Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Mar 10;17(3):e0264708. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0264708

Comparing actuarial and subjective healthy life expectancy estimates: A cross-sectional survey among the general population in Hungary

Zsombor Zrubka 1,2,*, Áron Kincses 3,4, Tamás Ferenci 1, Levente Kovács 1, László Gulácsi 1,2, Márta Péntek 1
Editor: Rashidul Alam Mahumud5
PMCID: PMC8912206  PMID: 35271611

Abstract

Background

Healthy life expectancy (HLE) is becoming an important indicator of population health. While actuarial estimates of HLE are frequently studied, there is scarcity of research on the subjective expectations of people about their HLE. The objective of this study is to compare actuarial and subjective HLE (sHLE) estimates in the ≥50-year-old Hungarian general population. Furthermore, we assessed subjective life expectancy (sLE) and explored determinants of the individual variance of sHLE and sLE.

Methods

We conducted a cross-sectional online survey in 2019. Subjective health expectations were measured at 60, 70, 80 and 90 years of age via the Global Activity Limitation Indicator (GALI). Point-estimates of sLE and background variables were also recorded. sHLE was estimated from GALI and sLE responses. Actuarial estimates of life expectancy (LE) and HLE for 2019 were provided by the Central Statistical Office of Hungary.

Results

Five hundred and four respondents (female 51.6%) were included. Mean (±SD) age was 63 (±7.5) years. Median LE (81.5 years, 95%CI 81.1–81.7) and sLE (82 years, 95%CI 80–85) were similar (p = 0.142), while median sHLE (66.8 years, 95%CI 65.5–68.3) was lower than HLE (72.7 years, 95%CI 82.4–82.9) by 5.9 years (p<0.001). Despite the greater median actuarial LE of women compared to men (p<0.001), we found no gender differences between the median sLE (p = 0.930), HLE (p = 0.417) and sHLE (p = 0.403) values. With less apparent gender differences among the predictors when compared to sLE, sHLE was mainly determined by self-perceived health, age and place of residence, while self-perceived health, close relatives’ longevity, social conditions, happiness and perceived lifestyle influenced sLE.

Conclusions

Along subjective life expectancy, subjective healthy life expectancy may be a feasible indicator and provide insights to individuals’ subjective expectations underlying the demographic estimates of the healthy life expectancy of the population.

Introduction

Since the second half of the 20th century, average life expectancy (LE) has been rising and the share of the elderly in the total population has been growing steadily. LE at birth in the most developed countries has reached 80 years. While LE at birth has increased globally by 5.5 years between 2000 and 2016, in the same period, health adjusted life expectancy (HALE) at birth has increased by 4.8 years suggesting that despite the increased healthy life span, people also live longer with disabilities [1]. Hence, healthy life expectancy (HLE) has been gaining importance and has been used for planning and evaluation of health policies [2,3]. The importance of health-related quality of life is emphasized by the World Health Organization’s (WHO) European Health 2020 policy framework [4]. Furthermore, the Lisbon Strategy of the European Union includes the target of adding on average two healthy life years across the EU by 2020—a target that still needs to be met [5].

WHO and Eurostat compute HLE via the Sullivan-method using standard life tables and cross-sectional gender- and age-specific morbidity data. For each corresponding age group in the life table, the proportion of time with disability is subtracted, and future years spent without disability are summarized [68]. While WHO computes HALE using disability estimates from the Global Burden of Disease Study [8], Eurostat’s Healthy Life Years (HLY) are based on the Global Activity Limitations Indicator (GALI) collected regularly by all European Member States in the European Union Statistics on Income and Living Conditions survey (EU-SILC) [6,9]. The GALI comprises a single question to assess long-standing activity limitations due to health problems [10,11]. Hence, the European HLY is also referred to as Disability Free Life Expectancy (DFLE) [6]. The Hungarian Central Statistical Office (HCSO) publishes HLY along with LE indicators annually [12].

While LE estimates provide information on the general status of the population, subjective life expectancy (sLE) has been studied as a proxy to gain insight into individual variances of LE [13]. The determinants of sLE include the longevity and health of forebears [1418], health status [16,1820], age [16,18,19], gender [16,18,2123], lifestyle-related risks [18,2426], socio-economic status [21,27] as well as a number of psychosocial and psychological factors, such as happiness, optimism, social relationships, depression or the sense of control [18,20,2831]. sLE has proven to be a predictor of actual life-expectancy and mortality [19,32,33], a determinant of decisions about retirement [34,35], consumption and savings [36] as well as health behaviours [37,38]. The deviation of sLE from actuarial LE has been associated with potentially important economic consequences due to altered perceptions on one’s financial prospects [14,26]. Furthermore, sLE influences the subjective value people attach to different levels of health problems [39,40].

Considering the breadth of research concerning sLE, there is scarcity of research on subjective healthy life expectancy (sHLE). However, subjective predictions about the onset and severity of disability might have also important implications for individuals’ current health-related and financial decisions as well as personal old age planning. These potential effects are of special importance if individuals’ ideas regarding future health (including both longevity and health problems) reflect an over- or underestimation compared to what can realistically be expected. Previous research on sHLE in the Netherlands [41,42] and Hungary [43,44] suggested that people underestimate their future health. However, these studies gauged health-related expectations via EQ-5D-3L, a generic health-related quality of life instrument [45,46], so their results are not directly comparable with Eurostat’s HLYs obtained via the GALI.

Therefore, our aim was to compare subjective and actuarial estimates of healthy life expectancy in the ≥50-year-old general population of Hungary, by adapting the GALI instrument to assess future health expectations. Furthermore, we assessed sLE and explored the determinants of the individual variance of sHLE and sLE.

Methods

Sample and study design

This study was performed on the ≥50-year-old subsample (study sample) of a larger cross-sectional online survey of health- and longevity-related expectations conducted among the ≥18-year-old general population of Hungary in 2019 (survey sample) [20]. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the Medical Research Council of Hungary (5113-2/2018/EKU). Participation was anonymous and respondents gave written consent prior completion of the survey. The survey sample comprised 1000 participants recruited from an online panel by a market-survey company using sampling quotas proportional to the general adult population in terms of gender, age (in the 18-65-year-old age group), educational level, type of settlement and geographical region. Those respondents were selected in the study sample, who were ≥50-year-old and provided coherent answers to questions related to future health expectations from 60 years of age. Younger individuals were excluded to avoid inflated measurement error when the onset of expected disability was ≤60 years.

Subjective and actuarial estimates of life expectancy and healthy life expectancy

We inquired sLE by asking a subjective point-estimate from each respondent [47] (Section A in S1 Appendix). Although survey answers were accepted without restriction, we included in our analysis respondents if their answers fell between their own age and the upper limit of 100 years.

The terms healthy, without limitation, or without disability will be used interchangeably throughout this paper. Future health expectations were measured with the adapted GALI [10], by asking the level of expected limitations due to health problems at the age of 60, 70, 80 and 90 years, whichever was higher than respondents’ age (Section B in S1 Appendix). GALI was also administered to describe respondent’s current health. As in HLY calculations [6], we considered any limitation as an indicator of disability. We imputed point estimates for respondents’ sHLE based on the average of possible ages without limitation, depending on their coherent response patterns of current limitations, future ages with and without expected limitations and the expected age of death. Altogether, we defined five coherent response patterns (and corresponding imputation types) based on plausible orderings of current health with or without limitations, the onset of future limitations and the age of expected death (Section C in S1 Appendix). We considered response patterns as incoherent if at any future age expected limitations were followed by expected healthy states or if subjective life expectancy was lower than current age. Incoherent respondents were excluded from the analysis. Examples for sLE imputation types and incongruent answer patterns are provided in Section D of S1 Appendix. The scatterplot of sHLE point estimates by age and imputation type are provided in the Figure of S1 Appendix. Throughout this paper the term sHLE will represent the subjective disability-free life expectancy from birth.

Abridged HLYs and actuarial life tables and for 2019 were provided by the HCSO. Both Eurostat and the HCSO calculates HLY’s from GALI results of the EU-SILC survey using the Sullivan method, the only methodological difference being that the HCSO uses the mid-year population in the calculations, while the Eurostat takes into account the beginnings of the year [48]. Yearly life expectancy and mortality data were available for both sexes up to 100 age years [49].

We estimated actuarial HLYs for each age year from abridged HLY tables available in standard format 5-year age groups up to 84 years and a single 85+ year-old age group. The estimation process is detailed in the S2 Appendix. Estimating HLYs for ages over 85 years was beyond the scope of our study. From the differences of life expectancy and healthy life expectancy curves, we computed actuarial life years with disability (LYD). From the difference of sLE and sHLE we also computed subjective life-years with disability. Throughout this paper, sLYD will denote indirect estimates of subjective life-years with disability, as this measure were not directly queried in the survey.

Sample survival curves

Subjective survival and subjective healthy survival functions were determined from sLE and sHLE via the Kaplan-Meier method.

In order to compare the subjective survival functions with actuarial survival projections for the sample, we constructed 20-year actuarial survival curves for 50–65 year-old male and female respondents via the cohort-compartment method (CCM) [50], using period life tables by si=x=5065Nxi=020(1qx+i)N, where si denotes the proportion of the sample for i∈{0,1,2,…20} years on from current age x∈{50,51,…65}, qx is the conditional probability of death at age x, Nx is the cohort sample size at age x and N is the total sample size. We obtained qx from the 2019 national life tables separately for males (S1 Table) and females (S2 Table).

We applied the following formula for the actuarial healthy survival projection for both sexes: hsi=x=5065Nxpxi=020(1hqx+i)N, where hsi denotes the proportion of the sample surviving without limitations due to health problems for i∈{0,1,2,…20} years on from current age x∈{50,51,…65}, hqx is the conditional probability of becoming limited at age x given the respondent was healthy until age x, Nx is the cohort sample size at age x, px is the proportion without limitations at age x and N is the total sample size. The parameters are displayed in S1 Table for males and S2 Table for females.

Explanatory variables

In addition to respondents’ age and gender, we inquired about the longevity of close family members. Health status was recorded by the self-perceived health item of MEHM [10,11]. We asked whether respondents were recipients of any informal or formal care due to health problems or ageing. Lifestyle risks were captured via a self-perceived item of own lifestyle relative to others’. Also, smoking habits, frequency of alcohol intake, body mass index (calculated from reported weight and height) and weekdays with at least 10 minutes of uninterrupted sport / physical activity were recorded. Socioeconomic status was described by respondents’ level of education, place of residence and net household income per capita according to the following: net monthly household income was asked in 10 equal intervals (0–1720 USD) and an open-ended top category (>1720 USD). Category midpoints were transformed to a continuous variable using the method of Parker and Fenwick for the top category [51]. Household income was divided by the number of household members without adjustment for the number of children, and grouped according to the lowest, middle and uppermost national quintiles of per capita net monthly household income [52]. Data were collected in local currency and converted to USD using the average currency exchange rate of 2019 (1 USD = 290.65 HUF) [53]. We recorded the number of adults and <18-year-old children as proxy variables for respondents’ social environment. Finally, we recorded the level of happiness on the 11-point numeric happiness scale. Higher scores indicated greater happiness [54].

Statistical analysis

Sample characteristics and key exploratory variables were summarized via descriptive methods.

The current and future health expectation patterns of respondents (Hx, Hx+10, Hx+20) by age-group and gender were tabulated. Hx represented current health, while Hx+10 and Hx+20 denoted health states in future time points with 10- and 20-year lag from the lower age-group boundary. For example, for the 60–69 years old age-group, Hx+10 denoted expected health at age 70, while Hx+20 denoted expected health at age 80. GALI categories were denoted as not limited, limited (but not severely) or severely limited. We denoted Hx+10 and Hx+20 as dead if sLE was shorter than the respective decade of Hx+10 or Hx+20. The proportion of respondents in future health states as well as the proportion transitioning between health states was compared via cross-tabulation and the Fischer’s exact test.

We applied survival methods for the analysis of sLE and sHLE point estimates. Median sLE and median sHLE was calculated and subjective survival curves of the two sexes were compared via the log-rank test. Median and mean values of subjective and actuarial values were compared via the non-parametric sign-test and one-sample t-test, respectively.

We also compared sLE, LE as well as sHLE and HLE for age years directly via explorative graphical methods. To reduce the scatter of subjective estimates around the actuarial data, we applied a local polynomial smoothing for the subjective estimates (including a 95%CI band) using an Epanechikov kernel. The differences between subjective and actuarial values were compared to 0 by age-group and gender by the one-sample t-test. Furthermore, we explored the correlation structure of sLE and sHLE via scatterplots, including the lines connecting pairs of actuarial LE / HLE data points. Estimates of sLE, sHLE and sLYD were compared via the two-sample t-tests between subgroups.

For the multivariate modelling of sHLE, instead of using Type 5 imputed values, we applied interval-censored regression, where those with zero or negative remaining healthy time were considered to be interval-censored to (-∞,0), i.e., left censored at respondents’ age (as for these subjects we did not have information when the limitation commenced), and non-censored for those where the remaining time was positive. A semiparametric proportional hazards model was used, with standard errors estimated through bootstrap [55]. sLE was modelled with multivariate Cox proportional hazards model. The model was stratified according to quantiles of age and close relatives’ life span as these violated the proportionality assumption; the stratified model was acceptable, as evidenced by the test of Grambsch and Therneau [56]. Finally, sLYD was modelled with multivariate ordinary least squares (OLS) regression using robust standard errors.

Statistical analyses were conducted by Stata 14 statistical software [57] and R version 4.0 [58] using package icenReg version 2.0.15 [59].

Results

Sample

Basic sociodemographic characteristics of the survey sample (N = 1000), the study sample (N = 504) and the Hungarian general population [49] are displayed in Table 1. In the study sample, mean (±SD) age was 63 (±7.5) years. Male respondents, the 60–69 years old age-group and respondents with secondary and tertiary education levels were over-represented compared to the ≥50-year-old general population. Over third of respondents who answered the income item (34.9%, N = 148/424) were from the highest income group (5th quintile). Mean (±SD) happiness score was 6.7 (±2.3). According to GALI, 43.7% (N = 220/504) of respondents experienced any long-standing limitation due to health problems. The distribution of key explanatory variables is provided in Table 2. Smoking (p = 0.026), excessive alcohol intake (p<0.001) and lack of exercise (p = 0.005) was more frequent among men than in women, while more women lived in single-adult households (p<0.001).

Table 1. Sample characteristics.

Study sample Population ≥50a Survey sample Population ≥18b
N % % N % %
Total 504 100 - 1000 100 100
Gender Male 244 48.4 42.5 455 45.5 47.1
Female 260 51.6 57.5 545 54.5 52.9
Age-group 18–29 - - - 101 10.1 17.2
30–39 - - - 156 15.6 16.0
40–49 - - - 201 20.1 19.6
50–59 150 29.8 32.0 165 16.5 15.1
60–69 261 51.8 34.5 275 27.5 16.3
≥70 93 18.5 33.5 102 10.2 15.8
Education Primary 152 30.2 55.0 300 30.0 45.4
Secondary 196 38.9 27.5 422 42.2 33.3
Tertiary 156 31.0 17.5 278 27.8 21.2
Place of residence Capital 113 22.4 17.3 223 22.3 18.3
City/town 268 53.2 53.2 523 52.3 52.4
Village 123 24.4 29.5 254 25.4 29.3
Region Central Hungary 171 33.9 29.2 338 33.8 31.0
Transdanubia 149 29.6 31.1 280 28.0 30.2
Great Plain and North 184 36.5 39.8 382 38.2 38.9
Income 1st quintile 74 14.7 10.6 206 20.6 15.8
2nd quintile 75 14.9 19.2 149 14.9 20.1
3rd quintile 51 10.1 27.1 94 9.4 23.9
4th quintile 76 15.1 23.7 120 12.0 21.7
5th quintile 148 29.4 19.4 258 25.8 18.5
Missing 80 15.9 - 173 17.3 -
Inclusion criteria Age ≥ 50 years 504 100 - 542 54.2 -
Consistent responder 504 100 - 914 91.4 -

a2019 Demographic Yearbook of Hungary [49], ≥50-year-old age group.

b2019 Demographic Yearbook of Hungary [49], distribution of age, gender and education are provided for the ≥18 age group, place of residence and region for the entire population of Hungary.

Table 2. Distribution of key explanatory variables.

Total Male Female Fischer
exacta
Variable Level N % N % N % p
GALIb Severely limited 29 5.8 18 7.4 11 4.2 0.340
Limited, but not severely 191 37.9 91 37.3 100 38.5
Not limited at all 284 56.3 135 55.3 149 57.3
Close relatives’ lifespan 55–74 years 148 29.4 77 31.6 71 27.3 0.524
75–85 years 222 44.0 102 41.8 120 46.2
85+ years 134 26.6 65 26.6 69 26.5
Self-perceived health Good (Very good / Good) 234 46.4 113 46.3 121 46.5 0.515
Bad (Fair / Bad / Very Bad) 270 53.6 131 53.7 139 53.5
Caregiver Has caregiver 58 11.5 27 11.1 31 11.9 0.960
No caregiver, but would need one 19 3.8 9 3.7 10 3.8
No caregiver, and do not need one 427 84.7 208 85.2 219 84.2
Self-reported lifestyle As healthy or healthier than others 423 83.9 202 82.8 221 85.0 0.289
Less healthy than others 81 16.1 42 17.2 39 15.0
Smoking Non-smoker 363 72.0 186 76.2 177 68.1 0.026
Current smoker 141 28.0 58 23.8 83 31.9
Alcohol consumption Max 3–4 days/week 440 87.3 191 78.3 249 95.8 <0.001
5+days/week 64 12.7 53 21.7 11 4.2
BMIc <30 (Not obese) 323 64.1 152 62.3 171 65.8 0.236
≥30 (Obese) 181 35.9 92 37.7 89 34.2
Physical activity Some exercise (≥ 1 day per week) 260 51.6 111 45.5 149 57.3 0.005
No exercise 244 48.4 133 54.5 111 42.7
N of adults in household Single adult 160 31.7 53 21.7 107 41.2 <0.001
Two or more adults 344 68.3 191 78.3 153 58.8
N of children in household No children 459 91.1 224 91.8 235 90.4 0.345
One or more children 45 8.9 20 8.2 25 9.6

aMale vs Female

bGALI: Global Activity Limitation Indicator; cBMI: Body Mass Index.

Future health expectations

The current self-perceived health status and subjective health expectation patterns by age-group and gender are shown in Fig 1. While the proportion of respondents without current limitation decreased significantly with age in men (p = 0.007), it was similar across age-groups in women (p = 0.941). Regarding future expectations, the proportion of healthy states (e.g., no limitations) decreased significantly with age (p<0.001) and time (p<0.001), while there was no difference between sexes (p = 0.538). However, in the 50–59 age-group, significantly more women than men expected limitations at the age of 70 (p = 0.002). While the proportion of expected deaths increased with age (p<0.001) and time (p<0.001), the difference between male and female respondents was not significant (p = 0.404). Severe limitations were expected infrequently (4.8%; 95%CI 3.5–6.3%) with increasing proportion over future time points (p = 0.026), but no significant differences between age-groups (p = 0.922) or sexes (p = 0.183). Moderate future limitations were expected by over one third of responders (36.0%, 95% CI 33.0–39.1%), with slight decrease over time (p = 0.049), a peak among the 60–69 years old age-group, and somewhat greater overall percentage in women (39.0%) than in men (32.8%) (p = 0.042).

Fig 1.

Fig 1

Current and future health expectation patterns by age group in A) men and B) women. Hx: current health status as measured by the Global Activity Limitations Indicator (GALI); H60/70/80/90: subjectively expected health status on the GALI for ages 60/70/80/90 years.

The overall percentage of respondents transitioning from no limitations to expected moderate limitations between subsequent time points was 33.2% (95% CI 29.1–37.5%), with considerable acceleration rate between time points (p<0.001) but no difference between age groups (p = 0.398) or sexes (p = 0.075). The percentage transitioning from no limitations to expected severe limitations was minimal (0.2%; 95%CI 0.0%-1.1%) with no difference between time points (p = 0.555), age groups (p>0.999) or sexes (p = 0.545). However, the percentage transitioning from no limitation to death was 15.0% (95%CI 12.1–18.4%), with marked acceleration between time points (p<0.001) and age groups (p<0.001) and no sex differences (p = 0.712). From moderate limitations to severe limitations, the transition rate was 8.2% (95%CI 5.7–11.4%), with some increase over time (p = 0.001) but with no significant difference between ages (p = 0.740) or sexes (p = 0.461). However, the percentage transitioning from moderate limitations to death was 33.5% (95%CI 28.8–38.4%), with steep increase with time (p<0.001) and age (p<0.001) and no difference between sexes (p>0.999). The transition from severe limitations to death was 51.1% (95%CI 35.8–66.3%), with no differences between groups.

Comparisons of actuarial and subjective life expectancy and healthy life expectancy estimates

Overall differences between actuarial and subjective life expectancy and healthy life expectancy estimates

Point estimates for sLE clustered at 5-year multiples, and for sHLE at 64.75 and 74.75 years. The distributions of sLE and sHLE by gender are depicted in S1 Fig. For the total sample, median sHLE (66.8 years; 95%CI 65.8–68.3) was lower than the median HLE (72.7 years; 95%CI 72.2–72.7) by 5.9 years (two-sided sign-test, p<0.001), and mean (±SD) sHLE (68.7±10.9 years) was lower by 4.0 years (two-sided paired t-test, p<0.001) compared to HLE (72.8±3.5 years). There was no significant difference (two-sided sign test, p = 0.142) between median sLE (82 years; 95%CI 80–85 years) and actuarial LE (81.5 years; 95%CI 81.1–81.7 years). However, the 1.8 years difference between mean (±SD) sLE (82.8±9.6 years) and LE (81.1±3.0 years) was significant (two-sided paired t-test, p<0.001). Median sLYD (12.25, 95%CI 10.25–14.25) was 4.3 years longer (two-sided sign-test, p<0.001), than LYD (7.9 years, 95%CI 7.9–8.0) and mean (±SD) sLYD (14.1±4.4 years) exceeded LYD (8.3±0.9 years) by 5.8 years (two-sided paired t-test, p<0.001).

Actuarial and subjective survival and healthy survival by gender

The actuarial and subjective survival and healthy survival functions for 50-65-year-olds are shown in Fig 2. For males, the actuarial survival estimate was below the lower boundary of the 95% CI range for nearly the entire 20-year period, while the subjective disability-free survival curves ran slightly above the actuarial estimate, within the 95% CI range. The differences were more nuanced for female respondents. While women overestimated both their overall and healthy survival in the forthcoming 10 years, the actuarial estimates fell within the 95% CI of the subjective survival curves between 10–20 years. The subjective survival curves of female respondents crossed the actuarial survival curves and run below the actuarial estimates on the 15 to 20-year year horizon.

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Comparison of 20-year actuarial and subjective curves of A) survival in men, B) survival in women, C) healthy survival in men and D) healthy survival in women in 50-65-year-olds. Actuarial survival curves were calculated for the sample age distribution via the cohort-compartment method, using conditional mortality data from the 2019 population life tables and estimated yearly conditional probabilities of becoming limited from the 2019 5-year-age-group healthy life expectancy (HLY) tables. Subjective survival curves were calculated from sample point estimates of subjective life expectancy (sLE) and imputed point-estimates of subjective healthy life expectancy (sHLE) from sLE point estimates and expected limitations at age 60,70 80 and 90 years measured by the adapted Global Activity Limitation Indicator (GALI) instrument.

For the entire sample, the difference between median sLE of men (82 years; 95%CI 80–85 years) and women (80 years; 95%CI 80–85 years) was not significant (log-rank test, p = 0.930), while median actuarial LE of males (79.1; 95%CI 78.7–79.5 years) and females (82.7; 95%CI 82.4–82.9 years) differed significantly (log-rank test, p<0.001). Neither median sHLE (log-rank test, p = 0.403) nor median actuarial HLE (log-rank test, p = 0.417) differed between sexes.

Actuarial vs subjective estimates by age group and gender

sHLE, sLE and sLYD values were rather similar between the two sexes, except for lower sLYD in men than in women in the 50-59-year-old age group (p = 0.002) (Fig 3). However, the differences compared to actuarial estimates showed gender differences. sLE was overestimated by men from 60 years of age, and by women over 70 years of age (Fig 3A). The mean (±SD, p) difference between sLE and LE in the 50–59, 60–69 and ≥70-year-old age groups was 2.4 (±1.4, p = 0.09), 4.3 (±0.9, p<0.001) and 2.8 (±1.1, p = 0.012) years in men and -0.6 (±1.2, p = 0.631), -0.34(±0.66, p<0.610) and 3.4 (±1.1, p = 0.002) years in women, respectively. sHLE and HLE differed in men by -1.8 (±1.6, p = 0.250), -4.1 (±0.9, p<0.001) and -3.4 (±1.2, p = 0.006) years and by -6.7 (±1.2, p<0.001), -4.4 (±0.8, p<0.001) and -2.3 (±1.2, p = 0.060) years in women (Fig 3B). sLYDs were higher than LYDs by 4.2 (±1.0, p<0.001), 8.4 (±1.0, p<0.001) and 6.2 (±1.1, p<0.001) in men and 6.1 (±1.3, p<0.001), 4.1 (±0.8, p<0.001) and 5.6 (±1.1, p<0.001) years in women in the 50–59, 60–69 and ≥70-year-old age groups, respectively (Fig 3C).

Fig 3.

Fig 3

Actuarial vs subjective A) life expectancy, B) healthy life expectancy and C) life years with disability and difference between actuarial and subjective D) life expectancy, E) healthy life expectancy and F) life years with disability relative to remaining actuarial time by age and gender. LE: life expectancy, sLE: subjective life expectancy, HLE: healthy life expectancy, sHLE: subjective healthy life expectancy, LYD: life years with disability, sLYD: subjective life years with disability, dLE: difference of actuarial and subjective life expectancy relative to actuarial remaining life expectancy, dHLE: difference of actuarial and subjective healthy life expectancy relative to actuarial remaining healthy life expectancy; dLYD: difference of actuarial and subjective life years with disability relative to actuarial remaining life years with disability.

Differences between actuarial and subjective estimates relative to actuarial remaining time

The respective differences between remaining sLE and LE, sHLE and HLE and sLYD and LYD relative to the respondents’ remaining actuarial LE, HLE and LYD are illustrated in Fig 3D (dLE), Fig 3E (dHLE) and Fig 3F (dLYD). While men overestimated remaining sLE on average by 23.3% (p<0.001), estimates of women were accurate up to 70 years of age (mean difference: -2.3%, p = 0.410). ≥70-year-old women overestimated sLE by 31.1%, (p<0.001). The overall difference between the accuracy of sLE estimates by men and women was significant (p<0.001). Both men and women underestimated remaining sHLE. Although men were more accurate than women in the 50–59 age-group (-16.9% vs -22.7%, p = 0.043), the overall difference was not significant across all age groups (-48.6% vs -47.1%, p = 0.885). Both men and women overestimated their remaining sLYDs. Despite the difference was not significant in the 50–59 (53.6% vs 54.2%, p = 0.972) and ≥70-year-old age groups (121.1% vs 80.8%, p = 0.130), the overall difference between men’s and women’s remaining sLYD estimates was large and highly significant (101.6% vs 52.2%, p<0.001).

Association between subjective life expectancy and healthy life expectancy

Corresponding sLE and sHLE values for each respondent were depicted on a scatterplot along with actuarial LE and HLE values (Fig 4). The Pearson correlation coefficient between sLE and sHLE in men and women was 0.60 and 0.48, respectively. We observed considerable individual variance in the difference between individual sLE and sHLE estimates (indirect sLYDs). According to the fitted local polynomial curves, on average, women’s sLE estimates were close to actuarial LE, if their sHLE estimates were in the actuarial range. Even those women, who expected to become disabled early, expected to live on average up to 80 years. sLE was below 80 years for those ~50-year-old women, who were disabled at the time of survey and increased proportionally for those who expected long healthy lives. sLE estimates of men were greater than actuarial LE when sHLE was estimated in the actuarial range and as women, even with early disability expectations, men expected to live up to 80 years on average. However, sLE for ~50-year-old men who were disabled at the time of survey was below 70 years. Male and female scatterplots are shown overlaid in S2 Fig.

Fig 4.

Fig 4

Healthy life expectancy vs life expectancy in A) wen and B) women.

Determinants of subjective estimates of life expectancy, healthy life expectancy and life years with disability

Although sHLE, sLE and sLYD estimates of men and women were similar, the regression analyses revealed multiple differences between the two sexes in terms of the determinants of subjective expectations (Table 3). We interpreted the hazard ratios (HR) in the survival models in terms of their effect on survival time: HR >1 suggested shorter, while HR<1 suggested longer sLE or sHLE versus baseline.

Table 3. Regression analysis of subjective healthy life expectancy, subjective life expectancy and subjective life years with disability by gender.
sHLEn sLEo sLYDp
Male Female Male Female Male Female
HRq p HR p HR p HR p Beta p Beta p
Age Age (years) 0.96* 0.013 0.97 0.068 - - - - -0.17 0.084 -0.32** 0.001
Close relatives’ lifespan a 55–74 years 1.37 0.247 1.12 0.638 - - - - -0.76 0.607 -0.80 0.631
≥85 years 0.77 0.387 0.98 0.940 - - - - 1.12 0.506 5.21** 0.004
Self-perceived health b Fair/Bad/Very Bad 3.74** <0.001 3.35** <0.001 1.42 0.095 1.63** 0.009 7.94** <0.001 5.60** 0.001
Caregiver c Has caregiver 4.49 0.126 1.28 0.618 2.41** 0.006 0.71 0.199 -1.04 0.781 7.90** 0.006
No caregiver, but needed 5.38 0.537 0.54 0.441 2.86* 0.034 1.53 0.364 2.14 0.551 5.42* 0.047
Self-perceived lifestyle d Less healthy than others 2.11 0.051 1.63 0.175 1.43 0.156 2.08** 0.005 0.42 0.829 -2.59 0.160
Smoking status e Current smoker 1.20 0.572 1.18 0.424 1.07 0.737 1.20 0.329 -0.19 0.917 -0.25 0.870
BMI f ≥30 (Obese) 0.87 0.581 0.91 0.677 0.56** 0.003 1.19 0.316 3.71** 0.009 -1.11 0.428
Physical activity g No exercise 0.88 0.571 0.74 0.186 0.98 0.912 0.84 0.326 -1.93 0.138 -0.76 0.582
Alcohol intake h ≥5 days/week 0.82 0.470 0.43 0.262 0.83 0.397 1.28 0.599 -1.15 0.478 -5.50 0.081
Education i Primary 0.74 0.353 0.88 0.628 1.14 0.520 0.92 0.696 -1.55 0.329 -0.35 0.833
Tertiary 0.77 0.373 0.99 0.970 0.79 0.310 0.80 0.258 0.48 0.775 2.63 0.127
Place of residence j City/Town 2.06* 0.039 1.14 0.558 1.26 0.307 1.12 0.570 1.60 0.398 0.87 0.567
Village 2.10 0.060 2.05** 0.009 1.00 0.996 0.81 0.370 3.86 0.066 2.98 0.135
Income k Q1 0.85 0.661 0.90 0.710 1.48 0.110 1.03 0.913 -2.72 0.166 -0.16 0.937
Q5 1.21 0.450 0.77 0.236 1.22 0.328 0.89 0.557 -1.58 0.328 -2.37 0.128
Household One or more adultsl 1.40 0.233 1.38 0.138 1.30 0.244 1.20 0.301 -0.16 0.931 0.62 0.702
One or more childrenm 2.48 0.069 0.97 0.925 0.52 0.050 1.00 0.991 6.25* 0.023 1.09 0.594
Happiness Happiness (0–10) 0.97 0.557 0.91 0.071 0.91* 0.032 0.88** 0.004 0.33 0.297 0.15 0.649
Intercept - - - - - - - - 15.50 0.020 23.86 0.002
N 204 220 204 220 204 220

* p>0.05

**p<0.01.

abase:75–84 years

bbase: Very good/Good

cbase: No, and not needed

dbase: Healthier or as healthy as others

ebase: Never smoked or quitted

fbase: <30

gbase: Some exercise

hbase: Lifetime abstinence to max 4 occasions/week

ibase: Secondary

jbase: Capital

kbase: Q2-Q4

lbase: Single adult

mbase: None

nsHLE: subjective healthy life expectancy

osLE: subjective life expectancy

psLYD: subjective life years with disability

qHR: hazard ratio.

sHLE was mainly determined by self-perceived health in both men and women. Place of residence other than the capital was also associated with shorter sHLE estimates in both men and women, although with differences between the coefficients of cities/towns and villages. While older age was associated with longer sHLE in men, the effect of age was not significant in women.

Wald tests comparing the lowest and highest categories suggested that longer lifespan of close relatives was associated with longer sLE in both men (p = 0.009) and women (p<0.001). In order to meet the proportional hazards assumption, the final sLE model was stratified by age groups and close relatives’ lifespan, so these variables were not included as predictors in the final sLE model. While greater happiness was associated with longer sLE in both men and women, the effect of health status and lifestyle was different between the two sexes. Low self-perceived health was associated with shorter sLE in women, but the association was not significant in men. However, having or needing a caregiver decreased significantly men’s but not women’s sLE. Less healthy self-perceived lifestyle compared to others was associated with significantly shorter sLE only in women, while obesity was associated with longer sLE in men.

Although low self-perceived health was a predictor of longer sLYD estimates in both men and women, other predictors were different between the two sexes. While higher age decreased the sLYD expectations of women, its effect was not significant in men. Long lifespan of close relatives and having or needed a caregiver were associated with longer sLYD estimates in women but on in men. On the other hand, in men, obesity was associated with longer sLYD estimates as well as living in a household with children.

Usual determinants of morbidity and mortality, such as education, income or lifestyle related factors, such as smoking, excessive drinking or lack of exercise were not associated with either sHLE, sLE or sLYD estimates. The adjusted survival curves illustrate the effect of age (Fig 5) and key predictor variables (Fig 6) on sLE and sHLE in both sexes.

Fig 5. Adjusted survival curves illustrating the effect of age on subjective life expectancy (sLE) and healthy life expectancy (sHLE) by gender sLE: Subjective life expectancy, sHLE: Subjective healthy life expectancy.

Fig 5

Fig 6. Adjusted survival curves illustrating the effect of age key predictors on life expectancy (sLE) and healthy life expectancy (sHLE) by gender sLE: Subjective life expectancy, sHLE: Subjective healthy life expectancy.

Fig 6

Discussion

We explored subjective health expectations of the ≥50-year-old online general population via a cross-sectional online survey. According to our best knowledge, this is the first study that explores sHLE using the adapted GALI instrument, and thereby, provides comparable sHLE estimates to HLYs reported by Eurostat [6]. We estimated sHLE from self-reported current long-standing limitations due to health problems, expected limitations at ages 60, 70, 80 and 90 as well as point-estimates for subjective life-expectancy. While estimates of sLE and LE were rather similar, median sHLE was 5.9 years shorter than median HLE. Despite the greater median actuarial LE of women compared to men, we found no gender differences between the median sLE, HLE and sHLE values. sHLE was mainly determined by self-perceived health status and place of residence. The predictors of sHLE and sLE were different, and gender differences in determinants of sLE were more apparent compared to those of sHLE.

In the 50–59 age group, men’s sHLE estimates were closer to actuarial HLE than women’s, while men and women underestimated sHLE to a similar extent from age 60. Previous studies of subjective health expectations based on the EQ-5D-3L instrument in the Netherlands also demonstrated that people underestimate future health beyond 70 years of age [41].

Similarly to findings in the US, men overestimated their sLE [22]. In our study sLYD was also overestimated by men. Women estimated accurately sLE up to age 70, while overestimated sLYD across all ages and sLE in the ≥70-year-old age group. Altogether, men from 60 and women from 70 years of age expected to succumb earlier to disability, but withstand it longer compared to their actuarial estimates. The subjective LE and LYD estimates of women were closer to actuarial estimates than those of men.

The individual variability of sHLE and sLE estimates was great among respondents. Among the well-established determinants of sLE such as the longevity of forebears [1418] self-perceived health status [16,1820], age [16,18,19], gender [16,18,2123], lifestyle-related risks [18,2426], socio-economic status [21,27] and psychosocial factors [18,20,2831], we found association of sHLE with self-perceived health, age while self-perceived health, close relatives’ longevity, social conditions, happiness and perceived lifestyle influenced sLE.

In our sample, self-perceived health was the strongest predictor of sHLE, which is an important similarity with previous studies. In the Dutch population age, gender, current health status, perceived lifestyle and close relatives’ lifespan were significant determinants of future health [41,42]. Furthermore, in addition to age, current health status, perceived lifestyle and close relatives’ lifespan, education, employment and income were significant predictors of future health (as measured with the EQ-5D-3L) in the Hungarian general population indicating broad similarities between countries with different health status, health systems and economic level [60]. In studies with chronic patients, treatment status and informal care also influenced future health expectations [43,44]. Living in country towns or villages was an indicator of shorter sHLE in our sample, while place of residence was not considered in previous studies [41,42,60]. The association of sHLE and the place of residence deserves further investigation in the light of the considerable territorial health inequalities of Hungary. Compared to the affluent districts of the capital, life expectancy at birth may be up to 11 years lower in rural regions [61].

Important determinants of health, such as education, income, and lifestyle-related factors (smoking, physical activity, alcohol intake and obesity) did not affect sHLE in our sample, while in the Dutch population shorter future health expectations were associated with the presence of objective lifestyle risks, such as smoking, lack of exercise or unhealthy diet [41,42]. Our results were indicative of the population’s ignorance about the negative consequences of unhealthy lifestyles, which may contribute to the unfavourable ranking of Hungary in Europe in terms of lifestyle risks and potentially preventable mortality [62]. The association of obesity with increased life expectancy in men is particularly alarming, and the association of sLE in women with self-perceived lifestyle but not with established risk factors may indicate the presence of health-related misbeliefs in the general population. These findings support the need for reinforced health education and health promotion activities in Hungary [63].

Our study revealed gender differences in future expectations about health, disability and longevity. In case of sHLE, the gender differences in terms of age and place of residence were subtle. Yet, we observed considerable differences between men and women in terms of how the social environment influenced sLE. Altogether, higher age was associated with shorter sHLE in men, but with decreased of sLYD in women. Becoming dependent on a caregiver was associated with the expectation of earlier death (shorter sLE) in men, but with extended survival of disability (longer sLYD) in women. Those men, who lived in a household with children, expected to live longer with disabilities, resembling the pattern of female respondents. Self-perceived health was associated with sLE in women but not in men. Also, the longevity of close relatives was associated with sLYD in women, but not in men. Other studies also revealed gender differences in subjective health expectations [4143,60]. Further studies of the gender-related differences in health expectations may improve our understanding about gender differences in health behaviours [64], or retirement and financial planning [65].

It has to be noted, that methodological differences may hamper direct comparison of our results with previous studies of subjective health expectations. Since GALI measures limitations due to health problems, while EQ-5D-3L is a measure of health-related quality of life, the two instruments may reflect different aspects of future health expectations, which is worthwhile to explore in greater depth. We are aware of a single study that measured subjective health expectations in terms of limitations on a 10-year horizon. The sample comprised the 40-55-year-old population [66]. Our study was conducted in the ≥50-year-old population, while other studies of future health also involved younger respondents from 18 years of age [41,42,60]. Due to the selection bias of survivors and the differences in timeframe for which the estimation is performed (i.e., regardless of age, all respondents estimated their health status for the same fixed ages), sHLE estimates can be particularly sensitive to the age distribution of the sample.

In our study, sHLE estimates were based on expectations of any future limitations. However, 4.8% of respondents expected severe limitations at some time-point. In the 2019 EU-SILC survey 17.3% of the 65+ years old Hungarian population reported severe limitations, while the range of country reports spanned between 6.5% and 25.9% [67]. While the difference may reflect the general sampling bias of online surveys [68], expectations about severe health states and death may indicate the health preferences of the general population [69] as well as how health systems can cope with the growing burden of serious health-related suffering [70,71].

The strength of our study is that our GALI-based sHLE estimates are comparable with Eurostat’s HLY or DFLE estimates. Furthermore, the sample size allowed the inclusion of a wide range of predictor variables in separate models for men and women, thereby allowing the explore the differences between the two sexes in great detail.

However, some limitations have to be mentioned. While sLE was based on point-estimates, and yearly mortality data were available for comparison with actuarial LE values, sHLE was estimated from expected GALI responses at four time points, current GALI and sLE values involving a number of data imputation steps. The start date of existing limitations was not available. Furthermore, yearly actuarial HLE values were estimated from 5-year HLY tables. Although we employed reasonable data transformations, some of our assumptions, such as the applying the same imputation rule for the start-date of existing limitations across all ages may have influenced results. However, by applying interval regression during the survival analysis of left-censored sHLE data, this effect was somewhat mitigated. Furthermore, we included the ≥50-year-old online population in our study and limited the scope of some analyses to 20-years. Therefore, subjective healthy life expectations of younger generations, and of those who are outside the reach of online surveys have remained unexplored.

Conclusions

In order to maintain the sustainability of healthcare and social systems under the pressure of growing expenditures and aging populations in Europe, is increasingly important to gain deeper understanding of people’s underlying subjective perceptions and expectations about their future health and longevity beyond the dynamics of objective demographic indicators.

We demonstrated that in addition to the already established sLE, sHLE may be a feasible indicator of individual health expectations. The study of sHLE may open new avenues for interdisciplinary collaboration between demographers and scientists from the field of public health, health psychology or health economics. According to our results, in the ≥50-year-old population, median subjective healthy life span was lower by 5.9 years compared to actuarial HLE estimates. The similar sHLE estimates of men and women did not reflect the gender differences in LE. sHLE and sLE were determined by different factors.

In Hungary, the association between geographical health inequalities, regional differences in sHLE and their cultural and structural determinants deserve more exploration. Furthermore, the lack of association between sHLE and objective lifestyle-related risk factors, such as smoking status, physical exercise, obesity or alcohol intake were an alarming finding, which may partly be related to the high prevalence of lifestyle related risks among the Hungarian general population. The determinants of subjective health expectancy and its association with actual health behaviours as well as health-related or economic decisions warrants further investigation.

Supporting information

S1 Fig

Distribution of A) subjective life expectancy (sLE) in men, B) sLE in women, C) subjective healthy life expectancy (sHLE) in men and D) sHLE in women.

(PDF)

S2 Fig. Subjective life expectancy (sLE) versus subjective healthy life expectancy (sHLE) in men and women.

(PDF)

S1 Table. Actuarial healthy life expectancy estimates for males by age years.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Actuarial healthy life expectancy estimates for males by age years.

(DOCX)

S1 Appendix. Details of survey items on subjective expectations and applied data transformations.

(PDF)

S2 Appendix. Estimating actuarial healthy life years (HLY) for each age year from abridged HLY tables.

(PDF)

S1 File

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. László Tóthfalusi and Dr. Miklós Farkas for their useful comments.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

Data collection was supported by the Higher Education Institutional Excellence Program of the Ministry of Human Capacities in the framework of the 'Financial and Public Services' research project (20764-3/2018/FEKUTSTRAT) at Corvinus University of Budapest. The conduct of this study was supported by the Higher Education Institutional Excellence Program of the Ministry for Innovation and Technology in the framework of the “Thematic Excellence Program” research project (TKP2020-NKA-02) at the Corvinus University of Budapest. Financial interests: In connection with writing this article, ZZ, ÁK, TF, LK, MP and LG received grant support from the Higher Education Institutional Excellence Program of the Ministry for Innovation and Technology “Thematic Excellence Program” (TKP2020-NKA-02) at the Corvinus University of Budapest. Non-financial interests: MP is member of the EuroQol Group, a not-for-profit organisation that develops and distributes instruments that assess and value health.

References

  • 1.The Global Health Observatory [Internet]. World Health Organisation. 2020 [cited Sept 22, 2020]. Available from: https://apps.who.int/gho/data/view.main.HALEXREGv?lang=en.
  • 2.Public Health Evaluation and Impact Assessment Consortium (PHEIAC). Evaluation of the Use and Impact of the European Community Health Indicators Final Report. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union, 2014.
  • 3.Stiefel MC, Perla RJ, Zell BL. A healthy bottom line: healthy life expectancy as an outcome measure for health improvement efforts. Milbank Q. 2010;88(1):30–53. Epub 2010/04/10. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0009.2010.00588.x ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2888015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.WHO. Health 2020 A European policy framework and strategy for the 21st century. Copenhagen, Denmark: WHO Regional Office for Europe, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.EPHA. Joint statement Europe, let’s do more for health! Brussels, Belgium: European Public Health Alliance, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Eurostat. Glossary: Healthy life years (HLY) 2015.
  • 7.Sullivan DF. A single index of mortality and morbidity. HSMHA Health Rep. 1971;86(4):347–54. . [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.WHO. WHO methods for life expectancy and healthy life expectancy. Geneva: Department of Health Statistics and Information Systems WHO, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Eurostat. European Union Statistics on Income and Living Conditions (EU-SILC) Luxembourg: Eurostat; 2020. [Sept 22, 2020]. Available from: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/web/microdata/european-union-statistics-on-income-and-living-conditions. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Eurostat. Glossary:Minimum European Health Module (MEHM) 2019 [24/02/2019]. Available from: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php/Glossary:Minimum_European_Health_Module_(MEHM).
  • 11.Cox B, van Oyen H, Cambois E, Jagger C, le Roy S, Robine JM, et al. The reliability of the Minimum European Health Module. Int J Public Health. 2009;54(2):55–60. doi: 10.1007/s00038-009-7104-y . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.KSH. Egészségi állapot (2005–2019) Budapest: Központi Statisztikai Hivatal; 2018. [October 24th, 2020]. Available from: https://www.ksh.hu/thm/2/indi2_8_1.html. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Perozek M. Using subjective expectations to forecast longevity: do survey respondents know something we don’t know? Demography. 2008;45(1):95–113. Epub 2008/04/09. doi: 10.1353/dem.2008.0010 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2831383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hamermesh DS. Expectations, Life Expectancy, and Economic Behavior. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 1985;100(2). doi: 10.2307/1885388 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Zick CD, Smith KR, Mayer RN, Taylor LB. Family, frailty, and fatal futures? Own-health and family-health predictors of subjective life expectancy. Res Aging. 2014;36(2):244–66. Epub 2015/02/05. doi: 10.1177/0164027513482948 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Donnelly R, Umberson D, Pudrovska T. Family Member Death and Subjective Life Expectancy Among Black and White Older Adults. J Aging Health. 2020;32(3–4):143–53. Epub 2018/11/20. doi: 10.1177/0898264318809798 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6525659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Robbins RA. Subjective life expectancy as a correlate of family life expectancy. Psychol Rep. 1988;62(2):442. Epub 1988/04/01. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1988.62.2.442 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Griffin B, Loh V, Hesketh B. A mental model of factors associated with subjective life expectancy. Soc Sci Med. 2013;82:79–86. Epub 2013/03/05. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.01.026 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.van Solinge H, Henkens K. Subjective life expectancy and actual mortality: results of a 10-year panel study among older workers. Eur J Ageing. 2018;15(2):155–64. Epub 2018/06/06. doi: 10.1007/s10433-017-0442-3 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5971026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Pentek M, Hajdu O, Rencz F, Beretzky Z, Brodszky V, Baji P, et al. Subjective expectations regarding ageing: a cross-sectional online population survey in Hungary. Eur J Health Econ. 2019;20(Suppl 1):17–30. doi: 10.1007/s10198-019-01059-w ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6544751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Choi JW, Kim JH, Yoo KB. Subjective expectations for future and mortality among middle-aged and older adults. Medicine (Baltimore). 2020;99(17):e19421. Epub 2020/04/26. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000019421 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7220670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mirowsky J. Subjective life expectancy in the US: correspondence to actuarial estimates by age, sex and race. Social Science & Medicine. 1999;49(7):967–79. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00193-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Philipov D, Scherbov S. Subjective length of life of European individuals at older ages: Temporal and gender distinctions. PLoS One. 2020;15(3):e0229975. Epub 2020/03/18. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0229975 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7077847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dieteren CM, Brouwer WBF, van Exel J. How do combinations of unhealthy behaviors relate to attitudinal factors and subjective health among the adult population in the Netherlands? BMC Public Health. 2020;20(1):441. Epub 2020/04/05. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-8429-y ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7126128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Falba TA, Busch SH. Survival expectations of the obese: Is excess mortality reflected in perceptions? Obes Res. 2005;13(4):754–61. Epub 2005/05/18. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.85 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Knoll MAZ, Shoffner D, O’Leary S. The Potential Effects of Obesity on Social Security Claiming Behavior and Retirement Benefits. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2018;73(4):723–32. Epub 2016/04/06. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw016 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Popham F, Mitchell R. Self-rated life expectancy and lifetime socio-economic position: cross-sectional analysis of the British household panel survey. Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36(1):58–65. Epub 2006/11/10. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl241 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Mirowsky J. Age, subjective life expectancy, and the sense of control: the horizon hypothesis. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1997;52(3):S125–34. Epub 1997/05/01. doi: 10.1093/geronb/52b.3.s125 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ross CE, Mirowsky J. Family relationships, social support and subjective life expectancy. J Health Soc Behav. 2002;43(4):469–89. Epub 2003/04/01. doi: 10.2307/3090238 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Joubert CE. Happiness, time consciousness, and subjective life expectancy. Percept Mot Skills. 1992;74(2):649–50. Epub 1992/04/01. doi: 10.2466/pms.1992.74.2.649 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bodner E, Bergman YS. Loneliness and depressive symptoms among older adults: The moderating role of subjective life expectancy. Psychiatry Res. 2016;237:78–82. Epub 2016/02/28. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.074 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Siegel M, Bradley EH, Kasl SV. Self-rated life expectancy as a predictor of mortality: evidence from the HRS and AHEAD surveys. Gerontology. 2003;49(4):265–71. Epub 2003/06/07. doi: 10.1159/000070409 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kim JH, Kim JM. Subjective life expectancy is a risk factor for perceived health status and mortality. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2017;15(1):190. Epub 2017/10/04. doi: 10.1186/s12955-017-0763-0 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5625600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.van Solinge H, Henkens K. [Living longer, working longer? The impact of subjective life expectancy on retirement intentions and behavior]. Tijdschr Gerontol Geriatr. 2010;41(5):204–13. Epub 2010/12/01. doi: 10.1007/BF03096212 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Vanajan A, Bultmann U, Henkens K. Why do older workers with chronic health conditions prefer to retire early? Age Ageing. 2020;49(3):403–10. Epub 2020/02/11. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afz180 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7187868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Salm M. Subjective mortality expectations and consumption and saving behaviours among the elderly. Canadian Journal of Economics/Revue canadienne d’économique. 2010;43(3):1040–57. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5982.2010.01605.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ziegelmann JP, Lippke S, Schwarzer R. Subjective residual life expectancy in health self-regulation. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2006;61(4):P195–201. Epub 2006/07/21. doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.4.p195 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Balia S. Survival expectations, subjective health and smoking: evidence from SHARE. Empirical Economics. 2014;47(2):753–80. doi: 10.1007/s00181-013-0750-1 WOS:000339971300016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Heintz E, Krol M, Levin LA. The impact of patients’ subjective life expectancy on time tradeoff valuations. Med Decis Making. 2013;33(2):261–70. Epub 2012/12/28. doi: 10.1177/0272989X12465673 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.van Nooten FE, Koolman X, Brouwer WB. The influence of subjective life expectancy on health state valuations using a 10 year TTO. Health Econ. 2009;18(5):549–58. Epub 2008/08/15. doi: 10.1002/hec.1385 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Brouwer WB, van Exel NJ. Expectations regarding length and health related quality of life: some empirical findings. Soc Sci Med. 2005;61(5):1083–94. Epub 2005/06/16. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.01.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Rappange DR, Brouwer WB, van Exel J. A long life in good health: subjective expectations regarding length and future health-related quality of life. Eur J Health Econ. 2016;17(5):577–89. Epub 2015/06/17. doi: 10.1007/s10198-015-0701-1 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4867149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Pentek M, Gulacsi L, Rojkovich B, Brodszky V, van Exel J, Brouwer WB. Subjective health expectations at biological therapy initiation: a survey of rheumatoid arthritis patients and rheumatologists. Eur J Health Econ. 2014;15 Suppl 1:S83–92. Epub 2014/05/17. doi: 10.1007/s10198-014-0597-1 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Pentek M, Brodszky V, Biro Z, Kolkedi Z, Dunai A, Nemeth J, et al. Subjective health expectations of patients with age-related macular degeneration treated with antiVEGF drugs. BMC Geriatr. 2017;17(1):233. Epub 2017/10/12. doi: 10.1186/s12877-017-0619-9 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5635533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.EuroQolGroup. EuroQol—a new facility for the measurement of health-related quality of life. Health Policy. 1990;16(3):199–208. doi: 10.1016/0168-8510(90)90421-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.EuroQol Research Foundation. EQ-5D-3L User Guide. Rotterdam: EuroQol Research Foundation, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Rappange DR, van Exel J, Brouwer WB. A short note on measuring subjective life expectancy: survival probabilities versus point estimates. Eur J Health Econ. 2017;18(1):7–12. Epub 2016/01/11. doi: 10.1007/s10198-015-0754-1 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5209395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Faragó M. Egészségesen várható élettartamok Magyarországon, 2011. Statisztikai Szemle. 2011;93(7):633–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.KSH. Demographic Yearbook, 2019. Budapest: KSH; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Alho J, Spencer B. Statistical Demography and Forecasting. New York: Springer Science + Business Media, Inc. 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Parker RN, Fenwick R. The Pareto Curve and Its Utility for Open-Ended Income Distributions in Survey Research. Social Forces. 1983;61(3). doi: 10.2307/2578140 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Office HCS. [A háztartások életszínvonala, 2017] 2018 [cited 2019 2 April]. Available from: https://www.ksh.hu/docs/hun/xftp/idoszaki/hazteletszinv/hazteletszinv17.pdf.
  • 53.Central Bank of Hungary. Annual exchange rates online: Central Bank of Hungary, [Sept 25, 2020]. Available from: https://www.mnb.hu/en/arfolyam-lekerdezes.
  • 54.Happiness in Nations: Overview of happiness surveys using Measure type: 112G / 11-step numeral Happiness [Internet]. Erasmus University Rotterdam, Happiness Economics Research Organisation. 1993 [cited 2019.03.20.]. Available from: http://worlddatabaseofhappiness.eur.nl.
  • 55.Pan W. Extending the Iterative Convex Minorant Algorithm to the Cox Model for Interval-Censored Data. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics. 1999;8(1):109–20. doi: 10.1080/10618600.1999.10474804 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Grambsch PM, Therneau TM. Proportional Hazards Tests and Diagnostics Based on Weighted Residuals. Biometrika. 1994;81(3). doi: 10.1093/biomet/81.3.515 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Anderson-Bergman C. icenReg: Regression Models for Interval Censored Data in R. Journal of Statistical Software. 2017;81(12):1–23. doi: 10.18637/jss.v081.i12 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Pentek M, Brodszky V, Gulacsi AL, Hajdu O, van Exel J, Brouwer W, et al. Subjective expectations regarding length and health-related quality of life in Hungary: results from an empirical investigation. Health Expect. 2014;17(5):696–709. Epub 2012/06/29. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-7625.2012.00797.x ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5060917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Pál V. Egészségünk földrajza—területi különbséges a hazai egészségi állapotban. Magyar Tudomány. 2017;178(2017/3):311–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.OECD/European Observatory on Health Systems and Policies. Hungary: Country Health Profile 2017. Paris / Brussels: OECD Publishing / European Observatory on Health Systems and Policies; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.OECD/European Observatory on Health Systems and Policies. State of Health in the EU. Paris / Brussels: OECD Publishing / European Observatory on Health Systems and Policies; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hiller J, Schatz K, Drexler H. Gender influence on health and risk behavior in primary prevention: a systematic review. Z Gesundh Wiss. 2017;25(4):339–49. Epub 2017/01/01. doi: 10.1007/s10389-017-0798-z ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7088168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Sundén AE, Surette BJ. Gender Differences in the Allocation of Assets in Retirement Savings Plans. The American Economic Review. 1998;88(2):207–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Huynh KP, Jung J. Subjective health expectations. Journal of Policy Modeling. 2015;37(4):693–711. doi: 10.1016/j.jpolmod.2015.04.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Eurostat. Self-perceived long-standing limitations in usual activities due to health problem by sex, age and income quintile Online: Eurostat; 2020 [October 24, 2020]. Available from: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/databrowser/view/hlth_silc_12/default/table?lang=en.
  • 68.Eysenbach G, Wyatt J. Using the Internet for surveys and health research. J Med Internet Res. 2002;4(2):E13. Epub 2003/01/30. doi: 10.2196/jmir.4.2.e13 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1761932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Richardson JR, McKie J, Peacock SJ, Iezzi A. Severity as an independent determinant of the social value of a health service. Eur J Health Econ. 2011;12(2):163–74. Epub 2010/05/11. doi: 10.1007/s10198-010-0249-z . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Clark D, Baur N, Clelland D, Garralda E, Lopez-Fidalgo J, Connor S, et al. Mapping Levels of Palliative Care Development in 198 Countries: The Situation in 2017. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2020;59(4):794–807 e4. Epub 2019/11/25. doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2019.11.009 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7105817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Sleeman KE, de Brito M, Etkind S, Nkhoma K, Guo P, Higginson IJ, et al. The escalating global burden of serious health-related suffering: projections to 2060 by world regions, age groups, and health conditions. The Lancet Global Health. 2019;7(7):e883–e92. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(19)30172-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Rashidul Alam Mahumud

6 Dec 2021

PONE-D-21-03658Comparing Actuarial and Subjective Healthy Life Expectancy Estimates: a Cross-Sectional Survey Among the General Population in HungaryPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Zrubka,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 20 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Rashidul Alam Mahumud, MPH, MSc, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please upload a copy of Figure 5 & 6, to which you refer in your text on page 26 & 27. If the figure is no longer to be included as part of the submission please remove all reference to it within the text.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Overall

Interesting paper and authors did hard work on it. However, it needs extensive revision to make publishable. It looks like directly copied from the report and too long which need entire revision.

The general comments are as follows:

Abstract

Methods line 29; GALI? write full form that appears first time.

Results line 32: 504? Replace it with the word.

Introduction

Page 3: line 56-57 (last sentence); looks like unnecessary.

Page 4: line 59; What are multiple methods? It might be better to introduce the paragraph with multiple methods and start describing them.

Page 4: line 68-83; this looks to long description, please merge the important things in a single sentence.

Page 5-6: Line 108-104; not necessary such details you can finish in a single sentence

Methods

Page 6: line 121; first sentence not needed.

Page 6: line 122 and 126; you already mentioned >50 in the objective; Why you collect 18-65 years? Make it clear.

Page 6: Please write sample and design section clearly and sequentially by referring one published article from the journal. It looks like mixed everything here.

Page 7: line 139-143; I suggest not to write the whole questions and answer here. (E.g. The opinion question was formulated …… and response was generated ……..)

Page 7-10: Line 132-201; I don’t think we need this much long explanation here.

Page 11-12: Line 221-247; explanatory variables section; please rewrite this no need to describe each questions in that details.

Page 20: line 376; Full form of GALI is already mentioned in the previous section so write short form GALI only.

Page 32: line 612: Can you make clear it clear?

Page 32: line 614: What is the reason behind ‘need of more explanation’?

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Mar 10;17(3):e0264708. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0264708.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


22 Jan 2022

Response to Reviewers

Title: Comparing Actuarial and Subjective Healthy Life Expectancy Estimates: a Cross-Sectional Survey Among the General Population in Hungary

Manuscript ID: PONE-D-21-03658

Dear Editors, Dear Reviewer,

Thank you very much for reviewing our manuscript. We supplied the missing figures, updated supplementary files and revised file-naming conventions as per the journal authors’ guide. Our responses to reviewers’ comments are detailed below.

Jan 20th, 2022

The Authors

Reviewer #1: Overall

Interesting paper and authors did hard work on it. However, it needs extensive revision to make publishable. It looks like directly copied from the report and too long which need entire revision.

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestions about making our manuscript more accessible by readers. We have abbreviated and simplified the entire paper and moved several technical details to supplementary files.

The general comments are as follows:

Abstract

Methods line 29; GALI? write full form that appears first time.

[Authors] Thank you for pointing out, we introduced the acronym in the abstract (see Revised Manuscript line 12)

Results line 32: 504? Replace it with the word.

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestion, we replaced to „five hundred and five”. (see Revised Manuscript line 16)

Introduction

Page 3: line 56-57 (last sentence); looks like unnecessary.

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestion, we shortened the sentence. (see Revised Manuscript lines 37-40)

Page 4: line 59; What are multiple methods? It might be better to introduce the paragraph with multiple methods and start describing them.

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestion, we re-edited and shortened this section. (see Revised Manuscript line 41)

Page 4: line 68-83; this looks to long description, please merge the important things in a single sentence.

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestion, we re-edited and shortened this section. (see Revised Manuscript lines 41-51)

Page 5-6: Line 108-104; not necessary such details you can finish in a single sentence

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestion, we re-edited and shortened this section. (see Revised Manuscript lines 70-73)

Methods

Page 6: line 121; first sentence not needed.

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestion, we re-edited and clarified this section. (see Revised Manuscript line 80)

Page 6: line 122 and 126; you already mentioned >50 in the objective; Why you collect 18-65 years? Make it clear.

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestion, we re-edited and clarified this section. The analysis was conducted on a sub-sample of a larger survey on health- and longevity-related expectations. While subjective life expectancy was inquired on the 18–65-year-old sample via a single point estimate (expected age of death), healthy life expectancy was computed from disability expectations for 60,70,80 and 90 years of age. To limit the inflation of measurement error for those respondents, who expected the onset of disability before age 60, we limited the study of healthy life expectancy to individuals who were 50+ years old. (e.g., healthy at age 18 vs age 50, if both expect disability by age 60). We added the following sentence:

„Those respondents were selected in the study sample, who were ≥50-year-old and provided coherent answers to questions related to future health expectations from 60 years of age. Younger individuals were excluded to avoid inflated measurement error when the onset of expected disability was �60 years.”

(see Revised Manuscript lines 88-91)

Page 6: Please write sample and design section clearly and sequentially by referring one published article from the journal. It looks like mixed everything here.

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestion, we re-edited this section. We hope that the sequence of reported information provides more clarity about our survey. (see Revised Manuscript lines 80-91)

Page 7: line 139-143; I suggest not to write the whole questions and answer here. (E.g. The opinion question was formulated …… and response was generated ……..)

Page 7-10: Line 132-201; I don’t think we need this much long explanation here.

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestion. We abbreviated the methods section and moved most of the technical details to supplementary files. (see Revised Manuscript lines 94-129)

Page 11-12: Line 221-247; explanatory variables section; please rewrite this no need to describe each questions in that details.

[Authors] Thank you for the suggestion. We abbreviated this section. (see Revised Manuscript lines 148-166)

Page 20: line 376; Full form of GALI is already mentioned in the previous section so write short form GALI only.

[Authors] Thank you for noting. We suggest keeping this caption text to Fig. 2 unchanged, so readers can interpret all figures without looking up information elsewhere in the manuscript. (see Revised Manuscript lines 287-296)

Page 32: line 612: Can you make clear it clear?

[Authors] Thank you for noting. We corrected the sentence as follows: „sHLE and sLE were determined by different factors.” (see Revised Manuscript line 532)

Page 32: line 614: What is the reason behind ‘need of more explanation’?

[Authors] Thank you for the question. There are considerable geographical inequalities of health in Hungary. One may hypothesize that low health expectations are determined culturally and contribute to the unhealthy lifestyles and overall inferior health of rural populations. However, low health expectations may as well be determined by structural factors, such as inferior health infrastructure and low access to healthcare, unemployment and poverty in rural Hungary. Therefore, we amended this sentence as follows:

“In Hungary, the association between geographical health inequalities, regional differences in sHLE and their cultural and structural determinants deserve more exploration.”

(see Revised Manuscript line 533-534)

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Rashidul Alam Mahumud

16 Feb 2022

Comparing Actuarial and Subjective Healthy Life Expectancy Estimates: a Cross-Sectional Survey Among the General Population in Hungary

PONE-D-21-03658R1

Dear Dr. Zrubka,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Rashidul Alam Mahumud, MPH, MSc, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Acceptance letter

Rashidul Alam Mahumud

2 Mar 2022

PONE-D-21-03658R1

Comparing Actuarial and Subjective Healthy Life Expectancy Estimates: a Cross-Sectional Survey Among the General Population in Hungary

Dear Dr. Zrubka:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Rashidul Alam Mahumud

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig

    Distribution of A) subjective life expectancy (sLE) in men, B) sLE in women, C) subjective healthy life expectancy (sHLE) in men and D) sHLE in women.

    (PDF)

    S2 Fig. Subjective life expectancy (sLE) versus subjective healthy life expectancy (sHLE) in men and women.

    (PDF)

    S1 Table. Actuarial healthy life expectancy estimates for males by age years.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. Actuarial healthy life expectancy estimates for males by age years.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Appendix. Details of survey items on subjective expectations and applied data transformations.

    (PDF)

    S2 Appendix. Estimating actuarial healthy life years (HLY) for each age year from abridged HLY tables.

    (PDF)

    S1 File

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES