Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2022 Mar 4;129(3):343–351. doi: 10.1007/s00702-022-02459-8

5-HTT genotype and inertia of negative affect in adolescents and young adults from the general population

T M Ollmann 1,✉,#, E Seidl 1,#, J Venz 1,2, L Pieper 1,2, C Voss 1, J Hoyer 1, H Kische 1, S R Poppenhäger 1, M A Schiele 3, K Domschke 3,4,#, K Beesdo-Baum 1,2,#
PMCID: PMC8930868  PMID: 35246765

Abstract

The study aims to replicate the previous found association of 5-HTTLPR and inertia of negative affect in daily life of adolescents and young adults. Data of 877 adolescents (aged 14–21 years) of the Behavior and Mind Health (BeMIND) study (epidemiological cohort study, Dresden, Germany) were genotyped for 5-HTTLPR/rs25531, grouped into SS/SLG/SLA/LGLA/LGLG vs. LALA, and provided ratings on negative affect items, depression and anxiety (Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System) eight times a day over 4 days. Multilevel regression models did not reveal an association of 5-HTTLPR genotype and inertia of negative affect, nor associations with inertia of anxiety or depression. Inertia of negative affect seems not to be a psychological mechanism through which 5-HTTLPR acts on psychopathology.

Keywords: Emotional inertia, 5-HTTLPR, Negative affect, Adolescence

Introduction

Change in dynamics of affective experience has gathered increasing attention within the last years since the static view of emotional experiences ignores the flow of affect influenced by internal and external factors. Fluctuations of emotions have been suggested to be associated with aspects of psychological wellbeing and maladaptation (Houben et al. 2015) as well as mental disorders (Trull et al. 2015). Thus, detecting and analyzing dynamic patterns of affective experience is crucial to improve our understanding of mental health problems.

The use of smartphone-based Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) has recently improved the assessment of such dynamic processes. Various measures have been proposed to depict different dynamical aspects of everyday life experiences (Dejonckheere et al. 2019). One frequently used measure is emotional inertia, which has previously been linked to mental health problems (Houben et al. 2015; Wichers et al. 2015). Emotional inertia stands for the resistance to change in affect over time and is usually calculated as the autoregressive coefficient (Lamers et al. 2018; Trull et al. 2015; Wichers et al. 2015).

Therefore, high emotional inertia is present when current emotional states are heavily influenced by previous states and low emotional inertia is present when current feelings are less predictable by previous feelings. Thus higher levels of emotional inertia might point towards the decouplement of emotional states from their adaptive function to respond flexibly to significant environmental events and regulation efforts to make disruptive emotions return to baseline (van Roekel et al. 2018).

Recently, emotional inertia has been associated with several mental health related constructs including low self-esteem (Koval et al. 2012; Kuppens et al. 2010), rumination (Koval et al. 2012; Trull et al. 2015) and depression (Koval et al. 2012; Kuppens et al. 2010). Higher levels of emotional inertia have been found in depressed adolescents compared to non-depressed adolescents (Koval et al. 2012; Kuppens et al. 2010). Furthermore, higher levels of emotional inertia predicted the onset of depression in early adolescents in a longitudinal study two years later (Kuppens et al. 2012), as well as depression severity in a non-clinical adult and clinical adolescent sample (Koval et al. 2012). Based on these findings, van Roekel et al. (2018) pursued the assumption that emotional inertia may be rooted in a known genetic risk factor for emotional dysfunction, namely a length polymorphism in the regulatory region of the serotonin transporter gene (serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphic region, 5-HTTLPR). 5-HTTLPR comprises a short allele (S) and a long allele (L) whereas the short allele is associated with less transcription of the serotonin transporter compared with the long allele (Lesch et al. 1996). A single nucleotide polymorphism rs25531 (A > G) is assumed to render the LG allele functionally equivalent to the S allele (i.e. reduced 5-HTT availability).

In their study, van Roekel et al. (2018) conducted an experience sampling study in the Netherlands among 236 high school adolescents investigating the association of 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 and emotional inertia and discerned S-allele carriers to be characterized by higher inertia for negative affect. The negative affect was operationalized by calculating a mean negative score using different emotional and affective states, including feeling anxious, irritated, worried, low, insecure, and guilty. The association between emotional inertia regarding negative affect and 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 was also found after adjusting for age, gender and depressive symptoms. The authors concluded that emotional inertia may represent a possible psychological pathway how 5-HTTLPR contributes to risk for depression or more broadly affective disorders. Following these results, the aim of the present study was to replicate this finding by van Roekel et al. (2018) using a larger population sample of adolescents and young adults.

Methods

Sample and procedures

The data were taken from the baseline investigation of the Behavior and Mind Health (BeMIND) study, an epidemiological cohort study of adolescents and young adults from Dresden, Germany. A random age and sex stratified general population sample of 14–21 year olds was drawn from the population registry in 2015 and N = 1180 participated in the baseline investigation conducted from 11/2015 to 12/2016 (response proportion: 21.7%; AAPOR formula RR1; cooperation rate: 43.4%; AAPOR formula coop1; AAPOR 2016). The overall aim of the study is to investigate developmental trajectories and risk factors of mental and behavioral disorders. Baseline assessments included categorical and dimensional diagnostic assessment on day 1, cognitive tasks and biosampling (blood or buccal swab) at day 2 approximately one week later, and an online questionnaire assessment as well as an Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) study part (see Appendix, Table 4) in between these two personal appointments. For the present analysis, participants were excluded if they did not participate in the EMA assessment (N = 26), if they showed a compliance rate of < 50% in EMA (no reliable and sufficient data; N = 82), if they did not provide blood/buccal swab samples/no genotype information available (N = 101), if they had no Caucasian descent by first generation (N = 115) or if information about the descent were not available (N = 35), resulting in a final analysis sample of 877 participants (74.2% of the total sample). Note that the numbers do not sum up since some participants fall under more than one category. All participants gave written informed consent or assent (in minors also legal guardians provided written informed consent). The study protocol and its amendments were reviewed and accepted by the ethics committee of the Technische Universität Dresden, Germany (TUD: EK381102014) and the study was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. Detailed information on the studies aims, procedures and sample characteristics can be found elsewhere (Beesdo-Baum et al. 2020).

Ecological momentary assessment

On eight occasions per day over four consecutive days (2 week-days and the weekend), smartphone-based EMA assessments were administered. Daily assessments included a time-based morning assessment, six daytime assessments, and one evening assessment. All items used for the following analyses were mandatorily assessed at each of the assessments, using a seek-bar which translated into a scale of 0–100. Sleep times and periods during which the participants did not want to be disturbed (e.g. school times) were considered while setting up an individual reminder scheme. Assessments were distributed symmetrically throughout the day but at unknown points of time for the participant. Each survey could be postponed three times for 5 min (15 min altogether), or the questionnaire could be omitted. To enhance the motivation for the execution of the EMA, participants were instructed face to face by trained study staff and a training day with three sets of questionnaires was taking place beforehand. Smartphones were returned to the study center by the participants and data stored on the smartphone was then transferred to the study server.

For the purpose of replicating the findings by van Roekel et al. (2018), a negative affect score was generated by calculating the mean of emotional/affect items and scales used in the EMA assessment. As van Roekel et al. (2018) did not use a validated negative affect scale, but added up items assessing negative affect, namely feelings of anxiety, irritation, worry, low mood, insecurity and guilt, the present study followed this procedure. However, the items differ as the current study used items of negative valence of the BeMIND EMA-item pool, namely anxiety (Patient-Reported Outcome Measurement Information System Version 1.0 Short Form; PROMIS-ANX; Pilkonis et al. 2011), depression (Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System Version 1.0 Short Form; PROMIS-DEP; Pilkonis et al. 2011), anger (Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System Version 1.0 Short Form; PROMIS-DEP; Pilkonis et al. 2011), wakefulness (short-form of the Multidimensional mental-state questionnaire; MDBF; Wilhelm and Schoebi 2007), pessimism (Skala Optimismus-Pessimismus-2; SOP2; Kemper et al. 2012), negative thoughts, and experiential avoidance (self-developed; full descriptions of the items used in this study are presented in Table 4 in the Appendix).

Table 4.

EMA items of the negative affect score

Construct Reference Item Answers (0–100)
Negative affect score
 Depression Patient-reported outcomes measurement information system (PROMIS emotional distress—depression—short-form, PROMIS-DEP) Since the last beep I felt worthless Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I felt helpless Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I felt depressed Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I felt hopeless Never—sometimes—always
 Anxiety Patient-reported outcomes measurement information system (PROMIS emotional distress—anxiety—short-form, PROMIS-ANX) Since the last beep I felt fearful Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I found it hard to focus on anything other than my anxiety Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I felt worried Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I felt uneasy Never—sometimes—always
 Anger Patient-reported outcomes measurement information system (PROMIS emotional distress—anger—short-form) Since the last beep I was irritated more than people knew Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I felt angry Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I felt like I was ready to explode Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I was grouchy Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I felt annoyed Never—sometimes—always
 Experiential avoidance Self-developed Since the last beep I was upset and concerned about my feelings or thoughts Never—sometimes—always
Since the last beep I tried hard to get rid of my feelings or thoughts Never—sometimes—always
 Pessimism Skala optimismus-pessimismus-2 (SOP2) Currently I am pessimistic Not at all—absolutely
 Mood awake–sleepy 6-item short-form of the Multidimensional mental-state questionnaire (MDBF) At the moment I feel… Tired—awake
Full of energy—shiftless
 Negative thoughts Self-developed Since the last beep I thought about negative or unpleasant things No negative thoughts—frequently negative thoughts

Genotyping

EDTA-blood samples were stored without delay at − 80 °C in a laboratory freezer. Whenever participants (or legal guardians) did not provide consent/assent to draw blood, they were asked to provide a buccal swab sample. The final analysis sample was genotyped for the 5-HTTLPR as well as the functionally related single nucleotide polymorphism rs25531 as described in published protocols (for details see Schiele et al. 2016). Genotypes were determined by two independent blinded investigators. Hardy–Weinberg criteria were fulfilled for the 5-HTTLPR genotype distribution (SS = 119, SL = 408, LL = 350, p = 0.99) as well as for the triallelic model (LALA = 289, LGLA/SLA = 421, LGLG/SLG/SS = 167, p = 0.53).

Statistical analysis

We applied sample weights to make sure that, after a weighting adjustment for sex and age, the age/sex distribution of the sample was representative for the population of the 14–21-year-old participants of Dresden. Two groups were built comprising LALA carriers in the high-expression group and SS, SLG, SLA, LGLA, LGLG carriers in the low-expression group (Baffa et al. 2010; Baune et al. 2008; Schiele et al. 2016, 2020; Wendland et al. 2006). A group comparison between the low-and high-expression group was conducted regarding sociodemographic characteristics including age (t test), sex distribution, education, social class, EMA compliance as well as negative affect score (survey design-based F test, Rao and Scott 1984).

Only EMA assessments with a time gap of less than 3 h in between each other were considered as truly consecutive EMA assessments. Hence, we also excluded between-day effects since night times were always longer than 3 h.

We investigated the relationship between 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 genotype and the carry over effect of affect from one assessment to the next one, namely inertia (Kuppens et al. 2010), with regard to the negative affect scale using multilevel regression modeling to account for the multilevel structure of the data (assessments nested in subjects). In the models, the affect score at sampling time t was predicted by the affect score at sampling time t−1 (t−1 affect scale person mean-centered; Enders and Tofighi 2007). The estimated slope for prediction of t affect score by t−1 affect score represents the autocorrelation of the respective time series of affect ratings, which is a direct operationalization of inertia (Kuppens et al. 2010). In detail, we added the interaction between person mean-centered t−1 negative affect score and genotype information as predictors to a multilevel regression model to predict the negative affect score at time t. The regression coefficient of the interaction between t−1 affect score and genotype information indicates the difference in autocorrelation between the two genotype groups. Age, sex and type of day (week/weekend), the interaction of t−1 affect score and age as well as the interaction of t−1 affect score and sex were additionally entered as covariates in these analyzes, thereby adjusting for influence of age and sex on the slope of the t−1 negative affect score.

We first used a random intercept model with fixed slopes across subjects within genotype group and levels of covariates since we were only interested in comparing the overall mean autocorrelation within genotype groups against each other, not in investigating variance of autocorrelations across subjects within groups. Hence, we omitted a random slope model in the spirit of model parsimony at first. Since this model may not carry all important facets of the data, we then investigated a random slope model corresponding to van Roekel et al. (2018), where both the intercept and slope values were allowed to vary between all subjects and the intercepts and slopes are predicted by dummy-coded genotype information in the level 2 model part.

Results

Sample characteristics and assessment distribution

Sample characteristics and EMA compliance are shown in Table 1. For the present analysis, data of 877 participants were utilized. As participants sometimes quit the EMA assessment somewhere in between, different amounts of assessments were available for the analysis with respect to the measured variables (between 23.692 and 23.457). Genotype groups did not differ significantly concerning age, sex, education, social class, compliance and negative affect score (all pss > 0.05).

Table 1.

Sample characteristics

5-HTTLPR/rs25531
Total sample LALA SS, SLG, LGLG, SLA, LALG
N 877 289 588
Age [mean (SD)] 17.92 (2.35) 17.99 (2.32) 17.89 (2.36)
Sex (female %) 49.54 49.00 49.81
Education (%)
 Low 0.70 0.20 0.95
 Middle 16.50 14.83 17.35
 High 78.03 80.20 76.92
 Other 4.77 4.77 4.77
Social class (%)
 Lowest 2.79 1.83 3.28
 Lower middle 13.41 14.57 12.82
 Middle 61.52 58.5 63.05
 Upper middle 21.67 24.29 20.34
 Upper 0.61 0.81 0.51
EMA compliance (%) 84.68 85.25 84.39
Negative affect (mean (SD)) 17.02 (7.76) 17.28 (8.57) 16.89 (7.32)

Data were weighted to improve representativeness for sex, age and compliance, but frequencies are reported unweighted

SD standard deviation, Delete "NA" since "NA" not used in the table NA negative affect

Association between 5-HTTLPR genotype and inertia regarding negative affect

As shown in Table 2 and illustrated in Fig. 1, no association was found between 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 genotype and inertia regarding negative affect in the parsimonious random intercept (b = 0.00, CI [− 0.05;0.05]) as well as in the random slope model (b = 0.01, CI [− 0.03; 0.05]).

Table 2.

5-HTTLPR/rs25531 genotype and inertia regarding negative affect, results of multilevel regression [with t affect scale as response variable and t−1 affect scale, dummy-coded genotype information, and their interaction as predictors]

Random intercept model Random slope model
b 95% CI b 95% CI
Intercept negative affect
 Value 16.463 12.351 20.574 16.526 12.413 20.639
 Genotype 0.485 − 0.708 1.678 0.492 − 0.702 1.686
 Age − 0.030 − 0.253 0.193 − 0.033 − 0.256 0.190
 Female 2.130 1.064 3.197 2.138 1.072 3.204
 Weekend − 0.661 − 0.939 − 0.383 − 0.721 − 0.982 − 0.460
Slope negative affect
 Value 0.367 0.166 0.569 0.241 0.094 0.388
 Genotype 0.000 − 0.050 0.050 0.009 − 0.033 0.051
 Age − 0.003 − 0.014 0.007 0.002 − 0.006 0.010
 Female 0.082 0.030 0.134 0.058 0.016 0.101

Week/weekend is included as level 1 covariate, age and sex are included as level 2 covariates. The Intercept sections show the main effect of covariates on affect scores, which is the effect of the covariate if all other covariates were held at zero. The Slope sections show the effect of the covariate on the slope of the t−1 affect score, i.e. the regression coefficients of the interaction between covariate and person mean-centered t−1 affect score in predicting t affect score

b regression coefficient, CI confidence interval

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Illustration of inertia on Negative Affect Score based on the random slope model. 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 genotype groups LALA vs. LGLG, SLG, SS, LGLA, SLA. Higher slopes represent higher inertia from one time point (Time T−1) to the next time point (Time T)

Post hoc analyses regarding associations between 5-HTTLPR genotype and inertia of specific emotion

Due to the unexpected lack of association between emotional inertia of negative affect and 5-HTTLPR/rs25531, post hoc analyses were conducted to examine whether there is an association between 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 and inertia of more specific emotional affect types. Specifically, the negative affect score was disentangled regarding inertia of depressive affect and inertia of anxious affect. This was based on the assumption by van Roekel et al. (2018), that emotional inertia not only constitutes a potential psychological pathway through which 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 impacts on the risk for depression but also concerning the risk of more broadly affective disorders, i.e. also comprising anxiety disorders. In addition, EMA studies often combine different items and constructs to generate a negative affect score, as was the case in the study by van Roekel et al. (2018) as well as in the present study. This has been criticized by Dejonckheere et al. (2019) since the variation of these emotions in regard to arousal (Russell 2003), associated appraisals (Moors 2013) and behavioral tendencies (Frijda et al. 1989) was not taken into account. The results of the more realistic random slope model imposing less model assumptions serve as basis for interpretation. However, also the post hoc analyses, as shown in Table 3 and illustrated in Fig. 2, revealed no association of 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 genotype and inertia of depression assessed by PROMIS-DEP in the two models (random intercept model: b = 0.01, CI [− 0.08, 0.10], random slope model: b = − 0.02, CI [− 0.07, 0.04]). A significant relationship was found, however, for 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 genotype and inertia of anxiety assessed by PROMIS-ANX in the parsimonious random intercept model where equal slopes are assumed across subjects within genotype group and levels of covariates. LALA genotype predicted significantly higher levels of inertia concerning anxiety compared to SS, SLG, SLA, LGLA, LGLG carriers (b = 0.09, CI [0.00, 0.17]). The association was reduced to near zero (b = 0.02, CI [− 0.04, 0.08]) and no longer significant in the more flexible random slope model where slopes are allowed to vary across all subjects. As mentioned above, the results of the random slope model are used for interpretation.

Table 3.

5-HTTLPR/rs25531 Genotype and Inertia Regarding Depression and Anxiety, results of Multilevel Regression [with t affect scale as response variable and t−1 affect scale, dummy-coded genotype information, and their interaction as predictors]

Random intercept model Random slope model
b 95% CI b 95% CI
Intercept PROMIS depression
 Value 5.519 1.561 9.477 5.467 1.506 9.428
 Genotype 0.877 − 0.344 2.098 0.927 − 0.297 2.151
 Age − 0.096 − 0.305 0.114 − 0.096 − 0.305 0.113
 Female 2.254 1.185 3.324 2.321 1.253 3.390
 Weekend − 0.275 − 0.554 0.003 − 0.232 − 0.475 0.011
Slope PROMIS depression
 Value 0.579 0.186 0.971 0.263 0.040 0.487
 Genotype 0.009 − 0.082 0.100 − 0.016 − 0.074 0.043
 Age − 0.023 − 0.044 − 0.002 − 0.008 − 0.021 0.004
 Female 0.177 0.082 0.273 0.137 0.074 0.200
Intercept PROMIS anxiety
 Value 5.012 1.658 8.367 4.986 1.615 8.358
 Genotype 0.922 − 0.016 1.859 0.954 0.013 1.895
 Age − 0.090 − 0.269 0.090 − 0.089 − 0.269 0.091
 Female 1.509 0.710 2.309 1.546 0.744 2.348
 Weekend − 0.245 − 0.489 − 0.002 − 0.243 − 0.467 − 0.019
Slope PROMIS anxiety
 Value 0.171 − 0.149 0.491 0.153 − 0.078 0.383
 Genotype 0.085 0.005 0.165 0.019 − 0.038 0.076
 Age − 0.003 − 0.019 0.014 − 0.003 − 0.016 0.009
 Female 0.133 0.053 0.213 0.077 0.022 0.132

Week/weekend is included as level 1 covariate, age and sex are included as level 2 covariates. The Intercept sections show the main effect of covariates on affect scores, which is the effect of the covariate if all other covariates were held at zero. The Slope sections show the effect of the covariate on the slope of the t−1 affect score, i.e. the regression coefficients of the interaction between covariate and person mean-centered t−1 affect score in predicting t affect score

b regression coefficient, CI confidence interval

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Illustration of inertia on A PROMIS Depression and B PROMIS Anxiety based on the random slope models. 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 genotype groups LALA vs. LGLG, SLG, SS, LGLA, SLA. Higher slopes represent higher inertia from one time point (Time T−1) to the next time point (Time T)

Discussion

This EMA study conducted in a general population sample of adolescents and young adults did not confirm the previously by van Roekel et al. (2018) reported association between the 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 S allele and inertia of negative affect. Thus, the results of the present study do not provide supporting evidence for the assumed psychological mechanism through which 5-HTTLPR indirectly increases risk for depression as hypothesized by van Roekel et al. (2018). Also, disentangling negative affect by examining inertia separately for depressive affect and anxious affect revealed no robust associations with 5-HTTLPR/rs25531.

The discrepancy of the present findings to the results found by van Roekel et al. (2018) could be due to various (methodological) reasons. First, rather than using a validated negative affect scale, the negative affect scores in both studies were generated by using different scales and a mix of items included in EMA, which are not identical. To combine different items and constructs to generate scales has been criticized by Dejonckheere et al. (2019) already, pointing out that this can lead to unreliable scales. This could further influence the quality of the scales and, therefore, the effects of inertia. However, the conducted post hoc analyses in the present study with regard to inertia of depressive and anxious affect revealed also no significant associations with 5-HTTLPR/rs25531. Nevertheless, more research is needed using, depending on the research topic, validated affect scales, such as the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (Watson et al. 1988) or focus more on clear and single constructs to account for different arousals, appraisals and action tendencies.

Another reason for not being able to replicate the findings of van Roekel et al. (2018) might be the use of different assessment modalities. While the present study utilized a visual analogue scale (1–100) that is able to measure affect on a very detailed level, van Roekel et al. (2018) used a seven-point scale, where people might tend to answer in the same manner more often because of missing options which would match their answer exactly. Furthermore, EMA compliance was lower in the original paper, with about 76% compared to about 85% in the present analysis sample after excluding participants with compliance < 50%. Estimating the impact of higher missing rates is not possible, as no information was given on how missings were handled. The present study considered EMA assessments as truly consecutive with a time gap less than 3 h. Possible longer durations due to missings between assessments may affect the measurement of inertia. Additionally, differences in sample characteristics regarding age (van Roekel M = 14.2 years, SD = 0.5 vs. M = 17.4 years, SD = 2.3), or ethnicity (van Roekel: 97.1% born in Netherlands, 1.3% in non-European country, no information about Caucasian descent vs. only Caucasian descent by first generation) might account for the observed discrepancies between studies.

In addition, emotional inertia is a complex feature, expected to be influenced by genetic as well as environmental factors. In line, a recent study investigated the genetic and environmental contribution towards adolescent daily emotional inertia in a sample of adolescent twins who provided data in respect to their positive and negative emotions daily over a month. The authors showed that non-shared environmental influences play a significant role, whereas genetic influences appear to be of minor importance (Zheng and Asbury 2019). In line, meta-analytic evaluations of single-gene candidate studies point towards small to negligible effects of single candidate genes regarding psychological phenotypes (Border et al. 2019).

Further, emotional inertia was also found to be susceptible to stress (Koval and Kuppens 2012; Kuppens et al. 2010). Thus the discrepancies between the present findings and the findings by van Roekel et al. (2018) could be partly due to environmental influences not considered in the present study and the study by van Roekel et al. (2018).

Besides state measures like emotional inertia, it is important to note, that studies have also shown a link between 5-HTTLPR or 5-HTTLPR/rs25531, respectively, and anxiety sensitivity, partly in interaction with early trauma (Klauke et al. 2011), anxious temperament (Schiele et al. 2020), as well as trait anxiety (Gonda and Bagdy 2006; for review see Gottschalk and Domschke 2016). However, findings have been inconsistent; Osher et al. (2000) found a link between the S allele and anxiety-related traits covering harm avoidance and neuroticism in an adult sample, whereas the general association of 5-HTTLPR and trait anxiety was not confirmed by a meta-analysis (Schinka et al. 2004) and in a recent study by Licht et al. (2020). In addition, 5-HTTLPR has been linked to stress reactivity. Gunthert et al. (2007) showed that on days when college students experienced more intense stressors, S-allele carriers reported more feelings of anxiety, in comparison with L-allele carriers. Consequently, further investigations into the link between 5-HTTLPR and emotional inertia should additionally consider trait measures of anxiety, stress reactivity or negative affect to clarify inconsistent findings.

Conclusions from the results of the present study should be drawn in light of some limitations as well as the fact that the present study did not allow for a one-to-one replication of the study by van Roekel et al. (2018) due to several reasons as detailed above. The limitations drawn from the present study concern first time slots where participants did not want to be interrupted by EMA, e.g. because of school or university duties. Thus, the representativeness of assessments of the daily life among the adolescents and young adults is limited. Additionally, it is worth noting that a qualified simplification of the computed multilevel model can alter results. However, the present study benefits from a detailed examination, investigating inertia not only regarding negative affect, but also exploring inertia of specific affects, i.e. depressive and anxious affect. A further advantage of our study is the large general population sample of adolescents in comparison to other studies investigating genetic effects applying EMA designs (Gunthert et al. 2007; van Roekel et al. 2018) as well as the study is close to a one-to-one replication.

To summarize, the present study could not replicate the previous finding of the 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 S allele being associated with inertia of negative affect (van Roekel et al. 2018). In addition, post hoc analysis revealed no significant relationship between 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 genotype and inertia of depressive and anxious affect. Thus, further research is needed to clarify the association of 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 and inertia regarding negative affect by using first, validated affect scales and second, considering environmental and contextual factors like stress).

Acknowledgements

The Behavior and Mind Health (BeMIND) study is part of the research program “The epidemiology of functional and dysfunctional behavioral and psychological factors in health and disease (EBP)” funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) project no. 01ER1303 and 01ER1703.

Appendix

See Table 4

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

We have no known conflict of interest to disclose.

Footnotes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

T. M. Ollmann and E. Seidl should be considered joint first author and K. Beesdo-Baum and K. Domschke should be considered joint senior authors.

Change history

3/22/2022

A Correction to this paper has been published: 10.1007/s00702-022-02489-2

Contributor Information

T. M. Ollmann, Email: theresa_magdalena.ollmann@tu-dresden.de

E. Seidl, Email: esther.seidl@tu-dresden.de

J. Venz, Email: john.venz@tu-dresden.de

L. Pieper, Email: lars.pieper@tu-dresden.de

C. Voss, Email: catharina.voss@tu-dresden.de

H. Kische, Email: hanna.kische@tu-dresden.de

S. R. Poppenhäger, Email: sophie.poppenhaeger@tu-dresden.de

M. A. Schiele, Email: miriam.schiele@uniklinik-freiburg.de

K. Domschke, Email: katharina.domschke@uniklinik-freiburg.de

K. Beesdo-Baum, Email: katja.beesdo-baum@tu-dresden.de

References

  1. AAPOR . Standard definitions: final dispositions of case codes and outcome rates for surveys. 9. Alexandria: The American Association for Public Opinion Research; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baffa A, Hohoff C, Baune BT, Müller-Tidow C, Tidow N, Freitag C, Zwanzger P, Deckert J, Arolt V, Domschke K. Norepinephrine and serotonin transporter genes: impact on treatment response in depression. Neuropsychobiology. 2010;62(2):121–131. doi: 10.1159/000317285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baune BT, Hohoff C, Mortensen LS, Deckert J, Arolt V, Domschke K. Serotonin transporter polymorhism (5-HTTLPR) association with melancholic depression: a female specific effect? Depress Anxiety. 2008;25(11):920–925. doi: 10.1002/da.20433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beesdo-Baum K, Voss C, Venz J, Hoyer J, Berwanger J, Kische H, Ollmann TM, Pieper L. The behavior and mind health (BeMIND) study: methods, design and baseline sample characteristics of a cohort study among adolescents and young adults. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2020;29(1):e1804. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Border R, Johnson EC, Evans LM, Smolen A, Berley N, Sullivan PF, Keller MC. No support for historical candidate gene or candidate gene-by-interaction hypotheses for major depression across multiple large samples. Am J Psychiatry. 2019;176(5):376–387. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18070881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Dejonckheere E, Mestdagh M, Houben M, Rutten I, Sels L, Kuppens P, Tuerlinckx F. Complex affect dynamics add limited information to the prediction of psychological well-being. Nat Hum Behav. 2019;3(5):478. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0555-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Enders CK, Tofighi D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. Psychol Methods. 2007;12(2):121. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Frijda NH, Kuipers P, Ter Schure E. Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;57(2):212. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.212. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gonda X, Bagdy G. Relationship between serotonin transporter gene 5HTTLPR polymorphism and the symptoms of neuroticism in a healthy population. Psychiatr Hungar Magyar Pszichiatriai Tarsasag Tudomanyos Folyoirata. 2006;21(5):379–385. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gottschalk MG, Domschke K. Novel developments in genetic and epigenetic mechanisms of anxiety. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2016;29(1):32–38. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gunthert KC, Conner TS, Armeli S, Tennen H, Covault J, Kranzler HR. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety reactivity in daily life: a daily process approach to gene-environment interaction. Psychosom Med. 2007;69(8):762–768. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318157ad42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Houben M, Van Den Noortgate W, Kuppens P. The relation between short-term emotion dynamics and psychological well-being: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2015;141(4):901. doi: 10.1037/a0038822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Kemper CJ, Beierlein C, Kovaleva A, Rammstedt B (2012). Eine Kurzskala zur Messung von Optimismus-Pessimismus: Die Skala Optimismus-Pessimismus-2 (SOP2)
  14. Klauke B, Deckert J, Reif A, Pauli P, Zwanzger P, Baumann C, Arolt V, Glöckner-Rist A, Domschke K. Serotonin transporter gene and childhood trauma—a G× E effect on anxiety sensitivity. Depress Anxiety. 2011;28(12):1048–1057. doi: 10.1002/da.20840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Koval P, Kuppens P. Changing emotion dynamics: Individual differences in the effect of anticipatory social stress on emotional inertia. Emotion. 2012;12(2):256–267. doi: 10.1037/a0024756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Koval P, Kuppens P, Allen NB, Sheeber L. Getting stuck in depression: the roles of rumination and emotional inertia. Cogn Emot. 2012;26(8):1412–1427. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.667392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kuppens P, Allen NB, Sheeber LB. Emotional inertia and psychological maladjustment. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(7):984–991. doi: 10.1177/0956797610372634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kuppens P, Sheeber LB, Yap MB, Whittle S, Simmons JG, Allen NB. Emotional inertia prospectively predicts the onset of depressive disorder in adolescence. Emotion. 2012;12(2):283. doi: 10.1037/a0025046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Lamers F, Swendsen J, Cui L, Husky M, Johns J, Zipunnikov V, Merikangas KR. Mood reactivity and affective dynamics in mood and anxiety disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 2018;127(7):659. doi: 10.1037/abn0000378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, Benjamin J, Müller CR, Hamer DH, Murphy DL. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274(5292):1527–1531. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Licht CL, Mortensen EL, Hjordt LV, Stenbæk DS, Arentzen TE, Nørremølle A, Knudsen GM. Serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) variation and sensory processing sensitivity—comparison with other anxiety-related temperamental dimensions. Mol Genet Genom Med. 2020;8(8):e1352. doi: 10.1002/mgg3.1352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Moors A. On the causal role of appraisal in emotion. Emot Rev. 2013;5(2):132–140. doi: 10.1177/1754073912463601. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Osher Y, Hamer D, Benjamin J. Association and linkage of anxiety-related traits with a functional polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region in Israeli sibling pairs. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5(2):216–219. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Pilkonis PA, Choi SW, Reise SP, Stover AM, Riley WT, Cella D, Group PC. Item banks for measuring emotional distress from the patient-reported outcomes measurement information system (PROMIS®): depression, anxiety, and anger. Assessment. 2011;18(3):263–283. doi: 10.1177/1073191111411667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Rao JN, Scott AJ. On chi-squared tests for multiway contingency tables with cell proportions estimated from survey data. Ann Stat. 1984;12:46–60. doi: 10.1214/aos/1176346391. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Russell JA. Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol Rev. 2003;110(1):145. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Schiele MA, Ziegler C, Holitschke K, Schartner C, Schmidt B, Weber H, Reif A, Romanos M, Pauli P, Zwanzger P, Deckert J. Influence of 5-HTT variation, childhood trauma and self-efficacy on anxiety traits: a gene-environment-coping interaction study. J Neural Trans. 2016;123(8):895–904. doi: 10.1007/s00702-016-1564-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Schiele MA, Herzog K, Kollert L, Böhnlein J, Repple J, Rosenkranz K, Leehr EJ, Ziegler C, Lueken U, Dannlowski U, Pauli P. Affective temperaments (TEMPS-A) in panic disorder and healthy probands: genetic modulation by 5-HTT variation. World J Biol Psychiatr. 2020;21(10):790–796. doi: 10.1080/15622975.2019.1705999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Schinka J, Busch R, Robichaux-Keene N. A meta-analysis of the association between the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and trait anxiety. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9(2):197–202. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Trull TJ, Lane SP, Koval P, Ebner-Priemer UW. Affective dynamics in psychopathology. Emot Rev. 2015;7(4):355–361. doi: 10.1177/1754073915590617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. van Roekel E, Verhagen M, Engels R, Kuppens P. Variation in the serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and inertia of negative and positive emotions in daily life. Emotion. 2018;18(2):229. doi: 10.1037/emo0000336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Wendland JR, Martin BJ, Kruse MR, Lesch KP, Murphy DL. Simultaneous genotyping of four functional loci of human SLC6A4, with a reappraisal of 5-HTTLPR and rs25531. Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11(3):224–226. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Wichers M, Wigman J, Myin-Germeys I. Micro-level affect dynamics in psychopathology viewed from complex dynamical system theory. Emot Rev. 2015;7(4):362–367. doi: 10.1177/1754073915590623. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Wilhelm P, Schoebi D. Assessing mood in daily life. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2007;23(4):258–267. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.23.4.258. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Zheng Y, Asbury K. Genetic and environmental influences on adolescent emotional inertia in daily life. J Youth Adolesc. 2019;48(9):1849–1860. doi: 10.1007/s10964-019-01075-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Neural Transmission are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES