Abstract
The context in which drinking occurs is a critical but relatively understudied factor in alcohol use disorder (AUD) etiology. In this article, I offer a social-contextual framework for examining AUD risk by reviewing studies on the unique antecedents and deleterious consequences of social versus solitary alcohol use in adolescents and young adults. Specifically, I provide evidence of distinct emotion regulatory functions across settings, with social drinking linked to enhancing positive emotions and social experiences and solitary drinking linked to coping with negative emotions. I end by considering the conceptual, methodological, and clinical implications of this social-contextual account of AUD risk.
Alcohol is consumed by 70% of people 18 years and older in the US (SAMHSA, 2018). While most US adults do not experience problems related to their alcohol use, 5.8% have an alcohol use disorder (AUD) (SAMHSA, 2018), defined as a “problematic pattern of alcohol use leading to clinically significant impairment or distress” (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Excessive alcohol use is a public health crisis (CDC, 2013).
One of the most challenging problems facing alcohol researchers is understanding why some individuals develop AUD. Major theories on the etiology (i.e., causes) of AUD have focused on intrapersonal (e.g., personality traits) and interpersonal (e.g., peer influence) factors that contribute to the development of pathological alcohol use (Sher et al., 2005). This review focuses on a critical but relatively understudied interpersonal factor in alcoholism etiology—the importance of considering whether alcohol consumption occurs in social versus solitary settings.
Most adolescents and young adults who drink alcohol only do so in social settings (Creswell et al., 2012; Fairbairn & Sayette, 2014), with ¾ citing “to have a good time with friends” as the primary motive for their alcohol use (O’Malley et al., 1998). A substantial minority, however, also consume alcohol while alone, with about 14% of adolescents (Mason et al., 2020) and 15–24% of young adults (Skrzynski & Creswell, 2020) reporting that they at times engage in solitary drinking (most commonly defined as drinking while no else is physically present). Consideration of the context in which drinking occurs has important implications for understanding risk processes for the development of AUD (Creswell et al., 2014).
Here, I offer a social-contextual framework for examining AUD risk by highlighting the unique antecedents and deleterious consequences of social versus solitary alcohol use (see Figure 1). Specifically, I (a) review studies linking unique individual difference factors to alcohol use across social and solitary settings, (b) provide evidence that drinking in both settings can be associated with heavy alcohol use and alcohol-related problems, (c) suggest that distinct mechanisms of risk for alcohol problems are associated with use in each setting, and (d) end with some broader considerations. I focus on adolescents and young adults, since nearly all of the research in this area has been conducted on these individuals.
Unique Individual Difference Factors
A growing literature has documented individual difference factors that influence who goes into one of two directions—either restricting alcohol use to social settings or also developing patterns of drinking alone. Among adolescents and young adults, drinking alcohol in social settings has been linked to positive emotionality and sociality (e.g., Cooper et al., 2016; Engels et al., 1999). In contrast, adolescents and young adults who engage in solitary drinking report more negative affect and more social discomfort than their social-only drinking peers (Skrzynski & Creswell, 2020). For example, adolescent solitary (versus social-only) drinkers have higher levels of trait negative emotionality (Creswell, Chung, Wright et al., 2015) and depressive symptoms (Tomlinson & Brown, 2012). Similarly, solitary drinking in young adults is associated with depressive symptoms, suicidal ideation, and lower distress tolerance (e.g., Gonzalez et al., 2009; Keough et al., 2015; Williams et al., 2015), and a recent longitudinal study demonstrated that negative affect prospectively predicted young adult solitary drinking four months later, even after accounting for baseline solitary drinking (Bilevicius et al., 2018). Further, solitary drinking in adolescents and young adults has been linked to measures of social discomfort, such as social anxiety (Skrzynski et al., 2018) and loneliness (Gonzalez & Skewes, 2013). In sum, there appear to be unique individual difference factors that are associated with alcohol use in social and solitary settings, with social drinking related to positive emotionality and sociality and solitary drinking related to negative emotionality and social discomfort.
Increased Alcohol Consumption and Alcohol Problems
Drinking in both social and solitary settings has been linked to heavy alcohol consumption and problems. Individuals often drink more alcohol in social settings than they do while alone (Monk et al., 2020), and per person alcohol consumption increases in larger versus smaller social groups (Smit et al., 2015), especially when socially normative information about heavy alcohol consumption is conveyed (Cullum et al., 2012). This increased alcohol consumption in social groups is not completely explained by social modeling (Kuendig & Kuntsche, 2012), but is also attributable to characteristics of the drinking individuals (e.g., anticipated social facilitation; Smith et al., 1995) and features of the social context (e.g., level of familiarity of group members; Fairbairn et al., 2018). Further, drinking in social settings is associated with negative alcohol-related outcomes that are likely markers of AUD, such as driving while intoxicated, alcohol-induced violence, sexual assault, and risky sexual behavior (Bersamin et al., 2012; Cleveland et al., 2019; Mair et al., 2015).
Solitary drinking has also been linked to increased alcohol use and problems (Skrzynski & Creswell, 2020). Adolescent and young adult solitary drinkers report significantly greater alcohol use than their social-only drinking peers (e.g., Creswell et al., 2015; Skrzynski et al., 2018). Importantly, longitudinal studies have demonstrated that any drinking while alone in adolescence prospectively predicts the development of alcohol problems over and above other baseline risk factors. For instance, Creswell and colleagues (2014) demonstrated that solitary (versus social-only) drinking adolescents (ages 12–18) subsequently met more criteria for AUD symptoms at age 25, even after controlling for adolescent alcohol use and problems. Similarly, Tucker and colleagues (2006) found that eighth-grade solitary (versus social-only) drinkers went on to experience more alcohol problems at age 23, even after accounting for eighth-grade alcohol use. What’s noteworthy is that adolescent and young adult solitary drinkers still spend the majority of their time drinking in social settings (Creswell et al., 2015; Skrzynski et al., 2018), yet engaging in occasional (typically ~25% of the time) solitary drinking is associated with increased alcohol use and the development of alcohol problems compared to their social-only drinking peers. Taken together, these findings indicate that alcohol use in both social and solitary settings is linked to the escalation of drinking and negative alcohol-related consequences. Importantly, the mechanisms of risk across these settings appear to be distinct.
Distinct Mechanisms of Risk
Adolescents and young adults report drinking in social settings to enhance positive emotions and social experiences (e.g., Cooper et al., 2016). Further, the belief that alcohol improves mood and social experiences is associated with problematic drinking (Leigh & Stacy, 1993) and, in prospective studies, is predictive of subsequent alcohol use and AUD symptoms years later (Patrick et al., 2011; Smith et al., 1995). Individuals who are highly sensitive to these emotional and social rewards of alcohol are thought to be at particular risk for developing alcohol problems (Grekin et al., 2006). Accordingly, individuals who experience greater mood enhancement from alcohol in the laboratory have been shown to escalate their drinking over a two-year period and develop AUD symptoms (King et al., 2011).
Individual difference factors that are associated with drinking in social settings (e.g., extraversion; Cooper et al., 2016) might also predict who obtains the most reward from alcohol in these contexts, placing them at greater risk to escalate their drinking. Sayette and colleagues (2012) conducted the largest study to date to evaluate alcohol’s social and emotional rewards in a laboratory social setting and to identify who is most sensitive to these effects. Healthy young adult drinkers (n=720) were assembled into three person groups of strangers and interacted with each other while consuming alcoholic or nonalcoholic beverages. Results provided robust support for alcohol’s emotional and social rewards. Further, some individuals were particularly likely to experience these rewards (Creswell et al., 2012). For instance, individuals high on extraversion reported greater positive mood and social bonding than individuals low on extraversion (Fairbairn et al., 2015). Moreover, social processes uniquely accounted for alcohol reward-sensitivity among these high extraversion individuals—they derived more reward from the smiles displayed by their groupmates and from instances when they shared a smile with a groupmate than those low on extraversion. These results extend previous studies demonstrating that individuals high on extraversion are more likely to endorse drinking for enhancement of positive mood and social experiences to suggest that these individuals actually experience more reward from alcohol in social settings. Longitudinal studies are now needed to determine whether this differential reward sensitivity prospectively predicts the escalation of drinking over time and the development of alcohol problems.
While drinking in social settings appears to be driven by the desire to enhance positive emotions and social experiences (Cooper et al., 1995), the most compelling theory for solitary drinking is one of self-medication, in which individuals drink alone to cope with negative affect (Mason et al., 2020; Skrzynski & Creswell, 2020). Adolescent and young adult solitary drinking is associated with drinking to cope motives (e.g., “because it helps when you feel depressed”) over and above social and enhancement motives (e.g., Cooper et al., 1995; O’Hara et al., 2015). Solitary drinking is also associated with drinking during negative but not positive affect (Creswell et al., 2014), beliefs in alcohol’s ability to mitigate negative affect (Christiansen et al., 2002), and the perceived inability to resist drinking while experiencing negative affect (Creswell, Chung, Wright et al. 2015). Further, adolescent and young adult solitary drinking is either not related to or negatively correlated with social and enhancement motives (Cooper et al., 1994; Armeli et al., 2018; O’Hara et al., 2015). Thus, in contrast to social drinking, which is more strongly motivated by social and emotional enhancement motives, solitary drinking in adolescents and young adults appears to be driven by a desire to cope with negative affect.
Future Directions
This review provides a framework for organizing research on how the social context of alcohol use in adolescents and young adults can improve our understanding of the development of alcohol problems. Additional studies are necessary to fill in some important gaps. For instance, other individual difference factors that may be associated with solitary versus social drinking should be explored, such as gender (Skrzynski & Creswell, 2020) and neurobiological factors linked to variation in negative/positive emotionality (e.g., Wright et al., 2019). Further, while there is some evidence for distinct alcohol problems for social versus solitary drinking (Mason et al., 2020), there also appears to be significant overlap (e.g., risky behavior; Bersamin et al., 2012; Tucker et al., 2006). Future studies are needed to determine if there are differential negative consequences across social and solitary settings. It is also noteworthy that the vast majority of studies in this area have been conducted on adolescents and young adults. It will be important to test the hypothesized framework in older individuals, especially given the increasing prevalence of social isolation associated with aging (Lou, Hawkley, Waite, & Cacioppo, 2012). Standardizing the definition of solitary drinking to mean drinking while no one else is physically (or virtually) present would help to further clarify associations between solitary drinking and relevant variables, as some researchers have defined solitary drinking as drinking with non-drinking others and/or among non-interacting others (see Skrzynski & Creswell, 2020).
In general, more rigorous tests are needed to understand the mechanisms underlying social and solitary drinking and the pathways by which drinking in each setting leads to adverse outcomes. Notably, the vast majority of studies conducted thus far on solitary drinking are cross-sectional, precluding causal interpretations. There is much that remains unknown. For instance, how do solitary drinkers experience alcohol intoxication in solitary versus social settings? The evidence reviewed above suggests that solitary drinkers may not expect or obtain the same kind of social rewards from alcohol in social settings, but this needs to be tested in experimental studies that manipulate the context of alcohol consumption. Such studies would also shed light on whether solitary drinkers actually experience negative affect relief when drinking alone and, if so, whether certain individuals (e.g., those with high negative affectivity) are especially sensitive alcohol’s tension reduction effects (e.g., Mohr et al., 2001). It’s important to note that while young adults in prior laboratory studies have reported increased negative affect in solitary versus social drinking contexts (Fairbairn & Sayette, 2014), drinking alone might have been an aversive experience for the majority of these participants who (based on prevalence rates) were likely social-only drinkers.
It will also be important to understand the factors that influence when solitary drinkers choose to drink in each setting. Studies using experimental mood manipulations could test the self-medication model of solitary drinking to determine whether heightened negative affect increases the preference to drink alone. Longitudinal, repeated measure designs that query individuals in real-time (e.g., using ecological momentary assessment [EMA]) would be useful to better understand the antecedents to solitary drinking, how drinking alone is experienced in the moment, and the pathways by which solitary drinking leads to negative outcomes. These study designs, although still correlational, can establish temporal precedence among antecedents and consequences of solitary drinking thus providing stronger information about the causal processes operating in the day-to-day lives of young people. For instance, using similar methodology, Mohr and colleagues (2001) demonstrated that individuals engaged in more solitary drinking on days with more negative interpersonal experiences and engaged in more social drinking on days with more positive interpersonal experiences. These types of intensive longitudinal designs would be particularly useful with adolescent populations who are not legally permitted to drink alcohol in experimental lab settings.
Much is also unknown about the social drinking pathway to alcohol problems. Notably, the vast majority of prior laboratory alcohol administration studies have asked young adults to consume alcohol while alone (Fairbairn & Sayette, 2014). This is a highly unusual way for most young adults to experience alcohol intoxication, and this solitary setting precludes measuring many of the subjectively pleasant effects of alcohol that confer increased risk for alcohol misuse (e.g., increased sociability) (Creswell et al., 2012). Use of laboratory social drinking paradigms may permit laboratory research to become even more informative in predicting risk to develop AUD. Additionally, while individual difference factors (e.g., genetics, personality) that predict enhanced reward from alcohol consumed in social settings have been identified (Creswell et al., 2012; Fairbairn et al., 2015), it remains unclear whether they are clinically meaningful, and longitudinal studies are needed.
Perhaps most importantly, studies should be conducted that test the hypothesized framework in its entirety within the same sample of participants, as prior studies have tended to focus either on social or solitary drinkers. Alcohol administration paradigms combined with EMA protocols that assess social and solitary drinkers in real-time and over a long enough timeframe to detect the development of AUD symptoms would help establish the necessary directional and causal relationships presented in Figure 1. These designs would permit comparisons of the acute effects of alcohol in solitary versus social-only drinkers across solitary and social drinking settings in the laboratory and real-world to further test the proposed mechanisms of risk and to determine whether the hypothesized individual difference factors differentially predict who experiences the most reward from alcohol in each setting. EMA methods could test hypothesized pathways during drinking episodes in real life and determine whether these associations predict the escalation of drinking and the development of alcohol problems over time.
In addition to the methodological implications discussed above, this framework has important conceptual and clinical implications. Conceptually, these data suggest that the context of drinking matters, with social and solitary alcohol consumption being psychologically distinct phenomena with qualitatively different antecedents and perhaps unique consequences (Cooper et al., 1995; Mason et al., 2020; Skrzynski & Creswell, 2020). One intriguing possibility is that existing theories of alcohol use (e.g., alcohol myopia theory [alcohol’s tendency to constrain attention to the most salient aspects of the environment; Steele & Josephs, 1990]; appraisal disruption theory [alcohol’s tendency to disrupt initial appraisal of stressful information; Sayette, 1993]) might apply more to solitary versus social drinking settings. Further, the proposed framework might also aid in our understanding of risk pathways for other drugs of abuse (Creswell, Chung, Clark et al., 2015; Mason et al., 2020).
Clinically, knowing more about an individual’s pattern of social and solitary drinking would aid in understanding the purposes that drinking serves, which is useful for identifying alternative reinforcement options to target in treatment (Creswell et al., 2020). Specifically, clinicians could frame conversations around drinking contexts. When individuals feel motivated to drink alone, they could be encouraged to reflect on whether this desire is related to the experience of negative emotions and, if so, more adaptive ways to cope with such negative emotions could be implemented (e.g., distress tolerance skills; Cavicchioli et al., 2019). Similarly, individuals could be asked to reflect on their experiences while drinking in social settings in order to identify why alcohol consumption in such contexts may be particularly rewarding for them. Interventions for social drinking settings could focus on more adaptive ways to increase positive emotions and social reward without drinking to excess (e.g., the ability to have higher quality social interactions when drinking moderately in social settings; Conroy & de Visser, 2018). Taken as a whole, the context of alcohol use deserves careful consideration as a factor that facilitates our understanding of the development of alcohol problems in adolescents and young adults.
Recommended Reading.
Creswell et al. (2014). (See References). A large prospective study showing that adolescent solitary drinking predicts the development of AUD symptoms in young adulthood.
Skrzysnki & Creswell (2020). (See References). A meta-analytic review documenting links between adolescent/young adult solitary drinking and negative psychosocial problems, drinking to cope motives, and increased alcohol use and problems.
Fairbairn et al. (2015). (See References). An experiment that manipulated alcohol consumption (versus placebo or control beverages) in a laboratory social setting and found differential sensitivity to alcohol’s social rewards for individuals high on trait extraversion.
Acknowledgements
Thanks to Michael Sayette and Sheldon Cohen for their feedback on an early draft of this paper.
References
- American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. [Google Scholar]
- Armeli S, Covault J, & Tennen H. (2018). Long-term changes in the effects of episode-specific drinking to cope motivation on daily well-being. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 32(7), 715–726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bersamin MM, Paschall MJ, Saltz RF, & Zamboanga BL (2012). Young adults and casual sex: The relevance of college drinking settings. Journal of Sex Research, 49(2–3), 274–281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bilevicius E, Single A, Rapinda KK, Bristow LA, & Keough MT (2018). Frequent solitary drinking mediates the associations between negative affect and harmful drinking in emerging adults. Addictive Behaviors, 87, 115–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner JD, & Terlecki MA (2016). Social anxiety and alcohol-related impairment: The mediational impact of solitary drinking. Addictive Behaviors, 58, 7–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavicchioli M, Movalli M, Vassena G, Ramella P, Prudenziati F, & Maffei C. (2019). The therapeutic role of emotion regulation and coping strategies during a stand-alone DBT skills training program for alcohol use disorder and concurrent substance use disorders. Addictive Behaviors, 98, 106035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Alcohol and Public Health: Alcohol-Related Disease Impact (ARDI). Average for United States 2006–2010 Alcohol-Attributable Deaths Due to Excessive Alcohol Use. Available at: https://nccd.cdc.gov/DPH_ARDI/Default/Report.aspx?T=AAM&P=f6d7eda7-036e-4553-9968-9b17ffad620e&R=d7a9b303-48e9-4440-bf47-070a4827e1fd&M=8E1C5233-5640-4EE8-9247-1ECA7DA325B9&F=&D=
- Christiansen M, Vik PW, & Jarchow A. (2002). College student heavy drinking in social contexts versus alone. Addictive Behaviors, 27(3), 393–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cleveland MJ, Testa M, & Hone LS (2019). Examining the roles of heavy episodic drinking, drinking venues, and sociosexuality in college men’s sexual aggression. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 80(2), 177–185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conroy D, & de Visser RO (2018). Benefits and drawbacks of social non-drinking identified by British university students. Drug and Alcohol Review, 37, S89–S97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML, Kuntsche E, Levitt A, Barber LL, & Wolf S. (2016). Motivational models of substance use: A review of theory and research on motives for using alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco. In Sher KJ (Ed.), Oxford library of psychology. The Oxford handbook of substance use and substance use disorders (p. 375–421). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML, Frone MR, Russell M, & Mudar P. (1995). Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 990–1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell KG, Chung T, Clark DB, & Martin CS (2014). Solitary alcohol use in teens is associated with drinking in response to negative affect and predicts alcohol problems in young adulthood. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(5), 602–610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell KG, Chung T, Clark DB, & Martin CS (2015). Solitary cannabis use in adolescence as a correlate and predictor of cannabis problems. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 156, 120–125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell KG, Chung T, Skrzynski CJ, Bachrach RL, Jackson KM, Clark DB, & Martin CS (2020). Drinking beyond the binge threshold in a clinical sample of adolescents. Addiction. Advance online publication. 10.1111/add.14979 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell KG, Chung T, Wright AGC, Black JJ, Clark DB, & Martin CS (2015). Personality, negative affect coping, and drinking alone: A structural equation modeling approach to examine correlates of adolescent solitary drinking. Addiction, 110(5), 775–783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell KG, Sayette MA, Manuck SB, Ferrell RE, Hill SY, & Dimoff JD (2012). DRD4 polymorphism moderates the effect of alcohol consumption on social bonding. PLoS ONE, 7(2), e29814, 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cullum J, O’Grady M, Armeli S, & Tennen H. (2012). The role of context-specific norms and group size in alcohol consumption and compliance drinking during natural drinking events. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 34(4), 304–312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engels RC, Knibbe RA, & Drop MJ (1999). Visiting public drinking places: an explorative study into the functions of pub-going for late adolescents. Substance Use & Misuse, 34(9), 1261–1280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fairbairn CE, Bresin K, Kang D, Rosen IG, Ariss T, Luczak SE, ... & Eckland NS (2018). A multimodal investigation of contextual effects on alcohol’s emotional rewards. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(4), 359–373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fairbairn CE, & Sayette MA (2014). A social-attributional analysis of alcohol response. Psychological Bulletin, 140(5), 1361–1382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fairbairn CE, Sayette MA, Wright AGC, Levine JM, Cohn JF, & Creswell KG (2015). Extraversion and the rewarding effects of alcohol in a social context. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 660–673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gonzalez VM, Collins RL, & Bradizza CM (2009). Solitary and social heavy drinking, suicidal ideation, and drinking motives in underage college drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 34(12), 993–999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gonzalez VM, & Skewes MC (2013). Solitary heavy drinking, social relationships, and negative mood regulation in college drinkers. Addiction Research & Theory, 21(4), 285–294. [Google Scholar]
- Grekin ER, Sher KJ, & Wood PK (2006). Personality and substance dependence symptoms: Modeling substance-specific traits. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(4), 415–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keough MT, Battista SR, O’Connor RM, Sherry SB, & Stewart SH (2016). Getting the party started—Alone: Solitary predrinking mediates the effect of social anxiety on alcohol-related problems. Addictive Behaviors, 55, 19–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keough MT, O’Connor RM, Sherry SB, & Stewart SH (2015). Context counts: Solitary drinking explains the association between depressive symptoms and alcohol-related problems in undergraduates. Addictive Behaviors, 42, 216–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuendig H, & Kuntsche E. (2012). Solitary versus social drinking: An experimental study on effects of social exposures on in situ alcohol consumption. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 36(4), 732–738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leigh BC, & Stacy AW (1993). Alcohol outcome expectancies: Scale construction and predictive utility in higher order confirmatory models. Psychological Assessment, 5(2), 216–229. [Google Scholar]
- Luo Y, Hawkley LC, Waite LJ, & Cacioppo JT (2012). Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: A national longitudinal study. Social Science & Medicine, 74(6), 907–914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mair C, Lipperman‐Kreda S, Gruenewald PJ, Bersamin M, & Grube JW (2015). Adolescent drinking risks associated with specific drinking contexts. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39(9), 1705–1711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason WA, Stevens AL, & Fleming CB (2020). A systematic review of research on adolescent solitary alcohol and marijuana use in the United States. Addiction, 115(1), 19–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohr CD, Armeli S, Tennen H, Carney MA, Affleck G, & Hromi A. (2001). Daily interpersonal experiences, context, and alcohol consumption: Crying in your beer and toasting good times. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(3), 489–500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monk RL, Qureshi A, & Heim D. (2020). An examination of the extent to which mood and context are associated with real-time alcohol consumption. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 208. Advance online publication. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.107880 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Hara RE, Armeli S, & Tennen H. (2015). College students’ drinking motives and social-contextual factors: Comparing associations across levels of analysis. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29(2), 420–429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patrick ME, Schulenberg JE, O’malley PM, Johnston LD, & Bachman JG (2011). Adolescents’ reported reasons for alcohol and marijuana use as predictors of substance use and problems in adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 72(1), 106–116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sayette MA (1993). An appraisal-disruption model of alcohol’s effects on stress responses in social drinkers. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 459–476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sayette MA, Creswell KG, Dimoff JD, Fairbairn CE, Cohn JF, Heckman BW, Kirchner TR, Levine JM, & Moreland RL (2012). Alcohol and group formation: A multimodal investigation of the effects of alcohol on emotion and social bonding. Psychological Science, 23(8), 869–878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sayette MA, Fairbairn CE, & Creswell KG (2016). Alcohol and emotion: The importance of social context. In Kopetz CE & Lejuez CW (Eds.), Addiction: A Social Psychological Perspective. (pp. 98–119), New York: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sher KJ, Grekin ER, & Williams NA (2005). The development of alcohol use disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 493–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skrzynski CJ, & Creswell KG (2020). Associations between solitary drinking and increased alcohol consumption, alcohol problems, and drinking to cope motives in adolescents and young adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Addiction. Advance online publication. 10.1111/add.15055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skrzynski C, Creswell KG, Bachrach RL, Chung T. (2018). Social deficits moderate the relationship between drinking to cope and solitary drinking in underage drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 78, 124–130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smit K, Groefsema M, Luijten M, Engels R, & Kuntsche E. (2015). Drinking motives moderate the effect of the social environment on alcohol use: an event-level study among young adults. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 76(6), 971–980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith GT, Goldman MS, Greenbaum PE, & Christiansen BA (1995). Expectancy for social facilitation from drinking: The divergent paths of high-expectancy and low-expectancy adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(1), 32–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele CM, & Josephs RA (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American Psychologist, 45(8), 921–933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA). 2018 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH). Table 2.1B—Tobacco Product and Alcohol Use in Lifetime, Past Year, and Past Month among Persons Aged 12 or Older, by Age Group: Percentages, 2017 and 2018. Available at: https://www.samhsa.gov/data/sites/default/files/cbhsq-reports/NSDUHDetailedTabs2018R2/NSDUHDetTabsSect2pe2018.htm#tab2-1b
- Tomlinson KL, & Brown SA (2012). Self-medication or social learning? A comparison of models to predict early adolescent drinking. Addictive behaviors, 37(2), 179–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker JS, Ellickson PL, Collins RL, & Klein DJ (2006). Does solitary substance use increase adolescents’ risk for poor psychosocial and behavioral outcomes? A 9-year longitudinal study comparing solitary and social users. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(4), 363–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams CL, Vik PW, & Wong MM (2015). Distress tolerance in social versus solitary college student drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 50, 89–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright AGC, Creswell KG, Flory JD, Muldoon M, & Manuck SB (2019). Neurobiological functioning and the personality trait hierarchy: Central serotonergic responsivity and the stability meta-trait. Psychological Science, 30(10), 1413–1423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]