Abstract
Neural synchrony in brain circuits is the mainstay of cognition, including memory processes. Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder that disrupts neural synchrony in specific circuits, associated with memory dysfunction before a substantial neural loss. Recognition memory impairment is a prominent cognitive symptom in the early stages of AD. The entorhinal–hippocampal circuit is critically engaged in recognition memory and is known as one of the earliest circuits involved due to AD pathology. Notably, the olfactory bulb is closely connected with the entorhinal–hippocampal circuit and is suggested as one of the earliest regions affected by AD. Therefore, we recorded simultaneous local field potential from the olfactory bulb (OB), entorhinal cortex (EC), and dorsal hippocampus (dHPC) to explore the functional connectivity in the OB-EC-dHPC circuit during novel object recognition (NOR) task performance in a rat model of AD. Animals that received amyloid-beta (Aβ) showed a significant impairment in task performance and a marked reduction in OB survived cells. We revealed that Aβ reduced coherence and synchrony in the OB-EC-dHPC circuit at theta and gamma bands during NOR performance. Importantly, our results exhibit that disrupted functional connectivity in the OB-EC-dHPC circuit was correlated with impaired recognition memory induced by Aβ. These findings can elucidate dynamic changes in neural activities underlying AD, helping to find novel diagnostic and therapeutic targets.
Subject terms: Alzheimer's disease, Learning and memory, Neural circuits, Neuroscience, Olfactory bulb
Introduction
Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disease and one of the most common causes of dementia with devastating morbidity and mortality1. AD is characterized by escalating cognitive decline, commonly anterograde amnestic cognitive impairments. Patients present with varying manifestations depending on the disease stage and primarily affected structures2. However, AD patients frequently present with deficits in recognition memory3, which notably can occur in prodromal stages as an early cognitive symptom4,5. Therefore, altered recognition memory is potentially associated with changes in regions' activities earlier than significant changes in volume or architecture, which could serve as biomarkers.
Several hypotheses have been introduced to explain the etiopathogenesis and clinical course of AD. The amyloid hypothesis is a widely accepted explanation based on extracellular aggregation and deposition of amyloid-beta peptides (Aβ) as a causal factor in AD6. The Aβ interferes with neuronal homeostasis, causes synaptic dysfunction and dysregulated neural excitability7, and triggers other pathological events8,9, leading to cognitive impairments. Compelling evidence demonstrated that cognitive impairment in AD might not be related to neuronal degeneration alone but also occur due to deficits in synchronized neural activities10,11. However, little is known about neural connectivity in circuits related to recognition memory.
Organized communications and information transfer during memory processes in both local circuits and distant networks in the brain are allocated by synchronized rhythmic activities10. AD is associated with significantly disrupted connectivity in such neuronal communications12, which can be present from early preclinical stages before progression to overt Alzheimer's dementia13. Notably, AD tends to involve specific neural assemblies and networks that are more vulnerable to Aβ aggregates14. Two structures that are proposed affected in AD early stages are the entorhinal cortex (EC)15 and hippocampus (HPC)16, both pivotal for proper memory functions, notably, object recognition17. Functional and structural changes in EC and HPC can be detected years before significant clinical symptoms, and therefore, suggested as potential biomarkers for AD diagnosis18.
Recent evidence indicates that olfactory network dysfunction may precede hippocampal formation damages19–21. Olfactory dysfunction is shown to be present as a disturbed sense of smell, proposed as an early sign of neurodegenerative diseases, including AD21. Functional changes in the olfactory system, particularly the olfactory bulb (OB), are shown in AD patients, with or even without smell problems21. Thus, OB can be a key candidate for investigating early functional changes in the brain induced by AD pathology.
Notably, the OB rhythmic oscillations, which are phase-locked to respiration, are global, propagate throughout the brain, coupled to fast cortical oscillations (e.g., gamma) and most importantly, synchronize distant regions' activities22–25. Therefore, OB could be considered a pivotal regulator of neural activity in the brain during cognitive performances26,27. On the other hand, the OB is closely connected with the EC-HPC circuit, which has synchronized oscillatory activity during spatial recognition22,28. Hence, exploring the OB network in recognition memory can provide useful information about dynamic functional connectivity alterations in AD.
The novel object recognition (NOR) task is widely accepted to evaluate memory deficits in AD animal models29. This task is based on the natural propensity of the animal to explore a new object, more than a familiar one, and does not concern spatial reference memory30. Given close structural and functional connections between OB and hippocampal formation, we hypothesized that the Aβ could disrupt OB-EC-HPC circuit connectivity in the AD rat model, resulting in recognition memory impairment. Therefore, to explore the circuit's connectivity in NOR performance, we simultaneously recorded local field potentials (LFPs) from OB, EC, and dorsal HPC (dHPC) during NOR task performance in an animal model of AD (Fig. 1 illustrates study protocol). This study can provide a better understanding of task-related dynamic circuit connectivity in AD, particularly in those regions that are more vulnerable to Aβ aggregates.
Results
Histopathological findings
Immunostaining of Aβ1–42 of EC and dHPC sections demonstrated a high level of Aβ plaques in Aβ animals compared to the saline group (Fig. 2). This result verifies our model induction. Moreover, we evaluated the pathology of Aβ as well as the number of survived cells in the OB subregions in both groups. As shown in Fig. 3, generally animals that received Aβ showed a significant increase in plaque accumulation. Our findings showed that Aβ pathology affected granule cell layer (GCL), glomerular layer (GLM), external plexiform layer (EPL), and mitral cell layer (MCL) containing glomerular, mitral, and tufted cells rather than the rest of the subregions. Similarly, the number of survived cells in the Aβ group is significantly lower than in the saline group, illustrating that the Aβ caused a degenerative condition in the OB. Focusing on subregions illustrates no significant changes across different subregions of OB in the Aβ group. Altogether, these results demonstrate that Aβ1-42 infusion results in Aβ plaque formation and neural loss, hallmarks of AD pathology in our region of interests (i.e., OB, EC, and dHPC), and confirm model induction.
Recognition memory performance
The ability to recognize a previously exposed entity can be used to determine memory alterations. Due to innate exploratory behavior, animals naturally tend to spend more time exploring a novel object than a familiar object, meaning animals remember a previously explored object31. The novel object recognition (NOR) evaluates an animal's behavior in exposure to a novel and a familiar object by assessing the discrimination index (DI)30. NOR can be used for evaluating different types of memory, including recognition memory.
Moreover, when the novel object was tested 50 min after training, we found that Aβ animals spent significantly less time exploring the novel object (mean ± SEM of time: 1.98 ± 0.23 s vs. 4.45 ± 0.61 s, p = 0.003) and more time exploring the familiar object than rats that received saline (average percentage of time spent for the novel object in Aβ and control animals was 54.94% and 78.76%, respectively. p = 0.015; mean ± SEM of DI in Aβ and control animals was 0.18 ± 0.05 and 0.42 ± 0.03, respectively, p = 0.001; Fig. 4). However, no significant changes were found for traveled distance in the open field box (mean ± SEM of total traveled distance in Aβ and control animals was 1459 ± 131.3 cm and 1627 ± 85.06 cm, respectively, p = 0.31), indicating locomotor activity was intact and not different among groups. These results showed that recognition memory is impaired in animals that received Aβ compared to the control group.
Aβ dropped coherence in OB-EC-dHPC circuit during NOR task performance
The EC-dHPC is a critical circuit for memory performance, particularly object recognition memory17,32. Accordingly, to explore whether Aβ affects the interaction between EC-dHPC during recognition memory, we examined the coherence of the circuit during NOR task performance (Fig. 5A–C). Our analysis showed animals that received Aβ had significantly lower coherence values in EC-dHPC at theta and gamma bands (p = 0.004 and p = 0.0014, respectively; Fig. 5D,E, upper sections) while exploring the novel object. On the other hand, OB sends axonal projection to the hippocampal formation areas, particularly to the EC33. Therefore, we evaluated the functional connectivity between OB and EC-dHPC during recognition memory performance. In Aβ animals, the coherence of OB with EC and dHPC was significantly reduced at both theta and gamma frequency bands (OB-EC: p = 0.04 and p = 0.001, respectively and OB-dHPC: p = 0.004 and p = 0.001, respectively; Fig. 5D,E middle and bottom sections). These results indicate that Aβ can disrupt the functional connectivity in the OB-EC-dHPC circuit, notably in theta and gamma bands.
We then assessed the correlation between recognition memory performance and coherence in the OB-EC-dHPC circuit to find a possible relationship between LFPs and the behavior (Fig. 5F). Accordingly, DI (as a measure of NOR performance accuracy) was positively correlated with EC-dHPC coherence for the saline group at theta and gamma (r = 0.79, p = 0.02, and r = 0.8, p = 0.02, respectively), and for Aβ animals at theta (r = 0.86, p = 0.02). In the OB-EC circuit, this correlation was significantly positive for the saline group at theta (r = 0.939, p = 0.009) and also for Aβ animals at both theta and gamma (r = 0.79, p = 0.03, and r = 0.67, p = 0.08, respectively). Finally, in the OB-dHPC circuit, the results showed a negative correlation for the saline group at theta (r = − 0.91, p = 0.01) and for Aβ animals in gamma (r = − 0.86, p = 0.03).
Synchrony in OB-EC-dHPC circuit during recognition memory performance
To address the effect of Aβ on signal synchrony, we explored the time-lagged cross-correlation in the OB-EC-dHPC circuit. During NOR performance, Aβ animals exhibited a significant drop of cross-correlation at both theta and gamma bands in EC-dHPC (p = 0.048 and p = 0.025, respectively), OB-EC (p = 0.043 and p = 0.047, respectively), and OB-dHPC (p = 0.012 and p = 0.006, respectively) circuits (Fig. 6A,B). We then computed the lag of information influx in this circuit. Similarly, our analyses indicated a significant reduction in the lag of information influx from OB to EC (p = 0.014) and EC to dHPC (p < 0.001) in Aβ animals compared to the control group. These results show that the Aβ impaired the amount and the direction of information influx.
Moreover, we found that NOR performance in Aβ animals was correlated with cross-correlation (referred to as correlation in Fig. 6C,D) in the OB-EC-dHPC circuit at theta and gamma frequency bands: In the EC-dHPC circuit, for Aβ animals, DI was negatively and positively correlated with cross-correlation at theta (r = − 0.72, p = 0.05) and gamma (r = 0.92, p = 0.01), respectively; for the saline group, this correlation was significantly positive at theta range (r = 0.84, p = 0.03). Regarding the OB-EC circuit, the correlation between NOR performance and cross-correlation was positive for both Aβ and saline groups at theta (r = 0.88, p = 0.009, and r = 0.67, p = 0.06, respectively) and for Aβ animals at gamma (r = 0.83, p = 0.02). In the OB-dHPC circuit, DI was correlated with cross-correlation positively for both Aβ and saline groups at theta and gamma (r = 0.83, p = 0.04, and r = 0.79, p = 0.02, respectively), and negatively for the Aβ group at gamma (r = − 0.91, p = 0.01). These observations suggested that Aβ strongly disrupts synchrony and direction of information influx within OB-EC-dHPC regions, which is correlated with NOR performance.
Discussion
The current study explored the OB-EC-dHPC circuit connectivity during the NOR task in the rat model of AD. The EC-dHPC is a critical circuit for object recognition17,32. Moreover, the OB is closely connected with hippocampal formation, particularly EC, a hub for the hippocampus28,33,34. Initially, we found that coherence and synchrony of the EC-dHPC circuit are decreased in Aβ animals compared to the control group at theta and gamma bands during NOR task performance. Further, we assessed the OB connectivity with the EC-dHPC circuit. Aβ, demonstrate impaired object recognition performance. Further, we found that Aβ reduced coherence and synchrony of OB-EC and OB-dHPC at theta and gamma frequencies during task performance. Notably, we revealed that disrupted functional connectivity in the OB-EC-dHPC circuit is correlated with recognition memory impairment induced by Aβ. Our results provide direct evidence regarding Aβ-induced synchrony disruption in the OB-EC-dHPC circuit during the NOR task, which is associated with impaired NOR.
Synchronized neural activities in brain circuits are essential for organized cognitive functions, which are affected in patients with AD10,13. These pathological events can be observed from the early stages of the disease, known as mild cognitive impairment (MCI)13,35. AD patients exhibit altered organized communication in both local and long-range circuits associated with cognitive deficits11. Several studies in these patients demonstrate changes in functional connectivity of oscillatory activities at resting-state and also during cognitive performances36,37. For instance, AD patients showed decreased global synchrony in alpha, beta, and gamma frequency bands during resting-state38. Also, in task-related neural synchrony, AD patients showed lower synchronization in alpha, and beta ranges activity during information maintenance of working memory39. This evidence demonstrated a disconnected state in the brain's networks occurring in AD11.
AD pathology has a propensity to alter the function and structure of specific brain regions14. For instance, change in hippocampal synchrony occurs in the prodromal stages of AD, which is proposed as an early biomarker for the disease13. Moreover, hypotrophic changes in EC and HPC can be detected in MCI patients, who progressed to Alzheimer's dementia40. In this line, the EC is known as one the earliest regions affected by AD41,42, and its dysfunction can be responsible for initial memory symptoms43. The EC is implicated in converging spatial and sensory information44, and alterations in its function due to AD may explain why these patients are showing deficits in recognition memory at preclinical stages45. Consistent with previous observations, the present study demonstrated that coherent oscillatory activity in EC-dHPC was correlated with NOR task performance. Also, we found that Aβ animals with impaired NOR performance showed remarkably lower coherence in this circuit at both theta and gamma ranges. This indicates coherent activity at low (theta) and high (gamma) frequencies between EC-dHPC is required for object recognition memory. In Aβ animals, we found disrupted oscillatory coupling at theta and gamma ranges between EC and dHPC associated with impaired object recognition.
Recent evidence suggests that pathologic and degenerative changes in OB may occur even before EC damage19,20. This is based on observations that hyposmia and anosmia occur in preclinical AD, before significant memory symptoms46,47, and are associated with olfactory network impairments48. Consequently, OB involvement can induce pathological changes in connected structures, including EC and HPC48.
Intact OB oscillations, notably in the gamma band, are required for odor-related processing and memory and learning processes49. The OB is closely connected with regions responsible for spatial coding and memory processes, such as EC via direct reciprocal connections33 and HPC via EC via the perforant pathway50. Therefore, changes in OB activities can influence EC and HPC functions. Our results illustrated that oscillatory coupling in OB-EC and OB-dHPC at theta and gamma range is required for NOR. Also, task performance was correlated with the circuit's synchronized activity at the theta and gamma range. Accordingly, Aβ animals in our study demonstrated disrupted synchrony in theta and gamma range in OB connection with EC-dHPC circuit during task performance. One paradoxical results of the current study were that in some frequency ranges a negative correlation between connectivity parameters and object recognition performance was found. Our findings were consistent with the former observation claiming that connectivity between brain circuits involving the hippocampus, rather than being directly associated with memory performance, is reflective of a ‘compensatory response for preserving optimal performance’51. Thus, animals with less memory function had employed more values of connectivity at least in some frequency ranges to improve the impaired function. We assumed that this effect may not only be restricted to the memory impairment model but also animals with intact memory performance could recruit the brain oscillation for cognitive performance. However further investigations are required to address how brain function gained neural oscillations.
It has been previously shown that soluble Aβ can impair OB activities52. The Aβ is a potential synaptic regulator, which acts as positive and negative regulators in presynaptic and postsynaptic activities, respectively53. Aβ accumulation can change the proportion of abnormally hypo- and hyperactive neurons in cortical circuits12, inducing synaptic decline52. On the other hand, it has been postulated that synaptic activity can modulate the Aβ levels, particularly in the interstitial fluid54. For instance, gamma oscillations are suggested to counteract amyloid load and AD-associated pathology by inducing neuronal and glial responses55. Moreover, interactions between subregions of OB cells play an important role in generating oscillations at a wide range of frequencies particularly theta and gamma bands56,57. For example, it has been shown that inhibitory activity in glomerular cells entrained slow oscillations which were temporally coupled with gamma range during odor sniffing58. On the other hand, principal cells within the GLM receive glutaminergic input from olfactory neurons. Besides, granule cells in the GCL exert lateral inhibition across larger distances neurons. Frequent sniffing induces slow inhibition in mitral and tufted cells59–62 in the EPL and MCL, resulting in faster, gamma range activities in the OB58 which can propagate to deep brain structures such as the ventral hippocampus and entorhinal cortex. Our investigation regarding Aβ pathology and the number of alive cells indicated that GLM, EPL and MCL layers containing glomerular, mitral, and tufted cells had dominantly affected by Aβ deposition. Taken together, the relationship between Aβ and OB dysfunction is reciprocal and not one-way: Aβ can impair OB activities, and this impairment can result in Aβ overproduction and accumulation63. Several mechanisms can potentially justify our observations regarding impaired neural synchrony induced by Aβ in the OB-EC-dHPC at low- and high-frequency ranges during the NOR task:
Firstly, OB rhythmic activity dynamically modulates other brain areas, notably the EC-dHPC, during a cognitive performance28. Thus, disrupted coupling due to Aβ in this circuit, probably via its direct effect on OB synapses and regardless of neural loss, may be responsible for impaired recognition memory. Reciprocally, EC damages induced by Aβ can disrupt the activity of OB21, which potentially exaggerates connectivity disruption in the circuit.
Secondly, OB has been known as a critical center for adult neurogenesis and acts as a hub in differentiating neural precursors for other brain regions64. Previously demonstrated that olfactory bulbectomy causes dendritic remodeling and altered synaptic plasticity in EC and HPC, resulting in decreased adult neurogenesis and spatial memory deficits65. The olfactory bulbectomy significantly reduced dendritic arborization in EC and reduced neural excitability66. Hence, disordered OB architecture and function can induce neuronal degeneration and hypotrophy in adjacent areas, including EC66 and HPC65. This observation corroborates our assumption that OB rhythmic activity is required for EC-dHPC function and plasticity. The OB-EC functional connectivity can potentially be responsible for reinforcing synaptic plasticity in the circuit, which can be disrupted by Aβ deposition.
Thirdly, specific structures in olfactory regions may act as a hub for propagating the proteinopathy in the neurodegenerative diseases to other areas, such as EC and HPC67. Aβ could behave prion-like and spread to adjacent areas via neural pathways68. Therefore, Aβ accumulation can spread to EC and then HPC, inducing pathological changes in these structures and functions.
It is noteworthy to mention that none of these mechanisms act spontaneously, and a combination is probably responsible for the symptomatology of AD. More studies are required to confirm these mechanisms and their potential application in predicting or even preventing AD complications.
In conclusion, we showed that Aβ disrupts neural synchrony leading to cognitive dysfunction. In particular, this study provided direct evidence regarding disrupted oscillatory connectivity between OB and EC-dHPC circuit at theta and gamma bands in association with impaired NOR performance. Recognition memory deficit related to altered OB-EC-dHPC connectivity can bring attention to the dynamic role of OB in integrating brain activities. We performed this experiment after the fourth week of model induction, considered an early stage of AD. Thus, observed changes in the dynamic activity of OB may be assumed as an early biomarker for AD, although more chronological studies are needed.
Although we showed a marked reduction in OB's cellular population, these results cannot differentiate between functional and non-functional neurons nor concern the alterations in synaptic activities. Further studies are needed to address that AD pathology-induced neural dysfunction in OB can present before irreversible structural damages and overt dementia. Thus, targeting these structures for early diagnosis and therapeutic interventions can prevent further damage and reduce neural injuries associated with cognitive problems. Our findings open a path for more selective targeting of structures, such as OB in AD, given its causal role in organization brain communication.
Materials and methods
Animals
The experiments were performed on adult pathogen-free male Wistar rats weighing 210-230 g obtained from Tarbiat Modares University (Tehran, Iran) and housed in 21 ± 2 °C, 12-h light–dark cycle, with free access to food and water. All experiments and procedures were done following the Tarbiat Modares University guidelines for animal care and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medical Sciences, Tarbiat Modares University (IR.MODARES.REC.1398.037). All animal procedures were in accordance with ARRIVE guidelines.
Surgery and electrode implantation
Rats were anesthetized using intraperitoneal injections of ketamine (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg) at day 0 (see Fig. 1A for timeline). We placed the anesthetized animals on a stereotaxic apparatus, and a longitudinal incision was made, then the skull was exposed by pulling the skin back. After drilling the skull on intended positions, stainless-steel recording electrodes (127 µm in diameter, A.M. System Inc., USA) were implanted unilaterally according to the stereotaxic coordinates of OB (AP: 8.5 mm, L: − 1 mm, DV: − 1.5 mm), the lateral portion of EC (AP: − 7.04 mm, L: − 5.5 mm, DV: − 6.5 mm) and dHPC (AP: − 3.6 mm, L: − 2.2 mm, DV: − 2.7 mm)69. We then implanted a stainless-steel screw at the right side of the parietal bone as a reference point. Finally, acrylic dental adhesive was poured around the electrodes and screws.
To confirm electrodes position, brains were carefully removed and fixed with 4% paraformaldehyde for 48 h. After preparing a 20 μm coronal section using a cryo micro-slicer, we visually compared them with the matching slices of the rat brain atlas69 (Fig. 1B). Animals with misplaced electrodes and animals that did not perform the tasks were excluded from the study. Data presented here are taken from twelve rats.
AD model induction
Aβ aggregation and plaque formation in the brain is one of the hallmarks and key components of AD pathology, which could be responsible for other pathologies6,70, including tau aggregation8,9,71. Aβ can accumulate intra- and extracellularly, causing devastating impairment in synaptic and neuronal functions, respectively. More importantly, Aβ induces neural apoptosis, resulting in neural degeneration72,73. Aβ1-42 is a critical agent frequently found in AD patients' brains70 that can induce neuronal apoptosis and AD-associated pathology72,74.
AD model was induced by Aβ1–42 (Cat No. A9810, Sigma-Aldrich, USA) dissolved in normal saline at the concentration of 4 μg/μl. The solution was kept at room temperature for 3 days before administration75. Aβ1–42 or saline was injected in a volume of 2 μl over 5 min via a microsyringe pump (Stoelting, Lane Dale, IL, USA) connected to the 25-gauge stainless steel needle bilaterally into the lateral cerebral ventricles according to stereotaxic coordination (AP: 0.8 mm, L: ± 1.6 mm, DV: − 3 mm).
Behavioral assessment
In the assessment locomotor activity and habituation phase of the NOR test, animals were placed individually in the center of the open field box to explore the area freely. To evaluate locomotor activity, animals were individually placed into the open field maze and allowed to explore freely for 10 min. Total traveled was calculated using the graphical interface of MATLAB76.
The NOR test is a reliable, sensitive, easy-to-perform behavioral method to evaluate spatial recognition memory77 in the AD animal model. This test does not concern the spatial reference memory and avoids inducing excessive stress29, negatively affecting memory and learning. Although it is a one-trial test, it consists of two phases: after habituation with the environment, in the first phase, rats first were placed in an open field box (50 cm high, 75 × 75 cm) center containing a couple of the same objects in the opposite corners 10 cm away from walls. Two types of objects, box-shaped (Fig. 1A right) and cylindrical shaped with 10 (diameter) × 20 (height) cm were used in this study (Fig. 1A left). Rats were exploring the objects for 4 min. In the second phase, after a 50-min delay, rats underwent a 4-min test identical to the previous, except one of the training phase objects was replaced with a novel object. Animals with intact memory spend more time beside the novel object than a previously-encountered object77. In this regard, we calculated the discrimination index (DI) as follows:
We also counted the percent and absolute time that animals explored the novel object. To calculate the object exploration time and number, we considered the while that nose-object distance was continuously less than 2.5 cm.
Four visual cues were placed 50 cm away from the mazes. The behavioral performances were recorded using a ceiling-mounted camera above the platform.
Histopathological assessment
Immunofluorescence
Rats were transcardially perfused with cold phosphate-buffered saline (PBS, 10 mM, pH = 7.4), followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS under urethane (1.2 g/Kg) anesthesia. Brains were post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for four hours at 4 °C. Cryoprotection was done with 30% sucrose solution in PBS. Coronal sections through the OB, EC, and dHPC were prepared using a freezing microtome (8 µm thick).
For immunofluorescent labeling, the sections were washed with PBS, permeabilized with Triton- × 100 0.2% for 20 min, and blocked in PBS, 0.2% Triton-X100, 10% normal serum during one hour at RT, followed by overnight incubation with primary antibody for anti-Aβ antibody (A8717, 1:700) at 4 °C. After successive washing with PBS, slides were incubated with proper fluorescent-labeled secondary antibodies (goat anti-rabbit, Alexa Fluor® 488; 1:2000, ab150077) for one hour at room temperature. Nuclei were counterstained with 4',6-diamidino-2- phenylindole (DAPI; Invitrogen Corp., USA), mounted, and images were taken using Olympus fluorescence microscopy and DP72 camera (BX51 TRF, USA).
Four sections of each animal were analyzed using ImageJ software (NIH, USA) for either plaque number or fluorescence intensity quantification. Aβ fluorescence intensity was assessed by measuring the mean gray value of OB, EC, and dHPC areas. Each value was normalized to the mean of tissue background intensity and presented as the percent of the saline group.
Nissl staining
Nissl staining was used to evaluate the number of survived cells in both groups. The procedure was performed according to manufacture protocol. Briefly, the sections were rehydrated by graded series alcohols (96%, 80%, and 70%) and stained with 0.1% Cresyl Fast Violet (Merck, Germany) at room temperature for 5 min. Afterward, the sections were washed and then dehydrated by graded series alcohols (70%, 80%, 96%, and 100%) cleared in xylene, cover-slipped using Entellan (Merck, chemical, Germany), and photographed. Images were captured from non-overlapping, consecutive microscopic fields using light Olympus BX-51 microscope and DP72 camera at 400 × magnification. A grid (50 μm × 50 μm) was randomly assigned to the images, and seven squares were counted for survived cells. The number of survived cells is expressed as the number of cells/mm2.
Signal processing
LFPs were simultaneously obtained from OB, EC, and dHPC via a fixed miniature buffer head stage with high-input impedance (BIODAC-A, TRITA Health Technology Co., Tehran, Iran). The signals were amplified using 1000 amplification gain, low-pass filtered < 250 Hz, and digitized at 1 kHz through a recording system (BIODAC-ESR18622, TRITA Health Technology Co., Tehran, Iran)78,79. EEG lab toolbox was applied for pre-processing the signals, including noise rejection and baseline correction80.
To find whether functional coupling in EC-dHPC circuit during recognition memory performance, we generated an inter-regional coherence map on a NOR maze when rats spontaneously explored the objects. LFP was binned into a positional frame, and the mean pixel coherence was color-coded. Accordingly, we selected 2 s before and 1 s after exploring the novel object. Finally, we averaged the coupling parameters per animal.
To measure the coherence, we computed magnitude-squared coherence using the mscohere function of MATLAB. Synchrony in the OB-EC-dHPC network was calculated using cross-correlation analysis, defined as the xcorr function in MATLAB software (the "coeff" option was selected for normalizing values). Max values of correlation in each trial were considered as synchrony parameters. All values of maximum cross-correlation were averaged per animal.
Statistical analysis
GraphPad Prism (version 6.0) was applied to analyze the data and create graphs statistically. The normality distribution of the data was examined via the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. As all data showed normal distribution, a t-test comparison was made. One sample t-test was applied to compare the mean values with the constant value of 0 or 100. The Pearson's correlation coefficient was applied to assess the correlation between behavioral results and connectivity measures. All data evaluated for the existence of outliers for Pearson's correlation and removed if a significant outlier exists. The p-value less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Acknowledgements
We want to thank Mr. Reza Vaziri, Farhad Farkhondeh, Mohsen Sharifi, and Ms. Forough Foolad for their technical assistance. The authors of this study need to express special thanks to Mr. Alireza Mani for his generous help. This study was supported by Tarbiat Modares University (Grant Number IG-39709). The funders had no role in the design, experiments, and analyses of the study.
Author contributions
M.S. conceptualized and designed the study, acquired, analyzed, and interpreted the data; drafted the manuscript. F.T. analyzed and interpreted the data, wrote the first draft and final version of the manuscript. M.A. analyzed and interpreted the data, drafted the manuscript. S.D. acquired the data and drafted the manuscript. K.D. acquired the data. M.N. software; analyzed and interpreted the data. M.J. and J.M. conceptualized and supervised the study. M.R.R. conceptualized, designed, and supervised the study, interpreted the data. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Farhad Tabasi and Maryam Abdolsamadi.
References
- 1.Prince MJ, et al. World Alzheimer Report 2015-The Global Impact of Dementia: An analysis of prevalence, Incidence, Cost and Trends. Alzheimer's Disease International; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Knopman DS, et al. Alzheimer disease. Nat. Rev. Dis. Prim. 2021;7:33. doi: 10.1038/s41572-021-00269-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.2015 Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement11, 332–384, 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.02.003 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 4.Goldstein FC, Loring DW, Thomas T, Saleh S, Hajjar I. Recognition memory performance as a cognitive marker of prodromal Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2019;72:507–514. doi: 10.3233/JAD-190468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Russo MJ, et al. Usefulness of discriminability and response bias indices for the evaluation of recognition memory in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2017;43:1–14. doi: 10.1159/000452255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Braak H, Braak E. Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiol. Aging. 1997;18:351–357. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(97)00056-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Maestú F, de Haan W, Busche MA, DeFelipe J. Neuronal excitation/inhibition imbalance: Core element of a translational perspective on Alzheimer pathophysiology. Ageing Res. Rev. 2021;69:101372. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2021.101372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bolmont T, et al. Induction of tau pathology by intracerebral infusion of amyloid-β-containing brain extract and by amyloid-β deposition in APP × tau transgenic mice. Am. J. Pathol. 2007;171:2012–2020. doi: 10.2353/ajpath.2007.070403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Guo J-P, Arai T, Miklossy J, McGeer PL. Aβ and tau form soluble complexes that may promote self aggregation of both into the insoluble forms observed in Alzheimer’s disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2006;103:1953–1958. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509386103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neural synchrony in brain disorders: Relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron. 2006;52:155–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Delbeuck X, Van der Linden M, Collette F. Alzheimer'disease as a disconnection syndrome? Neuropsychol. Rev. 2003;13:79–92. doi: 10.1023/A:1023832305702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Busche MA, et al. Clusters of hyperactive neurons near amyloid plaques in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Science. 2008;321:1686–1689. doi: 10.1126/science.1162844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Goutagny R, et al. Alterations in hippocampal network oscillations and theta-gamma coupling arise before Aβ overproduction in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2013;37:1896–1902. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Seeley WW, Crawford RK, Zhou J, Miller BL, Greicius MD. Neurodegenerative diseases target large-scale human brain networks. Neuron. 2009;62:42–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Stranahan, A. M. & Mattson, M. P. Selective vulnerability of neurons in layer II of the entorhinal cortex during aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neural Plast.2010 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 16.Balestrieri JVL, Nonato MB, Gheler L, Prandini MN. Structural volume of hippocampus and Alzheimer’s disease. Rev. Assoc. Med. Bras. 2020;66:512–515. doi: 10.1590/1806-9282.66.4.512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bird CM. The role of the hippocampus in recognition memory. Cortex. 2017;93:155–165. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Devanand DP, et al. Hippocampal and entorhinal atrophy in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2007;68:828. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000256697.20968.d7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Christen-Zaech S, et al. Early olfactory involvement in Alzheimer’s disease. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 2003;30:20–25. doi: 10.1017/S0317167100002389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ubeda-Bañon I, et al. The human olfactory system in two proteinopathies: Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Trans. Neurodegener. 2020;9:22. doi: 10.1186/s40035-020-00200-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Zou Y-M, Da Lu L-PL, Zhang H-H, Zhou Y-Y. Olfactory dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2016;12:869. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S104886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gourévitch B, Kay LM, Martin C. Directional coupling from the olfactory bulb to the hippocampus during a go/no-go odor discrimination task. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;103:2633–2641. doi: 10.1152/jn.01075.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Franks KH, Chuah MI, King AE, Vickers JC. Connectivity of pathology: The olfactory system as a model for network-driven mechanisms of Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:234. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Salimi M, et al. Distraction of olfactory bulb-medial prefrontal cortex circuit may induce anxiety-like behavior in allergic rhinitis. PloS One. 2019;14:e0221978. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0221978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ghazvineh S, et al. Rhythmic air-puff into nasal cavity modulates activity across multiple brain areas: A non-invasive brain stimulation method to reduce ventilator-induced memory impairment. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 2021;287:103627. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2021.103627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Biskamp J, Bartos M, Sauer J-F. Organization of prefrontal network activity by respiration-related oscillations. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:45508. doi: 10.1038/srep45508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Zelano C, et al. Nasal respiration entrains human limbic oscillations and modulates cognitive function. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:12448–12467. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2586-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Salimi M, et al. The olfactory bulb modulates entorhinal cortex oscillations during spatial working memory. J. Physiol. Sci. 2021;71:21. doi: 10.1186/s12576-021-00805-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zhang R, et al. Novel object recognition as a facile behavior test for evaluating drug effects in AβPP/PS1 Alzheimer's disease mouse model. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2012;31:801–812. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-120151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ennaceur A, Delacour J. A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: Behavioral data. Behav. Brain Res. 1988;31:47–59. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90157-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Antunes M, Biala G. The novel object recognition memory: Neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications. Cogn. Process. 2012;13:93–110. doi: 10.1007/s10339-011-0430-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Vnek N, Gleason T, Kromer L, Rothblat L. Entorhinal–hippocampal connections and object memory in the rat: Acquisition versus retention. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:3193–3199. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.15-04-03193.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gretenkord S, et al. Coordinated electrical activity in the olfactory bulb gates the oscillatory entrainment of entorhinal networks in neonatal mice. PLoS Biol. 2019;17:e2006994. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2006994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hozumi S, et al. Characteristics of changes in cholinergic function and impairment of learning and memory-related behavior induced by olfactory bulbectomy. Behav. Brain Res. 2003;138:9–15. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00183-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Galluzzi S, et al. Supporting evidence for using biomarkers in the diagnosis of MCI due to AD. J. Neurol. 2013;260:640–650. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6694-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Jeong J. EEG dynamics in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004;115:1490–1505. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Babiloni C, et al. Abnormal fronto-parietal coupling of brain rhythms in mild Alzheimer's disease: A multicentric EEG study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2004;19:2583–2590. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03333.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Koenig T, et al. Decreased EEG synchronization in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging. 2005;26:165–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Pijnenburg Y, et al. EEG synchronization likelihood in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease during a working memory task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004;115:1332–1339. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.12.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Du AT, et al. Higher atrophy rate of entorhinal cortex than hippocampus in AD. Neurology. 2004;62:422. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000106462.72282.90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Khan UA, et al. Molecular drivers and cortical spread of lateral entorhinal cortex dysfunction in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:304–311. doi: 10.1038/nn.3606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Zhou M, Zhang F, Zhao L, Qian J, Dong C. Entorhinal cortex: A good biomarker of mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;27:185–195. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2015-0019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Van Hoesen GW, Hyman BT, Damasio AR. Entorhinal cortex pathology in Alzheimer's disease. Hippocampus. 1991;1:1–8. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450010102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Moser EI, Kropff E, Moser M-B. Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;31:69–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.061307.090723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Tsao A, Moser M-B, Moser EI. Traces of experience in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:399–405. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kjelvik G, et al. The brain structural and cognitive basis of odor identification deficits in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. BMC Neurol. 2014;14:1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12883-014-0168-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Doty RL. Olfactory dysfunction in neurodegenerative diseases: Is there a common pathological substrate? Lancet Neurol. 2017;16:478–488. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30123-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Masurkar AV, Devanand D. Olfactory dysfunction in the elderly: Basic circuitry and alterations with normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Geriatr. Rep. 2014;3:91–100. doi: 10.1007/s13670-014-0080-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Beshel J, Kopell N, Kay LM. Olfactory bulb gamma oscillations are enhanced with task demands. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:8358–8365. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1199-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Witter MP. The perforant path: Projections from the entorhinal cortex to the dentate gyrus. Prog. Brain Res. 2007;163:43–61. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)63003-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Tamura M, Spellman TJ, Rosen AM, Gogos JA, Gordon JA. Hippocampal-prefrontal theta-gamma coupling during performance of a spatial working memory task. Nat. Commun. 2017;8:2182. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02108-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Alvarado-Martínez R, Salgado-Puga K, Peña-Ortega F. Amyloid beta inhibits olfactory bulb activity and the ability to smell. PloS One. 2013;8:e75745. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Palop JJ, Mucke L. Amyloid-beta-induced neuronal dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease: From synapses toward neural networks. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:812–818. doi: 10.1038/nn.2583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Cirrito JR, et al. Synaptic activity regulates interstitial fluid amyloid-β levels in vivo. Neuron. 2005;48:913–922. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Iaccarino HF, et al. Gamma frequency entrainment attenuates amyloid load and modifies microglia. Nature. 2016;540:230–235. doi: 10.1038/nature20587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Whittington MA, Traub RD. Interneuron diversity series: Inhibitory interneurons and network oscillations in vitro. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:676–682. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2003.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Klausberger T, Somogyi P. Neuronal diversity and temporal dynamics: The unity of hippocampal circuit operations. Science. 2008;321:53–57. doi: 10.1126/science.1149381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Fukunaga I, Herb JT, Kollo M, Boyden ES, Schaefer AT. Independent control of gamma and theta activity by distinct interneuron networks in the olfactory bulb. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:1208–1216. doi: 10.1038/nn.3760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Shusterman R, Smear MC, Koulakov AA, Rinberg D. Precise olfactory responses tile the sniff cycle. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:1039–1044. doi: 10.1038/nn.2877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Luo M, Katz LC. Response correlation maps of neurons in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2001;32:1165–1179. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00537-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Margrie TW, Sakmann B, Urban NN. Action potential propagation in mitral cell lateral dendrites is decremental and controls recurrent and lateral inhibition in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2001;98:319–324. doi: 10.1073/pnas.011523098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Yokoi M, Mori K, Nakanishi S. Refinement of odor molecule tuning by dendrodendritic synaptic inhibition in the olfactory bulb. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 1995;92:3371–3375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.8.3371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Franks KH, Chuah MI, King AE, Vickers JC. Connectivity of pathology: The olfactory system as a model for network-driven mechanisms of Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2015 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Lledo PM, Lazarini F. Neuronal replacement in microcircuits of the adult olfactory system. C. R. Biol. 2007;330:510–520. doi: 10.1016/j.crvi.2007.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Morales-Medina JC, et al. Impaired structural hippocampal plasticity is associated with emotional and memory deficits in the olfactory bulbectomized rat. Neuroscience. 2013;236:233–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Morales-Medina JC, Juarez I, Iannitti T, Flores G. Olfactory bulbectomy induces neuronal rearrangement in the entorhinal cortex in the rat. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 2013;52:80–86. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2013.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Braak H, Del Tredici K. The preclinical phase of the pathological process underlying sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2015;138:2814–2833. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Jaunmuktane Z, Brandner S. Invited Review: The role of prion-like mechanisms in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 2020;46:522–545. doi: 10.1111/nan.12592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates: Hard Cover. Elsevier; 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Di Fede G, et al. Molecular subtypes of Alzheimer’s disease. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:3269. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-21641-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Llorens-Marítin M, Jurado J, Hernández F, Ávila J. GSK-3β, a pivotal kinase in Alzheimer disease. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2014 doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2014.00046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Yu H-B, Li Z-B, Zhang H-X, Wang X-L. Role of potassium channels in Aβ1–40-activated apoptotic pathway in cultured cortical neurons. J. Neurosci. Res. 2006;84:1475–1484. doi: 10.1002/jnr.21054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kechko OI, et al. Beta-amyloid induces apoptosis of neuronal cells by inhibition of the Arg/N-end rule pathway proteolytic activity. Aging. 2019;11:6134–6152. doi: 10.18632/aging.102177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Kravenska Y, et al. The monomers, oligomers, and fibrils of amyloid-β inhibit the activity of mitoBKCa channels by a membrane-mediated mechanism. Biochim. et Biophys. Acta (BBA): Biomembr. 2020;1862:183337. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamem.2020.183337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Jafari A, et al. Brain mitochondrial ATP-insensitive large conductance Ca+2-activated K+ channel properties are altered in a rat model of amyloid-β neurotoxicity. Exp. Neurol. 2015;269:8–16. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2014.12.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Patel TP, et al. An open-source toolbox for automated phenotyping of mice in behavioral tasks. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Ennaceur A, Delacour J. A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: Behavioral data. Behav. Brain Res. 1988;31:47–59. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90157-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Salimi M, et al. Allergic rhinitis impairs working memory in association with drop of hippocampal–Prefrontal coupling. Brain Res. 2021;1758:147368. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2021.147368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Dehdar K, et al. Allergen-induced anxiety-like behavior is associated with disruption of medial prefrontal cortex: Amygdala circuit. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:19586. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-55539-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2004;134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.