Skip to main content
BJPsych Open logoLink to BJPsych Open
. 2022 Mar 15;8(2):e70. doi: 10.1192/bjo.2022.36

Normal amygdala morphology in dissociative identity disorder

Antje A T S Reinders 1,✉,*, Lora I Dimitrova 2,*, Yolanda R Schlumpf 3, Eline M Vissia 4, Ellert R S Nijenhuis 5, Lutz Jäncke 6, Sima Chalavi 7, Dick J Veltman 8
PMCID: PMC8935907  PMID: 35287776

Summary

Studies investigating the structure of the amygdala in relation to dissociation in psychiatric disorders are limited and have reported normal or preserved, increased or decreased global volumes. Thus, a more detailed investigation of the amygdala is warranted. Amygdala global and subregional volumes were compared between individuals with dissociative identity disorder (DID: n = 32) and healthy controls (n = 42). Analyses of covariance did not show volumetric differences between the DID and control groups. Although several unknowns make it challenging to interpret our findings, we propose that the finding of normal amygdala volume is a genuine finding because other studies using this data-set have presented robust morphological aberrations in relation to the diagnosis of DID.

Keywords: Subregions, global volume, DID, dissociation, FreeSurfer


The hippocampus and amygdala were the first neurostructural regions to be studied in dissociative disorders, including dissociative identity disorder (DID). A recent systematic review1 proposed decreased hippocampal volumes as a neurostructural biomarker for dissociative amnesia in DID. A later study2 confirmed this proposal and specified that findings of smaller bilateral global hippocampus are likely to be driven by decreases in subregions of the hippocampus, namely the bilateral CA1, right CA4, right granule cell molecular layer of the dentate gyrus and left pre-subiculum. The study further proposed decreased bilateral CA1 subfield volumes as a biomarker for dissociative amnesia in DID.

Studies that investigated the structure of the amygdala in DID and other disorders that involve dissociation are more limited and less consistent1. Grey matter volumes of the amygdala in relation to dissociation have been found to be normal or preserved,3,4 increased or decreased.1 Findings that global amygdala volume is normal in DID could be explained by low numbers of participants in the studies, preventing results from reaching statistical significance, or by adding a mixture of increased and decreased subfield volumes to a net finding of normal global amygdala volumes. The latter possibility is supported by a recent study in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), a disorder that is closely related to DID,5 that found a mixture of increased and decreased amygdala subregional volumes.6 Studying the amygdala in dissociation is important because the amygdala has been assigned a pivotal role in neurofunctional biological models for dissociation in which it is hypothesised that dissociation involves emotional overmodulation of the amygdala by midline prefrontal regions.7,8

In the current study we investigated amygdala volumes in individuals with DID and addressed two aims. Our first aim was to explore whether our previous finding of normal amygdala volume in this disorder3 might be due to low statistical power. To this end, we doubled the sample size. The second aim was to study both global and subfield amygdala volumes to investigate whether a mixture of increased and decreased subfield volumes caused a net result of normal global volumes.

Method

Participants

Data from a total of 75 women (only female participants with DID volunteered) were collected. There were 32 female volunteers with DID and 43 healthy controls matched for age, gender, years of education and ethnicity. Data were collected in The Netherlands at the University Medical Centre in Groningen (UMCG) and the Amsterdam Medical Centre (AMC) and in Switzerland at the University Hospital in Zurich (UHZ).2,9,10 All participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and as dictated by ethical requirements of the Medical Ethical Committees of UMCG (reference number: METC2008.211) and AMC (reference number: MEC09/155) and by the cantonal ethical commission of Zurich (Kantonale Ethikkommission Zürich; reference number: E-13/2008). All participants were given the right to withdraw and were fully debriefed in line with the ethical requirements of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Participants and data included in the current study are identical to those in the investigations of the hippocampus as a neurostructural biomarker of dissociation2 and whole-brain morphological studies.9,10 In sum: participants with DID were diagnosed by trained clinicians using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Dissociative Disorders (SCID-D) and all had a comorbid diagnosis of PTSD or of PTSD in remission and other comorbidity as confirmed by participants and their personal therapists.9,10 The control group was recruited through local newspaper advertisements. Exclusion criteria for all participants included age outside the range of 18–65 years, pregnancy, systemic or neurological illness, claustrophobia, metal implants in the body and substance misuse. Additional exclusion criteria for the control group included the presence of dissociative symptoms and a history of trauma, past or current psychiatric disorders and medication use. Participants in the control group were required to have no or limited (somatoform) dissociative symptoms and potentially traumatising experiences.9,10

Data acquisition

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data were collected using 3 T Philips whole-body scanners (Philips Medical Systems, Best, Netherlands) from centres in The Netherlands (AMC and UMCG) and Switzerland (UHZ). An optimised T1-weighted anatomical MRI protocol for the three participating centres was used:11 three-dimensional magnetisation-prepared rapid gradient-echo imaging (3-D MP-RAGE), repetition time TR = 9.95 ms, echo time TE = 5.6 ms, flip angle 8°, voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm3, number of slices 160, total scan time 10 min 14 s. Ratios of DID to control participants were approximately equal across the centres and the number of participants per group did not differ across centres (χ2 = 1.01, P = 0.603).

Volumetric analysis

MRI data were processed using FreeSurfer version 7.0 for MacOS (surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu). This version allows the extraction of both global and subregions of the amygdala. Following full surface reconstruction and volumetric segmentation, volumetric measures for the whole amygdala, the lateral nucleus, basal nucleus, accessory basal nucleus, anterior amygdaloid area, central nucleus, medial nucleus, cortical nucleus, corticoamygdaloid transition and paralaminar nucleus for each hemisphere were extracted. Further, the total intercranial volume (TIV) was calculated. Full details on the methodology are published elsewhere.12 For one participant from the control group, FreeSurfer was not able to complete the amygdala segmentation. Therefore, this participant was excluded from subsequent statistical analyses.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were performed using SPSS version 26 (www.ibm.com/uk-en/products/spss-statistics). Between-group differences in amygdala volumes for each hemisphere were tested with analyses of covariance (ANCOVA). Amygdala volumes acted as the dependent variable, group and centre as fixed categorical effects, and age and estimated TIV as continuous covariates. Group differences were investigated by comparing the estimated marginal means of the main effects with Bonferroni post hoc correction across all subregions and global volumes.

Results

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics and the findings of the between-group analyses (ANCOVA) on amygdala global volumes and volumes of amygdala subregions. We did not find any significant differences between the DID and control groups for either the global amygdala volumes or for the volumes of amygdala subregions. There was only one trend showing decreased volume for the DID group, and that was in the left corticoamygdaloid transition area (F(1,66) = 3.839, P = 0.054, ηp2 = 0.55), with a mean decrease of 9.090 mm3.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics and analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) between participants with dissociative identity disorder (DID) and healthy controls on amygdala volume

Mean volume, mm3 (s.d.) Between-group ANCOVA
DID group (n = 32) Control group (n = 42) F (d.f.) P-value ηp2 Mean difference, mm3 95% CI
Global amygdala
Left 1657.23 (151.45) 1706.07 (175.31) 1.007 (1.66) 0.319 0.015 39.480 −39.075 to 118.035
Right 1700.45 (146.38) 1756.31 (166.70) 1.499 (1.73) 0.225 0.022 45.701 −28.820 to 120.222
Lateral nucleus
Left 618.43 (58.89) 629.21 (62.46) 0.288 (1.66) 0.593 0.004 7.657 −20.827 to 36.140
Right 620.23 (45.38) 635.91 (58.75) 1.506 (1.66) 0.224 0.022 15.374 −9.639 to 40.386
Basal nucleus
Left 418.50 (45.49) 431.21 (45.05) 1.034 (1.66) 0.313 0.015 11.034 −10.630 to 32.698
Right 423.26 (40.89) 438.64 (43.54) 1.236 (1.66) 0.270 0.018 11.047 −8.790 to 30.883
Accessory basal nucleus
Left 254.66 (25.74) 264.97 (33.80) 1.274 (1.66) 0.263 0.019 8.394 −6.451 to 23.240
Right 266.77 (29.35) 278.52 (31.85) 1.408 (1.73) 0.240 0.021 8.609 −5.877 to 23.096
Anterior amygdaloid area
Left 52.48 (7.10) 53.32 (7.49) 0.089 (1.66) 0.766 0.001 0.530 −3.017 to 4.077
Right 57.56 (8.87) 58.86 (5.87) 1.082 (1.66) 0.302 0.016 1.849 −1.699 to 5.396
Central nucleus
Left 46.81 (6.40) 46.42 (8.87) 0.121 (1.66) 0.729 0.002 −0.684 −4.610 to 3.243
Right 51.30 (7.86) 51.47 (8.04) 0.024 (1.66) 0.878 0.0 −0.298 −4.148 to 3.553
Medial nucleus
Left 24.81 (5.76) 26.05 (8.25) 0.495 (1.73) 0.484 0.007 1.272 −2.336 to 4.880
Right 28.49 (6.82) 28.97 (8.37) 0.201 (1.66) 0.656 0.003 0.854 −2.950 to 4.658
Cortical nucleus
Left 26.37 (3.93) 27.58 (5.76) 0.620 (1.66) 0.434 0.009 0.979 −1.504 to 3.463
Right 29.37 (3.97) 30.23 (4.80) 0.233 (1.66) 0.631 0.004 0.524 −1.643 to 2.691
Corticoamygdaloid transition
Left 169.18 (16.92) 180.10 (21.38) 3.839 (1.66) 0.054a 0.055 9.090 −0.172 to 18.352
Right 177.85 (20.76) 187.24 (23.26) 1.905 (1.66) 0.172 0.028 7.277 −3.248 to 17.803
Paralaminar nucleus
Left 46.00 (5.24) 47.18 (4.88) 1.037 (1.66) 0.312 0.015 1.208 −1.160 to 3.576
Right 45.61 (4.52) 46.46 (4.51) 0.193 (1.66) 0.662 0.003 0.465 −1.648 to 2.578

ηp2, partial eta squared.

a.

0.05 < P ≤ 0.1.

Discussion

The current study confirms our previous finding of normal amygdala volumes in DID.3,4

Although the hippocampus is sensitive to excessive stress hormones, which may explain its decreased volumes in DID,2 the structure of the amygdala might be less sensitive to stress hormones than previously thought.13 Several unknowns add to the difficulty in interpreting our findings. They include the potential influence of different kinds of stress (e.g. attachment loss, physical abuse and emotional neglect), the sensitivity of the structure of the amygdala to the frequency and intensity of its activation and to ontogenetic developmental phases, and lifetime prefrontal inhibition of amygdala activation.8,14 The last, which is a potentially neuroprotective effect, might be more pronounced in individuals with DID, who predominantly function as one or more dissociative identities that successfully avoid emotional cues, which might relate to frequent prefrontal inhibition of amygdala activity. These unknowns all open pathways for future research.

The trend for decreased volume in the corticoamygdaloid transition area in our study might be due to scanner differences between the three centres as in the study by Morey and colleagues,6 they found that the covariates age and scanner were significant for the corticoamygdaloid transition area. Although we were careful to use identical scanner sequences at all three centres and included centre as a covariate, residual variance related to scanner differences in the corticoamygdaloid transition area cannot be excluded and could contribute to our finding of a trend. Age was the second covariate found in the study by Morey and colleagues to be significantly associated with amygdala volume. The effect of age on amygdala volumes in a sample of individuals with DID has been independently discussed15 for reported decreased amygdala volume.16 However, in the current study age is not a contaminating factor in the finding of normal amygdala volumes because the DID and control group were carefully matched (t(72) = −0.55, P = 0.581).2

This short report is part of a sequence of brain imaging papers that originated from a multicentre collaboration between two centres in The Netherlands and one in Switzerland. We found that structural imaging can aid a diagnosis of DID,10 that there is no evidence for DID to be a neurodevelopmental disorder9 and that hippocampal subregion CA1 can be proposed as a biomarker for dissociative amnesia.2 The findings in these studies were all statistically significant, indicating that this data-set contains robust morphological aberration in relation to the diagnosis of DID and that normal amygdala volumes are a genuine finding. Therefore, we conclude that our previously reported normal amygdala volumes in DID3 are upheld under increased statistical power and after investigating the independent contributions of subregions of the amygdala to its global volume.

Acknowledgements

We thank all the participants and their therapists. We thank Drs Nel Draijer, Mechteld Giesen, Ekaterina Weder and Eva Zimmermann for arranging participant inclusion and scanning as well as for the assessment of diagnostic interviews and their support as research clinicians.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author contributions

All authors have approved the final version for publication and are accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved. A.A.T.S.R.: conceptualisation, data acquisition, funding acquisition, interpretation, methodology, project administration, resources, supervision, visualisation, writing – review and editing. L.I.D.: formal analysis, methodology, interpretation, software, visualisation, writing original draft and review and editing. Y.R.S. and S.C.: data acquisition, interpretation, methodology, project administration, software, writing – review and editing. E.M.V.: conceptualisation, data acquisition, formal analysis, interpretation, methodology, project administration, writing original draft and review and editing. E.R.S.N. and L.J.: conceptualisation, data acquisition, interpretation, methodology, writing – review and editing. D.J.V.: conceptualisation, funding acquisition, formal analysis, interpretation, supervision, writing – review and editing.

Funding

This article represents independent research part-funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the National Health Service, the NIHR or the Department of Health. A.A.T.S.R. was supported by The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (www.nwo.nl), NWO-VENI grant no. 451-07-009. S.C. was supported by a David Caul graduate research grant from the International Society for the Study of Trauma and Dissociation (ISSTD) (www.isst-d.org/about/awards.htm). Y.R.S. was supported by the Forschungskredit UZH (www.researchers.uzh.ch/de/funding/phd/fkcandoc.html).

Declaration of interest

D.J.V. and A.A.T.S.R. are members of the BJPsych Open editorial board and did not take part in the review or decision-making process of this paper.

References

  • 1.Roydeva MI, Reinders AATS. Biomarkers of pathological dissociation: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 123: 120–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Dimitrova LI, Dean SL, Schlumpf YR, Vissia EM, Nijenhuis ERS, Chatzi V, et al. A neurostructural biomarker of dissociative amnesia: a hippocampal study in dissociative identity disorder. Psychol Med [Epub ahead of print] 24 Jun 2021. Available from: 10.1017/S0033291721002154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Chalavi S, Vissia EM, Giesen ME, Nijenhuis ERS, Draijer N, Barker GJ, et al. Similar cortical but not subcortical gray matter abnormalities in women with posttraumatic stress disorder with versus without dissociative identity disorder. Psychiatry Res Neuroimag 2015; 231: 308–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Nicholson TR, Aybek S, Kempton MJ, Daly EM, Murphy DG, David AS, et al. A structural MRI study of motor conversion disorder: evidence of reduction in thalamic volume. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2014; 85: 227–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Spiegel D, Lewis-Fernández R, Lanius R, Vermetten E, Simeon D, Friedman M. Dissociative disorders in DSM-5. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2013; 9: 299–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Morey RA, Clarke EK, Haswell CC, Phillips RD, Clausen AN, Mufford MS, et al. Amygdala nuclei volume and shape in military veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimag 2020; 5: 281–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lanius RA, Vermetten E, Loewenstein RJ, Brand B, Christian S, Bremner JD, et al. Emotion modulation in PTSD: clinical and neurobiological evidence for a dissociative subtype. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167: 640–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Reinders AATS, Willemsen ATM, den Boer JA, Vos HPJ, Veltman DJ, Loewenstein RJ. Opposite brain emotion-regulation patterns in identity states of dissociative identity disorder: a PET study and neurobiological model. Psychiatry Res 2014; 223: 236–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Reinders AATS, Chalavi S, Schlumpf YR, Vissia EM, Nijenhuis ERS, Jäncke L, et al. Neurodevelopmental origins of abnormal cortical morphology in dissociative identity disorder. Acta Psychiatri Scand 2018; 137: 157–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Reinders AATS, Marquand AF, Schlumpf YR, Chalavi S, Vissia EM, Nijenhuis ERS, et al. Aiding the diagnosis of dissociative identity disorder: pattern recognition study of brain biomarkers. Br J Psychiatry 2019; 215: 536–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Chalavi S, Simmons A, Dijkstra H, Barker GJ, Reinders AATS. Quantitative and qualitative assessment of structural magnetic resonance imaging data in a two-center study. BMC Med Imaging 2012; 12(1): 27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fischl B. FreeSurfer. NeuroImage 2012; 62: 774–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Barry TJ, Murray L, Fearon P, Moutsiana C, Johnstone T, Halligan SL. Amygdala volume and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis reactivity to social stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017; 85: 96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Nijenhuis E. Toward an ecology of dissociation in the context of trauma: Implications for the psychobiological study of dissociative disorders. In Dissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: Past, Present, Future (eds Dorahy MJ, Gold SN, O'Neil J). Routledge, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Spiegel D. Reduced hippocampal and amygdalar volume in dissociative identity disorder: not such clear evidence. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163: 2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Vermetten E, Schmahl C, Lindner S, Loewenstein RJ, Bremner JD. Hippocampal and amygdalar volumes in dissociative identity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163: 630–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from BJPsych Open are provided here courtesy of Royal College of Psychiatrists

RESOURCES