Skip to main content
Journal of Clinical Medicine logoLink to Journal of Clinical Medicine
. 2022 Feb 23;11(5):1204. doi: 10.3390/jcm11051204

Higher-Level Executive Functions in Healthy Elderly and Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Systematic Review

Ilaria Corbo 1, Maria Casagrande 2,*
Editor: Giovambattista Desideri
PMCID: PMC8911402  PMID: 35268294

Abstract

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) is a clinical syndrome characterized by a moderate decline in one or more cognitive functions with a preserved autonomy in daily life activities. MCI exhibits cognitive, behavioral, psychological symptoms. The executive functions (EFs) are key functions for everyday life and physical and mental health and allow for the behavior to adapt to external changes. Higher-level executive functions develop from basic EFs (inhibition, working memory, attentional control, and cognitive flexibility). They are planning, reasoning, problem solving, and fluid intelligence (Gf). This systematic review investigates the relationship between higher-level executive functions and healthy and pathological aging, assuming the role of executive functions deficits as a predictor of cognitive decline. The systematic review was conducted according to the PRISMA Statement. A total of 73 studies were identified. The results indicate that 65.8% of the studies confirm significant EFs alterations in MCI (56.8% planning, 50% reasoning, 100% problem solving, 71.4% fluid intelligence). These results seem to highlight a strong prevalence of higher-level executive functions deficits in MCI elderly than in healthy elderly.

Keywords: Mild Cognitive Impairment, ageing, elderly, executive functions, higher-level executive functions, planning, reasoning, fluid intelligence, problem solving

1. Introduction

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) is a syndrome characterized by a clinical profile intermediate between healthy aging and pathological aging. Individuals with MCI do not meet the diagnostic criteria of dementia, but they have worse cognitive functioning than physiological and normal aging [1]. The most common onset symptom is memory impairment, as in Alzheimer’s disease (AD), followed by other impairments [1]. However, cognitive deficits can be detected in cognitive functions other than memory. Petersen et al. [2] divided MCI into four groups based on the number and the type of impaired functions.

The most studied type is amnesic MCI, in which the subject has a memory disorder that can be at a single domain (aMCI) or multiple domains (aMCI–md) [3]. In the latter case, there are other impairments in addition to the memory deficits. On the other hand, if the subject does not have a memory deficit, we speak of non-amnestic MCI, which can be at a single (naMCI) or multiple (naMCI–md) domain based on the functions involved [2]. In 8–12% of cases, MCI evolves into Alzheimer’s disease. Hence, studying this syndrome is fundamental to predicting AD progression [2].

People with aMCI exhibit a reduced thickness of the entorhinal cortex, fusiform gyrus, and hippocampus compared to naMCI and healthy elderly, and reduced thickness of cingulate gyrus and amygdala compared to healthy elderly. A decreased thickness of precuneus is present in both MCI types [4]. These alterations are similar to the Alzheimer’s disease modifications, thus confirming how the MCI is a transitional phase between healthy and pathological aging from an anatomical point of view [5]. The patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment show behavioral and psychological symptoms, in addition to cognitive impairments involving memory and executive functions deficits [6].

The executive functions (EFs) are key functions for everyday life and physical and mental health, which allow adapting the behavior to external changes. The EFs are coordinated and integrated by different neural systems [7,8,9]. Executive functions deficits are the most common cognitive diseases, which can be found in several pathologies [10]. The Prefrontal Cortex (PFC) is the main area regulating EFs [7,9], and its damage can lead to the dysexecutive syndrome. This syndrome may be characterized by behavioral symptoms (e.g., social, sexual, and food behavior disorders, confabulation, and anosognosia) and/or cognitive symptoms (e.g., planning, shifting, theory of mind, and sustained attention disorders) [10]. Godefroy et al. [10] observe that planning is the most compromised cognitive ability in MCI and AD.

According to Diamond’s model, the EFs have four major components: inhibition, working memory, attentional control, and cognitive flexibility. Higher-level executive functions develop from these components. They are planning, reasoning, and problem-solving. Fluid intelligence (Gf) is considered a synonym of the latter two functions [7].

Planning is the ability to organize the behavior in certain situations, and think about the future to achieve a goal through a series of intermediate steps [11]. Shallice and Burgess [12] have described the planning process via four steps: (1) goal articulation; (2) plan formulation; (3) marker creation and triggering; (4) evaluation of initial goals achievement. The planning ability involves the right frontal area and the left frontal lobe. It requires sustained attention, inhibition of automatic responses [13], and the ability to hypothesize different scenarios and their consequences [14].

Reasoning is the ability to convert implicit information into explicit ones, clarify the process if necessary [15], and come to conclusions [16]. It can be divided into inductive and deductive reasoning, and the involved areas are left inferior and middle frontal gyrus, left middle and lateral temporal gyrus, left superior temporal, and cingulate gyrus [16]. Inductive reasoning involves the ability to make predictions regarding new situations based on pre-existent knowledge and is fundamental for categorization, scientific inference, probability, and decision-making [17]. Deductive reasoning is the ability to make logical inferences, and it is one of the major components of intelligence [18].

Problem solving is the capacity to achieve a goal through a sequence of cognitive operations (step by step) or insight [19]. Ordinary problem solving is a way to solve problems based on previous experience and uses well-known solutions. On the other hand, insight is an unconventional way to solve problems and is defined as an unconscious process [20]. Ordinary problem solving is regulated by the Frontoparietal Cognitive Control Network, which includes the middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, and inferior parietal lobule, and it involves a greater activation of Default Mode Network (DMN). Insight in problem solving is regulated by the same areas that regulate the ordinary problem solving with the addition of the anterior cingulate cortex and temporoparietal junction, and it involves a higher activation of visuospatial attention and visual perception areas [19].

Fluid Intelligence is the ability to solve problems via pre-existent acquired information [7,21]. The fluid intelligence is one of the two factors that derive from “general intelligence” together with crystallized intelligence (Gc). Crystallized intelligence is directly related to learning, while the fluid intelligence is the adaptation of the knowledge to a new context. Additionally, the two factors differ in their maximum peak: the crystallized intelligence peaks in early adulthood and is less affected by aging [21]; the fluid intelligence peaks in adolescence, is sensitive to physiological aging, and decreases more during this phase, but it can be stimulated by schooling, education, behavioral training, and stimulant drugs [21,22,23]. The most involved areas in Gf are the left anterior frontal lobe, inferior parietal lobule, and left frontoparietal regions, which are part of the Dorsal Attention Network (DAN). The DAN selects the internal stimuli based on goals or expectations and directs them to the appropriate cognitive or motor response [22,23].

This work proposes to analyze the executive functions in healthy elderly and Mild Cognitive Impairment since there is no other study of our knowledge that observes all the EFs together in these populations. Furthermore, the present study was carried out to evaluate how higher-level EFs could affect the everyday life of healthy and pathological elderly, and how they affect the independence in functional activity (IADL). IADL is one of the criteria of Mild Cognitive Impairment diagnosis [1], which is useful to distinguish it from dementia and could lead to an early diagnosis. This systematic review aims to investigate the relationship between higher-level executive functions and healthy and pathological aging, assuming the role of executive functions deficits as a predictor of cognitive decline. An additional goal is to establish what tests best discriminate healthy elderly from Mild Cognitive Impairment. Moreover, the present review points at evaluating how higher-level executive functions are compromised in both MCI and healthy elderly but with a worse outcome in pathological aging.

2. Materials and Methods

The review process was conducted according to the PRISMA Statement [24,25].

2.1. Research Strategies

A systematic search of the international literature was conducted in the following electronic databases by selecting articles published in peer-review journals: PsycINFO, Scopus, MEDLINE, and Web of Sciences. The last search was conducted on 13 July 2021.

A list of keywords and MeSH terms was generated to identify studies (“Mild Cognitive Impairment” AND “executive function*”); (“Mild Cognitive Impairment” AND “planning”); (“Mild Cognitive Impairment” AND “reasoning”); (“Mild Cognitive Impairment” AND “problem solving”); (“Mild Cognitive Impairment” AND “fluid intelligence”). Restrictions were made, limiting the research to academic publications with English and Italian full text, without restrictions regarding gender and ethnicity. Additionally, the bibliographical references of retrieved papers, reviews, and meta-analyses were screened manually to assess whether they included relevant studies in the review. The number of selected articles is shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Number of selected articles in databases.

Database
PsychINFO 1581
MEDLINE 4067
Scopus 2881
Web of Sciences 2740

2.2. Eligibility Criteria

A total of 11,269 articles were obtained from the search procedure. The first step allowed 5337 duplicates to be eliminated using the Mendeley software. Then, the list of potential articles produced by systematic research was revised. The reading of the title and abstract allowed the first exclusion of 5198 non-inherent studies. A further selection was made by reading the full text (See Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow diagram, PRISMA Statement [24,25].

The inclusion criteria were: adult population (age equal to or higher than 50 years), diagnosis of Mild Cognitive Impairment; healthy subjects; use of higher-level executive functions measurements.

The exclusion criteria were: participants with medical conditions that could potentially influence the investigated relationship (for example, metabolic disorders; cardiovascular disorders; chronic disorders; cancer); participants diagnosed with dementia (Alzheimer Disease; Parkinson’s Disease; Vascular Dementia; Frontotemporal Dementia; Dementia with Lewy Bodies; Huntington’s Disease), psychiatric disorders, neurological disorders, strokes; use of drugs that affect the nervous system and traumatic brain injury; methodological flaws; lack of essential data; assessment made by caregivers; MCI participants included in healthy elderly or AD groups; reviews, dissertations, editorials, comments, replies; trials; age < 50 years and animal models.

2.3. Data Collection

According to the PICOS approach [24], the following information was extracted from each study: authors and year of publication; characteristics of participants (including age, gender, Mini Mental State Examination—MMSE score); diagnostic criteria; experimental paradigm; results.

The extracted data are included in Table 2.

Table 2.

Results of selected studies.

Authors Group Age (SD) (%F) MMSE (SD) Diagnostic Criteria Test Results
Ambra et al. [26] aMCI 15 69.4 (7.59) 33.33 - [27] RCPM No difference in planning has been found
HC 31 69.2 (7.2) 45.16 -
Ávila et al. [28] HC 26 70.58 (7.17) - 26.85 (3.04) [6] TOL No difference in planning has been found
aMCI 38 73.03 (7) - 26.58 (2.03)
aMCI+ 29 77 (7.43) - 23.52 (3.17)
Beaver et al. [29] HC 65 72.34 (8.78) 63.1 - [30,31] Zoo Map Test No difference in planning has been found
MCI 19 70.53 (9.35) 52.6 -
MCI+ 33 71.37 (8.39) 48.5 -
Benavides-Varela et al. [32] MCI 43 75.44 42 26.39 (2.84) [30] RPM MCI: lower abstract reasoning than HC
HC 37 68.89 46 28.73 (1.17)
Berlot et al. [33] HC 20 74 (6.5) 50 - [34] Tower Test
(D-KEFS)
MCI: higher rule violations than HC
MCI 25 76.8 (7.3) 44 -
Beversdorf et al. [35] MCI 26 67.5 (8.9) 53.85 26.1 (1.7) MMSE > 24
CDR = 0.5
Matchstick Problem MCI: lower visuo-spatial problem solving than HC
HC 20 68.0 (8.3) 70 28.8 (1.4)
Bharat et al. [36] MCI 56 68.76 (7.59) 30.4 27.74 (2.43) [1] TOH MCI: higher time than HC
HC 59 67.13 (5.62) 32.3 30.83 (0.64)
Borella et al. [37] MCI 15 72.73 (5.28) 60 27.4 (1.45) [1,38] RCPM MCI: lower logical reasoning than HC
HC 18 69.72 (3.20) 61.11 29.5 (0.62)
Burton et al. [39] HC 158 73.57 (4.72) - 28.92 (1.17) [6,40] Block Design HC: performed better than naMCI, naMCI+ and aMCI+
aMCI 6 79.5 (5.65) - 26.83 (2.48)
naMCI 39 77.54 (5.61) - 28.67 (2.48)
aMCI+ 19 82 (5.04) - 28.16 (1.26)
naMCI+ 28 79.57 (4.86) - 28.68 (1.09)
Chang et al. [41] HC 36 69.33 (4.09) 58.33 - [31] Matrix Reasoning MCI-pa < MCI-na < HC
MCI-na 24 71.54 (8.85) 58.33 -
MCI-pa 22 72.82 (7.83) 50 -
Chao et al. [42] HC 65 68.69 (6.8) 61.6 - [6,43] Matrix Reasoning
Similarities
Matrix Reasoning
MCI: lower reasoning than HC
Similarities
No difference in reasoning has been found
MCI 54 73.46 (9.3) 54.6 -
Chow et al. [44] HC 52 75.19 (6.4) 48.07 - [40] Matrix Reasoning aMCI+: lower reasoning than HC
aMCI 34 76.41 (6.42) 58.82 -
aMCI+ 20 79.15 (5.57) 30 -
De Oliveira et al. [45] HC 61 70.66 (6.55) 57.37 28.38 (1.48) [6] Block Design
RCPM
Block Design
MCI: lower fluid
intelligence than HC
RCPM
MCI: lower fluid
intelligence than HC
MCI 38 72.32 (7.94) 63.15 25.79 (2.74)
De Paula et al. [46] MCI 60 73.7 (8.9) 53.33 24.23 (3.43) [30] TOL (Portella [47] and Krikorian version [48]) Portella et al., [47]
MCI: lower planning than HC
Krikorian et al., [48]
MCI: lower planning than HC
HC 60 74.1 (5.6) 55 27.08 (2.96)
Djordjevic et al. [49] HC 33 73.7 48.5 28.7 [38,50] Similarities
Block Design
Similarities
No difference in verbal abstract reasoning has been found
Block Design
No difference in nonverbal reasoning has been found
MCI 51 75.4 51 27.26
Dwolatzky et al. [51] HC 39 73.41 (8.0) 66.67 29.03 (1.11) [38] Pictorial Puzzles 2x2 MCI: lower accuracy in problem solving task than HC
MCI 30 77.15 (6.43) 43.33 27.63 (1.54)
Econoumou et al. [52] MCI 31 73.58 (6.17) - 28.10 (1.47) [38] Matrix Reasoning MCI: lower fluid intelligence than HC
HC 27 70.56 (8.87) - -
Espinosa et al. [53] HC 50 72.26 (7.85) 74 28.38 (1.68) [38] Action Program Test
Key Search Test
Zoo Map Test
Action Program Test
MCI: lower planning than HC
Key Search Test
No difference in planning has been found
Zoo Map Test
MCI: lower planning than HC
MCI 50 74.30 (6.93) 44 26.06 (2.68)
Garcia–Alvarez et al. [54] HC 124 73.17 (8.6) 60.48 28.49 (1.4) [1] TOL MCI: lower planning than HC
MCI 48 76.68 (10.3) 43.75 25.96 (2.03)
García et al. [55] MCI 5 82 (6.38) 40 24 (1.41) Memory impairment; normal daily living; no dementia Abstraction MCI: lower abstraction than HC
HC 5 74.25 (6.86) 40 28.25 (2.06)
Griffith et al. [56] HC 21 66.7 (7.2) 66.67 29.3 (1.0) [38,57] CLOX–1 No difference in planning has been found
MCI 21 68.1 (8.8) 52.38 28.4 (1.2)
Guild et al. [58] HC 48 70.65 (4.47) 54.17 28.88 (1.36) [59] Block Design Matrix Reasoning Block Design
No difference in IQ has been found
Matrix Reasoning
No difference in visuo-spatial reasoning has been found
aMCI 14 73.07 (6.44) 85.71 28.14 (1.46)
Hellmuth et al. [60] HC 41 68.2 (7.2) 68.29 29.6 (0.6) CDR ≥ 0.5 3 Similarities and
3 Proverbs
No difference in abstraction has been found
MCI 10 68.9 (8.8) 50 28.6 (1.8)
Heuer et al. [61] HC 118 69.4 (0.57) 58.47 29.54 (0.64) CDR ≥ 0.5 3 Similarities and
3 Proverbs
MCI: lower abstraction than HC
MCI 36 72.9 (1.12) 50 28.77 (0.24)
Jefferson et al. [62] HC 40 72.3 (5.5) 60 29.3 (0.9) [1,6] Similarities
Matrix Reasoning
Similarities
MCI: lower verbal abstract reasoning than HC
Matrix Reasoning
MCI: lower nonverbal abstract reasoning than HC
MCI 40 74.3 (7.5) 48 27.8 (1.8)
Jin et al. [63] HC 13 62.6 (7.0) 30.77 29.1 (0.6) MMSE > 24 Sudoku MCI: lower accuracy in problem solving complex task
aMCI 13 63.6 (7.8) 30.77 25.9 (1.8)
Junquera et al. [64] HC 51 71.2 (4.5) - 28.94 (1.36) [1,6] Zoo Maps Test
Similarities
Zoo Maps Test
aMCI+: lower planning than HC and aMCI
naMCI: lower planning than HC
Similarities
aMCI+: lower reasoning than HC
naMCI: lower reasoning than HC
aMCI 26 74.73 (4.53) - 28.54 (1.27)
aMCI+ 50 75.61 (6.46) - 26.20 (2.99)
naMCI+ 18 72.24 (6.14) - 27.77 (2.45)
Kramer et al. [65] HC 35 73.0 (5.3) - 29.5 (0.8) [38] 2 similarities and
2 proverbs
No difference in abstract reasoning has been found
aMCI 86 75.0 (6.1) - 28.5 (1.5)
Levinoff et al. [66] HC 40 74.1 (7.1) - 28.7 (1.2) [67] Similarities
Block Design
Similarities
MCI: lower in abstract verbal reasoning than HC
Block Design
No difference in fluid intelligence
MCI 73 74.0 (7.3) - 27.7 (1.9)
Li et al. [68] HC 28 71.25 (6.43) 60.71 27.61 (1.95) [1,30] Similarities aMCI: lower abstract reasoning than HC
aMCI 29 73.76 (6.42) 62.07 26.07 (2.33)
Li et al. [69] HC 111 73.56 (8.62) 65.8 26.0 (4.44) CDR = 0 Block Design aMCI: lower planning than HC
aMCI 111 75.30 (7.12) 66.7 25.28 (3.47)
Li et al. [70] HC 123 66.26 (9.96) 69.1 28.5 (1.42) [1] Similarities aMCI: lower abstract reasoning than HC
aMCI 106 74.24 (8.05) 48.6 26.03 (2.6)
naMCI 37 71.46 (9.63) 67.6 27.35 (2.20)
Lindbergh et al. [71] HC 35 74.7 (5.97) 66.7 - [30,34] Tower test (D-KEFS) MCI: lower planning than HC
MCI 25 78.6 (5.22) 92 -
Lui et al. [72] HC 93 74.2 (6.5) 85.25 26.6 (2.5) [30] ACED money management
MacCAT-T
ACED
MCI: lower reasoning than HC
MacCAT-T
No difference in reasoning has been found
MCI 92 77.8 (6.8) 71.74 25.3 (2.6)
Lussier et al. [73] HC 26 72.0 (6.4) 69 - [1,6] TOL MCI: lower planning than HC
MCI 22 75.8 (6.5) 36 -
Metzler-Baddeley et al. [74] HC 20 74.0 (6.5) 50 - [34] TOL MCI: higher rule violation than HC
MCI 46 76.8 (7.3) 44 -
Moreira et al. [75] HC 26 68.42 (8.39) 61.54 29.62 (0.7) [76] Proverbs MCI: lower abstraction than HC
MCI 32 68.03 (7.29) 46.87 27.69 (1.31)
Muñoz-Neira et al. [77] HC 30 71.93 (7.06) 50 28.77 (1.14) [6] Similarities No difference in abstraction has been found
MCI 14 71.71 (7.16) 42.9 26.29 (2.13)
Nishi et al. [78] MCI 30 69.8 (7.3) 73.33 26.5 (2.1) MMSE ≥ 24
CDR = 0.5
NINCDS-ADRDA [79]
RCPM MCI: lower reasoning than HC
HC 15 70.9 (4.2) 60 29.1 (1.6)
Nordlund et al. [80] HC 112 67.0 (5.5) - 29.3 (1.1) MMSE < 25 Similarities No difference in verbal abstraction has been found
MCI 35 64.0 (8.2) - 28.5 (1.5)
Nordlund et al. [81] HC 60 66.5 (6.2) 46.67 29.3 (1.1) MMSE < 25 WCST-CV
Similarities
WCST-CV
MCI: lower planning than HC
Similarities
No difference in abstraction has been found
MCI 60 66.4 (6.8) 46.67 28.4 (1.3)
Nordlund et al. [82] HC 50 65.1 (6.1) 54 29.3 (1.0) MMSE < 25 WCST-CV
Similarities
WCST-CV
MCI: lower planning than HC
Similarities
No difference in abstraction has been found
MCI 73 60.7 (6.8) 52.05 28.6 (1.3)
Okonkwo et al. [83] HC 43 66.76 (7.40) 62.79 29.38 (0.89) [1] CLOX–1 No difference in planning has been found
MCI 43 69.54 (8.22) 44.19 28.54 (1.46)
Okonkwo et al. [84] HC 56 64.63 (8.5) 67.9 29.55 (0.76) [30] CLOX–1
DRS–2 Conceptualization
Cognitive Competency Test
CLOX–1
No difference in planning has been found
DRS–2 Conceptualization
No difference in abstraction has been found
Cognitive Competency Test
No difference in verbal reasoning has been found
MCI 60 68.05 (6.77) 56.7 28.37 (1.5)
Pa et al. [85] HC 36 64.8 (8.2) 63.89 29.8 (0.6) [86] Matrix Reasoning
Similarities
Matrix Reasoning
No difference in reasoning has been found
Similarities
No difference in reasoning has been found
aMCI 26 68.0 (6.6) 50 28.7 (1.2)
Pa et al., [87] MCI 57 69.8 (9.3) 47.37 28.4 (1.5) [6] Abstraction No difference in abstraction has been found
HC 40 65.2 (8.9) 50 29.8 (0.5)
Papp et al. [88] HC 92 67.4 (8.8) 65.2 29.2 (1.01) MMSE 24–30
CDR ≤ 0.5
Groton Maze Learning Test MCI: higher exploratory errors, rule-breaks errors and lower difference in errors between trial 1 and trial 2
aMCI 59 69.9 (8.1) 45.8 27.7 (1.35)
Pertl et al. [89] MCI 22 75 50 27 [1,34] CLOX–1 MCI: lower planning than HC
HC 29 73 65.52 29
Pertl et al. [90] HC 19 74 - 29 [1,34] CLOX–1 No difference in planning has been found
MCI 17 79 70.59 27
Peters et al. [91] HC 20 72.0 (6.9) 70 29.6 (0.5) [31] TOL No difference in planning has been found
MCI 22 70.4 (7.1) 59.1 28.1 (1.4)
Rainville et al. [92] HC 42 69.9 (7.3) - 29.4 (0.9) [93] TOL MCI: higher rule breakings and abandoned than HC
MCI 51 68.9 (8.3) - 28.0 (1.6)
Royall et al. [94] HC 45 75.8 (6.0) 75.6 27.8 (2.1) CDR < 3
MMSE < 10
CLOX–1 MCI: lower planning than HC
MCI 40 78.6 (6.7) 72.5 24.8 (2.9)
Sánchez–Benavides et al. [95] HC 30 72.1 (4.7) 51 28.8 (1.2) [96] TOL–Drexel Version No difference in planning has been found
MCI 23 72.9 (7.4) 61 26.3 (2.1)
Sánchez–Benavides et al. 2014 [97] HC 356 64.9 (9.3) 59.6 28.7 (1.5) [79] TOL–Drexel Version MCI: lower total correct than HC
MCI: higher total moves, total initiation time, total execution time and total solving time than HC
MCI 79 72.8 (6.5) 57 25.7 (2.2)
Sanders et al. [98] HC 37 70.27 (7.93) 65.57 - [30,31] Zoo Map Test MCI: higher total errors than HC
MCI 37 72.89 (9.01) 45.94 -
Schmitter-Edgecombe et al. [99] MCI 38 70.58 (8.6) 55.26 - [30,31] Zoo Map Test MCI: lower planning than HC
HC 38 69.34 (7.95) 71.05 -
Schmitter-Edgecombe et al. [100] HC 51 70.94 (8.1) - - [30,31] CLOX–1 MCI: lower planning than HC
MCI 51 70.98 (8.42) - -
Serra et al. [101] aMCI 16 72.5 (6.5) 37.5 25.3 (1.2) [30] RCPM No difference in reasoning has been found
HC 13 64.1 (10.5) 30.77 28.9 (1.3)
Serra et al. [102] aMCI 15 70.9 (9.0) 27 25.4 (1.7) [1,6] RCPM No difference in reasoning has been found
naMCI 13 68.6 (5.7) 77 26.3 (1.6)
HC 28 63.4 (8.9) 37 28.4 (1.7)
Serrao et al. [103] HC 38 67.37 (5.89) - 27.88 (0.62) [1,6] Matrix Reasoning MCI: lower IQ than HC
MCI 61 68.92 (6.49) - 26.03 (0.44)
Sheldon et al. [104] aMCI 16 74.4 (7.4) 69 29.5 (0.7) [1] Means-Ends Problem Solving Test MCI: lower problem solving than MCI
HC 16 75.1 (5.7) 38 28.4 (1.2)
Sherod et al. [105] HC 85 67.2 (8.2) 65 29.4 (0.9) [30] CLOX–1
DRS–2 ConceptualizationCognitive Competency Test
CLOX–1
No difference in planning has been found
DRS–2 Conceptualization
MCI: lower abstraction than HC
Cognitive Competency Test
MCI: lower abstraction than HC
MCI 113 70.3 (7.4) 57 28.1 (1.9)
Tabert et al. [106] HC 83 66.9 (9.1) 59.4 29.3 (0.8) [38] Similarities
Mattis Identities and Oddities
Similarities
MCI: lower verbal abstract reasoning than HC
Mattis Identities and Oddities
No difference in nonverbal abstract reasoning has been found
MCI 148 67.0 (9.9) 55 27.5 (2.2)
Tam et al. [107] MCI 24 73.88 (10.8) 50 27.22 (1.65) MMSE > 24 CLOX–1 No difference in planning has been found
HC 24 73.25 (9.03) 62.5 28.63 (1.38)
Tripathi et al. [108] MCI 22 68.18 (5.7) 27.27 28.0 (2.37) [1] TOH No difference in planning has been found
HC 20 68.65 (6.0) 25 30.0 (1.0)
Urbanowitsch et al. [109] HC 143 73.94 (0.99) 52.45 28.91 (1.12) [110] Similarities MCI: lower reasoning than HC
MCI 63 74.21 (1.03) 50.79 28.07 (1.41)
Weakley et al. [111] MCI 32 69.34 (8.6) 66 - [1] Zoo Map Test No difference in planning has been found
HC 64 68.13 (9.16) 72 -
Wu et al. [112] HC 16 67.75 (5.64) 50 29.13 (1.09) [1] Matrix Reasoning
Block Design
Matrix Reasoning
No difference in IQ has been found
Block Design
MCI: lower IQ than HC
aMCI 13 69.0 (5.69) 53.85 26.23 (2.05)
Zamarian et al. [113] HC 18 65.1 (4.6) 61.11 29.8 (0.4) [1] CLOX–1 MCI: lower planning than HC
MCI 18 69.0 (7.5) 55.55 26.9 (1.2)
Zhang et al. [114] HC 32 73.5 (8.5) - 28.7 (1.8) [38] Trail Making Test (B-A)
Porteus Maze Test
Verbal Fluency Test (fruits and animals)
Trail Making Test
MCI: lower planning than HC
Porteus Maze Test
MCI: lower planning than HC
Verbal Fluency Test
MCI: lower planning than HC
MCI 32 73.7 (8.2) - 27.4 (2.0)
Zhang et al. [115] aMCI 34 67.9 (6.7) 58.82 28.3 (0.5) [1] Abstraction–MoCA
CDT
Abstraction
No difference in abstraction has been found
Clock Drawing Test
No difference in planning has been found
HC 36 67.4 (5.0) 50 29.5 (0.7)
Zheng et al. [116] aMCI 34 67.9 (6.7) 58.82 28.3 (1.5) [40] CDT No difference in planning has been found
HC 36 67.4 (5.0) 50 29.5 (0.7)
Zheng et al. [117] aMCI 50 69.8 (6.8) 68 27.9 (1.5) [40] CDT No difference in planning has been found
HC 48 69.2 (5.1) 60.41 29.5 (0.7)

SD = standard deviations; MMSE = Mini Mental State-Examination; MCI = Mild Cognitive Impairment; aMCI = amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment; naMCI = non amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment; MCI+ = Mild Cognitive Impairment multiple domains; aMCI+ = amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment multiple domain; naMCI+ = non amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment multiple domains; MCI-na = normal awareness for memory deficits; MCI-pa = poor awareness for memory deficits; RCPM = Raven’s Progressive Coloured Matrices; RPM = Raven’s Progressive Matrices; TOL = Tower of London; TOH = Tower of Hanoi; Tower Test (D-KEFS) = Tower test (Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System); CDT = Clock Drawing Test; WCST-CV = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test–Computer Version; MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment; CDR = Clinical Dementia Rating Scale; NINCDS-ADRDA = National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disease and Stroke/Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Associations; AACD = Ageing-Associated Cognitive Decline; DSM = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; CLOX-1 = Clock Drawing Task; DRS-2 =Dementia Rating Scale-2; ACED money management = Assessment of Capacity for Everyday Decision-Making money management; MacCAT-T = The MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool for Treatment.

2.4. Quality Assessment

A quality assessment was carried out to analyze the eligibility of each article to reduce the risk bias. The analysis used five criteria to screen each study selected for systematic review: sampling bias, executive function measurements, diagnostic criteria, selective reporting bias, and methodological bias. Each criterion score ranges from 1 (low risk) to 3 (high risk). The overall quality shall be calculated by adding all the scores obtaining a global score ranging from 5 to 15. The study was considered at low risk of bias if the score was 5, while a score in the 6–10 interval was considered an indicator of a moderate risk of bias. The quality assessment was subdivided into planning, reasoning, fluid intelligence, and problem-solving measurements. The risk of bias is reported in Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Risk Bias of selected articles, considering all higher executive functions.

2.4.1. Quality Assessment of Planning

Figure 3 shows the percentage of articles adopting planning tests fulfilling each quality criterion by the risk of bias assessment. On average, the quality of the studies was good since 36 out of 37 studies (97.3%) exhibited low scores on the risk of bias. The high percentage of studies with low or no risk of bias increases the validity of this systematic review. Despite one study (2.7%) showing moderate scores, no study reports a moderate risk of bias in more than two items. A large percentage of the studies adopted valid and reliable tools to measure planning and included an appropriate sample size. Moreover, most studies were adequately controlled for confounding variables. The higher risk bias was in the “EFs measurements” and the lower in “methodological bias”. In the overall bias, the score ranged from 5 to 7 for every article included.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Risk of Bias for Planning.

2.4.2. Quality Assessment of Reasoning

Figure 4 shows the percentage of articles adopting reasoning tests fulfilling each quality criterion of risk of bias assessment. On average, the quality of the studies was good since 28 out of 32 studies (87.5%) exhibited low scores on the risk of bias. The high percentage of studies with low or no risk of bias increases the validity of this systematic review. Despite four studies (12.5%) showing moderate scores, no study reports a moderate risk of bias in more than two items. A large percentage of the studies used valid and reliable tools to measure reasoning and included an appropriate sample size. Moreover, most studies were adequately controlled for confounding variables. The higher risk bias was in the “methodological bias” and the lower in “sampling bias” “EFs measurements” and “diagnostic criteria”. In the overall bias, the score ranged from 5 to 7 for every article included.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Risk of Bias for Reasoning.

2.4.3. Quality Assessment of Problem Solving

Figure 5 shows the percentage of articles adopting a problem-solving task fulfilling each quality criterion of risk of bias assessment. On average, the quality of the studies was good since six out of six studies (100%) exhibited low scores on the risk of bias. The high percentage of studies with low or no risk of bias increases the validity of this systematic review. No study reports a moderate risk of bias in more than one item. A large percentage of the studies used valid and reliable tools to measure problem solving and included an appropriate sample size. Moreover, most studies were adequately controlled for confounding variables. The higher risk bias was in the “EFs measurements” and the lower in “sampling bias”, “methodological bias”, and “diagnostic criteria”. In the overall bias, the score ranged from 5 to 6 for every article included.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Risk of Bias for Problem Solving.

2.4.4. Quality Assessment of Fluid Intelligence

Figure 6 shows the percentage of articles adopting fluid intelligence measurements fulfilling each quality criterion of risk of bias assessment. On average, the quality of the studies was good since six out of seven studies (85.7%) exhibited low scores on the risk of bias. The high percentage of studies with low or no risk of bias increases the validity of this systematic review. Despite one study (14.3%) showing moderate scores, no study reports a moderate risk of bias in more than two items. A large percentage of the studies used valid and reliable tools to measure fluid intelligence and included an appropriate sample size. Moreover, most studies were adequately controlled for confounding variables. The higher risk bias was in the “methodological bias” and the lower in “EFs measurements” and “diagnostic criteria”. In the overall bias, the score ranged from 5 to 7 for every article included.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Risk of Bias for Fluid Intelligence.

3. Results

3.1. Studies Selection

The flow chart shows the number of studies identified from the databases and the number of studies examined, assessed for eligibility, and included in the review with the reasons for possible exclusions (see Figure 1). A total of 73 studies were identified.

Of the 73 selected studies, 30 analyzed planning, 31 reasoning, six problem solving, and seven fluid intelligence. Nine studies used different executive function measures.

Results will be presented in two subsections, according to the higher-level executive functions and the MCI subtype.

3.2. Planning (N = 37)

Thirty-seven studies have measured planning in healthy elderly and MCI participants with an overall sample of 3491 participants (1919 HC and 1572 MCI) with a mean age that ranges from 60.7 years [82] to 79 years [90].

Thirteen studies used “CLOX-1” or “Clock Drawing Test (CDT)” [56,83,84,89,90,94,100,105,107,113,115,116,117]; two studies used the “Wisconsin Card Sorting Test–Computer Version (WCST–CV)” [81,82]; 10 studies used the “Tower of London (TOL)” [28,33,46,54,73,74,91,92,95,96], six studies used “Zoo Map Test” [29,53,64,98,99,111], two studies used “Tower of Hanoi (TOH)” [36,108]; one study used “Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (RCPM)” [26]; one study used “Tower Test (D-KEFS)” [71]; one study used “Groton Maze Learning Test” [88]; one study used “Trail Making Test (B-A)” [114]; one study used “Porteus Maze Test” [114]; one study used “Verbal Fluency Test (fruits and animals version)” [114]; one study used “Action Program Test” [53]; and one study used “Key search Test” [53].

Fifteen studies did not report any significant difference between groups [26,28,29,56,83,84,89,90,95,105,107,108,110,115,116,117]. One study [53] performed three tests to assess planning ability and observed a worse performance in MCI in only two of them (Action Program Test and Zoo Map Test). The remaining 21 studies reported poor performance in MCI than healthy subjects [33,36,46,54,64,71,73,74,81,82,88,89,90,94,97,98,99,100,113,114].

Nine studies [28,36,46,54,71,73,74,92,97] of the thirteen that analyzed the planning abilities with tower tests (“Tower of London”, “Tower of Hanoi”, and “Tower Test (D-KEFS)”) highlighted a poorer performance in MCI than healthy groups. Metzler-Baddeley et al. [74] and Berlot et al. [33] observed more rule violations during the performance of the task in MCI, while Bharath et al. [36] reported a longer time to complete the test. De Paula et al. [46] used two versions of “Tower of London” (designed by Portella et al. [47] and Krikorian et al. [48]) and observed a lower planning ability in MCI subjects. Rainville et al. [92] pointed out a higher rule breaking and abandonment rate in MCI. Sánchez–Benavides et al. [96] saw in Mild Cognitive Impairment subjects higher total moves, total initiation time, total exclusion time, total solving time, and lower total correct rates than healthy subjects. Also, Garcia-Alvarez et al. [54], Lindbergh et al. [71], and Lussier et al. [73] found poor planning in Mild Cognitive Impairment subjects.

Four studies [89,94,100,113] that used “CLOX-1” found a lower planning capacity in Mild Cognitive Impairment samples.

Three studies used the “Zoo Map Test” [64,98,99]. Sanders et al. [98] highlighted higher total errors in MCI than healthy controls. Junquera et al. [64] analyzed the differences between healthy subjects, aMCI, naMCI, and aMCI multiple domains: aMCI multiple domains showed lower planning than healthy elderly and aMCI single domain, while naMCI subjects showed a poor planning ability than healthy elderly. Sanders et al. [98] observed decreased planning ability in MCI compared to healthy elderly.

Espinosa et al., [53] analyzed the differences between healthy subjects and MCI using the “Zoo Map Test” and “Action Program Test”, in both tests, MCI had lower planning than healthy controls. Nordlund et al. [81,82] found a poor planning ability, assessed with the “Wisconsin Card Sorting Test–Computer Version (WCST–CV)”, in MCI subjects than healthy elderly. Papp et al. [88] used the “Groton Maze Learning Test” to evaluate planning and underlined that participants with MCI exhibit higher exploratory errors, more rule-breaks errors, and reduced differences between trial 1 and trial 2 than healthy subjects. Zhang et al. [114] evaluated the differences between healthy subjects and MCI with “Trail Making Test (B-A)”, “Porteus Maze Test”, and “Verbal Fluency (fruits and animals)”, and in each test, Mild Cognitive Impairment showed lower planning than healthy elderly.

3.3. Reasoning (N = 32)

Thirty-two studies measured reasoning in healthy elderly and Mild Cognitive Impairment subjects, with an overall sample of 3371 participants (1676 HC and 1695 MCI) and a mean age ranging from 60.7 years [82] to 82 years [55].

Seventeen studies used “Similarities” [42,50,60,61,62,64,65,66,68,70,77,80,81,82,85,106,109]; six studies used “Matrix Reasoning” [41,42,44,58,62,85]; four studies used “Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (RCPM)” [37,78,101,102]; four studies used “Proverbs” [60,61,65,75]; two studies used “Cognitive Competency Test” [84,105]; two studies used “Abstraction–MoCA” [55,115]; two studies used “DRS–2 Conceptualization” [84,105]; one study used “ACED Money Management” [72]; one study used “MacCAT-T” [72]; one study used “Block Design” [49]; one study used “Mattis Identities and Oddities” [105]; one study used “Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPM)” [32]; one study used “Abstraction (Wechsler et al., [118], Kramer et al., [119])” [87].

Fifteen studies did not report any significant difference between samples [49,58,60,61,65,77,80,81,82,84,85,87,101,102,115].

Three studies [42,72,106] performed two tasks each and observed lower reasoning ability in MCI participants in only one of them.

The remaining 14 studies reported differences between MCI and healthy subjects [32,37,41,44,55,62,64,66,68,70,75,78,105,109].

Seven studies used “Similarities” [62,64,66,68,70,106,109] to assess reasoning in healthy elderly and Mild Cognitive Impairment samples and reported lower performance in reasoning in MCI subjects. Junquera et al. [64] analyzed the differences between healthy subjects, aMCI, naMCI, and aMCI multiple domains: both aMCI multiple domains and naMCI showed lower reasoning than healthy elderly. Four studies used “Matrix Reasoning” [41,42,44,62] to evaluate MCI and healthy subjects, and in each study, a decreased reasoning in Mild Cognitive Impairment subjects was highlighted. In particular, Chang [41] observed a higher performance in healthy subjects than MCI with normal awareness for memory (MCI-na), which in turn were better than MCI with poor awareness for memory (MCI-pa). Two studies used “Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices–RCPM” [37,78] and observed a reduced reasoning ability in Mild Cognitive Impairment participants, as well as Benavides-Varela et al., [32] that used “Raven’s Progressive Matrices–RPM”. Sherod et al. [105] analyzed the differences between healthy subjects and MCI with the “DRS–2 Conceptualization” and “Cognitive Competency Test”, and in both tests MCI had lower abstraction than healthy controls. Lui et al., [72] used “ACED Money Management” and reported a reduced reasoning ability in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Moreira et al. [75] used “Proverbs” to evaluate the reasoning in healthy elderly and MCI participants and observed higher abstraction ability in healthy subjects than MCI. García et al. [55] used “Abstraction (MoCA)” and pointed out a reduced ability in abstraction in MCI subjects.

3.4. Problem Solving (N = 6)

Six studies have assessed problem solving in MCI and a control group, with an overall sample composed of 344 participants (236 MCI and 108 HC) and a mean age ranging from 62.6 years [63] to 82 years [39]. Each study [35,51,63,88,104] used a different task to evaluate problem solving, and they all showed differences between the samples.

Beversdorf et al., [35] used the “Matchstick Problem” to evaluate visuospatial problem solving and highlighted lower capacity in the MCI sample to solve problems. Burton et al., [39] used “Block Design” to evaluate problem solving ability in healthy elderly, aMCI single and multiple domains, naMCI single and multiple domain participants. They observed better performance in healthy subjects than amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment multiple domains, non-amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment single, and multiple domains, but not to aMCI single domain subjects. Dwolatzky et al., [51] used “Pictorial Puzzles (2x2)” and reported a reduced accuracy in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Jin et al., [63] evaluated problem solving with “Sudoku (Nikoli Publishing)” and highlighted a decreased accuracy in complex tasks in aMCI subjects compared to healthy subjects. Papp et al., [88] used the “Groton Maze Learning Test” to evaluate problem solving and underlined that participants with MCI have higher exploratory errors, more rule-breaks errors, and a lower difference between trial 1 and trial 2 than healthy subjects. Sheldon et al., [104] used the “Means-Ends Problem Solving Test” and observed a reduced problem-solving ability in MCI subjects.

3.5. Fluid Intelligence (N = 7)

Seven studies have measured fluid intelligence in healthy elderly and Mild Cognitive Impairment subjects, with an overall sample of 682 participants (341 HC and 341 MCI) and a mean age ranging from 67.37 years [103] to 75.3 years [69]. Five studies used “Block Design” [45,58,66,69,112], three studies used “Matrix Reasoning” [52,103,112], and one study used “Raven Coloured Matrices” to evaluate fluid intelligence.

Two studies [58,66] did not report any significant difference between samples, one study performed multiple tests and showed conflicting results [110], while the others [45,52,69,103] reported lower performance in fluid intelligence in MCI subjects.

3.6. Amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment (N = 20)

Twenty studies analyzed the differences between aMCI and healthy elderly in higher-level executive functions, with an overall sample of 1783 participants (969 HC and 814 aMCI) and a mean age ranging from 62.6 years [64] to 82 years [39]. Eleven of these (55%) reported a significant difference between healthy and pathological elderly. Eight studies [26,28,64,69,88,115,116,117] compared planning ability in aMCI and healthy elderly, and only three of these (37.5%) [64,69,88] reported a worse performance in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Ten studies [44,58,64,65,68,70,85,101,102,115] analyzed reasoning in amnesic MCI and a control group, four of these (40%) [44,64,68,70] highlighted a poorer reasoning ability in MCI. Four studies [39,63,88,104] investigated problem solving in amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment and healthy control, and all (100%) of them highlighted a significant difference between groups. Two studies [58,112] evaluated fluid intelligence, and only one [112] observed a lower Gf in aMCI. Additionally, only four [28,39,44,64] of these studies have distinguished between amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment single and multiple domains. In two of them [44,64], aMCI+ have reported a poorer performance regarding aMCI single domain with respect to healthy subjects.

3.7. Non-Amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment (N = 4)

Four studies analyzed the differences between naMCI and healthy elderly in higher-level executive functions, with an overall sample of 495 participants (360 HC and 135 naMCI) and a mean age ranging from 63.4 years [102] to 79.57 years [39]. Three of these (75%) reported a significant difference between groups. One study [64] evaluated planning in naMCI and healthy elderly, highlighting a poorer performance in MCI sample. The most analyzed higher-level executive function in non-amnesic MCI is reasoning, which is evaluated in three studies [64,70,102], but only one (33.3%) [64] of them highlighted a lower reasoning ability in naMCI. Finally, one study [39] analyzed problem solving and observed a worse performance in naMCI compared to healthy elderly. Only one study [39] set apart non-amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment multiple domain from naMCI single domain, showing no difference between the two groups. No one evaluated fluid intelligence in non-amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment, and no one analyzed them without the amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment sample.

4. Discussion

The purpose of this systematic review was to investigate the relationship between higher-level executive functions and healthy and pathological aging, assuming the role of executive functions deficits as a predictor of the general cognitive decline. Results showed that not all the studies found a prevalence of higher-level executive functions deficits in individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment diagnosis compared to healthy elderly; however, 64.4% of the studies confirm a significant presence of alterations in MCI (56.8% planning, 50% reasoning, 100% problem solving, 71.4% fluid intelligence).

Despite the scarce number of observations that do not allow reliable conclusions, the evaluation of problem solving showed significant results. These data must be interpreted with caution because the studies [35,39,51,63,88,104] used different tasks to evaluate this ability. One interesting finding was observed by Burton et al. [39] that compared healthy subjects, amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment single and multiple domains, and non-amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment single and multiple domains to analyze problem solving. According to the literature, the authors reported lower problem-solving capacity in participants with aMCI multiple domains and naMCI single and multiple domains compared to healthy elderly, while the aMCI single domain subjects did not report any significant difference with the others. These results could be attributed to the Mild Cognitive Impairment [1], in which the only impaired cognitive domain is memory. On the other hand, Jin et al. [63] found a significant difference between aMCI and healthy control group; the author reported a positive linear correlation between blood oxygen levels in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and precuneus in aMCI subjects during simple (r = 0.95) and complex (r = 0.90) problem solving tasks. In addition, healthy elderly showed a deactivation of these areas while the aMCI showed an activation. These regions are included in the Default Mode Network (DMN) and, taking into account the close relationship with the hippocampus, these activations in aMCI may be explained as a compensatory memory mechanism.

The results of fluid intelligence must be interpreted with caution due to the small number of studies that measured this variable [45,52,58,66,69,103,112]. A possible source of error about fluid intelligence ability is linked to the type of assessment carried out: this review includes studies that evaluated the intelligence quotient employing tests commonly used to assess fluid intelligence.

Despite this, Li et al., [69], through the means regression and the cluster analysis, observed that the “Block Design” test could predict conversion from a healthy state to amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment. Another important finding is observed by Wu et al., [111] that studied, in amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment and healthy elderly, the Resting State-Executive Control Network (RS-ECN), a network that is adjacent to DMN and the other major attention networks and with which it shares some anatomical areas. The aMCI showed a decreased functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), inferior parietal lobule (IPC), lateral parietal and anterior insula, precuneus, middle frontal gyrus, left and right dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC); these regions are strictly involved in the Ventral Attention Network and more generally in executive functions. Moreover, the author [112] also observed increased functional connectivity of different areas of the Default Mode Network, the Ventral Attention Network (VAN), and the Dorsal Attention Network: the right anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC), left and right ventral lateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), superior parietal cortex, posterior parietal lobule, occipital and temporal. Even if these regions are not involved in fluid intelligence, they are still implicated in planning, reasoning, problem solving, abstract thinking, and other executive functions, with particular reference to the DLPFC. The overall results of fluid intelligence, although not uniform, pursued a trend towards higher prevalence of this ability deficit in Mild Cognitive Impairment.

The results about planning are projected to highlight a negative trend in MCI that reported lower ability than healthy elderly. However, these data must be interpreted with caution because not all tests provided statistically significant results, and some studies used inappropriate tests. In particular, some studies used the “Clock Drawing Test” and “CLOX-1”, which are not specific for planning evaluation but are instead typically used in neuropsychological batteries to investigate other cognitive functions. The “Clock Drawing Test” is commonly used to assess praxis and visuospatial skills, while the “CLOX-1” is the version that evaluates the executive functions (e.g., goal selection, planning, selective attention, and motor sequencing) [120]. Despite this distinction, four studies [57,116,117,118] used the “Clock Drawing Test” to assess executive functions, and neither of these reported any significant difference between MCI and healthy elderly. In addition, the studies that used “CLOX–1” [83,84,89,104,106] did not report significant differences between MCI and healthy subjects; only a few of studies [89,94,100,113] highlighted lower planning ability in pathological aging. These results could be explained by the low sensitivity of this test in discriminating between MCI and healthy elderly. However, the studies that used the “Clock Drawing Test” and the “CLOX-1” were included too, since both original validations [120,121] considered the test adequate to assess planning ability.

On the other hand, the “Zoo Map Test” and the tower tests (“Tower of London”, “Tower of Hanoi”, and “Tower Test (D-KEFS)”) seem well to discriminate the differences between healthy and MCI participants. Junquera et al., [64] analyzed the differences between healthy subjects and single and multiple domain aMCI and naMCI participants. Subjects with aMCI multiple domains and single and multiple domains naMCI subjects exhibited lower planning ability than healthy elderly. Two studies [33,74] observed more rule violations during tasks in MCI; in addition, Rainville et al., [92] pointed out a higher rule breaking and abandonment rate in MCI than healthy participants. Metzler-Baddeley et al., [74] have also observed a correlation between the number of rule violations in the TOL and the variation of mean diffusivity in the bilateral anterior cingulum and the fornix.

Although not all results showed a statistically significant difference, many reasoning deficits can be observed in MCI. Most studies used the “Similarities” test to evaluate reasoning, which identifies the relationship between a couple of words. Seven of these studies [62,64,66,68,70,106,109] reported significant differences between healthy and Mild Cognitive Impairment elderly. Chang [41] compared healthy and MCI participants with and without awareness for memory problems and observed a higher performance in healthy subjects than in MCI with normal awareness for memory (MCI-na), which in turn were better than MCI with poor awareness for memory (MCI-pa). In addition, MCI-pa showed reduced white matter integrity of left dorsal frontal–striatal tract, right dorsal frontal–striatal tract, left anterior thalamocortical radiations–ventral part, corpus callosum–inferior parietal lobule, and corpus callosum–ventral prefrontal regions. Nishi et al., [78] found a correlation between reasoning task execution and reduced glucose reuptake in the right middle frontal gyrus and higher activation in the same area.

Not all the studies that analyzed higher-level executive functions highlighted significant differences. Generally, it may be concluded that elderly with Mild Cognitive Impairment exhibit poorer performance than healthy elderly. Due to small observations, problem solving and fluid intelligence results do not allow reliable conclusions. Despite this, the results appear promising, showing higher executive function deficits in MCI. Though numerous and highlighting a worse performance in MCI, planning and reasoning results do not always show significant differences between groups. This could be related to low sensitivity measures to discriminate MCI from normal aging.

Limits

Despite the encouraging results, this review holds some limitations. The major limitation is the lack of quantitative analysis (meta-analysis), which is difficult to carry out because of the large number of different tests and diagnostic criteria adopted by the studies. The absence of a standardized protocol to evaluate the higher-level executive functions represents another limitation, leading to the administration of rarely used tests and, consequently, to hardly generalizable results. An additional limiting factor of this review is task impurity and, therefore, the difficulty of separately evaluating each higher-level EF. For example, the “Matrix Reasoning” is used to evaluate: reasoning [42,44,85], visuospatial reasoning [58], non-verbal abstract reasoning [62], intelligence quotient [103,111], and fluid intelligence [52]. A further limit can be related to publication bias. Lastly, this review is based on Diamond’s model [7], and therefore it focuses on some executive functions excluding all others, such as decision-making.

5. Conclusions

The results of this systematic review seem to highlight a higher prevalence of higher-level executive functions disease in elderly with Mild Cognitive Impairment than in healthy elderly, confirming results already observed with other executive functions, such as cognitive and motor inhibition, conflict control, and cognitive flexibility [122], although some of these EFs are also compromised in healthy elderly [123]. MCI shows modifications over every aspect investigated in this research, highlighting significant differences that could worsen the quality of life. As far as we know, this study is the first to evaluate these aspects in healthy and MCI elderly. Certainly, a future goal will be to establish and create a standardized protocol to discriminate MCI from healthy elderly. Such a protocol should accurately measure reasoning, planning, problem solving, and fluid intelligence since these functions are treated as a single construct included in executive functions. An important goal for the next studies will be to figure out if higher-level executive functions diseases are early symptoms of Mild Cognitive Impairment or, on the other hand, MCI leads to poorer higher-level executive functions abilities as a consequence of the more significant alterations of the nervous system occurring in pathologically older age than in healthy elderly.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, I.C. and M.C.; methodology, I.C. and M.C.; writing—review and editing, I.C. and M.C.; supervision, M.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by Ph.D. Founding Program, Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Petersen R.C. Mild Cognitive Impairment. N. Engl. J. Med. 2011;364:2227–2234. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcp0910237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Petersen R.C. Mild Cognitive Impairment. Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2016;22:404–418. doi: 10.1212/CON.0000000000000313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.de Mendonça A., Guerreiro M., Ribeiro F., Mendes T., Garcia C. Mild cognitive impairment. J. Mol. Neurosci. 2004;23:143–147. doi: 10.1385/JMN:23:1-2:143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Csukly G., Sirály E., Fodor Z., Horváth A., Salacz P., Hidasi Z., Csibri É., Rudas G., Szabó Á. The Differentiation of Amnestic Type MCI from the Non-Amnestic Types by Structural MRI. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016;8:52. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Haroutunian V., Hoffman L.B., Been M.S. Is there a neuropathology difference between Mild Cognitive Impairment and dementia? Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2009;11:171. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2009.11.2/vharoutunian. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Winblad B., Palmer K., Kivipelto M., Jelic V., Fratiglioni L., Wahlund L.O., Petersen R.C. Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: Report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Intern. Med. 2004;256:240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013;64:135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Miyake A., Friedman N.P. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012;21:8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Funahashi S., Andreau J.M. Prefrontal cortex and neural mechanisms of executive function. J. Physiol. Paris. 2013;107:471–482. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Godefroy O., Azouvi P., Robert P., Roussel M., LeGall D., Meulemans T., Behalf of the Groupe de Réflexion sur l’Evaluation des Fonctions Exécutives Study Group Dysexecutive syndrome: Diagnostic criteria and validation study. Ann. Neurol. 2010;68:855–864. doi: 10.1002/ana.22117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Owen A.M. Cognitive planning in humans: Neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neuropharmacological perspectives. Prog. Neurobiol. 1997;53:431–450. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(97)00042-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Shallice T.I.M., Burgess P.W. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain. 1991;114:727–741. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.2.727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Gouveia P.A.R., Brucki S.M.D., Malheiros S.M.F., Bueno O.F.A. Disorders in planning and strategy application in frontal lobe lesion patients. Brain Cogn. 2007;63:240–246. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Carlin D., Bonerba J., Phipps M., Alexander G., Shapiro M., Grafman J. Planning impairments in frontal lobe dementia and frontal lobe lesion patients. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:655–665. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00102-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wertheim J., Ragni M. The neurocognitive correlates of human reasoning: A meta-analysis of conditional and syllogistic inferences. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000;32:1061–1078. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Goel V., Gold B., Kapur S., Houle S. Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1998;10:293–302. doi: 10.1162/089892998562744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hayes B.K., Heit E., Swendsen H. Inductive reasoning. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2010;1:278–292. doi: 10.1002/wcs.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Johnson-Laird P. Deductive reasoning. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2010;1:8–17. doi: 10.1002/wcs.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lin J., Wen X., Cui X., Xiang Y., Xie J., Chen Y., Mo L. Common and specific neural correlates underlying insight and ordinary problem solving. Brain Imaging Behav. 2021;15:1374–1387. doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00337-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Öllinger M., Jones G., Knoblich G. Investigating the effect of mental set on insight problem solving. Exp. Psychol. 2008;55:269–282. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.55.4.269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cattell R.B. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. J. Educ. Psychol. 1963;54:1. doi: 10.1037/h0046743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Santarnecchi E., Emmendorfer A., Pascual-Leone A. Dissecting the parieto-frontal correlates of fluid intelligence: A comprehensive ALE meta-analysis study. Intelligence. 2017;63:9–28. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.04.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Santarnecchi E., Emmendorfer A., Tadayon S., Rossi S., Rossi A., Pascual-Leone A. Network connectivity correlates of variability in fluid intelligence performance. Intelligence. 2017;65:35–47. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.10.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Liberati A., Altman D.G., Tetzlaff J., Mulrow C., Gøtzsche P.C., Ioannidis J.P.A., Clarke M., Devereaux P.J., Kleijnen J., Moher D. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2009;62:e1–e34. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Moher D., Liberati A., Tetzlaff J., Altman D.G., The PRISMA Group Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ambra F.I., Iavarone A., Ronga B., Chieffi S., Carnevale G., Iaccarino L., Garofalo E. Qualitative patterns at Raven’s colored progressive matrices in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2016;28:561–565. doi: 10.1007/s40520-015-0438-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Musicco M., Sorbi S., Bonavita V., Caltagirone C. Validation of the guidelines for the diagnosis of dementia and Alzheimer’s Disease of the Italian Neurological Society. Study in 72 Italian neurological centres and 1549 patients. Neurol. Sci. 2004;25:289–295. doi: 10.1007/s10072-004-0356-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ávila R.T., de Paula J.J., Bicalho M.A., Moraes E.N., Nicolato R., Malloy-Diniz L.F., Diniz B.S. Working Memory and Cognitive Flexibility Mediates Visuoconstructional Abilities in Older Adults with Heterogeneous Cognitive Ability. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS. 2015;21:392–398. doi: 10.1017/S135561771500034X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Beaver J., Wilson K.B., Schmitter-Edgecombe M. Characterising omission errors in everyday task completion and cognitive correlates in individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment and dementia. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2019;29:804–820. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2017.1337039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Petersen R.C., Doody R., Kurz A., Mohs R.C., Morris J.C., Rabins P.V., Winblad B. Current concepts in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Arch. Neurol. 2001;58:1985–1992. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.12.1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Petersen R.C., Morris J.C. Mild cognitive impairment as a clinical entity and treatment target. Arch. Neurol. 2005;62:1160–1163. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.7.1160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Benavides-Varela S., Burgio F., Weis L., Mitolo M., Palmer K., Toffano R., Semenza C. The role of limbic structures in financial abilities of Mild Cognitive Impairment patients. NeuroImage Clin. 2020;26:102222. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Berlot R., Metzler-Baddeley C., Ikram M.A., Jones D.K., O’Sullivan M.J. Global efficiency of structural networks mediates cognitive control in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016;8:292. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Albert M.S. Changes in cognition. Neurobiol. Aging. 2011;32:S58–S63. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Beversdorf D.Q., Ferguson J.L.W., Hillier A., Sharma U.K., Nagaraja H.N., Bornstein R.A., Scharre D.W. Problem solving ability in patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 2007;20:44–47. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e31802e5101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bharath S., Sadanand S., Kumar K.J., Balachandar R., Joshi H., Varghese M. Clinical and neuropsychological profile of persons with Mild Cognitive Impairment, a hospital based study from a lower and middle income country. Asian J. Psychiatry. 2017;30:185–189. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2017.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Borella E., Carretti B., Mitolo M., Zavagnin M., Caffarra P., Mammarella N., Piras F. Characterizing cognitive inhibitory deficits in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Psychiatry Res. 2017;251:342–348. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Petersen R.C., Smith G.E., Waring S.C., Ivnik R.J., Tangalos E.G., Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 1999;56:303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Burton C.L., Strauss E., Bunce D., Hunter M.A., Hultsch D.F. Functional abilities in older adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment. Gerontology. 2009;55:570–581. doi: 10.1159/000228918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Petersen R.C. Mild Cognitive Impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Intern. Med. 2004;256:183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Chang Y.-L., Chao R.-Y., Hsu Y.-C., Chen T.-F., Tseng W.-Y.I. White matter network disruption and cognitive correlates underlying impaired memory awareness in Mild Cognitive Impairment. NeuroImage Clin. 2021;30:102626. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Chao R.-Y., Chen T.-F., Chang Y.-L. Executive Function Predicts the Validity of Subjective Memory Complaints in Older Adults beyond Demographic, Emotional, and Clinical Factors. J. Prev. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2021;8:161–168. doi: 10.14283/jpad.2020.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Jak A.J., Bondi M.W., Delano-Wood L., Wierenga C., Corey-Bloom J., Salmon D.P., Delis D.C. Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining Mild Cognitive Impairment. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2009;17:368–375. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31819431d5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Chow R., Rabi R., Paracha S., Vasquez B.P., Hasher L., Alain C., Anderson N.D. Reaction time intra-individual variability reveals inhibitory deficits in single- and multiple-domain amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2021;77:71–83. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbab051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.De Oliveira M.O., Nitrini R., Yassuda M.S., Brucki S.M.D. Vocabulary is an appropriate measure of premorbid intelligence in a sample with heterogeneous educational level in brazil. Behav. Neurol. 2014;2014:875960. doi: 10.1155/2014/875960. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.De Paula J.J., Moreira L., Nicolato R., de Marco L.A., Côrrea H., Romano-Silva M.A., Malloy-Diniz L.F. The Tower of London Test: Different scoring criteria for diagnosing Alzheimer’s disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment. Psychol. Rep. 2012;110:477–488. doi: 10.2466/03.10.13.PR0.110.2.477-488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Portella M.J., Marcos-Bars T., Rami-González L., Navarro-Odriozola V., Gastó-Ferrer C., Samalero M. “Torre de Londres”: Planificatión mental, validez y efecto techo. Rev. Neurol. 2003;37:2010–2313. doi: 10.33588/rn.3703.2003156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Krikorian R., Bartok J., Gay N. Tower of London procedure: A standard method and developmental data. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1994;16:840–850. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Djordjevic J., Jones-Gotman M., De Sousa K., Chertkow H. Olfaction in patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 2008;29:693–706. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Chertkow H. Mild cognitive impairment. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2002;15:401–407. doi: 10.1097/00019052-200208000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Dwolatzky T., Whitehead V., Doniger G.M., Simon E.S., Schweiger A., Jaffe D., Chertkow H. Validity of a novel computerized cognitive battery for Mild Cognitive Impairment. BMC Geriatr. 2003;3:4. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-3-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Economou A., Papageorgiou S.G., Karageorgiou C., Vassilopoulos D. Nonepisodic memory deficits in amnestic MCI. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 2007;20:99–106. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e31804c6fe7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Espinosa A., Alegret M., Boada M., Vinyes G., Valero S., Martínez-Lage P., Tárraga L. Ecological assessment of executive functions in Mild Cognitive Impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS. 2009;15:751–757. doi: 10.1017/S135561770999035X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Garcia-Alvarez L., Gomar J.J., Sousa A., Garcia-Portilla M.P., Goldberg T.E. Breadth and depth of working memory and executive function compromises in Mild Cognitive Impairment and their relationships to frontal lobe morphometry and functional competence. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2019;11:170–179. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2018.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.García S., Cuetos F., Novelli A., Martínez C. A new and short protocol to achieve the early diagnosis of Mild Cognitive Impairment. Neurol. Sci. 2021;42:3687–3694. doi: 10.1007/s10072-021-05044-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Griffith H.R., Belue K., Sicola A., Krzywanski S., Zamrini E., Harrell L., Marson D.C. Impaired financial abilities in Mild Cognitive Impairment: A direct assessment approach. Neurology. 2003;60:449–457. doi: 10.1212/WNL.60.3.449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ritchie K., Artero S., Touchon J. Classification criteria for Mild Cognitive Impairment: A population-based validation study. Neurology. 2001;56:37–42. doi: 10.1212/WNL.56.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Guild E.B., Vasquez B.P., Maione A.M., Mah L., Ween J., Anderson N.D. Dynamic working memory performance in individuals with single-domain amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2014;36:751–760. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.941790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Petersen R.C., Smith G.E., Waring S.C., Ivnik R.J., Kokmen E., Tangelos E.G. Aging, memory, and Mild Cognitive Impairment. Int. Psychogeriatr. 1997;9:65–69. doi: 10.1017/S1041610297004717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Hellmuth J., Mirsky J., Heuer H.W., Matlin A., Jafari A., Garbutt S., Boxer A.L. Multicenter validation of a bedside antisaccade task as a measure of executive function. Neurology. 2012;78:1824–1831. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318258f785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Heuer H.W., Mirsky J.B., Kong E.L., Dickerson B.C., Miller B.L., Kramer J.H., Boxer A.L. Antisaccade task reflects cortical involvement in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Neurology. 2013;81:1235–1243. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a6cbfe. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Jefferson A.L., Lambe S., Moser D.J., Byerly L.K., Ozonoff A., Karlawish J.H. Decisional capacity for research participation in individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2008;56:1236–1243. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01752.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Jin G., Li K., Hu Y., Qin Y., Wang X., Xiang J., Zhong N. Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: Functional MR imaging study of response in posterior cingulate cortex and adjacent precuneus during problem-solving tasks. Radiology. 2011;261:525–533. doi: 10.1148/radiol.11102186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Junquera A., García-Zamora E., Olazarán J., Parra M.A., Fernández-Guinea S. Role of Executive Functions in the Conversion from Mild Cognitive Impairment to Dementia. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. JAD. 2020;77:641–653. doi: 10.3233/JAD-200586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kramer J.H., Nelson A., Johnson J.K., Yaffe K., Glenn S., Rosen H.J., Miller B.L. Multiple cognitive deficits in amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2006;22:306–311. doi: 10.1159/000095303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Levinoff E.J., Phillips N.A., Verret L., Babins L., Kelner N., Akerib V., Chertkow H. Cognitive estimation impairment in Alzheimer disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment. Neuropsychology. 2006;20:123–132. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.1.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Petersen R.C. Mild cognitive impairment: Transition between aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurologia. 2000;15:93–101. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(00)82678-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Li H.-J., Wang P.-Y., Jiang Y., Chan R.C.K., Wang H.-L., Li J. Neurological soft signs in persons with amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment and the relationships to neuropsychological functions. Behav. Brain Funct. BBF. 2012;8:29. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-8-29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Li W., Sun L., Xiao S. Prevalence, Incidence, Influence Factors, and Cognitive Characteristics of Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment among Older Adult: A 1-Year Follow-Up Study in China. Front. Psychiatry. 2020;11:75. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Li X., Shen M., Jin Y., Jia S., Zhou Z., Han Z., Jiao J. Validity and Reliability of the New Chinese Version of the Frontal Assessment Battery-Phonemic. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2021;80:371–381. doi: 10.3233/JAD-201028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Lindbergh C.A., Puente A.N., Gray J.C., Mackillop J., Miller L.S. Delay and probability discounting as candidate markers for dementia: An initial investigation. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2014;29:651–662. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acu043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Lui V.W.C., Lam L.C.W., Chau R.C.M., Fung A.W.T., Wong B.M.L., Leung G.T.Y., Appelbaum P.S. Structured assessment of mental capacity to make financial decisions in Chinese older persons with Mild Cognitive Impairment and mild Alzheimer disease. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 2013;26:69–77. doi: 10.1177/0891988713481268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Lussier M., Adam S., Chikhaoui B., Consel C., Gagnon M., Gilbert B., Bier N. Smart Home Technology: A New Approach for Performance Measurements of Activities of Daily Living and Prediction of Mild Cognitive Impairment in Older Adults. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. JAD. 2019;68:85–96. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Metzler-Baddeley C., Jones D.K., Steventon J., Westacott L., Aggleton J.P., O’Sullivan M.J. Cingulum microstructure predicts cognitive control in older age and Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:17612–17619. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3299-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Moreira H.S., Costa A.S., Machado Á., Castro S.L., Lima C.F., Vicente S.G. Distinguishing Mild Cognitive Impairment from healthy aging and Alzheimer’s Disease: The contribution of the INECO Frontal Screening (IFS) PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0221873. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0221873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Petersen R.C., Caracciolo B., Brayne C., Gauthier S., Jelic V., Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment: A concept in evolution. J. Intern. Med. 2014;275:214–228. doi: 10.1111/joim.12190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Muñoz-Neira C., Chaparro F.H., Delgado C., Brown J., Slachevsky A. Test your memory—Spanish version (TYM-S): A validation study of a self-administered cognitive screening test. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2014;29:730–740. doi: 10.1002/gps.4055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Nishi H., Sawamoto N., Namiki C., Yoshida H., Dinh H.D.T., Ishizu K., Fukuyama H. Correlation between cognitive deficits and glucose hypometabolism in Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Neuroimaging. 2010;20:29–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2008.00328.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.McKhann G., Drachman D., Folstein M., Katzman R., Price D., Stadlan E.M. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group* under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939. doi: 10.1212/WNL.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Nordlund A., Rolstad S., Hellström P., Sjögren M., Hansen S., Wallin A. The Goteborg MCI study: Mild cognitive impairment is a heterogeneous condition. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2005;76:1485–1490. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.050385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Nordlund A., Rolstad S., Klang O., Lind K., Hansen S., Wallin A. Cognitive profiles of Mild Cognitive Impairment with and without vascular disease. Neuropsychology. 2007;21:706–712. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.6.706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Nordlund A., Rolstad S., Klang O., Lind K., Pedersen M., Blennow K., Wallin A. Episodic memory and speed/attention deficits are associated with Alzheimer-typical CSF abnormalities in MCI. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS. 2008;14:582–590. doi: 10.1017/S135561770808079X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Okonkwo O.C., Wadley V.G., Griffith H.R., Ball K., Marson D.C. Cognitive correlates of financial abilities in Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2006;54:1745–1750. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00916.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Okonkwo O.C., Griffith H.R., Belue K., Lanza S., Zamrini E.Y., Harrell L.E., Marson D.C. Cognitive models of medical decision-making capacity in patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS. 2008;14:297–308. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Pa J., Boxer A., Chao L.L., Gazzaley A., Freeman K., Kramer J., Johnson J.K. Clinical-neuroimaging characteristics of dysexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment. Ann. Neurol. 2009;65:414–423. doi: 10.1002/ana.21591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. American Psychiatric Publishing Inc.; Washington, DC, USA: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Pa J., Possin K.L., Wilson S.M., Quitania L.C., Kramer J.H., Boxer A.L., Johnson J.K. Gray matter correlates of set-shifting among neurodegenerative disease, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and healthy older adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS. 2010;16:640–650. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Papp K.V., Snyder P.J., Maruff P., Bartkowiak J., Pietrzak R.H. Detecting subtle changes in visuospatial executive function and learning in the amnestic variant of Mild Cognitive Impairment. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e21688. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Pertl M.-T., Benke T., Zamarian L., Delazer M. Decision Making and Ratio Processing in Patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. JAD. 2015;48:765–779. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Pertl M.-T., Benke T., Zamarian L., Delazer M. Effects of Healthy Aging and Mild Cognitive Impairment on a Real-Life Decision-Making Task. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. JAD. 2017;58:1077–1087. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Peters F., Villeneuve S., Belleville S. Predicting progression to dementia in elderly subjects with Mild Cognitive Impairment using both cognitive and neuroimaging predictors. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. JAD. 2014;38:307–318. doi: 10.3233/JAD-130842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Rainville C., Lepage E., Gauthier S., Kergoat M.-J., Belleville S. Executive function deficits in persons with Mild Cognitive Impairment: A study with a Tower of London task. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2012;34:306–324. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.639298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Petersen R.C. Mild cognitive impairment clinical trials. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 2003;2:646–653. doi: 10.1038/nrd1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Royall D.R., Matsuoka T., Palmer R.F., Kato Y., Taniguchi S., Ogawa M., Narumoto J. Greater than the sum of its parts: δ Improves upon a battery’s diagnostic performance. Neuropsychology. 2015;29:683–692. doi: 10.1037/neu0000153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Sánchez-Benavides G., Gómez-Ansón B., Quintana M., Vives Y., Manero R.M., Sainz A., Peña-Casanova J. Problem-solving abilities and frontal lobe cortical thickness in healthy aging and Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS. 2010;16:836–845. doi: 10.1017/S135561771000069X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Peña-Casanova J., Monllau A., Böhm P., Aguilar M., Sol J.M., Hernández G., Blesa R. Diagnostic value and test-retest reliability of the Blessed Dementia Rating Scale for Alzheimer’s disease: Data from the NORMACODEM project. Neurologia. 2005;20:349–355. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Sánchez-Benavides G., Peña-Casanova J., Casals-Coll M., Gramunt N., Molinuevo J.L., Gómez-Ansón B., Blesa R. Cognitive and neuroimaging profiles in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: Data from the Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project) J. Alzheimer’s Dis. JAD. 2014;41:887–901. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Sanders C., Low C., Schmitter-Edgecombe M. Assessment of planning abilities in individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment using an open-ended problem-solving task. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2014;36:1084–1097. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.983462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Schmitter-Edgecombe M., McAlister C., Weakley A. Naturalistic assessment of everyday functioning in individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment: The day-out task. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:631–641. doi: 10.1037/a0029352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Schmitter-Edgecombe M., Parsey C.M. Cognitive correlates of functional abilities in individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment: Comparison of questionnaire, direct observation, and performance-based measures. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2014;28:726–746. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2014.911964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Serra L., Cercignani M., Lenzi D., Perri R., Fadda L., Caltagirone C., Bozzali M. Grey and white matter changes at different stages of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. JAD. 2010;19:147–159. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-1223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Serra L., Giulietti G., Cercignani M., Spanò B., Torso M., Castelli D., Bozzali M. Mild cognitive impairment: Same identity for different entities. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. JAD. 2013;33:1157–1165. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-121663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Serrao V.T., Brucki S.M.D., Campanholo K.R., Mansur L.L., Nitrini R., Miotto E.C. Performance of a sample of patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) and healthy elderly on a lexical decision test (LDT) as a measure of pre-morbid intelligence. Dement. Neuropsychol. 2015;9:265–269. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642015dn93000009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Sheldon S., Vandermorris S., Al-Haj M., Cohen S., Winocur G., Moscovitch M. Ill-defined problem solving in amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: Linking episodic memory to effective solution generation. Neuropsychologia. 2015;68:168–175. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Sherod M.G., Griffith H.R., Copeland J., Belue K., Krzywanski S., Zamrini E.Y., Marson D.C. Neurocognitive predictors of financial capacity across the dementia spectrum: Normal aging, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS. 2009;15:258–267. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Tabert M.H., Manly J.J., Liu X., Pelton G.H., Rosenblum S., Jacobs M., Devanand D.P. Neuropsychological prediction of conversion to Alzheimer disease in patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2006;63:916–924. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Tam J.W., Schmitter-Edgecombe M. Event-based prospective memory and everyday forgetting in healthy older adults and individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2013;35:279–290. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2013.770823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Tripathi R., Kumar K., Balachandar R., Marimuthu P., Varghese M., Bharath S. Neuropsychological markers of Mild Cognitive Impairment: A clinic based study from urban India. Ann. Indian Acad. Neurol. 2015;18:177–180. doi: 10.4103/0972-2327.150566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Urbanowitsch N., Degen C., Toro P., Schröder J. Neurological soft signs in aging, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease—The impact of cognitive decline and cognitive reserve. Front. Psychiatry. 2015;6:12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Levy R. Working Party of the International Psychogeriatric Association. Aging-associated cognitive decline. Int. Psychogeriatr. 1994;6:63–68. doi: 10.1017/S1041610294001626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Weakley A., Schmitter-Edgecombe M. Naturalistic assessment of task interruption in individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment. Neuropsychology. 2019;33:1–12. doi: 10.1037/neu0000481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Wu L., Soder R.B., Schoemaker D., Carbonnell F., Sziklas V., Rowley J., Rosa-Neto P. Resting state executive control network adaptations in amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. JAD. 2014;40:993–1004. doi: 10.3233/JAD-131574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Zamarian L., Semenza C., Domahs F., Benke T., Delazer M. Alzheimer’s disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Effects of shifting and interference in simple arithmetic. J. Neurol. Sci. 2007;263:79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2007.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Zhang Y., Han B., Verhaeghen P., Nilsson L.-G. Executive functioning in older adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment: MCI has effects on planning, but not on inhibition. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2007;14:557–570. doi: 10.1080/13825580600788118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Zhang Z., Zheng H., Liang K., Wang H., Kong S., Hu J., Sun G. Functional degeneration in dorsal and ventral attention systems in amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: An fMRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 2015;585:160–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.11.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Zheng D., Dong X., Sun H., Xu Y., Ma Y., Wang X. The overall impairment of core executive function components in patients with amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: A cross-sectional study. BMC Neurol. 2012;12:138. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-12-138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Zheng D., Sun H., Dong X., Liu B., Xu Y., Chen S., Wang X. Executive dysfunction and gray matter atrophy in amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. Neurobiol. Aging. 2014;35:548–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Wechsler D. WAiS-iii. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX, USA: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Kramer J. Abstraction-is it teachable? ‘The devil is in the detail’; Proceedings of the 16th Conference on Software Engineering Education and Training; Madrid, Spain. 20–22 March 2003; Washington, DC, USA: IEEE Computer Society; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Royall D.R., Cordes J.A., Polk M. CLOX: An executive clock drawing task. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1998;64:588–594. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.64.5.588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Freedman M., Leach L., Kaplan E., Winocur G., Shulman K., Delis D.C. Clock Drawing: A Neuropsychological Analysis. Oxford University Press; New York, NY, USA: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Guarino A., Forte G., Giovannoli J., Casagrande M. Executive functions in the elderly with Mild Cognitive Impairment: A systematic review on motor and cognitive inhibition, conflict control and cognitive flexibility. Aging Ment. Health. 2020;24:1028–1045. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2019.1584785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Casagrande M., Agostini F., Favieri F., Forte G., Giovannoli J., Guarino A., Martella D. Age-related changes in hemispherical specialization for attentional networks. Brain Sci. 2021;11:1115. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11091115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Clinical Medicine are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES