Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Anxiety Stress Coping. 2021 Sep 15;35(4):395–408. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2021.1977797

Anxiety Sensitivity, Distress Intolerance, and Negative Interpretation Bias Strengthen the Relationship Between Trait Anxiety and Depersonalization

Noah J French 1, Jeremy W Eberle 1, Bethany A Teachman 1
PMCID: PMC8920942  NIHMSID: NIHMS1739023  PMID: 34524043

Abstract

Background and Objectives:

Depersonalization is common in anxiety disorders, but little is known about factors that influence co-occurring anxiety and depersonalization.

Design:

We investigated trait moderators of the relationships between state and trait anxiety and depersonalization to better understand their co-occurrence and to identify potential points of intervention.

Methods:

Adults recruited on Amazon Mechanical Turk (N = 303) completed two computer tasks designed to increase variability in state anxiety and depersonalization as well as several self-report questionnaires.

Results:

As hypothesized, anxiety positively predicted depersonalization at both a state level and trait level. Moreover, as hypothesized, the trait anxiety-trait depersonalization relationship was strengthened by greater anxiety sensitivity; distress intolerance; and negative interpretation bias for anxiety sensations, and for depersonalization sensations. None of these hypothesized trait moderators significantly strengthened the state anxiety-state depersonalization relationship.

Conclusions:

These findings suggest that, on a trait level, anxiety and depersonalization more frequently co-occur when people catastrophically misinterpret their symptoms or have lower emotional distress tolerance.

Keywords: anxiety, depersonalization, derealization, anxiety sensitivity, distress tolerance, interpretation bias

Introduction

Depersonalization is a subjective experience of unreality with respect to one’s sense of self and is often accompanied by derealization, that experience of unreality with respect to one’s external world.1 People experiencing depersonalization often have the unsettling sense that they are an outside observer to their own thoughts and actions and that their world is foggy and dreamlike (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Depersonalization often co-occurs with anxiety but, historically, little attention has been paid to dissociative experiences among people with anxiety, including why dissociation occurs for some but not all people who experience anxiety. This study sought to replicate the established co-occurrence between trait anxiety and depersonalization symptoms (e.g., Michal et al., 2005; Sierra et al., 2012) and to test proposed trait moderators, which might explain why certain people are more prone to dissociation when anxious. Further, using mild symptom induction tasks, the study tested these moderators on the relationship between in-the-moment, state expressions of anxiety and depersonalization.

Depersonalization frequently occurs in a variety of anxiety and anxiety-related disorders, such as generalized anxiety disorder (Michal et al., 2016), social anxiety disorder (Michal et al., 2005), hypochondriasis (Torch, 1978), and obsessive-compulsive disorder (Boysan, 2014). It is also one of the defining features (but not necessary for diagnosis) of panic attacks and agoraphobia (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The co-occurrence of anxiety and depersonalization also seems to hold in nonclinical populations (Trueman, 1984a). In fact, of all emotional states examined, Simeon et al. (2003) found anxiety to be the best predictor of depersonalization. Yet, not all people with anxiety experience depersonalization when highly anxious. Segui et al. (2000) found that only 24.1% of a panic disorder sample reported depersonalization during their panic attacks, and consequently proposed that “panic disorder with depersonalization” be considered a distinct subtype of panic disorder. Complicating things further, Sierra et al. (2012) found that the association between trait anxiety and depersonalization symptoms was apparent in depersonalization/derealization patients with milder depersonalization symptoms, but not in those with more severe depersonalization symptoms. Clearly, the relationship between anxiety severity and depersonalization severity is more complex than a simple linear function.

Why do high levels of anxiety predict depersonalization for some people? A few conceptual models have been proposed, though none has been rigorously tested. We focus on three potential moderators: distress tolerance, negative interpretation bias, and anxiety sensitivity. They were selected because of empirical research linking these constructs to anxiety and depersonalization,2 and because each has a theoretical basis to suggest it may moderate the anxiety-depersonalization relationship.

First, a number of theorists hypothesize that depersonalization is an evolution-prepared psychological defense against extreme trauma, emotional distress, or anxiety (e.g., Boysan, 2014; Griffin et al., 1997; Sierra, 2009; Snyder, 1999; Zeidner & Endler, 1996). According to this view, when someone experiences distress above a given threshold, depersonalization “kicks in” to numb the anxiety and reduce the emotional intensity of the experience. This protective model of depersonalization has some empirical support. People who experienced depersonalization during a traumatic event reported lower intensity of other unpleasant symptoms during that event, indicating that depersonalization may have helped defend them from the full impact of the trauma (Shilony & Grossman, 1993), though causality cannot be inferred from this study’s correlational design. This suggests that distress tolerance, the ability to withstand negative affect or other adverse psychological or physical states, might help explain variability in the anxiety-depersonalization relationship; if the protective model of depersonalization holds, greater distress intolerance might strengthen this relationship.

Second, in their cognitive-behavioral model of depersonalization/derealization disorder, which has early empirical support (Hunter et al., 2014), Hunter et al. (2003) posit that threat interpretations of otherwise transient depersonalization (e.g., believing that one is “going crazy”) intensify subsequent anxiety and perpetuate an anxiety-depersonalization cycle. Indeed, individuals with chronic depersonalization sometimes report fears of having a brain disease or impending insanity (Hunter et al., 2003; Roth, 1960). Beyond the Hunter et al. model, there is evidence that threat interpretations about the psychosocial consequences of anxiety symptoms predict elevated levels of depersonalization (Marks et al., 1991), suggesting that threat interpretations of both anxiety and depersonalization symptoms will be similarly predictive. Thus, it appears that negative interpretation bias, the tendency to construe ambiguous situations in a negative or threatening way, may feed the vicious cycle of anxiety and depersonalization. In fact, early trials of cognitive therapy (Schweden et al., 2016) and cognitive behavioral therapy (Hunter et al., 2003), both of which target interpretation biases, have had success in treating patients with co-occurring anxiety and depersonalization.

Third, anxiety sensitivity, the fear of fear (i.e., fear of experiencing anxiety-related sensations), is a construct similar to, and yet distinct from, negative interpretation bias for anxiety symptoms. Olthuis and colleagues suggest anxiety sensitivity “…is the trait and negative interpretation biases are one of the cognitive processes by which this trait predisposes an individual to anxiety symptoms” (see Olthuis et al., 2012; p. 333). Anxiety sensitivity is often predictive of intense anxiety (i.e., panic symptoms) in response to tasks that provoke physical anxiety sensations (McNally, 2002). Anxiety sensitivity, too, may therefore strengthen the anxiety-depersonalization relationship, through its association with negative interpretation bias for anxiety sensations or by elevating anxiety level in response to anxiety sensations, thereby heightening the risk of triggering depersonalization.

Overview of Present Study and Hypotheses

The present study recruited participants online to test the relationship between anxiety and depersonalization (at both state and trait levels) and trait-level moderators of that relationship. Participants completed, in randomized order, two mild symptom induction tasks designed to increase variability in state anxiety (imagining an upcoming anxiety-provoking event) and depersonalization (staring intently at a dot), with intermixed self-reports of state anxiety and depersonalization, followed by reports of trait anxiety, depression, and the potential moderators: anxiety sensitivity, distress tolerance, and negative interpretation bias for anxiety sensations and for depersonalization sensations. We also measured state negative valence and arousal, to better understand how the symptom induction tasks affected participants.

We hypothesized that each induction task would elevate both negative valence and arousal, along with state anxiety and depersonalization, relative to pre-induction ratings (Hypothesis A). Specifically, we expected the anxiety induction to increase anxiety and the depersonalization induction to increase depersonalization. Next, we hypothesized that there would be a significant, positive relation between anxiety and depersonalization (Hypothesis B). Finally, we hypothesized that greater distress intolerance (Hypothesis C1), negative interpretation bias for depersonalization symptoms (Hypothesis C2), negative interpretation bias for anxiety symptoms (Hypothesis C3), and anxiety sensitivity (Hypothesis C4) would strengthen the relation; this proposed moderation is Hypothesis C.

In addition to testing these predictions for trait levels of anxiety and depersonalization, it is worthwhile to test these predictions at a state level, using repeated measures data to examine within-person change. Indeed, the conceptual models that informed our design imply within-person effects. Moreover, for many psychological constructs, between-person effects and within-person effects are not equivalent (Molenaar, 2004), so it is important not to assume that anxiety-depersonalization relationships will operate identically at state and trait levels; thus, we examine this empirically. Further, evidence for state depersonalization-state anxiety co-occurrence is conflicting (Hoyer et al., 2013; Lickel et al., 2008; Sierra et al., 2002), warranting further study. We hypothesized that the direction and moderators of the relationship between anxiety and depersonalization would be the same on the state level as on the trait level.

Method

Participants and Design

We collected data from 453 participants over three waves via Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk). In line with suggested practices for MTurk research (Kennedy et al., 2018), we manually sorted through responses at the end of each wave of data collection to flag those we suspected of being the work of automated bots. In the end, we were left with 303 responses for analyses. See Section S1.1 of the supplement for a detailed description of our method for excluding suspicious responses. Of these 303 participants, 65.4% identified as male, and 66.3% identified as White/European origin. Additionally, 12.5% identified as Hispanic or Latino. The mean age was 34.56 years (SD = 9.80). See Table 1 for full demographic information.

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics by Condition

Characteristic Induction Order 1
(n = 164)
Induction Order 2
(n = 139)

Age (years): M (SD) 34.75 (9.50) 34.33 (10.17)
Gender: n (%)
 Female 50 (30.5) 51 (36.7)
 Male 112 (68.3) 86 (61.9)
 Transgender Female 0 (0.0) 1 (0.7)
 Transgender Male 0 (0.0) 1 (0.7)
 Other 2 (1.2) 0 (0.0)
 Prefer not to answer 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0)
Race: n (%)a
 American Indian/Alaska Native 4 (2.4) 5 (3.6)
 East Asian 15 (9.1) 8 (5.8)
 South Asian 22 (13.4) 18 (12.9)
 Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander 2 (1.2) 1 (0.7)
 Black/African origin 19 (11.6) 12 (8.6)
 White/European origin 105 (64.0) 96 (69.1)
 Other or Unknown 1 (0.6) 3 (2.2)
 Prefer not to answer 4 (2.4) 0 (0.0)
Ethnicity: n (%)
 Hispanic or Latino 25 (15.2) 13 (9.4)
 Not Hispanic or Latino 138 (84.1) 121 (87.1)
 Unknown 1 (0.6) 2 (1.4)
 Prefer not to answer 0 (0.0) 3 (2.2)

Note. Induction Order 1 = depersonalization induction first, Induction Order 2 = anxiety induction first.

a

Percentages total over 100.0, because participants were able to select multiple races.

Recruitment

The study was advertised on MTurk as “[a] psychology study lasting 15–20 minutes that involves completing short tasks on the computer and answering questionnaires.” Participants were offered $3 for completing the study. Enrollment was restricted to adults (age 18+) who had at least 90% of their previously completed MTurk tasks approved for payment. There were no restrictions on country of residence, though the study was only provided in English.

Measures

State Anxiety, Depersonalization, Negative Valence, and Arousal

State anxiety, depersonalization, negative valence, and arousal were assessed with self-report sliders. The state anxiety slider spanned from Calm to Anxious, the state depersonalization slider from Present/Grounded to Detached/Unreal, the state negative valence slider from Pleasant to Unpleasant, and the state arousal slider from Resting/Relaxed to Excited/Worked Up. State valence and arousal were included to more broadly understand how the induction tasks (described below) affected participants and to distract participants from the main state phenomena of interest: anxiety and depersonalization. See Table 2 for descriptive statistics for the state-level measures and Figures S1 and S2 for visualizations of how mean state scores changed over time over the course of the induction tasks.

Table 2.

Mean (SD) State/Trait Scores by Condition

Construct / Time Point Induction Order 1
(n = 164)
Induction Order 2
(n = 139)

State Anxiety
 Time 0 24.27 (25.91) 23.73 (23.93)
 Time 1 34.09 (29.44) 65.32 (28.52)
 Time 2 69.07 (25.37) 35.89 (28.51)
State Depersonalization
 Time 0 22.21 (24.07) 21.63 (22.90)
 Time 1 38.60 (30.02) 47.17 (25.52)
 Time 2 51.47 (27.86) 38.60 (30.17)
State Negative Valence
 Time 0 29.68 (23.95) 28.09 (23.88)
 Time 1 43.15 (27.88) 57.29 (25.65)
 Time 2 64.85 (23.11) 43.26 (27.37)
State Arousal
 Time 0 27.46 (25.63) 28.18 (26.57)
 Time 1 33.02 (26.81) 59.58 (26.09)
 Time 2 62.70 (23.79) 34.76 (27.25)
Trait Anxiety 1.97 (1.84) 2.47 (1.77)
Trait Depression 1.72 (1.87) 2.11 (1.84)
Trait Depersonalization 1.10 (1.67) 1.52 (1.79)
Anxiety Sensitivity 27.44 (17.45) 27.83 (16.93)
Distress Tolerance 11.02 (4.99) 11.52 (4.58)
Neg. Interpretation Bias for Anxiety Sensations 1.07 (1.00) 1.19 (1.02)
Neg. Interpretation Bias for Depersonalization Sensations 1.10 (1.11) 1.19 (1.04)

Note. Induction Order 1 = depersonalization induction first, Induction Order 2 = anxiety induction first. Time 0 = baseline, Time 1 = after first induction, Time 2 = after second induction.

Trait Anxiety and Depersonalization

Trait anxiety was assessed with the Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4; Kroenke et al., 2009), a self-report of anxiety and depression symptoms over the past two weeks with four 4-point Likert items ranging from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day). Two anxiety items comprise the Anxiety subscale, for which a sum of 3 or greater reflects potential generalized anxiety, panic, social anxiety, or posttraumatic stress disorder. The sum of the anxiety items was used in analyses. Internal consistency3 for the anxiety items was good, ωt = .81, 95% CI = [.75, .85]. Trait depersonalization was assessed with the two-item version of the Cambridge Depersonalization Scale (CDS-2; Michal et al., 2011), a-self report of depersonalization symptoms over the past two weeks with two 4-point Likert items ranging from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day). A sum of at least 3 reflects potentially clinically significant depersonalization. Internal consistency was good, ωt = .87, 95% CI = [.83, .90]. Prior studies support the construct and criterion validity of the GAD-2 and PHQ-2 (Kroenke et al., 2003, 2007, 2009; Löwe et al., 2005). Further, their internal consistency, discriminant validity, 1–4-week test-retest reliability, and sensitivity to treatment change have been found comparable to those of longer measures (Kroenke et al., 2010; Staples et al., 2019). Additionally, the 2-item version of the Cambridge Depersonalization Scale differentiates patients with clinically significant depersonalization well from other groups, and shows high reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.92; Michal et al., 2010).

Anxiety Sensitivity.

Anxiety sensitivity, the fear of arousal-related sensations, was assessed with the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (Taylor et al., 2007), a multidimensional self-report of anxiety sensitivity with eighteen 5-point Likert items ranging from 0 (very little) to 4 (very much). Participants rate how much they agree with items such as “It is important for me to not appear nervous” and “It scares me when my heart beats rapidly.” Six items comprise physical concerns, six items comprise cognitive concerns, and six items comprise social concerns. The sum of all 18 items was analyzed. Internal consistency was good, ωt = .95, 95% CI = [.93, .95].

Distress Tolerance.

Distress tolerance, an individual’s perceived ability to experience and endure negative psychological states, was assessed with the Distress Tolerance Scale-Short Form (DTS-SF; Garner et al., 2018), a self-report of overall distress tolerance with four 5-point Likert items ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree). For consistency with the other study measures, we reverse-coded the DTS-SF in our implementation, such that 1 represented strongly disagree and 5 represented strongly agree. The sum of the four items was used in analysis, and—because of the reverse coding—higher scores represent lower distress tolerance. Internal consistency was good, ωt = .89, 95% CI = [.86, .91].

Negative Interpretation Biases for Anxiety and Depersonalization Sensations.

Negative interpretation biases for anxiety and depersonalization sensations were assessed with the Anxiety and Depersonalization Sensations Interpretation Questionnaire (ADSIQ), a reading judgement task developed for the present study. The task presents eight ambiguous situations, four of which involve anxiety sensations (e.g., “You notice that your heart rate has risen and skips a beat”) and four of which involve depersonalization sensations (e.g., “Out of the blue, you feel strange, as if you were not real or as if you were cut off from the world”). After each situation, participants read three possible explanations, two of which are benign interpretations (e.g., “You’ve been working too hard and need a rest”) and one of which is a threat interpretation (e.g., “You are going out of your mind”). Participants rate the extent they think each explanation for a situation would be likely to be true if they found themselves in that situation on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (not at all likely) to 4 (extremely likely). The structure of the questionnaire is based on the Body Sensations Interpretation Questionnaire (Clark et al., 1997), but all items were developed or modified specifically for the ADSIQ. See Section S1.2 for the questionnaire in full. The mean of the threat interpretation items for anxiety sensation situations and the mean of the threat interpretation items for depersonalization sensation situations were used in analyses. Internal consistency was good for the anxiety sensations items, ωt = .84, 95% CI = [.81, .87], and for the depersonalization sensations items, ωt = 0.87, 95% CI = [.85, .90]. See Table 2 for descriptive statistics of all trait scale scores.

Conditions

We are aware of no task that induces both state anxiety and state depersonalization that would be feasible and ethical to administer in an online format, so we chose to use two separate symptom inductions: one for state anxiety and one for state depersonalization. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions to control for potential order effects of the inductions. Participants in Induction Order 1 completed the depersonalization induction first and the anxiety induction second. Those in Induction Order 2 completed the anxiety induction first and the depersonalization induction second.

Anxiety Induction

Participants were instructed to pick an upcoming situation they expected would make them feel anxious, worried, or scared and to describe the situation in a few words. Then, for 45 seconds, participants were instructed to imagine the situation as vividly as possible, playing out specific parts of the situation in their heads and imagining what they would see hear, smell, think, and feel in that situation. After the 45 seconds, a button appeared allowing participants to proceed. The induction was adapted from prior online research on anxiety (Eberle et al., 2021; Ji et al., 2020). See Section S1.3 for the induction instructions in full.

Depersonalization Induction

Participants were instructed to position themselves a comfortable distance from their computer and stare intently at a dot on their screen for three minutes. The instructions informed participants that they may blink, but that they should try their best to not look away from the dot. A 3-minute countdown timer—intended to improve compliance by updating in participants’ peripheral vision and assuring them the task was progressing—was visible. After 3 minutes, a button appeared allowing participants to proceed. The induction was adapted from Miller et al. (1994), who found that 3 minutes of dot-staring increased self-reported state depersonalization in participants with panic disorder and (to a lesser extent) in non-anxious control participants. See Section S1.4 for the induction instructions in full.

Procedure

Participants provided written informed consent and answered questions about demographic characteristics. Next, they completed the state-level measures, their first symptom induction, the state-level measures a second time, their second symptom induction, and the state-level measures a third and final time. Participants were then guided through a 30-second diaphragmatic breathing exercise intended to return them closer to a physiological baseline. Following the breathing exercise, participants completed a battery of trait questionnaires (PHQ-4, CDS-2, ASI-3, DTS-SF, and ADSIQ) in randomized order. Finally, participants were debriefed and paid. The University of Virginia Institutional Review Board approved all procedures.

Statistical Analysis

State anxiety, state depersonalization/derealization, state negative valence, and state arousal were each measured three times per participant: immediately before the first induction (Time 0), between the first and second induction (Time 1), and after the second induction (Time 2). All hypotheses involving the state scores were tested with linear mixed-effects models fitted using the lme4 package (ver. 1.1–23; Bates et al., 2015) in R. All simple and multiple linear regression models were tested with the stats package (ver. 3.6.2) in R. All significance tests are two-tailed with an alpha level of .05. See Section S1.5 for a description of how data were transformed to fit model assumptions. The analytic plan and hypotheses were preregistered prior to data collection (https://osf.io/xgazd), and data and analysis scripts are publicly available (https://osf.io/j486g). Missing data were handled by calculating the mean of available items or by listwise deletion if no items were available. See Section S1.6 for further detail.

Hypothesis A

State anxiety and depersonalization are related sensations, so we first tested whether it was necessary to separate state anxiety from depersonalization scores in our manipulation checks for the two symptom inductions. To do this, we used a mixed-effects model for each induction that tested the effect of time on state outcome (including state anxiety and state depersonalization in the same model). We entered time (0 = pre-induction or 1 = post-induction), outcome (state anxiety or state depersonalization), Time × Outcome, and induction order (Induction Order 1 or Induction Order 2) as fixed effects, and we entered a random intercept for each participant. Time × Outcome was significant for both inductions, suggesting that the inductions had differential effects on state depersonalization versus state anxiety. Thus, we separated state anxiety from state depersonalization scores in our manipulation checks. To isolate the effects of time for each level of outcome in the combined models described above, we used the method and online tool (quantpsy.org/interact/hlm2.htm) developed by Preacher et al. (2006). Finally, we used four mixed-effects models to test the hypothesized increases in state negative valence and arousal—one model per outcome per induction. We entered time and induction order as fixed effects, and we entered a random intercept for each participant.

Hypothesis B

We used mixed-effects modeling to test the effect of state anxiety on state depersonalization. We computed level 1 (person-mean-centered) and level 2 (person mean) variables for each participant’s state anxiety scores to fully disaggregate within-person from between-person effects.4 State anxiety (person-mean-centered), state anxiety person mean, and time (coded as 0, 1, or 2) were entered as fixed effects, and we entered a random intercept for each participant.5 The state anxiety (person-mean-centered) term was the effect of interest. We used simple linear regression to test the effect of trait anxiety on trait depersonalization.

Hypothesis C

We used mixed-effects modeling to test the moderating role of each of the proposed trait moderators (anxiety sensitivity, distress intolerance, negative interpretation bias for anxiety sensations, and negative interpretation bias for depersonalization sensations) on the effect of state anxiety on state depersonalization. A separate model was constructed for each moderator. We entered state anxiety (person-mean-centered), state anxiety person mean, moderator, State Anxiety (person-mean-centered) × Moderator, and time (coded as 0, 1, or 2) as fixed effects, and we entered a random intercept for each participant. The State Anxiety (person-mean-centered) × Moderator interaction was the effect of interest. We used multiple linear regression to test the moderating role of each of the four proposed trait moderators on the effect of trait anxiety on trait depersonalization. Trait anxiety, moderator, and Trait Anxiety × Moderator were entered as predictors. The Trait Anxiety × Moderator interaction was the effect of interest.

Results

Hypothesis A: Effects of Inductions on State Outcomes

The depersonalization induction and the anxiety induction each had differential effects on state depersonalization versus state anxiety, β = 0.18, SE = 0.04, t(906.00) = 4.25, p < .001, 95% CI [0.09, 0.26]; β = −0.25, SE = 0.04, t(906.00) = −6.35, p < .001, 95% CI [−0.33, −0.17]; respectively. Therefore, for our manipulation checks, we estimated the effect of each induction on state anxiety separately from state depersonalization. As expected, the depersonalization induction increased state depersonalization on average, β = 0.07, p = .024, 95% CI [0.01, 0.12], but—counter to hypotheses—decreased state anxiety on average, β = −0.11, p < .001, 95% CI [−0.17, −0.05]. The anxiety induction increased both state anxiety, β = 0.58, p < .001, 95% CI [0.52, 0.63], and depersonalization, β = 0.32, p < .001, 95% CI [0.27, 0.38], on average, as hypothesized. In the tests of the remaining hypothesized increases in state outcomes, the depersonalization induction had no significant effect on state negative valence and—counter to hypotheses—decreased state arousal on average, β = −0.13, p < .001, 95% CI [−0.19, −0.06]. Finally, the anxiety induction increased both negative valence, β = 0.44, p < .001, 95% CI [0.38, 0.50], and arousal, β = 0.51, p < .001, 95% CI [0.45, 0.57], on average, as expected (Table 3).

Table 3.

Hypothesis A: Effects of Time on State Outcomes

Induction State Outcome β (SE) df t p 95% CI

Depersonalization Induction Anxiety −0.11 (0.03) 906.00 −3.76 <.001*** [−0.17, −0.05]
Depersonalization 0.07 (0.03) 906.00 2.26 .024* [0.01, 0.12]
Negative Valence 0.01 (0.03) 302.00 0.45 .654 [−0.05, 0.08]
Arousal −0.13 (0.03) 302.00 −3.91 <.001*** [−0.19, −0.06]
Anxiety Induction Anxiety 0.58 (0.03) 906.00 20.63 <.001*** [0.52, 0.63]
Depersonalization 0.33 (0.03) 906.00 11.65 <.001*** [0.27, 0.38]
Negative Valence 0.44 (0.03) 302.00 14.89 <.001*** [0.38, 0.50]
Arousal 0.51 (0.03) 302.00 15.93 <.001*** [0.45, 0.57]

Note. Time was coded as either 0 = immediately before induction or 1 = immediately after induction.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

Hypothesis B: Effects of State/Trait Anxiety on State/Trait Depersonalization

As hypothesized, state anxiety scores (person-mean-centered) positively predicted state depersonalization scores, β = 0.43, SE = 0.02, t(604.00) = 19.46, p < .001, 95% CI [0.39, 0.47]. The relationship was mirrored on the trait level, β = 0.60, SE = 0.05, t(300) = 13.16, p < .001, 95% CI [0.51, 0.70].6

Hypothesis C: Moderation of Anxiety-Depersonalization Relationship at State and Trait Levels

None of the hypothesized trait moderators significantly impacted the relation between state anxiety scores and state depersonalization scores (see Table 4). Trait-level analyses, however, revealed that, as hypothesized, the trait anxiety-trait depersonalization relationship was strengthened by greater anxiety sensitivity, β = 0.25, p < .001, 95% CI [0.17, 0.34]; distress intolerance, β = 0.15, p = .004, 95% CI [0.05, 0.25]; negative interpretation bias for anxiety sensations,7 β = −0.21, p < .001, 95% CI [−0.30, −0.13]; and negative interpretation bias for depersonalization sensations,8 β = −0.27, p < .001, 95% CI [−0.35, −0.19] (see Table 4). Figure S3 shows how the strength of the trait anxiety-trait depersonalization relation varies for different levels of anxiety sensitivity, a pattern generally consistent across the other three moderators.

Table 4.

Hypothesis C: Moderation of Anxiety-Depersonalization Relationship at State and Trait Levels

Trait Moderator β (SE) df t p 95% CI

State Level

Anxiety Sensitivity 0.03 (0.02) 601.00 1.48 .139 [−0.01, 0.06]
Distress Intolerance 0.02 (0.02) 601.00 0.92 .357 [−0.02, 0.05]
Negative Interpretation Bias for Anxiety Sensationsa 0.00 (0.02) 603.00 −0.23 .819 [−0.04, 0.03]
Negative Interpretation Bias for Depersonalization Sensationsa 0.00 (0.02) 603.00 0.16 .876 [−0.03, 0.04]

Trait Level

Anxiety Sensitivity 0.25 (0.04) 298 5.70 <.001*** [0.17, 0.34]
Distress Intolerance 0.15 (0.05) 298 2.92 .004** [0.05, 0.25]
Negative Interpretation Bias for Anxiety Sensationsa −0.21 (0.04) 298 −4.79 <.001*** [−0.30, −0.13]
Negative Interpretation Bias for Depersonalization Sensationsa −0.27 (0.04) 298 −6.54 <.001*** [−0.35, −0.19]

Note. Effects on state depersonalization were estimated with mixed-effects models. Effects on trait depersonalization were estimated with multiple linear regression models.

a

Negative Interpretation Bias for Anxiety Sensations and Negative Interpretation Bias for Depersonalization Sensations were inverse-transformed to meet model assumptions. Therefore, negative values for β suggest positive effects of Trait Anxiety × Negative Interpretation Bias for Anxiety Sensations and Trait Anxiety × Negative Interpretation Bias for Depersonalization Sensations on Trait Depersonalization.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

Discussion

The present study investigated trait moderators of the relationships between state and trait anxiety and depersonalization to better understand their co-occurrence and to identify potential points of intervention. As hypothesized, the depersonalization induction increased state depersonalization, but—unexpectedly—decreased state anxiety and arousal, and had no significant effect on state negative valence. The anxiety induction increased all of these state scores as hypothesized. As anticipated, anxiety positively predicted depersonalization at both a state and trait level, and the trait anxiety-trait depersonalization relationship was strengthened by greater anxiety sensitivity, distress intolerance, and negative interpretation bias for anxiety sensations and for depersonalization sensations. There were no significant effects of the moderators on the state anxiety-state depersonalization relationship.

For years, theorists have posited that depersonalization—or dissociation in general—is an evolution-prepared defense mechanism meant to make intense stressors more tolerable (e.g., Boysan, 2014; Griffin et al., 1997; Sierra, 2009; Snyder, 1999; Zeidner & Endler, 1996). This protective model of depersonalization would imply that individual differences in the severity of depersonalization symptoms at a given intensity of distress depend on each person’s threshold for emotional distress, which we operationalized as distress tolerance (measured by the DTS-SF). In our data, lower distress tolerance strengthened the relationship between trait anxiety and trait depersonalization, which lends additional empirical support for the protective model of depersonalization. It is worth noting, though, that in cases of chronic depersonalization (as in depersonalization/derealization disorder), this model of depersonalization might be less applicable, as there is often no clear psychological stressor from which the constant depersonalization could be protecting (Dell, 2011). Further, distress tolerance was not manipulated, so causal inferences about its role are not possible with this design.

Hunter et al. (2003) proposed a cognitive-behavioral model of depersonalization, which contends that threat interpretations for depersonalization symptoms lead to elevated anxiety and, therefore, an increase in depersonalization symptoms, fueling the anxiety-depersonalization cycle. The present study tested aspects of this cognitive-behavioral model (with a larger sample size than Hunter et al., 2014) and also tested an extension of the model: that threat interpretations of anxiety symptoms (not just depersonalization symptoms) might also feed into the anxiety-depersonalization cycle, by—presumably—increasing levels of subsequent anxiety, which would in turn increase the severity of depersonalization symptoms. In our data, individuals who reported more catastrophic interpretations for anxiety symptoms and depersonalization symptoms had a stronger trait anxiety-trait depersonalization relationship. The relationship was also strengthened by higher levels of anxiety sensitivity. The present study therefore provides further empirical evidence for the cognitive-behavioral model of depersonalization and suggests that threat interpretations and fear of anxiety sensations also feed the anxiety-depersonalization cycle.

The protective and cognitive-behavioral models of depersonalization are not incompatible, and developing an integrated model might be warranted. Such a model would suggest that anxiety, trauma, and stress lead to symptoms of depersonalization, especially for individuals who stand to benefit most from the protective function of depersonalization (i.e., people with low distress tolerance). Additionally, individuals high in anxiety sensitivity and prone to negative interpretation bias for their original anxiety symptoms would experience an elevated anxiety response as they catastrophically misinterpret what their anxiety symptoms mean, thereby increasing their likelihood for experiencing symptoms of depersonalization. Once depersonalization symptoms occur, those prone to negative interpretation bias for their depersonalization symptoms would experience an increase in subsequent anxiety symptoms as they catastrophically misinterpret what their depersonalization symptoms mean. The process would start again, with these elevated anxiety levels.

Few studies to date have investigated state depersonalization. One reason for this may be the lack of a validated state depersonalization questionnaire, leading researchers to adapt pre-existing trait measures of depersonalization for an in-the-moment timescale (e.g., Hoyer et al., 2013). To increase variability in symptoms (which we expected to be too low and invariant at baseline for the statistical models), participants in the present study completed mild symptom induction tasks. The anxiety induction increased state anxiety, depersonalization, negative valence, and arousal, as hypothesized. The depersonalization induction yielded more unexpected effects. It increased state depersonalization slightly, as predicted, but significantly decreased state anxiety and arousal, counter to hypotheses. The induction results may suggest that change in state anxiety more strongly predicts change in state depersonalization than the reverse. This inference must be made with considerable caution, however, given the nature of the symptom inductions, which were intentionally low intensity, because the study was conducted online and remotely. Additionally, demand characteristics might have had differential confounding effects on state outcomes. That is, it is likely that participants would recognize the anxious imagery activity was meant to raise anxiety, and less likely that participants would recognize the dot-staring activity was meant to make them feel more “detached/unreal.”

Further research is needed to determine the nature of the anxiety-depersonalization relationship at the state level and why the factors that moderated the trait-level relationship had nonsignificant effects on the state-level relationship. It could be that the trait moderators tested are only risk-factors for more long-term, pathological co-occurring anxiety and depersonalization, and have little effect on their co-occurrence in a given moment. Alternatively, the mismatch between the hypothesized trait-level moderators and state-level symptom measures may have reduced the power of analyses. Future studies could examine whether state-level appraisals (i.e., how people are actually interpreting their symptoms in a given moment) rather than trait-level tendencies to interpret hypothetical sensations in a threatening or benign way (what was captured by the ADSIQ) impact the state anxiety-state depersonalization relationship.

Limitations and Future Research Directions

Limitations of the symptom induction tasks and study design may have also contributed to the nonsignificant state-level results. Given its relatively weak effects on state outcomes, the inclusion of the dot-staring task may have limited the power of analyses. More generally, the low-intensity symptom induction tasks may not have produced enough variability in state symptoms for the moderation analyses to reveal significant effects, especially in an unselected sample. Future research might benefit from trying different methods of inducing state symptoms or using a single and more potent induction task, such as hyperventilation, which—according to results from a pilot study—may increase both state anxiety and depersonalization (Lickel et al., 2008). Additionally, future researchers may want to use longer versions of questionnaires for trait anxiety and depersonalization to examine the robustness of the present study’s results with more comprehensive measures. Finally, the ADSIQ requires further validation as a tool for measuring threat interpretations of anxiety and depersonalization sensations.

Conclusion

Although clinical implications from this study are necessarily limited by the use of an unselected sample, the present study raises interesting possibilities for future tests of possible intervention targets with clinical samples. First, distress tolerance training (Linehan, 1993, 2015) might help to disrupt the anxiety-depersonalization cycle. Second, training more benign interpretations of depersonalization symptoms, as suggested by Hunter et al. (2003), along with Anxiety Sensitivity Amelioration Training (Schmidt et al., 2007) or cognitive bias modification to train benign interpretations of anxiety sensitivity symptoms (e.g., Steinman & Teachman, 2010) might improve outcomes when anxiety and depersonalization co-occur. As research grows on depersonalization, a historically treatment-resistant symptom, it will be important to consider its close relationship with anxiety and ways their co-occurrence may affect the course of treatment. The current results suggest a series of moderating factors that make co-occurrence more likely.

Supplementary Material

Supp 1

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Program for Anxiety, Cognition, and Treatment at the University of Virginia for their support and feedback throughout the study and Gus Sjobeck for his help with statistical analyses. This research was supported by a grant awarded to the first author from the University of Virginia’s Harrison Undergraduate Research Awards program and a NIMH R01MH113752 grant awarded to the third author. Preregistration for the study can be found at https://osf.io/xgazd, and data and analysis scripts can be found at https://osf.io/j486g. The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

1

We know of no evidence that distinguishes between individuals with predominant depersonalization versus derealization (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), and many authors do not regard the two as separate constructs (Radovic & Radovic, 2002). Therefore, for brevity, we use depersonalization to describe the range of low-intensity dissociative experiences that encompass feelings of unreality with respect to oneself and/or one’s external world.

2

For example, greater anxiety sensitivity and lesser distress tolerance confer higher risk for anxiety (Allan et al., 2014; Keough et al., 2010; McNally, 2002; Michel et al., 2016). Negative interpretation bias is associated with more severe symptoms in social anxiety (Chen et al., 2020), panic disorder (Teachman et al., 2007), and generalized anxiety disorder (Hayes et al., 2010). Depersonalization has been linked to higher anxiety sensitivity and interpretation bias (Schweden et al., 2018; Weiner & McKay, 2013).

3

Internal consistency was calculated based on McDonald’s omega total, as recommended by methodologists (Dunn et al., 2014). Calculations were done using the MBESS package (ver. 4.7.0; Kelley, 2007) in R (ver. 3.5.2).

4

This decision, based on Wang and Maxwell (2015), deviates from the preregistration and reflects the authors’ more advanced statistical training since publishing the preregistration.

5

Adding only a random intercept (no random slope for time) for each participant also deviates from the preregistration and was done across all mixed-effects models to eliminate convergence and singularity warnings.

6

Had we used the natural logarithm of trait anxiety scores (instead of untransformed trait anxiety scores) for this model, assumptions of linearity and dependent variable continuousness would have been violated, though the predicted effect of trait anxiety on trait depersonalization would have still held, β = 0.59, SE = 0.05, t(300) = 12.7, p < .001, 95% CI [0.50, 0.68].

7

We inverse-transformed scores on the anxiety sensations subscale of the ADSIQ prior to standardizing to meet model assumptions. Therefore, the negative β suggests that greater negative interpretation bias for anxiety sensations strengthens the trait anxiety-trait depersonalization relationship.

8

The above is also true for scores on the depersonalization sensations subscale of the ADSIQ, meaning the negative β suggests that greater negative interpretation bias for depersonalization sensations also strengthens the trait anxiety-trait depersonalization relationship.

References

  1. Aderibigbe YA, Bloch RM, & Walker WR (2001). Prevalence of depersonalization and derealization experiences in a rural population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 36(2), 63–69. 10.1007/s001270050291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allan NP, Macatee RJ, Norr AM, & Schmidt NB (2014). Direct and Interactive Effects of Distress Tolerance and Anxiety Sensitivity on Generalized Anxiety and Depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(5), 530–540. 10.1007/s10608-014-9623-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, & Walker S (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boysan M (2014). Dissociative experiences are associated with obsessive-compulsive symptoms in a non-clinical sample: A latent profile analysis. Archives of Neuropsychiatry, 51, 253–262. 10.4274/npa.y6884 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chen J, Short M, & Kemps E (2020). Interpretation bias in social anxiety: A systematic review and meta-analysis. In Journal of Affective Disorders (Vol. 276, pp. 1119–1130). Elsevier B.V. 10.1016/j.jad.2020.07.121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Clark DM, Salkovskis PM, Ost LG, Breitholtz E, Koehler KA, Westling BE, Jeavons A, & Gelder M (1997). Misinterpretation of body sensations in panic disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(2), 203–213. 10.1037//0022-006x.65.2.203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dell PF (2011). Depersonalization: A New Look at a Neglected Syndrome, by M. Sierra. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 12(4), 401–403. 10.1080/15299732.2011.573763 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dunn TJ, Baguley T, & Brunsden V (2014). From alpha to omega: A practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. British Journal of Psychology, 105(3), 399–412. 10.1111/bjop.12046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Eberle JW, Daniel KE, Baee S, Behan HC, Silverman AL, Calicho-Mamani C, Baglione AN, Werntz A, French NJ, Ji JL, Hohensee N, Tong X, Boukhechba M, Funk DH, Barnes LE, & Teachman BA (2021). Web-based interpretation training to reduce anxiety: A sequential multiple-assignment randomized trial. Manuscript in Preparation. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Garner LE, Van Kirk N, Tifft ED, Krompinger JW, Mathes BM, Fraire M, Falkenstein MJ, Brennan BP, Crosby JM, & Elias JA (2018). Validation of the distress tolerance scale-short form in obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74(6), 916–925. 10.1002/jclp.22554 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Griffin MG, Resick PA, & Mechanic MB (1997). Objective assessment of peritraumatic dissociation: Psychophysiological indicators. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(8), 1081–1088. 10.1176/ajp.154.8.1081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hayes S, Hirsch CR, Krebs G, & Mathews A (2010). The effects of modifying interpretation bias on worry in generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(3), 171–178. 10.1016/j.brat.2009.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hoyer J, Braeuer D, Crawcour S, Klumbies E, & Kirschbaum C (2013). Depersonalization/derealization during acute social stress in social phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27(2), 178–187. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hunter ECM, Phillips ML, Chalder T, Sierra M, & David AS (2003). Depersonalisation disorder: A cognitive-behavioural conceptualisation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(12), 1451–1467. 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00066-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hunter ECM, Salkovskis PM, & David AS (2014). Attributions, appraisals and attention for symptoms in depersonalisation disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 53(1), 20–29. 10.1016/j.brat.2013.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hunter ECM, Sierra M, & David AS (2004). The epidemiology of depersonalisation and derealisation - A systematic review. In Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (Vol. 39, Issue 1, pp. 9–18). 10.1007/s00127-004-0701-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ji JL, Baee S, Zhang D, Meyer MJ, Funk D, Portnow S, Barnes L, & Teachman BA (2020). Multi-session online interpretation bias training for anxiety in a community sample. Manuscript Submitted for Publication. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kelley K (2007). Methods for the behavioral, educational, and social sciences: An R package. Behavior Research Methods, 39(4), 979–984. 10.3758/BF03192993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kennedy R, Clifford S, Burleigh T, Waggoner PD, Jewell R, & Winter N (2018). The shape of and solutions to the MTurk quality crisis. 10.2139/ssrn.3272468 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Keough ME, Riccardi CJ, Timpano KR, Mitchell MA, & Schmidt NB (2010). Anxiety Symptomatology: The Association With Distress Tolerance and Anxiety Sensitivity. Behavior Therapy, 41(4), 567–574. 10.1016/j.beth.2010.04.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, & Williams JBW (2003). The patient health questionnaire-2: Validity of a two-item depression screener. Medical Care, 41(11), 1284–1292. 10.1097/01.MLR.0000093487.78664.3C [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, & Löwe B (2009). An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: The PHQ-4. Psychosomatics, 50(6), 613–621. 10.1176/appi.psy.50.6.613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Monahan PO, & Löwe B (2007). Anxiety disorders in primary care: Prevalence, impairment, comorbidity, and detection. Annals of Internal Medicine, 146(5), 317–325. 10.7326/0003-4819-146-5-200703060-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Leyro TM, Zvolensky MJ, & Bernstein A (2010). Distress Tolerance and Psychopathological Symptoms and Disorders: A Review of the Empirical Literature Among Adults. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 576–600. 10.1037/a0019712 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lickel J, Nelson E, Lickel AH, & Deacon B (2008). Interoceptive Exposure Exercises for Evoking Depersonalization and Derealization: A Pilot Study. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 22(4), 321–330. 10.1891/0889-8391.22.4.321 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Linehan MM (1993). Diagnosis and treatment of mental disorders. Skills training manual for treating borderline personality disorder. Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  28. Linehan MM (2015). DBT Skills Training Manual (2nd ed.). Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  29. Löwe B, Kroenke K, & Gräfe K (2005). Detecting and monitoring depression with a two-item questionnaire (PHQ-2). Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 58(2), 163–171. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Marks MP, Başoǧlu M, Alkubaisy T, Şengün S, & Marks IM (1991). Are anxiety symptoms and catastrophic cognitions directly related? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 5(3), 247–254. 10.1016/0887-6185(91)90005-E [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. McNally RJ (2002). Anxiety sensitivity and panic disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 52(10), 938–946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Merikangas KR, Jin R, He JP, Kessler RC, Lee S, Sampson NA, Viana MC, Andrade LH, Hu C, Karam EG, Ladea M, Medina-Mora ME, Ono Y, Posada-Villa J, Sagar R, Wells JE, & Zarkov Z (2011). Prevalence and correlates of bipolar spectrum disorder in the World Mental Health Survey Initiative. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(3), 241–251. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Michal M, Adler J, Wiltink J, Reiner I, Tschan R, Wölfling K, Weimert S, Tuin I, Subic-Wrana C, Beutel ME, & Zwerenz R (2016). A case series of 223 patients with depersonalization-derealization syndrome. BMC Psychiatry, 16(1), 203. 10.1186/s12888-016-0908-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Michal M, Kaufhold J, Grabhorn R, Krakow K, Overbeck G, & Heidenreich T (2005). Depersonalization and social anxiety. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 193(9), 629–632. 10.1097/01.nmd.0000178038.87332.ec [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Michal M, Wiltink J, Subic-Wrana C, Zwerenz R, Tuin I, Lichy M, Brähler E, & Beutel ME (2009). Prevalence, correlates, and predictors of depersonalization experiences in the German general population. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 197(7), 499–506. 10.1097/nmd.0b013e3181aacd94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Michal M, Wiltink J, Till Y, Wild PS, Blettner M, & Beutel ME (2011). Distinctiveness and overlap of depersonalization with anxiety and depression in a community sample: Results from the Gutenberg Heart Study. Psychiatry Research, 188(2), 264–268. 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Michal M, Zwerenz R, Tschan R, Edinger J, Lichy M, Knebel A, Tuin I, & Beutel M (2010). Screening nach Depersonalisation-Derealisation mittels zweier Items der Cambridge Depersonalisation Scale. PPmP Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie, 60(5), 175–179. 10.1055/s-0029-1224098 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Michel NM, Rowa K, Young L, & McCabe RE (2016). Emotional distress tolerance across anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 40, 94–103. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.04.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Miller PP, Brown TA, DiNardo PA, & Barlow DH (1994). The experimental induction of depersonalization and derealization in panic disorder and nonanxious subjects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(5), 511–519. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)90138-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Molenaar PCM (2004). A Manifesto on Psychology as Idiographic Science: Bringing the Person Back Into Scientific Psychology, This Time Forever. Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research & Perspective, 2(4), 201–218. 10.1207/s15366359mea0204_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Olthuis JV, Stewart SH, Watt MC, Sabourin BC, Keogh E, Olthuis JV, Stewart SH, Watt MC, Sabourin BC, & Keogh E (2012). Anxiety sensitivity and negative interpretation biases: Their shared and unique associations with anxiety symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 34, 332–342. 10.1007/s10862-012-9286-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Preacher KJ, Curran PJ, & Bauer DJ (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31(4), 437–448. 10.3102/10769986031004437 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Radovic F, & Radovic S (2002). Feelings of unreality: A conceptual and phenomenological analysis of the language of depersonalization. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 9(3), 271–279. 10.1353/ppp.2003.0048 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Roth M (1960). The phobic anxiety-depersonalization syndrome and some general aetiological problems in psychiatry. Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 1, 293–306. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14439416 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Schmidt NB, Eggleston AM, Woolaway-Bickel K, Fitzpatrick KK, Vasey MW, & Richey JA (2007). Anxiety Sensitivity Amelioration Training (ASAT): A longitudinal primary prevention program targeting cognitive vulnerability. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21(3), 302–319. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schweden TLK, Pittig A, Bräuer D, Klumbies E, Kirschbaum C, & Hoyer J (2016). Reduction of depersonalization during social stress through cognitive therapy for social anxiety disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 43, 99–105. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schweden TLK, Wolfradt U, Jahnke S, & Hoyer J (2018). Depersonalization under Academic Stress: Frequency, Predictors, and Consequences. Psychopathology, 51(4), 252–261. 10.1159/000489468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Segui J, Marquez M, Garcia L, Canet J, Salvador-Carulla L, & Ortiz M (2000). Depersonalization in panic disorder: A clinical study. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 41(3), 172–178. 10.1016/S0010-440X(00)90044-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Shilony E, & Grossman FK (1993). Depersonalization as a defense mechanism in survivors of trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6(1), 119–128. 10.1007/BF02093366 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sierra M (2009). Depersonalization: A New Look at a Neglected Syndrome. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  51. Sierra M, Medford N, Wyatt G, & David AS (2012). Depersonalization disorder and anxiety: A special relationship? Psychiatry Research, 197(1–2), 123–127. 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sierra M, Senior C, Dalton J, McDonough M, Bond A, Phillips ML, O’Dwyer AM, & David AS (2002). Autonomic response in depersonalization disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 833–838. 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.833 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Simeon D, Gross S, Guralnik O, Stein DJ, Schmeidler J, & Hollander E (1997). Feeling unreal: 30 cases of DSM-III-R depersonalization disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(8), 1107–1113. 10.1176/ajp.154.8.1107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Simeon D, Riggio-Rosen A, Guralnik O, Knutelska M, & Nelson D (2003). Depersonalization disorder: Dissociation and affect. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 4(4), 63–76. 10.1300/J229v04n04_05 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Snyder CR (Ed.). (1999). Coping: The psychology of what works. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  56. Steinman SA, & Teachman BA (2010). Modifying interpretations among individuals high in anxiety sensitivity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(1), 71–78. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.08.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Taylor S, Zvolensky MJ, Cox BJ, Deacon B, Heimberg RG, Ledley DR, Abramowitz JS, Holaway RM, Sandin B, Stewart SH, Coles M, Eng W, Daly ES, Arrindell WA, Bouvard M, & Cardenas SJ (2007). Robust Dimensions of Anxiety Sensitivity: Development and Initial Validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychological Assessment, 19(2), 176–188. 10.1037/1040-3590.19.2.176 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Teachman BA, Smith-Janik SB, & Saporito J (2007). Information processing biases and panic disorder: Relationships among cognitive and symptom measures. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(8), 1791–1811. 10.1016/j.brat.2007.01.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Torch EM (1978). Review of the relationship between obsession and depersonalization. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 58(2), 191–198. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1978.tb06931.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Trueman D (1984a). Anxiety and depersonalization and derealization experiences. Psychological Reports, 54(1), 91–96. 10.2466/pr0.1984.54.1.91 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Trueman D (1984b). Depersonalization in a nonclinical population. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 116(1), 107–112. 10.1080/00223980.1984.9923624 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Wang LP, & Maxwell SE (2015). On disaggregating between-person and within-person effects with longitudinal data using multilevel models. Psychological Methods, 20(1), 63–83. 10.1037/met0000030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Weiner E, & McKay D (2013). A Preliminary Evaluation of Repeated Exposure for Depersonalization and Derealization. Behavior Modification, 37(2), 226–242. 10.1177/0145445512461651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Zeidner M, & Endler NS (Eds.). (1996). Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applications. John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp 1

RESOURCES