanding Society - UK Household Longitudinal Study: A Biosocial Component, Oxfordshire A REC, Reference: 10/H0604/2)”.

4. Thank you for stating the following in the Funding Section of your manuscript:

“: This paper is part of a PhD project that is funded by the St Leonard’s PhD scholarship, University of St Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom.”

We note that you have provided funding information that is not currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form.

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows:

“This paper is part of a PhD project that is funded by the St Leonard’s PhD scholarship, University of St Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

Response: We have removed the Funding statement from the acknowledgement section of the manuscript. The funding statement in the online submission form is correct as it stands: “This paper is part of a PhD project that is funded by the St Leonard’s PhD scholarship, University of St Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom.”

5. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available.

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

Response: We cannot make the data underlying our analysis publicly available due to ethical and legal restrictions. We are using the “Understanding Society: The UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS)” dataset which is an initiative funded by the Economic and Social Research Council and various Government Departments, with scientific leadership by the Institute for Social and Economic Research, University of Essex, and survey delivery by NatCen Social Research and Kantar Public. These data are protected by a copyright license and strictly distributed by the UK Data Service which is the largest digital repository for quantitative and qualitative social science and humanities research data in the UK. Therefore, data underlying our analysis can only be accessed through the UK Data Service for authorized researchers from the following URL: https://beta.ukdataservice.ac.uk/datacatalogue/series/series?id=2000053

We have added the above explanation into the cover letter.

6. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

Response: We have made changes to our Data Availability Statement because the data cannot be made publicly available by the researchers due to ethical and legal considerations. We have added the updated data availability statement at the end of the cover letter.

7. Please note that in order to use the direct billing option the corresponding author must be affiliated with the chosen institute. Please either amend your manuscript to change the affiliation or corresponding author, or email us at plosone@plos.org with a request to remove this option.

Response: The corresponding author is affiliated with the chosen institute (University of St. Andrews). We have amended the information provided on the system and in the Title page to reflect that by adding a full stop after the St. Andrews.

8. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please move it to the Methods section and delete it from any other section. Please ensure that your ethics statement is included in your manuscript, as the ethics statement entered into the online submission form will not be published alongside your manuscript.

Response: We have added the ethics statement into the Methods section of the manuscript on line 357 and removed it from the Acknowledgement section.

9. We note that Figure 3 in your submission contain [map/satellite] images which may be copyrighted. All PLOS content is published under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which means that the manuscript, images, and Supporting Information files will be freely available online, and any third party is permitted to access, download, copy, distribute, and use these materials in any way, even commercially, with proper attribution. For these reasons, we cannot publish previously copyrighted maps or satellite images created using proprietary data, such as Google software (Google Maps, Street View, and Earth). For more information, see our copyright guidelines: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/licenses-and-copyright.

We require you to either (1) present written permission from the copyright holder to publish these figures specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license, or (2) remove the figures from your submission:

a. You may seek permission from the original copyright holder of Figure 3 to publish the content specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license.

We recommend that you contact the original copyright holder with the Content Permission Form (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=7c09/content-permission-form.pdf) and the following text:

“I request permission for the open-access journal PLOS ONE to publish XXX under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CCAL) CC BY 4.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Please be aware that this license allows unrestricted use and distribution, even commercially, by third parties. Please reply and provide explicit written permission to publish XXX under a CC BY license and complete the attached form.”

Please upload the completed Content Permission Form or other proof of granted permissions as an ""Other"" file with your submission.

In the figure caption of the copyrighted figure, please include the following text: “Reprinted from [ref] under a CC BY license, with permission from [name of publisher], original copyright [original copyright year].”

b. If you are unable to obtain permission from the original copyright holder to publish these figures under the CC BY 4.0 license or if the copyright holder’s requirements are incompatible with the CC BY 4.0 license, please either i) remove the figure or ii) supply a replacement figure that complies with the CC BY 4.0 license. Please check copyright information on all replacement figures and update the figure caption with source information. If applicable, please specify in the figure caption text when a figure is similar but not identical to the original image and is therefore for illustrative purposes only.

Response: Figure 3 is not copied from any source, it is produced by the authors in ArcGIS Pro software using the air pollution data downloaded from DEFRA office and the local authorities and LSOAs shape file for UK downloaded from the Office for National Statistics. Both data sets are available as open governmental data.

DEFRA air pollution data are open and publicly available from https://uk-air.defra.gov.uk/data/ under Open Government License (OGL), which allows anyone to copy, publish, distribute and transmit the information; adapt the information; exploit the information commercially and non-commercially for example, by combining it with other information, or by including it in own product or application. These data can therefore be used freely, as long as there is acknowledgement to DEFRA through citation.

The local authorities and LSOAs shape files for UK downloaded from the Office for National Statistics has an Open Government Licence and UK Government Licensing Terms and conditions of supply. The following statement is stated on their website (URL: https://www.ons.gov.uk/methodology/geography/licences ): “Under the terms of the Open Government Licence and UK Government Licensing Framework (launched 30 September 2010), if you wish to use or re-use ONS material, whether commercially or privately, you may do so freely without a specific application for a licence, subject to the conditions of the Open Government Licence and the Framework. If you are reproducing ONS content you must include a source accreditation to ONS. Digital boundary products and reference maps are supplied under the Open Government Licence. You must use the following copyright statements when you reproduce or use this material: Source: Office for National Statistics licensed under the Open Government Licence v.3.0”.

We have added a footnote under Figure 3 to say that Figure 3 is produced by the authors using air pollution data from DEFRA and local authorities and LSOAs of UK shapefiles from the Office for National Statistics, both of which were downloaded under the Open Government Licence v.3.0.

The following resources for replacing copyrighted map figures may be helpful:

USGS National Map Viewer (public domain): http://viewer.nationalmap.gov/viewer/

The Gateway to Astronaut Photography of Earth (public domain): http://eol.jsc.nasa.gov/sseop/clickmap/

Maps at the CIA (public domain): https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-world-factbook/index.html and https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/cia-maps-publications/index.html

NASA Earth Observatory (public domain): http://earthobservatory.nasa.gov/

Landsat: http://landsat.visibleearth.nasa.gov/

USGS EROS (Earth Resources Observatory and Science (EROS) Center) (public domain): http://eros.usgs.gov/#

Natural Earth (public domain): http://www.naturalearthdata.com/

:

Additional Editor Comments:

We have received reviews from two reviewers for your manuscript “Air pollution and individuals’ mental well-being in the adult population in United Kingdom: A spatial-temporal longitudinal study and the moderating effect of ethnicity”. At this time, the manuscript will require substantial revision before it can be considered for publication. The reviewers recommended that you make substantial amendments to your manuscript. Please respond within the next 14 days to all comments raised by the reviewers. You can also submit a revised version of your manuscript at that time. We encourage you to submit your documents with tracked changes to highlight the revisions.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

________________________________________

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This study is interesting because the authors utilized the datasets from a large cohort to investigate the associations between mental health outcomes and exposures to NO2, SO2, PM2.5, and PM10. The study is properly designed, and data analyses are adequate with new findings. The data interpretations and discussions are adequate.

Specific comments:

1. Line 99: Please break this sentence into two separately focusing impacts of PM1 and PM2.5. A part of PM2.5 cannot cross blood brain barriers.

Response: We have added 2 sentences on line 93 to explain the specific impacts of PM2.5 and PM1 as follows: “Specifically, exposure to ambient PM2.5 results in depressive responses and increased hippocampal pro-inflammatory cytokines. (11) Exposure to PM1 leads to increased inflammation and reactive oxygen species (ROS) generation and impacts learning and memory (12)”.

2. Fig 4 title: correct subscripts.

Response: The subscripts have been corrected in Fig 4 and its title

3. Line 209: Please provide a reference justifying that 1x1 km spatial resolution is sufficient for selected air pollutants for this type of association studies. Please add more information on this.

Response: We have added two references to support that 1*1 km spatial resolution is sufficient for air pollution modelling and we have provided more information on line 247 as follows: “These air pollution maps at 1x1 km resolution are modelled each year by DEFRA under the “Defra's Modelling of Ambient Air Quality (MAAQ) contract” and are used to provide policy support in the UK and to fulfil the UK's reporting obligations to Europe (51). The 1x1 km air pollution raster data are the finest spatial resolution data that can be downloaded from DEFRA and are sufficient to obtain good modelling estimates (52, 53)”.

4. Lines 224 – 234: Besides citing references please also add some information stating the connections between these factors and targeted pollutants and health outcomes.

Response: We have added an explanation about the connections between the covariates and air pollution and mental well-being outcomes on line 294 as follows: “Specifically, poor mental well-being, stress, and depression have been associated with younger or older ages, women sex, cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking, physical inactivity, lower education, divorced/widowed marital status, lower household income, and belonging to an ethnic minority group (6, 31-33), which in turn confounds the association between air pollution and mental well-being outcomes”.

5. Table 5: correct subscripts for air pollutants.

Response: The subscripts for air pollutants have been corrected in Table 5 which is now referred to as Table 6.

Reviewer #2: The authors tried to examine the effects of ambient air pollution on mental health by measuring mental well-being by a suitable tool. I also agree with authors that evidence base of the association between air pollution and mental health across the globe is week. In that context this study is important and has good scientific merits. The largest advantage of the study is the large sample size with repeated measure over 11 years. Though exposure misclassification is an issue as authors highlighted as population exposure assigned to individual, but that was only available option to them. However, there are lot of methodological issues, unless that are addressed, I cannot recommend for publication. These are the following issues:

Response: We have asked the data owner again to give us access to the lowest possible geography they have which is the Lower Super Output Areas (LSOAs) which is used to divide areas in the UK based on population size with the minimum allowed population size being 1000 people. Therefore, the paper now presents analysis at the LSOAs as well as the local authorities level. Results remained the same as we show in the paper, however, this approach reduces the exposure bias.

We have also addressed the analysis issues as shown below:

1. I couldn’t understand why they dichotomized each item of GHQ12 tools. Instead, they could have added all considering each item measured in an ordinal scale of 4 points to have better precision in measurement. By doing so they reduced the true variability mechanical which led to decrease in precision of the measurement to some extent.

Response: There are 2 methods used in the literature to calculate the GHQ12 scale scores. The most used method is the (0-0-1-1) which dichotomises first the responses of individuals to each of the 12 questions and then sum them up resulting in an overall score that ranges between 0 and 12. The second method sums directly the scores (0-1-2-3) resulting in an overall score that ranges between 0 and 36. In the UKHLS data they have both methods. However, given that most of the literature uses the (0-0-1-1) method, we have used this.

Nevertheless, to address the decrease in precision issue, we have now used both types of GHQ12 scale in our analysis and added an in-depth explanation under the “Individuals’ reported mental well-being” section starting on line 222-242 regarding the GHQ12 scores generation methods and how we used it in our study. All changes to this section are marked in Tracked changes.

2. The most serious flaw of the analysis is not to report the distribution of the response of interest. As far as existing literature is concern that is supposed to be a negatively skewed distribution. Though I don’t know what their distribution was looked like, but my guess it is too both side truncated(bounded) negatively skewed distribution. Whatever means and sds reported by them in the discussion indicated presence of severe skewness. Question is how valid a Gaussian Mixed Effects Model is under this background of response distribution. No such attempt was made by the authors to check such validity of the multilevel model they used. Without that one cannot rely on their estimates or findings.

Response: The responses to the GHQ12 scale whether in terms of (0-12) or (0-36) scores were right skewed. After thorough review of the literature that used GHQ12 scale in their analysis, we have decided to do the analysis based on dichotomisation using cut-off values suggested by the literature to reflect the most accurate estimates of the GHQ12 scale in measuring the mental well-being of individuals. Therefore, we have constructed three GHQ12 binary variables and performed analysis on them. All the details of the construction of these three GHQ12 binary outcome variables are described in detail under the “Individuals’ reported mental well-being” section and marked in Track changes in the manuscript.

We should note that the analysis for the three GHQ12 outcomes were concordant and that these results were also concordant with the previous analysis carried in the older version of this manuscript. Therefore, conclusions remained the same.

3. I recommend a Beta-Binomial mixed effects model to be used for this analysis which is commonly used by people with tool-based measurement of psychological features in the literature. Authors can convert the score into a proportion scale (0,1) by minimax normalization method. If any score attends exactly 0, or 1 do the necessary step to shrink them within (0,1). Report the results in terms OR per 10ug/m3 increase in pollutant. OR should be interpreted as the ratio of the odds of poor mental health wellbeing for every 10ug/m3 increase in ambient concentration of the pollutant. Please go through the relevant literature will guide you. As data wouldn’t be binary, the interpretation in terms of OR is a bit tricky.

Response: As reported in the previous response, we changed our analysis method to construct three binary GHQ12 outcomes rather than using the GHQ12 scale as a continuous variable following the methods of multiple published articles that used the GHQ12 questionnaire as detailed in the “Individuals’ reported mental well-being” section of the manuscript as follows: “For the present study, we used both methods for the GHQ12 scale, the (0-0-1-1) and the (0-1-2-3) method. Given that the scores of the GHQ12 scale are right-skewed and based on relevant literature, we dichotomised the overall GHQ12 scale using two cut off points for the GHQ12 (0-12): our sample mean GHQ12 (0-12) score = 1.8 ∼ 2 (GHQ0-12 ≥ 2) and the GHQ12 (0-12) score of greater than or equal to 4 (GHQ0-12 ≥ 4) as an indication of poor mental well-being (Godinho et al., 2011; Holi et al., 2003; Hori et al., 2016; Maheswaran et al., 2015; Nerdrum et al., 2006; Puustinen et al., 2011). The GHQ12 (0-36) score was dichotomised based on one cut off point of greater than or equal to 12 (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12) as an indication of poor mental well-being (Berglund et al., 2015; Wrede et al., 2021).”

Therefore, the three GHQ12 outcomes are now binary variables and not continuous variables as in the previous version of the manuscript. Thus, the beta-binomial method is not applicable anymore. Instead, we used for the analysis of these three GHQ12 binary outcomes multilevel mixed effects logit models, which are normally used by the literature to analyse binary variables. Please see below a list of some papers that used GHQ12 as a dichotomised binary variable and used logit models for analysis.

Results in our paper are now reported in terms of ORs and 95% CIs per 10 µg/m3 increase in air pollutants to fit the used logit models.

We must note that we didn’t keep the analysis of the GHQ12 scale as a continuous variable and thus we didn’t use the reviewer suggested beta-binomial method because all relevant literature that uses the GHQ12 scale use it as a binary variable based on cut off thresholds validated by a large body of research studies to show poor mental well-being as we explained in the new version of the manuscript. We have also included below a list of research studies that use GHQ12 dichotomisation methods and logit models for analysis.

Reference list that used dichotomization of the GHQ12 scales and assessed associations using logit models:

Berglund, E., Lytsy, P., & Westerling, R. (2015). Health and wellbeing in informal caregivers and non-caregivers: a comparative cross-sectional study of the Swedish general population. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 13(1), 109. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-015-0309-2

Godinho, E. L., Farias, L. C., Aguiar, J. C., Martelli-Júnior, H., Bonan, P. R., Ferreira, R. C., De Paula, A. M., Martins, A. M., & Guimarães, A. L. (2011). No association between periodontal disease and GHQ-12 in a Brazilian Police population. Med Oral Patol Oral Cir Bucal, 16(6), e857-863.

Holi, M. M., Marttunen, M., & Aalberg, V. (2003). Comparison of the GHQ-36, the GHQ-12 and the SCL-90 as psychiatric screening instruments in the Finnish population. Nord J Psychiatry, 57(3), 233-238. https://doi.org/10.1080/08039480310001418

Hori, D., Tsujiguchi, H., Kambayashi, Y., Hamagishi, T., Kitaoka, M., Mitoma, J., Asakura, H., Suzuki, F., Anyenda, E. O., Nguyen, T. T. T., Hibino, Y., Shibata, A., Hayashi, K., Sagara, T., Sasahara, S., Matsuzaki, I., Hatta, K., Konoshita, T., & Nakamura, H. (2016). The associations between lifestyles and mental health using the General Health Questionnaire 12-items are different dependently on age and sex: a population-based cross-sectional study in Kanazawa, Japan. Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine, 21(6), 410-421. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12199-016-0541-3

Maheswaran, H., Kupek, E., & Petrou, S. (2015). Self-reported health and socio-economic inequalities in England, 1996–2009: Repeated national cross-sectional study. Social Science & Medicine, 136-137, 135-146. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.05.026

Nerdrum, P., Rustøen, T., & Rønnestad, M. H. (2006). Student Psychological Distress: A psychometric study of 1750 Norwegian 1st‐year undergraduate students. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 50(1), 95-109. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313830500372075

Puustinen, P. J., Koponen, H., Kautiainen, H., Mäntyselkä, P., & Vanhala, M. (2011). Psychological distress measured by the GHQ-12 and mortality: a prospective population-based study. Scand J Public Health, 39(6), 577-581. https://doi.org/10.1177/1403494811414244

Wrede, O., Löve, J., Jonasson, J. M., Panneh, M., & Priebe, G. (2021). Promoting mental health in migrants: a GHQ12-evaluation of a community health program in Sweden. BMC Public Health, 21(1), 262. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-10284-z

4. After understanding the data structure, I feel multilevel model may not be needed. There is no reason to believe that the temporal effects will vary over geography. Both temporal and cluster effects can be assumed independent considering two independent random intercepts. Secondly, the levels should be finalized by testing the variances step by step (H0: sigma_b=0 vs H1:sigma_b!=0). If nested model, then nested variances should be tested. Look at random effects model literature. ICC based assessment may not be correct always as that ignores the nested structure.

Response: The models now have the following nested structure in random intercept: repeated individual responses nested in Local authorities or LSOAs (i.e. two random intercepts in the models, one for the individual ID and one for the geography level of local authority or LSOAs).

A multilevel model is needed for our analysis due to the nested structure of the data. As we explain in the manuscript, our data is made up of repeated individual responses over 10 or less data collection waves. Hence there is a need to account the models for the individual ID in random intercept.

Second, individuals are nested within geographical areas (local authorities or LSOAs) and also air pollution is linked at a geographical scale (once at the local authority level and once at the LSOAs level). Therefore, there is a need for another random intercept for the geographical scale (local authority or LSOA) to account for the nested structure of the data and obtain accurate estimates of the effect of air pollution on mental well-being.

In constructing our multilevel models, we didn’t just rely on the ICC but also on the nested structure of the data and the contextual linkage of air pollution. The ICC was used just to show that accounting for individuals ID in random intercepts is necessary given the high observed ICC while that of the household ID might not be needed given the low observed ICC.

We have added into the manuscript on line 335-343 an explanation of why a multilevel model is needed supported by a reference from the literature as follows: “This type of analysis was chosen as it fits the longitudinal panel design of the study which involves repeated individual responses across time linked to air pollution data at the LSOA or local authority level whereby repeated individual responses are nested within LSOAs or local authorities. The random intercept for the individual ID is necessary in the multilevel models given the high homogeneity in the individual’s responses across time (ICC=0.42 and ICC=0.49; Table 5), while the local authority or LSOAs random intercept is needed to allow for less biased assessments of the contextual-linked air pollution effect on mental well-being (Jerrett et al., 2014); resulting in three-levels mixed-effect logit models”.

Jerrett, M., McConnell, R., Wolch, J., Chang, R., Lam, C., Dunton, G., Gilliland, F., Lurmann, F., Islam, T., & Berhane, K. (2014). Traffic-related air pollution and obesity formation in children: a longitudinal, multilevel analysis. Environmental Health, 13(1), 49. https://doi.org/10.1186/1476-069X-13-49

5. I am confused with within vs between effects of pollutants. After reading their process of computation I understand they all are same measured with different level of precision. 1) manually removed the temporal variation and estimated effect size 2) manually removed spatial variation and estimated effect size 3) statistically removed spatiotemporal variation and estimated effects size. Where are the differences? First fix the questions you are asking. The possible questions could be 1) Do the effects of pollution vary over geography? 2) Do the effects vary over duration of exposure? Stratification by interaction term should be the solution of it.

Response: The between effects are the average air pollution exposure across the 11 years of follow-up. Therefore, the measure can provide evidence of whether more polluted local-authorities or LSOAs affect the mental well-being of individuals.

The within effect is the annual deviation of air pollution from the average 11 years air pollution per each local authority or LSOA; thus this measure shows whether fluctuations of air pollution across time within each local authority or LSOA affect individuals’ mental well-being. For example, if person A has poorer well-being than person B – this could be any for a reason/characteristic which make them different; Person A (or B) well-being might become poorer if air pollution conditions change from one year to another, keeping all other conditions/variables fixed: this is the within effects.

We have amended the research questions on line 126-135 and on line 157-170 to reflect the analysis done as follows: “Accordingly, this study investigates longitudinally the overall and the spatial-temporal (between-within) effects of long-term (11 years) exposure to NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollution in the UK on individuals’ reported mental well-being measured using the 12 items General Health Questionnaire (GHQ12) scale. Unlike other studies that assess the effect of air pollution on well-being using one geographical scale, our study aims to assess the effect of air pollution exposure on mental well-being at two geographical scales, coarse local authorities (council areas in Scotland) and detailed Lower Super Output Areas (LSOAs; data zones in Scotland). This will allow us to compare the results between the two geographical scales and explore more deeply the local-contextual patterns of the effect of air pollution on mental well-being. Additionally, our study aims to consider both space and time by determining whether living in more polluted geographical areas (local authorities and LSOAs) is the driving cause for poor mental well-being (between) or whether it is the variation in air pollution across time within each geographical area (within) that is causing poor mental well-being; thus providing detailed spatial-temporal evidence for policymaking decisions”.

We have also added two equations under the data analysis section on line 361-373 to explain the between-within effects in the multilevel mixed effects logit models.

6. Supplementary analysis should be done by either baseline logistic model or ordered logistic, not by binary logistic model. By multiple binary logistic models’ precision of the estimates would be compromised.

Response: This supplementary material has now been removed, and multilevel mixed effects logit models have been moved into the main paper as per the response to points 2 and 3 above. The three GHQ12 outcomes are binary outcomes coded as 0=good mental well-being and 1=poor mental well-being, therefore we have used for analysis multilevel mixed effects logit models as per the relevant literature that uses the same GHQ12 scale measure. All changes are marked in Track changes in the manuscript.

7. In the abstract authors reported regression coefficients but named as OR, I guess. As higher score means worst in mental wellbeing, one should expect a positive association with pollution, OR should be >1.

Response: We apologise for this typo mistake. However, given that the analysis is changed and is now performed on three GHQ12 binary outcomes using multilevel mixed effect logit models, we are reporting in this new version of the abstract ORs not coefficients.

8. Line 385-389: Authors stating in text about high score but reporting regression coefficients doesn’t go with sentence. Is it the mean difference from reference ethnic group? If so, state accordingly.

Response: The sentence was corrected on line 577 after carrying out the new analysis of the three GHQ12 binary outcomes as follows: “It is worth to note that examining the association between ethnicity and mental well-being revealed higher odds of poor mental well-being among people from Pakistani/Bangladeshi (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12: OR=1.17, 95%CI=1.06-1.29; GHQ0-12 ≥ 2: OR=1.09, 95%CI=1.00-1.19; GHQ0-12 ≥ 4: OR=1.13, 95%CI=1.02-1.25) and mixed ethnicities (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12: OR=1.15, 95%CI=1.01-1.32; GHQ0-12 ≥ 2: OR=1.21, 95%CI=1.08-1.37; GHQ0-12 ≥ 4: OR=1.26, 95%CI=1.10-1.43) origin in comparison to the British-white, even after adjusting for socio-demographics and lifestyle covariates (S1 Table 1)”.

9. Looking at the correlation matrix among pollutant, it is worthy to try multipollutant model with SO2, NO2 and PM2.5. Look at variance inflation factor, if less than 8/10, don’t bother, it will work. Best way to assess is add one at a time if AIC decreases you can retain. In case of multicollinearity AIC should increase after adding them in a single model.

Response: Given that the correlation between SO2 and the other three pollutants is low to moderate, we have added to our analysis bi-pollutant models adjusting the NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 models to SO2 pollutant. The results of this analysis are shown in Table 7 and Table 9 in the manuscript.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response_to_reviewers_R1.docx

Decision Letter 1

Bijaya Kumar Padhi

10 Feb 2022

Air pollution and individuals’ mental well-being in the adult population in United Kingdom: A spatial-temporal longitudinal study and the moderating effect of ethnicity

PONE-D-21-31292R1

Dear Dr. Abed Al Ahad,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Bijaya Kumar Padhi, PhD, MPH

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Bijaya Kumar Padhi

15 Feb 2022

PONE-D-21-31292R1

Air pollution and individuals’ mental well-being in the adult population in United Kingdom: A spatial-temporal longitudinal study and the moderating effect of ethnicity

Dear Dr. Abed Al Ahad:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Bijaya Kumar Padhi

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Additional analysis and sensitivity check-ups.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response_to_reviewers_R1.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    We cannot make the data underlying our analysis publicly available due to ethical and legal restrictions. We are using the “Understanding Society: The UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS)” dataset which is an initiative funded by the Economic and Social Research Council and various Government Departments, with scientific leadership by the Institute for Social and Economic Research, University of Essex, and survey delivery by NatCen Social Research and Kantar Public. These data are protected by a copyright license and strictly distributed by the UK Data Service which is the largest digital repository for quantitative and qualitative social science and humanities research data in the UK. Therefore, data underlying our analysis can only be accessed through the UK Data Service for authorized researchers from the following URL: https://beta.ukdataservice.ac.uk/datacatalogue/series/series?id=2000053. We confirm that authors did not have any special access privileges that others would not have. The data can be accessed following registration on the UK Data Archive. An application to access geography data such as the local authority and the Lower Super Output Areas is required using the UK Data Archive platform.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES